CHAPTER 4 Methodology and Results Given that social capital has been declining since the early 1970s, it would make sense that social capital-diminishing news stories have been increasing while social capital-enhancing news stories have been decreasing. If social capital-diminishing stories have been increasing, that would suggest that news stories have placed more and more emphasis on catastrophes or political scandals and are also increasingly framed in a strategic, episodic, and individualoriented manner and increasingly filled with images of people working alone. The hypothesis of this paper is that because of these changes in television news, Americans have become more cynical not only about the political process and political figures but also about fellow citizens in their community and aboutcommunity associations. RESEARCH DESIGN In designing my coding scheme, I primarily drew upon the literature of television news framing, which I discussed in the previous chapter. I used Iyengar’s concept of episodic and thematic frames, Cappella and Jamieson’s strategic and issue frames, and Patterson’s use of measuring negativity in news stories. I also use Gerbner’s Violence Index, which includes 44 counting the images of violent acts, to create my own method of coding television images. Applying the concepts of television framing and priming to individual and group behavior, I included in my codebook a method to code for individual-oriented and group-oriented stories and individual and group images. I honed my coding scheme through a number of pretests involving current television news broadcasts. After watching a week of CBS News from February 17 to 23, 2003, I ran a test run of my coding scheme. I was able to refine my coding scheme to include more story types and instances of government interference in group-oriented frames and images. As discussed in Chapter 3, seeing or hearing about groups of people who are working together restricted by authority (i.e., police officers beating protesters) might prime viewers to avoid engaging in organized activities. As shown in Chapter 2, rates of participation and trust start declining in the early 1970s and continue to decline in the 1980s. In order to prove that a correlation exists between social capital-diminishing news and a decline in social capital, I need to show that there has been a change in television news framing that starts before the early 1970s and that this change continues through the 1980s. Thus, to test my hypothesis, I randomly selected a week of news every five years from 1965 to 1990. I chose the weeks of January 17 to January 23, 1965; December 13 to December 19, 1970; February 16 to February 22, 1975; June 8 to June 14, 1980; April 14 to April 20, 1985; and April 15 to April 21, 1990. Due to the limitations of this project, a week of news from every five years was the most reasonable amount news I could code. Vanderbilt News Archive has CBS news archives dating back to August 1968; however, the cost of accessing their archive made it impossible for me to use their news videos. Instead of using Vanderbilt’s news archive, I found a free source of television news archives at the Motion 45 Picture and Television research center of the Library of Congress. The Library of Congress’s CBS Evening news archive only dates back to 1975, which is why I only coded news stories from 1975, 1980, 1985, and 1990. I was also unable to locate complete transcripts of CBS news; the University of Michigan owned abbreviated transcripts from CBS evening news dating back to 1970. These transcripts were not particularly helpful because each news broadcast was incomplete and because my coding scheme involved coding images from the news broadcasts. Since I am only looking at a fifteen year period with a rather small sample of news broadcasts, my small selection of news may not be entirely representative of news broadcasts during the period; however, given the time and financial restraints of this project, I am only able to code a total of four weeks of news. Ideally, this project would have covered the period from 1965 to 1990 and would have had a random selection of several weeks from each year, instead of week, of news broadcasts. A greater amount of news would have insured a higher level of confidence in my data. In other words, one could be more confident that my sample can be generalized to the entire newscasts from 1975, 1980, 1985, and 1990 if I had selected a larger number of newscasts from those years. SOCIAL CAPITAL-ENHANCING NEWS STORIES Because of the decline in social capital in the past thirty years, my hypothesis predicts that social capital-enhancing news stories should also be declining. Issue, thematic, and grouporiented frames, human-interest and state and local stories, and images of groups of people working together are all likely to prime the viewer’s sense of community and not activate Figure 4.1: Trends in Issue and Strategic Frames, 1975-1990. 46 50 45 40 Percentage 35 30 25 20 15 10 Issue Frames 5 Strategic Frames 0 1975 1980 1985 1990 political and interpersonal mistrust. My hypothesis is that these characteristics of news stories have decreased during the period of 1975 to 1990. The results of measuring the change in social capital-enhancing and social capitaldiminishing is mixed. From my data, there is little support for the hypothesis that the components of social capital-enhancing news stories have decreased; in fact, there has even been an increase in some features such as images of formal and informal groups. Frames that I classified as social capital-enhancing, as discussed in Chapter 3, remained overall constant for the thirty year period that I examined. Figures 4.1 and 4.2 show that issue and thematic frames were steady at an average of 34 percent and 8 percent, respectively. Like many of the categories I measured, group-oriented frames did not conclusively decline or increase between 1975 and 1990—the data on group-oriented frames fluctuates during Figure 4.2: Trends in Episodic and Thematic Frames, 1975-1990. 