Perception – Gain Control Task Name Description Steady state visual evoked potentials to magnocellular vs. parvocellular biased stimuli. Magnocellular and parvocellular visual pathway function assessed using steady-state visual evoked potentials. This task that takes ~ 15 min. The magnocellular system responds to low luminance contrast and the parvocellular system does not begin to respond until contrast has reached about 16%. In this task, isolated checks are modulated at low contrasts to bias responding towards the magnocellular visual pathway and are modulated around a high contrast "pedestal" to bias responding towards the parvocellular pathway. Signal-to-noise ratio, which is the amplitude of the response corrected for noise, is calculated for each contrast and is the dependent variable. (Butler et al., 2001) (Butler et al., 2005) (Zemon & Gordon, 2006) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Butler, P. D., Zemon, V., Schechter, I., Saperstein, A. M., Hoptman, M. J., Lim, K. O., et al. (2005). Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 62(5), 495-504. Cognitive Construct Validity The magnocellular-biased condition produces a steeply rising increase in response to low contrast stimuli which reaches a saturation-level once luminance contrast reaches ~16% (Butler et al., 2001; Butler et al., 2005; Zemon & Gordon, 2006). This leads to a characteristic Sshaped, non-linear contrast gain control curve. The initial steeply rising part of the curve reflects substantial amplification of low contrast stimuli, permitting M-pathway neurons to respond robustly even at low contrasts. This magnocellular-pathway response is an example of gain control because response levels are optimized within a limited dynamic signaling range and the sensory signal is amplified. (Butler et al., 2001) (Butler et al., 2005) (Zemon & Gordon, 2006) Neural Construct Validity Reliability The contrast response curves obtained in human studies in healthy controls (Butler et al., 2001; Butler et al., 2005; Zemon & Gordon, 2006) (Fox, Sato, & Daw, 1990) to magnocellular- and parvocellular-biased stimuli are very similar to what is seen in single-cell recordings in monkeys supporting the concept that magnocellular and parvocellular responses are being examined. In addition, visual pathways within the brain use glutamate as their primary neurotransmitter and NMDA appears to have a central role in gain control. For instance, NMDA receptors amplify responses to isolated stimuli as well as amplifying the effects of lateral inhibition (e.g., increase surround antagonism of center receptive field responses) (Daw, Stein, & Fox, 1993) (Kwon, Nelson, Toth, & Sur, 1992). Thus, an NMDA deficit would result in decreased amplification and less lateral inhibition. Microinufsion of NMDA antagonists into cat lateral geniculate nucleus or primary visual cortex produced shallower gain at low contrast and a much lower plateau indicating decreased signal amplification in electrophysiological studies There has been some work on test retest reliability (Butler et al., unpublished observations). Twenty participants (10 controls and 10 schizophrenia patients) have been tested twice. The correlation between signal-to-noise ratios (the dependent measure which is a measure of amplitude corrected for noise) obtained in first and second test sessions was significant (total group: r=0.81, p<0.0001; patients alone: r=0.72, n=14, p=0.004; controls alone: r=0.88, n=6, p=0.02). Psychometric Characteristics The task does not appear to have practice effects. It is a passive viewing task in which electrophysiological responses are obtained. The responses do not habituate even following repeated presentation of the stimulus conditions. It is not a learned task, so that presentation at multiple test dates is not affected by prior testing. There do not appear to be floor effects because most people, including patients, give at least a small response at low contrast and a slightly higher response as contrast increases. There is not a ceiling effect because the amplitude of the electrophysiological response does not have a ceiling. Animal Model Stage of Research Single cell recordings from magnocellular and parvocellular neurons in monkey lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) show characteristic electrophysiological responses. Magnocellular neurons show steep increases in responding at low contrast and saturation at higher contrast. Parvocellular neurons show a shallow slope at low contrast and a nonsaturating response. The curves found in the proposed task are very similar to those seen in monkey work. Studies in cat LGN show decreased slope and decreased plateau following infusion of NMDA antagonists (Daw et al., 1993) (Kwon et al., 1992) similar to what is seen in schizophrenia patients in