Student Number: 1035697 MSc Dissertation Title: Polygenic association in ADHD for the prediction of cognitive performance Word Count: 10,659 1035697 MSc. Dissertation 2 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Contents Abstract .................................................................................................................................... 5 Introduction .............................................................................................................................. 6 Epidemiology and Impairment .............................................................................................. 6 Diagnostic Classification ........................................................................................................ 7 Dimensional Trait .................................................................................................................. 8 Comorbidities ........................................................................................................................ 8 Aetiology and Heritability ...................................................................................................... 9 Molecular Genetic Investigation in ADHD ............................................................................. 9 Genome-wide Association Studies (GWA) .......................................................................... 10 Candidate Gene Approaches ............................................................................................... 11 Rare Variants ....................................................................................................................... 12 Challenges in Detecting Associations with Common Genetic Variants............................... 13 Candidate Cognitive Endophenotypes in ADHD.................................................................. 15 Polygenic Association .......................................................................................................... 17 Aims ..................................................................................................................................... 18 Method .................................................................................................................................... 19 Sample Selection ................................................................................................................. 19 Clinical Measures................................................................................................................. 20 Medication .......................................................................................................................... 20 Cognitive Measures ............................................................................................................. 20 Genotyping & QC process ................................................................................................... 22 TDT Testing and Covariates ................................................................................................. 23 Pseudo-controls and Imputation......................................................................................... 24 Polygenic Signal ................................................................................................................... 24 Major Allele Frequency ....................................................................................................... 26 Results.................................................................................................................................... 27 Discussion.............................................................................................................................. 28 Power................................................................................................................................... 29 Limitations ........................................................................................................................... 30 Use of ADHD Symptom Counts to Generate Polygenic Signals........................................... 30 Major Allele Bias .................................................................................................................. 31 Case/Pseudo-control ........................................................................................................... 32 Future Directions ................................................................................................................. 32 Conclusions.......................................................................................................................... 33 References............................................................................................................................. 35 3 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Appendix ................................................................................................................................ 44 Appendix 1 – List of Full Gene Names ................................................................................. 44 Appendix 2 – List of Abbreviations ...................................................................................... 45 4 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Polygenic association in ADHD for the prediction of cognitive performance Abstract Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with multiple cognitive performance deficits. Among these Kuntsi et al (2010, Archives of General Psychiatry) selected the most promising indicators for a multivariate familial factor analysis. Two familial cognitive impairment factors were identified, which accounted for 85% and 13% of the familial variance on ADHD. The first, large factor captured speed and variability of reaction times (RT) and the second factor captured commission (Ce) and omission (Oe) errors on a go/no-go task. Using the same sample we now evaluate whether polygenic signals for ADHD generated from genome-wide SNP data are associated with the most promising cognitive variables. Analysis used data from the International Multi-Centre ADHD Gene (IMAGE) project. Transmission disequilibrium test (TDT) was conducted and the data used to generate polygenic scores in a training sample at a range of different inclusion thresholds. The best performing threshold (10% of SNP associations) was selected using crossvalidation and used produce a polygenic signal which was then apply to predict cognitive performance using regression analysis in an independent test set. This study was not able to detect a positive polygenic signal for ADHD, or able to predict cognitive performance using this polygenic signal. Power calculations indicated that the training sample was insufficient in this pilot study, but demonstrated that the generation of a positive polygenic signal for ADHD should be achievable using currently available ADHD GWA samples. The study also demonstrates the effectiveness of case/pseudo-control imputation for dealing with major allele overtransmission bias, as well as suggesting a number of methodological improvements and directions for future polygenic investigations. 