Neural AO1 Serotonin Serotonin is thought to reduce aggression by inhibiting responses to emotional stimuli that might otherwise lead to an aggressive response. Low levels of serotonin in the brain have been associated with an increased susceptibility to impulsive behaviour, aggression and even violent suicide. Some drugs are thought to alter serotonin levels and thus increase aggressive behaviour. Man et al (1990) gave 35 healthy subjects dexfenfluramine, which is known to deplete serotonin. Using a questionnaire to assess hostility and aggression levels, they found that dexfenfluramine treatment in males (but not females) was associated with an increase in hostility and aggression. This demonstrates a clear link between lower levels of serotonin and aggressive behaviour. Dopamine There is some evidence that a link between high levels of dopamine and aggressive behaviour exists. Lavine (1997) claims that increases in dopamine activity via the use of amphetamines have also been associated with increases in aggressive behaviour. Antipsychotics which reduce dopamine activity in the brain have been shown to reduce aggressive behaviour in violent delinquents (Buitelaar, 2003). This demonstrates a clear link between higher levels of dopamine and aggressive behaviour. Hormonal AO1 Testosterone The male sex hormone testosterone is thought to influence aggression from young adulthood onwards due to its action on brain areas involved in controlling aggression. Dabbs et al (1987) measured salivary testosterone in violent and nonviolent criminals. Those with the highest testosterone levels had a history of primarily violent crime whereas those with lowest levels had committed only non-violent crimes. Students of non-prison population have found similar trends. Lindman (1987) found that young males who behaved aggressively when drunk had higher testosterone levels than those who did not act aggressively. LT3 - Neural and Hormonal Mechanisms in Aggression Commentary/Supportive Evidence (AO2) (3/4 pieces for 24 mark question) Evidence from non-human studies One strength of research into the link between lower levels of serotonin and aggressive behaviour is that supportive evidence comes from non-human studies. Raleigh et al (1991) conducted a study on vervet monkeys. They found that individuals fed on experimental diets high in tryptophan (which increases serotonin levels) exhibited decreased levels of aggression. Individuals fed on diets that were low in tryptophan exhibited increased aggressive behaviour. This is a strength because it suggests that the difference in aggression could be attributed to serotonin levels. As a consequence this increases the credibility of the research into the link between serotonin and aggressive behaviour. Evidence from Antidepressants A further strength of the research into the link between serotonin and aggression is that supportive evidence comes from research using antidepressants. Bond (2005) established that low levels of serotonin are associated with low impulse control and aggressive behaviour. Drugs that clinically raise serotonin levels (SSRIs) produce a concurrent lowering in aggression. This is a strength because it further establishes a link between lowered levels of serotonin and aggression indicating that this neurotransmitter (or lack of) plays a vital role in displays of aggressive behaviour. As a consequence, the credibility of the research into the link between serotonin and aggression is strengthened. MAID (A03) (At least one AID paragraph in each essay) Reductionism vs Holism One issue with the research into neural and hormonal mechanisms of aggression is that it is reductionist. This is because it reduces the complex behaviour of aggression down to simple neurotransmitter and hormonal imbalances in the brain. Whilst this allows for aggression to be studied in great detail, it does not consider other factors that may contribute to aggressive behaviour in humans. For example the complexity of human social behaviour is ignored. The processes of social learning theory and deindividuation have been shown to have an impact on aggressive behaviour. This casts doubt on the explanatory power of neural and hormonal mechanisms in its attempt to explain aggression. In order to truly understand this complex behaviour, the individual as a whole must be taken into account. Meta-analysis One issue with the research into the effect of dopamine on aggression is that the link between this neurotransmitter and aggressive behaviour is still unclear. Scerbo and Raine (1993) carried out a meta-analysis into neurotransmitter levels in antisocial children and adults. They reported a consistent link between lower levels of serotonin and aggression. However they found no significant rise or fall in levels of dopamine. Whilst this provides strong empirical support for the link between serotonin and aggression, it suggests that the role of dopamine in aggression may be more complex. As a consequence this reduces the credibility of the claim that dopamine levels have an impact on aggression. Inconsistent Evidence One weakness of research into the link between testosterone levels and aggression is that there are several issues with the supporting evidence. Albert et al (1993) argued that the research into this link is largely unreliable. In addition most studies showing positive correlations have involved small samples of men within prisons, using either self-report measures of aggression or judgements based solely on the crimes committed. This carries with it a wealth of issues such as population validity (only using male prisoners), social desirability (lying on their questionnaires) or lack of control over extraneous variables (not considering the circumstances in which the crimes were committed). L: As a consequence this casts doubt over the credibility of the research into the link between testosterone and aggression. Gender Bias: One issue with the research into the link between testosterone and aggression is that it is gender bias. For example most studies have involved only male participants, failing to investigate the effect testosterone has on females. A small area of research by Archer (2005) into this has established that the association between testosterone and aggression is actually higher for female than male samples. This lends credibility to the assumption that increased testosterone does in fact lead to aggression. However it reduces the explanatory power of the original research since it fails to consider the effect of testosterone on aggression in females.