47 80 70 60 Percentage 50 40 30 20 Episodic Frames 10 Thematic Frames 0 1975 1980 1985 1990 this period (Figure 4.4). Group reenactment of George Washington’s winter at Valley Forge and groups of people protesting inflation problems in France and Spain are news stories that contributed to the 20.9 percent of group-oriented stories in 1975; this percentage drops to 16.4 percent in 1980 and swings back up to 19.4 percent in 1985 and 29.4 percent in 1990. Figure 4.5 documents that there is also no conclusive evidence on changes in positive news stories: my data shows that the percentage of positive new stories was in fluctuation, from 10.4 percent in 1975 to 5.2 percent in 1980 to 7.8 percent in 1985 to finally 11.8 percent in 1990. New stories that focused on state and local issues, which were also classified as social capital-enhancing stories, did not show a decline (Figure 4.7). The percentage of state and local stories, which included such stories as sheep ranchers’ problems with coyotes in Colorado in 1975, a tornado disaster in 48 Figure 4.3: Total Number of News Stories Compared to Human Interest Stories, 19751990. 140 120 100 Count 80 60 40 All news stories 20 Human interest news stories 0 1975 1985 1980 1990 Nebraska in 1980, and a chemical factory’s pollution in a Texas community in 1990, kept steady at around 14 percent in the four years that I sampled. My data suggests signs that some of the social capital-enhancing components had not decreased but in fact had increased over the past thirty years. Stories that I had classified as human-interest stories had increased steadily from 12.2 percent in 1975 to 29.4 percent in 1990. Human-interest stories are “soft” news stories that include news segments such as sled dog practices in upstate New York (1975), the Muppets leaving television (1980), small-town capitals in the United States (1985), and Greta Garbo’s death (1990). The increase in the percentage of human-interest stories can be attributed to the fact that the number of news stories declined from 1975 to 1990. Figure 4.3 shows that in 1975 and 1980, the sample of news broadcasts contained a total of 115 and 116 news stories, respectively; this figure declines to 103 49 Figure 4.4: Trends in Individual-oriented and Group-oriented Frames, 1975-1990. 40 35 30 Percentage 25 20 15 10 Individualoriented 5 Grouporiented 0 1975 1985 1980 1990 in 1985 and a mere 68 news stories in 1990. The drop in the number of news stories can be explained by changes in advertising in the early 1980s, which caused a growth in the number of commercials during the evening news.1 Thus, even though the actual count of human-interest stories that were broadcast during a week increased from 14 in 1975 to 18 in 1990, the percentage of human-interest stories increased more dramatically. Not only have the percentage of human-interest stories increased, contrary to my hypothesis, images of formal and informal groups have also become more prevalent. The percentage of group images, formal, informal, and political, has not changed; but the percentage of pictures of people in organized associations and people engaging in unofficial and unorganized group activity have grown in the past thirty years. Formal groups, which are 1 Neil Postman and Steve Powers, How to Watch TV News (New York: Penguin Books, 1992), 117-8. 50 Figure 4.5: Trends in Positive and Negative News Stories, 1975-1990. 35 30 Percentage 25 20 15 10 5 Negative Positive 0 1975 1980 1985 1990 featured in the pictures of such stories as a news report on a meeting of disgruntled conservatives seeking to break away from the Republican party (1975) and an association of Chinatown businesses in San Francisco protesting the destruction of a section of the freeway that served as a primary road into Chinatown (1990). These images increased from 8.9 percent in 1975 to 13.6 percent in 1990. The percentage of images of informal groups more than doubled during the same period, from 7.8 percent in 1975 to 16.9 percent in 1990. Pictures of a family doing activities together, such as images of an “average” American family that is plagued by the health problems caused by the chemical plant in the area eating a family dinner together (1990), and mingling with their co-workers and neighbors, such as college students socializing with each other while waiting in line to find jobs (1975), increased. The increase in the number of formal and informal images may be due in part to better technology in the television news industry. 51 Figure 4.6: Relative changes in social capital-enhancing television news, 1975-1990. Relative change 1975-1990 -3.9%* -2.7%* 8.5%* 1.3%* 0.2%* 14.3% 12.4% -0.9%* 4.6% 9.1% Issue Frames Thematic Frames Group-oriented Frames Positive Tone State/Local Stories Human-Interest Stories Stories about Political Groups Images of groups working together Images of informal groups Images of formal groups *These figures are inconclusive as percentages fluctuate greatly in 1980 and 1985. Please refer to Appendix B for the complete figures. Overall, the total amount of images I counted increased to 754 in 1985 and 634 in 1990, from low levels of 448 and 464 images in 1975 and 1980 respectively. More advanced technology allowed networks to capture more images for news broadcasts and also to move more equipment into a greater variety of locations. My hypothesis that social capital-enhancing components of news stories have declined in the past thirty years is not supported by my data. Figure 4.6 tracks the relative changes of social capital-enhancing television news and shows that my results are very mixed. In many instances, the results are inconclusive but in some cases such as human-interest stories and formal and informal group images, the results are contrary to my expectations. I shall next examine my data on social capital-diminishing news stories. SOCIAL CAPITAL-DIMINISHING NEWS STORIES 52 Figure 4.7: Trends in National and State/Local News, 1975-1990. 