this task (Butler et al 2005). There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention. (Fox et al., 1990). Thus, the magnocellular-biased task may be assessing NMDAmediated signal amplification. Indeed, schizophrenia patients show curves very similar to those seen following infusion of NMDA antagonists in animal studies. Zemon, V., & Gordon, J. (2006). Luminance-contrast mechanisms in humans: visual evoked potentials and a nonlinear model. Vision Res, 46(24), 4163-4180. ContrastContrast Effect (CCE) Task Gain control has been successfully studied using the Contrast-Contrast Effect (CCE) task in which contrast sensitivity for a ringed target can be influenced by the contrast of a circular surround (Chubb, Sperling, & Solomon, 1989). In this task, participants are asked to match a variable contrast patch to a central patch. When the surround is highcontrast, the inner target is perceived to be of lower contrast than when the same target is perceived without a surround (Chubb et al., 1989; Dakin, Carlin, & Hemsley, 2005). MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Dakin, S., Carlin, P., & Hemsley, D. (2005). Weak suppression of visual context in chronic schizophrenia. Curr Biol, 15(20), R822-824. Zenger-Landolt, B., & Heeger, D. J. (2003). This task is ideal for examining gain control in schizophrenia because: 1) reduced gain control, or contextual modulation, would be indicated by more accurate contrast judgments regarding the inner circle compared to controls; and 2) there is already evidence for reduced spatial context effects in vision in schizophrenia (Must, Janka, Benedek, & Keri, 2004; Uhlhaas et al., 2006). An important feature of this task is that full psychometric functions can be obtained for subjects. From these, separate indicators of precision (the minimum size of contrast differences that are detectable, which is indicated by the slope of the function) and bias (reflecting the amount of offset that is needed between the target and the surround to produce a perceptual match) can be obtained, allowing us to examine discrimination accuracy independent of (Daw et al., 1993) (Fox et al., 1990) (Kwon et al., 1992) Converging evidence from psychophysics and fMRI indicates that the contrastcontrast effect is linked to gain control within primary visual cortex (V1) (ZengerLandolt & Heeger, 2003). Further evidence indicates that the effect within V1 is likely due to both activity arising within V1 and to topdown feedback from higher, object-processing areas to V1 (Lotto & Purves, 2001). Because 90% of cells in V1 are subject to suppression from neighboring cells, tasks such as this that are known to act on V1 neurons are ideal methods for the study of gain control. (Zenger-Landolt & Heeger, 2003) (Lotto & Purves, 2001) This has not been assessed. An important feature of this task is that full psychometric functions can be obtained for subjects. From these, separate indicators of precision (the minimum size of contrast differences that are detectable, which is indicated by the slope of the function) and bias (reflecting the amount of offset that is needed between the target and the surround to produce a perceptual match) can be obtained, allowing us to examine discrimination accuracy independent of response bias (as with other signaldetection analyses). This suggests the absence of floor/ceiling effects Not Known There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention. Mismatch Negativity Response suppression in v1 agrees with psychophysics of surround masking. J Neurosci, 23(17), 6884-6893. response bias (as with other signal-detection analyses). Mismatch negativity (MMN) is an auditory event-related potential (ERP) elicited in an "oddball" task, in which a sequence of repetitive standard tones is interrupted by a physically different "deviant" tone that violates expectancies created by the standard. MMN can be recorded and quantified using standard ERP recording systems (e.g. Neuroscan, Biosemi, ANT). (Umbricht & Krljes, 2005) MMN indexes perception of stimulus deviance at the level of auditory cortex. Generation of MMN depends upon gain control (i.e. signal amplification) of neurons sensitive to stimulus deviance. Presentation of repetitive standards should, under normal conditions, lead to upward bias of the gain process. In schizophrenia, this bias mechanism appears to be impaired. MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Javitt, D. C., Spencer, K. M., Thaker, G. K., Winterer, G., & Hajos, M. (2008). Neurophysiological biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Drug Discov, 7(1), 68-83. Pakarinen, S., Takegata, R., Rinne, T., Huotilainen, M., & Naatanen, R. (2007). Measurement of extensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP). Clin Neurophysiol, 118(1), 177185. (Uhlhaas et al., 