5 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Introduction Epidemiology and Impairment Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder with a reported average childhood prevalence of 5%, although reports of prevalence range from 1%-20% (Polanczyk et al., 2007). Diagnosis persists into adulthood in approximately 15% of childhood cases, with 50% who no longer meet diagnostic criteria continuing to express residual symptoms and impairments into adult life (Faraone et al., 2006). Estimates for adult prevalence ranges from 1-6% (Weiss et al., 1985, Murphy and Barkley, 1996), averaging around 3% (Faraone and Biederman, 2005). Significant variability exists within the reported prevalence for ADHD, but is attributed to methodological differences rather than geographical factors. No significant differences are observed in average rates between North America, Europe, South America, Asia or Oceania (Polanczyk et al., 2007), or in children with different ethnic backgrounds (Lahey et al., 1994). Prevalence is highly sensitive to the diagnostic criteria used in defining the diagnosis, with DSM-III or ICD-10 definitions producing lower prevalence rates against DSM-IV criteria. Differing research methodologies, such as ignoring the requirement for impairment, or the use of a single informant opposed to multiple informants can also greatly influence reported prevalence rates. However, differences in prevalence are consistently reported to vary by gender and age, being elevated in males over females and more common in children than adolescents (Polanczyk et al., 2007). Although generally, ADHD prevalence declines with age, persistence of symptoms is equal in girls and boys (Biederman et al., 2004), with the ratio of male-females diagnosed with adult ADHD being around 1.6; comparable to that of child and adolescent ADHD (Scahill and Schwab-Stone, 2000). ADHD is characterised by age-inappropriate hyperactivity, inattention and impulsive behaviours, and is associated with a broad range of negative social, cognitive and functional outcomes (National Institutes of Health, 2000). Impairments may appear later than symptom onset, particularly in relation to inattention behaviours and scholastic achievement (Applegate et al., 1997). In adults ADHD has been associated with financial difficulties (National Institutes of Health, 2000), and modestly with unemployment and divorce (Biederman, 2004, Barkley et al., 2002). ADHD impairment in adults is represented differently to children, and may be expressed by poor nutrition, increased rates of smoking, aggressive driving behaviours, increased rates of accidents, legal problems, and workplace challenges (Faraone et al., 2006, Kessler et al., 2006). Many adult-specific impairments are not 6 1035697 MSc. Dissertation well captured by current diagnostic criteria, and can also be prone to poor measurement on account of the lack of insight and limitations of the self-report methodology used for much adult ADHD research (Kessler et al., 2006). The negative functional consequences of persistent ADHD may therefore be underestimated, and may represent a significant public health burden. Diagnostic Classification DSM-IV (APA, 2000) classification of ADHD consists of 9 inattention items, 6 hyperactivity items and 3 impulsivity items, grouped by inattention and hyperactivityimpulsivity subtypes. A clinical diagnosis of ADHD requires the presence of six or more symptoms on at least one subscale, which are maladaptive and inconsistent with developmental level, for a period of at least six months. Symptoms must have caused some impairment before age 7, in at least two or more settings such as school and home, and must not occur exclusively as part of another disorder. Clear evidence of significant implement in social, academic or occupational functioning must also be present. If scores of six symptoms are present on both subscales then a diagnosis of combined type ADHD is made, otherwise predominantly inattentive or predominantly hyperactive-impulsive diagnosis may be applied depending on symptom distribution. However, these subtypes have been shown to be unstable in longitudinal follow-up, with 50% changing between subtypes (Valo and Tannock, 2010), and those with the hyperactive subtype being particularly likely to shift to a combined type diagnosis (Lahey et al., 2005). This may therefore either represent a tendency for symptom range to broadening with age or more likely, an inaccurate representation of the underlying disorder by existing diagnostic criteria. Proposed revisions to ADHD classifications for DSM-5 recognise subtypes as unstable by redefining them as ‘current presentations’ (APA, 2010). However, the existing structure has been retained, with the addition of a restrictive inattentive presentation, requiring six symptoms on the inattentive scale but two or less on the hyperactive-impulsive scale, for purely inattentive children. DSM-IV criteria were not designed for the diagnosis of adult ADHD and so have been argued to be suboptimal for this purpose due to a child focused range of symptoms the requirement for severe impairment and the requirement for a high symptom count. Further, childhood level criteria may not account for the adoption of coping mechanisms and reduction of symptom counts in adult life. DSM-5 proposals directly address these arguments through increasing the requirement for age of symptom onset to 12, reducing symptom requirements for older adolescents and adults (ages 17 and older) 7 1035697 MSc. Dissertation