70 60 Percentage 50 40 30 20 10 National State/Local 0 1975 1980 1985 1990 The data for social capital-diminishing stories, like those for social capital-enhancing stories, is also varied. Many components of social capital-diminishing news stories such as strategic and individual-oriented frames have increased in the past thirty years, other features such as negative tone and stories classified as alerts and catastrophes either fluctuated or remained constant. Overall, the frames classified as social capital-diminishing increased, as my hypothesis had suggested. My data shows that the percentage of strategic frames, which Patterson and Cappella and Jamieson argue are responsible for growing political cynicism in Americans, doubled from 15.7 percent in 1975 to 22.4 percent in 1980 to 34 percent in 1985 to finally 32.4 percent in 1990 (Figure 4.1). Individual-oriented stories, which composed of 19.2 percent of news stories in 1975, also increased during this period to 29.4 percent in 1990. Data for episodic 53 Figure 4.8: Trends in New Stories Classified as Crime, Alert, Catastrophe, or Political Scandal, 1975-1990 35 30 Percentage 25 20 15 10 Crime Alert 5 Catastophe Political Scandal 0 1975 1985 1980 1990 stories from the same period do not show the same increase (Figurer 4.2). The percentages of episodically-framed news stories experienced some fluctuation: 67.8 percent of stories in 1975 had episodic frames; this figure increases to 70.7 percent in 1980 and decreases to 65.1 percent and 57.4 percent in 1985 and 1990. Group-oriented frames with government interference also did not change significantly as the percentage of group stories with instances of interference consisted of less than 10 percent of the news stories each year. Stories that featured formal groups in the story, however, increased dramatically in the thirty years. The percentage of stories that featured formal groups and organizations nearly doubled: 28.7 percent of news stories in 1975 contained instances of groups that are formally organized such as the Republican party and the American Federation of Labor and by 1990 the percentage rose to 51.5. Informal groups barely registered in my data; thus the findings are insignificant. 54 Figure 4.9: Relative Change in Social Capital-Diminishing Television News, 1975-1990. Relative change 1975-1990 16.7% -10.5* 10.3% 1.8%* 4.8% -32.3% 5.1% 2.2% 0.9%* -1.9%* 6.5%* 0.3%* -8.9%* Strategic Frames Episodic Frames Individual-oriented Frames Group-oriented Frames with Interference Negative Tone National Stories Crime Stories Alert Stories Catastrophe Stories Political Scandal Stories Individual Images Group Images with Interference Catastrophe Images *These figures are inconclusive because the percentages in 1980 and 1985 fluctuate greatly. Please refer to Appendix B for the complete figures. Negative tone, which as a component of social capital-diminishing stories should have declined, showed no significant change: from 1975 to 1990, negative news stories increased by 4 percent from 26.1 percent to 30.9 percent. Stories classified as political scandals, catastrophe, crime, and alert, which as social capital-diminishing stories should have increased substantially in the past thirty years, showed no significant or inconclusive changes, as pictured in Figure 4.8. In 1975, stories about the House Speaker’s secretary Pauline B. Girvin, indicted by on charges of fraud, and profiles of the men convicted in Watergate composed the 7.8 percent of stories that were coded as political scandals. In 1985, however, the percentage had increased to 16.5, due to the plethora of scandal coverage concerning President Reagan’s controversial visit to a World War II German cemetery where Nazi soldiers were buried and Reagan’s plan to supply aid to the Nicaraguan contras, an action 55 which he mistakenly claimed the Pope supported. The figures do not remain high, as only 5.9 percent of the news stories in 1990 were coded as political scandals. Stories concerning catastrophes experienced a similar fluctuation in the years that I examined. Catastrophic stories increased steadily from 18.2 percent (1975) to 29.1 percent (1985). The week of April 14 to April 20, 1985 was characterized by events such as a fire in a New York skyscraper, deaths caused by Mt. Vesuvius activity, and an armed Neo-Nazi, who had killed several people, on the loose in the Midwest, all of which I coded as catastrophic events. In 1990, however, the figure had fallen back to 19.1 percent. Crime and alert stories showed no significant changes as both increased by only five and two percent, respectively. My data on social capital-diminishing images are also rather inconclusive. Images of people working alone, which according to my hypothesis should have declined, fluctuated from 16.5 percent to 25.2 percent back down to 16.3 percent and up again to 23 percent in the four years that I coded. Like the group-oriented frames with interference news stories, the images of interference were also insignificant; thus, no conclusions can be gleamed from my data on images and news stories about groups and organizations that are restrained and prohibited to work together by authorities. As Figures 4.6 and 4.9 show, support for my hypothesis that my classifications of social capital-enhancing and social capital-diminishing news stories have changed significantly is mixed and inconclusive. According to my findings, social capital-diminishing news stories have not been increasing steadily nor have social capital-enhancing stories been decreasing steadily since 1975. Thus, my hypothesis that the changes in the news media might have affected social capital is not supported. As a result, the notion that the declines in social capital that past research has identified cannot be conclusively attributed to changes in television news. There 56 are numerous explanations as to why my research has not shown the correlation between social capital and television news, as I will discuss in the next chapter. 57