2006) (Must et al., 2004) At the neural level, MMN reflects current flow through open, unblocked NMDA receptors within auditory cortex. Similar mechanisms mediate gain control within visual cortex, suggesting a parallel phenomenon. MMN generation can be antagonized by administration of NMDA agonists such as PCP or ketamine in either human or animal models (Javitt, 2000; Javitt, Steinschneider, Schroeder, & Arezzo, 1996; Turetsky et al., 2007; Umbricht et al., 2000). MMN tracks auditory perceptual performance across a variety of dimensions (Pakarinen, Takegata, Rinne, Huotilainen, & Naatanen, 2007). Test-retest reliability of mismatch negativity for duration, frequency and intensity changes (Hall et al., 2006; Tervaniemi et al., 1999). Hall (average of 18 days): 0.34-0.66 ICCs Tervaniemi (average of 8.3 days): Correlations between 0.41 and 0.78. MMN is generated preattentively in the absence of a behavioral task. Floor effects depend upon quality of EEG recording and can be minimized through good electrophysiological technique. Testretest studies show no significant "learning" (i.e. repetition effects see references on test/retest reliability). As MMN amplitude increases with dF and standards/deviant ratio, ceiling effect is not relevant (Javitt, Spencer, Thaker, Winterer, & Hajos, 2008). Homologues exist in monkeys (Javitt et al., 1996) (definitely) and rodents (probably)(Ehrlichman, Maxwell, Majumdar, & Siegel, 2008; Umbricht, Vyssotki, Latanov, Nitsch, & Lipp, 2005). There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. Data already exists on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. There is evidence that performance on this task can improve in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. Prepulse Inhibition of Startle Individuals are presented with two auditory probes in sequences. The P50 response to the second probe is typically reduced in individuals with intact sensory gating. It is not clear whether this correlates with other measures of inhibition. (Swerdlow, Braff, Taaid, & Geyer, 1994) (Braff & Geyer, 1990) (Cadenhead, Carasso, Swerdlow, Geyer, & Braff, 1999) There is a large literature on the neural substrates of Prepulse inhibition in animals, and a large literature on pharmacological effects. There are now human imaging studies (at least one) looking at prepulse inhibition in the scanner. YES We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. (Joober, Zarate, Rouleau, Skamene, & Boksa, 2002) (Kumari, Antonova, & Geyer, 2008) (Campbell et al., 2007) (Kumari et al., 2007) (Kumari et al., 2003) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia (Swerdlow et al., 2006). This task has been studies with psychopharamcology methods Swerdlow, N. R., Light, G. A., Cadenhead, K. S., Sprock, J., Hsieh, M. H., & Braff, D. L. (2006). Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 63(12), 1325-1335. Turetsky, B. I., Calkins, M. E., Light, G. A., Olincy, A., Radant, A. D., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2007). Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr Bull, 33(1), 69-94. Contrast Sensitivity This is a classic psychophysical task that has Contrast is an example of gain control, which takes into Gain control in the visual system refers to processes I am not aware of any published data on The task does not have a ceiling effect, There have been numerous studies There is evidence that this specific task been used for over 50 years (Bodis-Wollner, 1972; Movshon & Kiorpes, 1988; Robson, 1966). There are different variations on the method. One method is use of a two-alternative forced choice design. In this design, contrast sensitivity functions can be obtained by presenting sine-wave gratings at several different spatial frequencies from low to high (e.g., 0.5 to 21 cycles/degree). Gratings are presented on one half (either the right or left side) of a visual display, with the other side having a uniform field. Participants are asked to state which side of the display contains the grating. Contrast is varied across trials using an up-and-down transformed response method to determine contrast sensitivity (e.g., the contrast at which the side the grating is on is correctly detected 79% of the time). MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Butler, P. D., Zemon, V., Schechter, I., Saperstein, A. M., Hoptman, M. J., Lim, K. O., et al. (2005). Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 62(5), 495-504. Keri, S., Antal, A., Szekeres, G., Benedek, G., & Janka, Z. (2002). Spatiotemporal visual account spatial and temporal context. Contrast sensitivity varies with spatial context (as well as with temporal context (Robson, 1966). In addition, gain control assists sensory subsystems in increasing contrast between adjacent and successive stimuli. that generally occur at early stages of processing such as at the level of the LGN or primary visual cortex and utilizes excitatory and inhibitory lateral interactions between neurons. Contrast sensitivity can be assessed and contrast sensitivity curves obtained from recordings of LGN neurons as well as from neurons in primary visual cortex (Kiorpes, Tang, Hawken, & Movshon, 2003). Contrast sensitivity curves produce an inverted U shaped function with a peak in the mid-range of spatial frequencies and falloff at lower and higher spatial frequencies. The fall-off at lower spatial frequencies is mainly a function of inhibitory lateral interactions between center and surround whereas fall-off at higher spatial frequencies is more a function of excitatory interactions (Burbeck & Kelly, 1980). test retest reliability. In our laboratory, 29 people have been tested twice and there was a significant correlation between first and second test time for all spatial frequencies (0.5 - 21c/degree) examined (r=0.670.4, p=0.0001-0.02 for the different spatial frequencies). but, in theory, could have a floor effect. Higher contrast sensitivity indicates better performance. Contrast sensitivity is the inverse of threshold so that a high contrast sensitivity of 100 corresponds to a low threshold of 1% contrast. There is no theoretical limit on how high contrast sensitivity can go. A contrast sensitivity of 500, for instance, would correspond to a threshold of 0.2% contrast. On the other hand, poor performance leads to low contrast sensitivity and corresponding high thresholds. For example, a contrast sensitivity of 1 would correspond to a threshold of 100% contrast. It is not possible to go higher than 100% contrast. However, in practice, unless there is a serious visual problem, people do not need 100% contrast to do this task. I am not aware of data on practice effects, though this is not a learned task so that practice effects should be minimal. done in a variety of species including goldfish, cat, and falcon (Movshon & Kiorpes, 1988). Contrast sensitivity can be obtained in macaques using behavioral methods (Movshon & Kiorpes, 1988). elicits deficits in schizophrenia (Butler et al., 2005; Keri, Antal, Szekeres, Benedek, & Janka, 2002; Slaghuis, 1998). We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention. processing in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci, 14(2), 190-196. REFERENCES: Bodis-Wollner, I. (1972). Contrast sensitivity and increment threshold. Perception, 1(1), 73-83. Braff, D. L., & Geyer, M. A. (1990). Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 47(2), 181-188. Burbeck, C. A., & Kelly, D. H. (1980). Spatiotemporal characteristics of visual mechanisms: excitatory-inhibitory model. J Opt Soc Am, 70(9), 1121-1126. Butler, P. D., Schechter, I., Zemon, V., Schwartz, S. G., Greenstein, V. C., Gordon, J., et al. (2001). Dysfunction of early-stage visual processing in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry, 158(7), 1126-1133. Butler, P. D., Zemon, V., Schechter, I., Saperstein, A. M., Hoptman, M. J., Lim, K. O., et al. (2005). Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 62(5), 495-504. Cadenhead, K. S., Carasso, B. S., Swerdlow, N. R., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (1999). Prepulse inhibition and habituation of the startle response are stable neurobiological measures in a normal male population. Biol Psychiatry, 45(3), 360-364. Campbell, L. E., Hughes, M., Budd, T. W., Cooper, G., Fulham, W. R., Karayanidis, F., et al. (2007). Primary and secondary neural networks of auditory prepulse inhibition: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of sensorimotor gating of the human acoustic startle response. Eur J Neurosci, 26(8), 2327-2333. Chubb, C., Sperling, G., & Solomon, J. A. (1989). Texture interactions determine perceived contrast. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 86(23), 9631-9635. Dakin, S., Carlin, P., & Hemsley, D. (2005). Weak suppression of visual context in chronic schizophrenia. Curr Biol, 15(20), R822-824. Daw, N. W., Stein, P. S., & Fox, K. (1993). The role of NMDA receptors in information processing. Annu Rev Neurosci, 16, 207-222. Ehrlichman, R. S., Maxwell, C. R., Majumdar, S., & Siegel, S. J. (2008). Deviance-elicited Changes in Event-related Potentials are Attenuated by Ketamine in Mice. J Cogn Neurosci. Fox, K., Sato, H., & Daw, N. (1990). The effect of varying stimulus intensity on NMDA-receptor activity in cat visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 64, 1413-1428. Hall, M. H., Schulze, K., Rijsdijk, F., Picchioni, M., Ettinger, U., Bramon, E., et al. (2006). Heritability and reliability of P300, P50 and duration mismatch negativity. Behav Genet, 36(6), 845-857. Javitt, D. C. (2000). Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dysfunction in schizophrenia. Audiol Neurootol, 5(3-4), 207-215. Javitt, D. C., Spencer, K. M., Thaker, G. K., Winterer, G., & Hajos, M. (2008). Neurophysiological biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Drug Discov, 7(1), 68-83. Javitt, D. C., Steinschneider, M., Schroeder, C. E., & Arezzo, J. C. (1996). Role of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 93(21), 11962-11967. Joober, R., Zarate, J. M., Rouleau, G. A., Skamene, E., & Boksa, P. (2002). Provisional mapping of quantitative trait loci modulating the acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(5), 765-781. Keri, S., Antal, A., Szekeres, G., Benedek, G., & Janka, Z. (2002). Spatiotemporal visual processing in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci, 14(2), 190-196. Kiorpes, L., Tang, C., Hawken, M. J., & Movshon, J. A. (2003). Ideal observer analysis of the development of spatial contrast sensitivity in macaque monkeys. J Vis, 3(10), 630-641. Kumari, V., Antonova, E., & Geyer, M. A. (2008). Prepulse inhibition and "psychosis-proneness" in healthy individuals: An fMRI study. Eur Psychiatry. Kumari, V., Antonova, E., Geyer, M. A., Ffytche, D., Williams, S. C., & Sharma, T. (2007). A fMRI investigation of startle gating deficits in schizophrenia patients treated with typical or atypical antipsychotics. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol, 10(4), 463-477. Kumari, V., Gray, J. A., Geyer, M. A., ffytche, D., Soni, W., Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., et al. (2003). Neural correlates of tactile prepulse inhibition: a functional MRI study in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Psychiatry Res, 122(2), 99-113. Kwon, Y. H., Nelson, S. B., Toth, L. J., & Sur, M. (1992). Effect of stimulus contrast and size on NMDA receptor activity in cat lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol, 68(1), 182-196. Lotto, R. B., & Purves, D. (2001). An empirical explanation of the Chubb illusion. J Cogn Neurosci, 13(5), 547-555. Movshon, J. A., & Kiorpes, L. (1988). Analysis of the development of spatial contrast sensitivity in monkey and human infants. J Opt Soc Am A, 5(12), 2166-2172. Must, A., Janka, Z., Benedek, G., & Keri, S. (2004). Reduced facilitation effect of collinear flankers on contrast detection reveals impaired lateral connectivity in the visual cortex of schizophrenia patients. Neurosci Lett, 357(2), 131-134. Pakarinen, S., Takegata, R., Rinne, T., Huotilainen, M., & Naatanen, R. (2007). Measurement of extensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP). Clin Neurophysiol, 118(1), 177-185. Robson, J. G. (1966). Spatial and temporal contrast-sensitivity functions of the visual system. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 56, 1141-1142. Slaghuis, W. L. (1998). Contrast sensitivity for stationary and drifting spatial frequency gratings in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol, 107(1), 49-62. Swerdlow, N. R., Braff, D. L., Taaid, N., & Geyer, M. A. (1994). Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 51(2), 139-154. Swerdlow, N. R., Light, G. A., Cadenhead, K. S., Sprock, J., Hsieh, M. H., & Braff, D. L. (2006). Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 63(12), 1325-1335. Tervaniemi, M., Lehtokoski, A., Sinkkonen, J., Virtanen, J., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Naatanen, R. (1999). Test-retest reliability of mismatch negativity for duration, frequency and intensity changes. Clin Neurophysiol, 110(8), 1388-1393. Turetsky, B. I., Calkins, M. E., Light, G. A., Olincy, A., Radant, A. D., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2007). Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr Bull, 33(1), 69-94. Uhlhaas, P. J., Linden, D. E., Singer, W., Haenschel, C., Lindner, M., Maurer, K., et al. (2006). Dysfunctional long-range coordination of neural activity during Gestalt perception in schizophrenia. J Neurosci, 26(31), 8168-8175. Umbricht, D., & Krljes, S. (2005). Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res, 76(1), 1-23. Umbricht, D., Schmid, L., Koller, R., Vollenweider, F. X., Hell, D., & Javitt, D. C. (2000). Ketamine-induced deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in healthy volunteers: implications for models of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 57(12), 1139-1147. Umbricht, D., Vyssotki, D., Latanov, A., Nitsch, R., & Lipp, H. P. (2005). Deviance-related electrophysiological activity in mice: is there mismatch negativity in mice? Clin Neurophysiol, 116(2), 353-363. Zemon, V., & Gordon, J. (2006). Luminance-contrast mechanisms in humans: visual evoked potentials and a nonlinear model. Vision Res, 46(24), 4163-4180. Zenger-Landolt, B., & Heeger, D. J. (2003). Response suppression in v1 agrees with psychophysics of surround masking. J Neurosci, 23(17), 6884-6893.