to four, adding adult specific examples to symptom items, and reducing the impairment criteria from “clinically significant” to “interfere with or reduce quality of…functioning” (APA, 2000, APA, 2010). With the demonstrated sensitivity of ADHD prevalence to methodological changes, these proposals are likely to increase prevalence rates across the whole age spectrum, but especially in adult ADHD, as well as increasing the heterogeneity of ADHD presentation. However, these proposed criteria may arguably better represent the underlying phenotype. Dimensional Trait ADHD-type symptoms appear throughout the population at subclinical levels, so ADHD should be considered a dimensional trait (Chen et al., 2008). This dimensional construct of ADHD would be far better at capturing heterogeneity in ADHD symptom presentation, but is not represented within current or proposed DSM-IV and DSM-5 categorical definitions. Further, it has been argued that the existing subtype structure over-represents inattention and under-represents impulsivity symptoms (Bell, 2011). This structure can also lead to diagnostic artefacts, such as where scores of five inattention symptoms and five hyperactiveimpulsive symptoms do not meet ADHD diagnostic thresholds. Quantitative genetic investigations support the concept of ADHD diagnostic criteria as representing the extreme end of a continuous dimensional trait (Levy et al., 1997) at which meaningful levels of impairment are likely to be present. Thus, although the epidemiology of ADHD is better explained as continuous trait, use of a categorical definition is justified when interested in clinically significant expressions of the syndrome (Haslam et al., 2006). Comorbidities ADHD is highly comorbid with other psychiatric disorders notably; with an estimated 60-100% reported to display one or more comorbid disorder (Gillberg et al., 2004). Comorbidities include both externalising and internalising disorders, with conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, anxiety, depression, bipolar disorder and substance abuse being most common (Bauermeister et al., 2007, Biederman et al., 1991, Biederman, 2004, Jensen et al., 1997, Gillberg et al., 2004, Faraone et al., 2001, Faraone et al., 1997). increased functional Typically those with comorbid problems, show impairments and have poorer long-term prognosis (Bauermeister et al., 2007). However, currently it is unclear to what extent comorbid 8 1035697 MSc. Dissertation problems are related on a phenotypic level, or if they predominantly arise through common underling genetic liabilities as a consequence of an atypical neurodevelopmental trajectory (Merwood and Asherson, in press). Evidence for familial associations (Biederman et al., 1991, Faraone et al., 1997, Faraone et al., 2001), and studies of co-occurrence of cognitive endophenotypes (Rommelse et al., 2009) indicate possible shared genetic aetiology for some comorbidities. The close relationship between ADHD and its common comorbidities is further reinforced by indications that stimulant medication treatment of childhood ADHD may reduce the number of comorbidities experienced in childhood (Biederman, 2003). However, a distinction may exist between those with aggressive/conduct type comorbidities and anxiety type comorbidities (Jensen et al., 1997). This distinction has been further supported by evidence that late onset ADHD may be associated with less severe symptoms overall, more anxiety symptoms and more internalising behaviours, but less with conduct problems related to authority and discipline (Karam et al., 2009). Further work including studies using polygenic methods is required to further understand the possible genetic relationships between ADHD and different comorbid disorders. Aetiology and Heritability The aetiology of ADHD is currently unknown but is likely to be a complex interaction between genetic and environmental factors, with a substantial genetic component. Familial investigations of ADHD show 1st degree relatives of children with ADHD have a 4-10 fold elevated risk (Chen et al., 2008), and twin studies estimate heritability rates of around 62-76% (Faraone et al., 2005, Wood and Neale, 2010). The two symptom dimensions of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention also show substantial overlap in genetic liability (McLoughlin et al., 2007). ADHD has been found to be highly heritable as either a categorical definition or dimensional trait, although no specific susceptibility genes have been unequivocally identified. Molecular Genetic Investigation in ADHD Early ADHD linkage (Smalley et al., 2002) and GWA studies (Neale et al., 2008) did not detect common genetic variations with large effect sizes associated with ADHD. This implies that genetic liability for ADHD is likely to be conferred from many thousands of genes of very small effect and may be due to additive and interaction effects (Kuntsi et al., 2006a). This is in line with findings from genetic 9 1035697 MSc. Dissertation investigations of most other psychiatric disorders, such as bipolar disorder (Sklar et al., 2008). Consequently, ADHD genetic research has tended to favour either candidate gene investigations, focused on genes with roles in neural systems impaired in ADHD (Brookes et al., 2006), or GWA meta-analyses with increasingly large samples to improve power to detect associations with small effect sizes. Recently, investigations looking for associations with rare structural variants of comparatively large effect size have begun to be published, indicating ADHD may be enriched for rare-variants. As a consequence, this research may indicate that the role of common genetic variants for genetic liability in ADHD may be smaller than has usually been assumed. An overview of current literature from the GWA, candidate gene, and rare variant research in ADHD is presented here, followed by a discussion of some problems facing genetic research in ADHD which make the detection of associations difficult. This review will then outline research on cognitive endophenotypes in ADHD, and the use of these markers in genetically sensitive designs. Finally this section will outline a method for directly testing the common variant model in ADHD: polygenic analysis; along with its advantages and potential applications. Genome-wide Association Studies (GWA) GWA studies have as yet been unable to detect associations between common genetic variations and ADHD in children (Neale et al., 2008, Mick et al., 2010), or adults (Lesch et al., 2008), at the accepted genome-wide significant threshold of 5 x 10^-8 (Dudbridge and Gusnanto, 2008). However, two GWA studies (Lesch et al., 2008, Franke et al., 2009) have reported suggestive associations with the CDH13 gene (see appendix for full gene names), which has been shown to code for a neural adhesion protein and has been linked with methamphetamine dependence (Uhl et al., 2008). CDH13 has also been implicated in ADHD in results from meta-analysis of genome-wide linkage studies which reported significant associations with the 16q21–16q24 region (Zhou et al., 2008). The largest GWA to date, combined four large ADHD samples (2,960 cases, 2,455 controls) and reported several associations on two regions of chromosome 7 and 8 at the <5x10^-6 level (Neale et al., 2010). Multiple other top-50 hits were also reported within these same two regions. The chromosome 7 SNPs fall within a “gene-poor” area of the genome but the closest gene, SHFM1, is expressed within the brain and has been linked to proteolysis within cells and congenital physical abnormalities. These functional roles imply that this gene is particularly important during embryonic development. The 10 1035697 MSc. Dissertation SNPs on chromosome 8 fall within the coding region for CHMP7, which has been shown to be expressed in the brain and has a role in protein sorting/recycling. Within 100kb of this region, other genes linked with apoptosis and cell adhesion are also located (TNFRSF10D, TNFRSF10A, LOXL2). Although these suggestive associations await replication, and do not meet genome-wide significance thresholds, the consistency of the top hits falling within these two regions offers promising new targets for candidate gene investigations and represents evidence for the role of common genetic variants in the genetic liability of ADHD. Furthermore, the chromosome 7 region reported in this study has also shown suggestive associations with major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder (Moskvina et al., 2009) providing further evidence for a close genetic link between ADHD and its common comorbidities. Some GWAs approaches have attempted to examine the genetic associations with ADHD related quantitative traits. Lasky-Su et al. (2008) and Faraone et al. (2007), explored associations with age-of-onset. No genome-wide significant hits were reported but a risk variant for DRD5 was linked to earlier age of onset, while nominal associations were reported for HTR2A and SLC9A9, which have been identified as ADHD candidate genes in other studies. Candidate Gene Approaches Candidate gene approaches have a reduced requirement for the highly conservative significance thresholds use in GWAs, but are limited by the need to specify target genes prior to carrying out the investigation. This prevents this method detecting novel associations outside the genes of interest, and restricts the method’s usefulness to providing converging evidence following GWA investigations or for the study of genes chosen though systemic pathway approaches. Brookes et al. (2006) identified 51 genes of interest in ADHD predominantly focusing dopaminergic (DA), noradrenergic (NA) and serotoninergic (5HT) pathways. Many were obvious targets for candidate gene investigations as stimulant medication used to treat ADHD has a known action on synaptic DA levels (Swanson et al., 2007), and 5-HT has been previously linked with poor impulse regulation (Lucki, 1998). A comprehensive meta-analysis of candidate gene studies by Gizer et al (2009) concluded there was sufficient evidence to indicate reliable associations with the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1; SLC6A3), two dopamine receptor genes (DRD4, DRD5), the serotonin transporter gene (HTT; SLC6A4), one serotonin 11 1035697 MSc. Dissertation receptor gene (5HT1B; HTR1B) and SNAP25, a gene which codes for proteins involved in synaptic plasticity and axonal growth. Yet, reports of associations with these genes have been inconsistent across studies; a likely consequence of sample heterogeneity and methodological differences in the criteria used to define ADHD. Further, there are indications that some associations may be stronger across genders or that the expression of some risk genes is developmentally variable. For example, it has been suggested a risk variant of the COMT gene which codes for Catechol-O-methyltransferase, an enzyme which degrades post-synaptic dopamine, adrenaline and noradrenaline, may be expressed differently across genders (Dempster et al., 2006). Although a general association with ADHD was not found by Gizer and colleagues during meta-analysis, in a study with a predominantly male sample (84%) the risk allele was associated with increased severity of ADHD symptoms (Palmason et al., 2010). This might indicate differential risk conferred by the variant across genders (Biederman et al., 2008), offering a potential mechanisms to explain the observed gender difference in ADHD. However, this might indicate further heterogeneity of genetic effects making reliable detection of associations in mixed samples more difficult. Rare Variants Recently evidence for the influence of rare (<1% population frequency) copy number variants (CNVs) on ADHD risk have been published. CNVs are a significant component of genetic variation and genome-wide analysis of CNVs can be accomplished in the same way as SNP based GWA studies with use of gene-chips containing CNV probes. Of particular interest for ADHD genetic research are large (>500 kb) CNVs that have been shown to have robust associations with other psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and autism (International Schizophrenia Consortium, 2008, Glessner et al., 2009). Larger CNVs also have the greatest callrate accuracy and show good concordance across platforms (Itsara et al., 2009), making good initial targets for this approach. The first analysis of this type did not report significant associations with ADHD (Elia et al., 2010), yet subsequent investigations have indicated ADHD samples to be significantly enriched for large CNVs against control samples (14% vs 7%; Williams et al., 2010). A subset of children with ADHD and severe intellectual disabilities, showed particularly high penetrance for large, rare CNVs (36%), yet rates of CNVs remained significantly elevated in children with ADHD but without an intellectual disability. This suggests some CNVs may be associated with ADHD 12 1035697 MSc. Dissertation independent of cognitive ability, while others are likely to contribute to both. These CNVs were shown to be both deletions and duplications, and to be a mixture of both inherited and de novo mutations (11:4), supporting arguments for a functional role in the development of the ADHD phenotype, and ADHD linked cognitive impairments. Of the CNVs reported in Williams et al. (2010), seventeen also show associations with either autism (Glessner et al., 2009, Daly et al., 2008, Roohi et al., 2009, Kalscheuer et al., 2007) or schizophrenia (Stefansson et al., 2008, International Schizophrenia Consortium, 2008, Ingason et al., 2011, McCarthy et al., 2009) in other studies, with CNVs associated with both ADHD and schizophrenia being predominantly located within the 16p13.11 region. One gene failing within this region is NDE1 which has previously been linked to neurodevelopmental processes and is known to interact with the DISC1 gene (Bradshaw et al., 2009). Investigation of rare CNVs is yielding promising results. However, while authors of CNV studies are keen to stress that research on common genetic variants should not be abandoned; these findings present the possibility that the genetic liability for ADHD could be explained predominantly through many thousands of rarevariants as opposed to the assumed common genetic variation model. If so this would make these associations very difficult to detect with existing GWA methods, and make the collection of increasingly large samples for common genetic variance studies fruitless. In practice, both models are likely to be viable routes to developing the phenotype, and evidence from other psychiatric disorders with greater depth of CNV research implies symptoms are likely to arise from large numbers of common variants of subtle effect in some cases and few larger effect rare variants in others. However, with the exact contribution of rare variants and common variants to the genetic liability of ADHD still underdetermined, there is value in directly testing the common variant model in ADHD using polygenic analysis. Further, issues with detecting genetic associations in ADHD and the advantages of using a polygenic analysis are described in the following sections. Challenges in Detecting Associations with Common Genetic Variants GWAs for ADHD have thus far failed to detect a genome-wide significant association with a common variant in ADHD, which is assumed to be due to a lack of power. Currently, samples for ADHD are substantially smaller than those of other psychiatric disorders, such as bipolar disorder and schizophrenia, which began to yield the first genome-wide significant associations at around 2-3 times the sample 13 1035697 MSc. Dissertation currently available in ADHD. Evidence from existing candidate gene studies suggests that effect sizes for individual common variants in ADHD may be around the 1.12-1.33 range. If common genetic variants do confer risk in ADHD, and effect sizes are equivalent to those in other disorders, it is estimated around 4000 cases may be required to have 80% power to detect a genome-wide significant association with a minor allele frequency of >25% (Corvin et al., 2010). Post-hoc power analysis from the largest sample to date with nearly 3000 cases, demonstrated effect sizes in ADHD must be very small as the study reported 98% power to detect effects contributing more than 0.5% of the variance in the phenotype (Neale et al., 2010). However, this fell to only 2% power for effect sizes of 0.1%, which is likely to be closer to the true effect size for many common variants in ADHD. Therefore, current assumptions are that stronger associations for ADHD will emerge with time as GWA samples sizes improve, but this has not yet ben conclusively proven. In light of the evidence that rare variants may also play a role in the genetic liability for ADHD, it remains justified to attempt to test these assumptions with existing available data before undertaking the hugely demanding task of collecting additional large GWA samples. A second issue which is likely to be making detection of genetic associations difficult is the heterogeneity of the ADHD phenotype. Symptom expression can vary widely, as can prevalence based upon the inconsistent application of diagnostic criteria across research investigations. Consequently, this can create difficulties in the replication of results between studies and make resolution of the genetic liability in ADHD challenging. The effects of population stratification in large samples, and the possibility of differing effect sizes for some variants across genders (Biederman et al., 2008) add further complexity. Equally, the expression of risk genes may show age dependent developmental effects, altering production rates of gene products and increasing phenotypic heterogeneity within the syndrome at different ages. Evidence of this effect has been presented for DAT1, a gene coding for the dopamine transporter protein, where the risk variant has stronger influence in adolescence (Barkley et al., 2006, Elia and Devoto, 2007). COMT activity has also been reported to correlate with age in animal models (Venero et al., 1991). Therefore, developmental differences in gene expression and the interaction with risk variants may account for the tendency of symptoms to decline into adulthood, and explain why ADHD is typically associated with childhood (Faraone et al., 2000). 14 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Candidate Cognitive Endophenotypes in ADHD The variable definitions of ADHD, combined with developmental variability and gene-environment interactions effects (Kuntsi et al., 2006a) increase phonotypical heterogeneity, reducing power to detect robust genetic associations. However, the study of neurobiological processes thought to underpin deficits in ADHD may help to counter phenotypic variability in the study of genetic associations (Tye et al., 2011). In this analysis, genetic risk markers for ADHD have been used to predicted cognitive performance on indices of established ADHD-linked cognitive endophenotypes. According to Gottesman and Gould (2003) endophenotypes must be associated with the clinical disorder, share overlapping genetic influences and be present in non-affected family members at a higher rate than the general population. Broadly, many impairments in ADHD relate to general categories of attentional-arousal problems or executive function deficits. Specifically, cognitive impairments include deficits in working memory, planning and organisation, set shifting, processing speed, attention regulation, variability in reaction times, impulsivity and response inhibition (Willcutt et al., 2005). A general IQ deficit of 7-12 points is also commonly associated with ADHD (Kuntsi et al., 2004), and measures of reaction times, response inhibition and sustained attention (indexed by commission errors and omission errors respectively), consistently show impaired performance in ADHD (Willcutt et al., 2005, Klein et al., 2006, Johnson et al., 2009, Kuntsi et al., 2009, Wood et al., 2010). Incentive factors have been shown to play a role in performance on reaction time tasks, as performance can be significantly improved under incentive conditions (Kuntsi et al., 2009, Uebel et al., 2010, Andreou et al., 2007). However, incentives have not been shown to influence performance on measures of omission or commission errors, suggesting different underling processes (Uebel et al., 2010) Variability in reaction time (RTV), demonstrates strong associations with ADHD in children and adolescents (Klein et al., 2006, Rommelse, 2008, Wood et al., 2010, Kuntsi et al., 2010) as well as adults (McLoughlin et al., 2010). This endophenotype has been shown to have a heritability of around 50-60%, and has an estimated familial correlation with ADHD of around 0.74 (Kuntsi et al., 2010). Measures of attention and executive function, including inhibition, have been demonstrated to be moderately heritable, and to share a genetic association with ADHD (Doyle et al., 2005). As there is little evidence of shared environmental effects in either ADHD or related cognitive variables, familial effects can be assumed to be largely genetic in origin (Andreou et al., 2007). Similarly, IQ deficits which are also 15 1035697 MSc. Dissertation moderately strongly heritable have been linked with ADHD due to shared familial genetic influences (Kuntsi et al., 2004, Polderman et al., 2006, Wood et al., 2010, Wood et al., 2011). In molecular genetic investigations of cognitive endophenotypes for ADHD, most research has been focused on risk variants of DAT1, DRD4 and COMT, which are among a number of key candidate genes for ADHD (Brookes et al., 2006). DRD4 has been associated with increased RTV, and to a lesser extent impulsiveness, but not response inhibition on the Go/NoGo task. DAT1 variants have also been associated with increased RTV in a number of investigations, and in some reports have been found to be associated with rates of omission and commission errors on the continuous performance task. In contrast, no associations have been found between COMT and RTV on constant performance or Go/NoGo tasks, but associations have been reported with choice impulsivity in delay discounting tasks. (See Kebir et al., 2009 for a detailed review of current literature). Therefore, although evidence for molecular genetic associations with cognitive endophenotypes is still limited, in combination with familial evidence, it provides initial indications that these cognitive endophenotypes are genetically linked to ADHD. Using multivariate familial factor analysis, Kuntsi and colleagues (2010) demonstrated that familial factors for cognitive performance indicators of mean reaction time (MRT), reaction time variability (RTV), omission errors and commission errors separated into two separate factors. Both of these factors were associated with ADHD. The larger factor accounted for the familial variance for MRT and RTV and shared 85% of its familial variance with ADHD; the second factor captured variance for omission and commission errors and shared 13% of its familial variance with ADHD. This study indicates that two separate processes may underlie cognitive impairments in ADHD. One factor captures speed and consistency of reaction time performance, and may relate to arousal regulation; while the second factor captures omission and commission errors and may relate to sustained attention and inhibition or executive function. This division of processes is supported by previous findings that incentives appear to normalise performance on indices of arousal-regulation, but not those of executive function (Kuntsi et al., 2009, Uebel et al., 2010). In further work it was demonstrated that genetic co-variance between reaction time variability or errors, and ADHD was largely independent of IQ (Wood et al., 2010, Wood et al., 2011). 16 1035697 MSc. Dissertation It is therefore warranted to make use of markers of brain function, such as cognitive performance deficits, as endophenotypes for the investigation of ADHD to understand the relationship between underlying endophenotypes and expression of the ADHD phenotype. Evidence that cognitive impairments continue into adulthood, even following remission of childhood diagnosis of ADHD (Faraone et al., 2006) suggests some neurobiological correlates may represent more stable phenotypes than the broad ADHD phenotype itself. Furthermore, it might be assumed that genotypes underlying cognitive performance deficits or specific neurobiological pathways would be less genetically diverse than the sum of all genotypic associations with the ADHD phenotype; theoretically increasing power to detect genetic associations (Gottesman and Gould, 2003). However, although this approach may reduce heterogeneity within samples, available GWA samples sizes are still insufficient to reliability detect novel genetic associations with ADHD or cognitive endophenotypes. To overcome some of these issues, this study adopted a method of using polygenic association using common genetic variants in ADHD to examine the genetic relationship with ADHD-linked endophenotypes of reaction time variability, omission errors, commission errors and IQ. In examining the genetic relationship between these underlying cognitive functions and the broad ADHD phenotype, this type of analysis may help to understand the pathways from genetic risk variants to cognitive impairments and expressed ADHD behaviours and identify broad loci for novel candidate gene investigations based these identified systemic pathways. Polygenic Association The use of polygenic association approaches using existing GWA data, can overcome some of the challenges associated with looking for genetic associations in ADHD, and offers a novel technique for the understanding of shared genetic liability between ADHD and its comorbidities or endophenotypes. Polygenic association utilises the suggestive genetic associations detected in GWA studies on “en masse” to produce a risk score, which is able to predict the phenotype or related quantitative trait in an independent test sample. From the many thousands of associations detected in a typical GWA study very few will reach genome-wide significance. However, many will be genuine associations with the phenotype but due to small effect sizes of individual variants they are not able to be differentiated from the many false associations present in the analysis. This necessitates the adoption of stringent significance thresholds in GWA studies, and forces most of these genuine 17 1035697 MSc. Dissertation associations to be ignored. Yet, through combining a large number of these small associations a useful polygenic ‘signal’ can emerge. This approach was successfully used by the International Schizophrenia Consortium, to discriminate cases from controls in an independent sample using a polygenic signal derived from schizophrenia GWA data (Purcell et al., 2009). Critically, this polygenic score had predictive power even though the signal explained only 3% of the underlying variance and no genome-wide significant associations were reported in the dataset. Given that the power of the polygenic signal comes from the combination of many associations, polygenic analysis lacks the fidelity of GWAs to provide evidence of specific associations with individual genetic markers. Rather, polygenic analysis can demonstrate that the overall common genetic variation plays a role in the heritability of disorder, and with large samples, may be able to estimate the relative contributions of common variants against rare variants to the observed heritability. It follows that if the contribution of the common genetic variant model in a disorder can be demonstrated, then assumptions regarding the potential of GWA studies to detect specific associations with expanding sample sizes are justified. More broadly this technique may be applied to investigate the genetic overlap between two different disorders, comorbid disorders, other traits or endophenotypes. In their polygenic investigation of schizophrenia The International Schizophrenia Consortium demonstrated they were able to use a schizophrenia polygenic signal to predict bipolar disorder status in an independent sample, suggesting a shared genetic liability between the two disorders and indicating a pleiotropic effect for common variants. The polygenic signal was also found not to be predictive of six other non-psychiatric diseases (coronary artery disease, Crohn’s disease, hypertension, rheumatoid arthritis, type I and type II diabetes), demonstrating a valuable degree of specificity in the investigation of genetically-related phenotypes. However, at the time of writing polygenic analysis methods have not been applied to psychiatric disorders other than schizophrenia, despite the substantial quantity of available GWA data for a range of phenotypes. This investigation now progresses the application of the polygenic analysis method in the investigation of cognitive performance indicators in ADHD. Aims This pilot study aimed to test the feasibility of conducting polygenic analysis in ADHD and to test methods for overcoming potential problems arising from the use of 18 1035697 MSc. Dissertation family based GWA data. An a priori hypothesis was made that risk markers for ADHD will be associated with impaired cognitive performance, and thus a polygenic signal for ADHD will be predictive of impaired cognitive performance on four indexes (reaction time variability, omission errors, commission errors, IQ) which reliability detect deficits in ADHD. Method Sample Selection Data from ADHD parent-proband trios was available from the International Multicentre ADHD Genetics (IMAGE) project. The IMAGE consortium consisted of 12 sites across eight countries (Belgium, Germany, Ireland, Israel, the Netherlands, Spain, Switzerland, and the United Kingdom), which collected phenotypic and genotype data on trios using standardised procedures. Eight of these 12 sites set up a further collaboration to collect additional cognitive performance data. Standard IMAGE exclusion criteria and QC procedures were applied to sample. Probands were aged between 6 and 17 years. Cases we excluded if they reported autism, epilepsy, a neurological disorder, a disorder with externalizing behaviours which could be mistaken for ADHD, or IQ <70. This study focused on probands with a diagnosis of ADHD combined type only, as these were the most common subgroup within the sample. Initially 862 QC’ed parent-proband trios were available before polygenic analysis exclusion criteria were applied. 43 cases were excluded as they met criteria for the inattentive or hyperactive subtypes (11 inattentive, 32 hyperactive). This sample of 819 was then divided into a training set for the generation of the polygenic signal, and an independent test set. All cases with cognitive performance data available were included in the test set (n=338). The remaining cases (n=454) comprised the training set, which was further subdivided into five subsets that were stratified by site so that an equal number of participants from each site was represented in each of the five subsets. Where the majority of the cases from a site were included with the cognitive test set, leaving a small number of cases (<10) without cognitive data, those remaining cases were excluded from the sample rather than populate the training subsets with individual cases from single site. This process resulted in 27 cases across four sites being removed. The final number of cases surviving all stages of QC and selection was 792. 19 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Clinical Measures Under standard IMAGE protocols all probands were assessed by a paediatrician or child psychiatrist using the Parental Account of Childhood Symptom (PACS; Taylor et al., 1986). The PACS is semi-structured and designed to be objective measure of child behaviour. The measure obtains detailed descriptions of the child’s typical behaviour in a range of situations, both in the past week and the past year. Situations are defined by external events or behaviours, with interviewers rating severity and frequency on a four point scale for each situation. The measure was administered by trained interviewers and had an inter-rater reliability ranging between 0.79 to 0.96. PACS scores were processed using a standardized algorithm to generate DSM-IV symptom counts based upon the 18 DSM-IV ADHD items. Information from the PACS was supplemented by items scoring in the mid to high range on the teacher rated Conners’ ADHD subscale to provide robustness through use of another rater of child behaviour in a second environment. Cases with potential comorbid autism spectrum disorders, which might confound the analysis, were excluded on the basis of atypical scores on both the social communication questionnaire, and the prosocial scale of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Medication As stimulant medication can alter the expression of ADHD symptoms and behaviours, probands were asked to not take stimulant medications for minimum of 48 hours prior to undertaking research assessments. Where this was not possible, interviewers endeavoured to rate the child’s typical behaviours during past medication free periods. Any proband without a medication free period in the past two years was excluded from the sample. Cognitive data was not used unless the participant was medication free during assessments. Cognitive Measures All participants completed four subtests (vocabulary, similarities, picture completion, block design) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children, Third Edition (WISC), or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, fourth edition (WAIS) for probands aged 16 or over, for an estimate of IQ. 20 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Eight sites conducted additional cognitive performance assessments in addition to genotype and behavioural assessments. Probands were assessed on a visual Go/No-Go task and a reaction time task: “The Fast Task”. The Go/No-Go task (Kuntsi et al., 2005) required participants to respond as quickly as possible to “go” stimuli, but not to “no-go” stimuli, whilst retaining accuracy. The rate of “go” to “no-go” stimuli was 4:1, with stimuli being presented for 300 milliseconds. Data was collected on number of errors and reaction time (RT). Three conditions matched on duration were run: slow (72 trials, 8-second inter-stimulus interval), fast (462 trials, 1 second inter-stimulus interval) and incentive. Only performance on fast and slow conditions was utilised for this analysis, as these conditions showed the strongest phenotypic and familial association with ADHD (Uebel et al., 2010). Presentation of fast and slow conditions was counterbalanced by participant. The Fast Task (Kuntsi et al., 2006b, Andreou et al., 2007), involved a baseline condition consisting of 72 trials with an 8-second inter-stimulus interval, and a fast-incentive condition with a 1-second inter-stimulus interval. Only reaction time data from the baseline condition was used in this analysis. Participants saw four empty circles on the screen at the beginning of the inter-stimulus interval and after 8 seconds one of these circles became coloured. Participants responded to this target signal by pressing a corresponding response key, after which the stimuli disappeared from the screen and a new trial started after a 2.5 second blank-screen period. At the start of the paradigm participants were informed both speed and accuracy of response was equally important. High familial correlations (0.69–0.83), have been reported across the fasttask and Go/NoGo task and between the fast and slow conditions of the Go/NoGo task, indicating performance on these cognitive indices measure the same two underlying factors (Wood et al., 2011). This is useful as it allow combination of data from similar, but not identical paradigms to increase power to detect associations with these unitary constructs. The use of composite variables is also beneficial to reduce the total number of variables in the analysis, limiting the potential burden of multiple testing corrections. On this basis standard deviation of reaction time, or ‘reaction time variability’ (RTV), was selected as the sole reaction time variable over mean reaction time (MRT), as RTV has consistently stronger and more robust associations with ADHD. Performance data was combined into three composite variables of omission errors (Oe), commission errors (Ce), and RTV. Missing data existed for some 21 1035697 MSc. Dissertation cognitive variables due to two teams not using the go/no-go task, two not using the fast-task, and due to occasional technical issues with data collection. Due to the limited number of cases with cognitive data available, efforts were taken to utilise the largest amount of data possible. Where data from both conditions was present a composite score using means from both conditions was used, however where a score from only one condition was available this score was used in place of a composite score to conserve the amount of data available for analysis. A total of 338 probands had at least one cognitive performance index available (Table 1). Table 1. Composition of cognitive performance variables and number of cases available for each index. Composite Variable Condition contributing to composite variable Number of cases Number of cases with data available with data available by condition in final composite score Omission errors (Oe) Go/NoGo fast Go/NoGo slow 249 244 249 Commission errors (Ce) Go/NoGo fast Go/NoGo slow 249 244 249 Std. deviation of reaction time (RTV) Go/NoGo slow fast-task baseline 244 147 277 Total number of cases with at least one cognitive performance indicator available 338 Genotyping & QC process Standard IMAGE genotyping and quality control (QC) procedures were applied to the data prior to conducting the polygenic analysis. These are fully documented in Neale et al. (2008), but a summary is provided here for convenience. Perlegen 600K genotype platform was used to genotype DNA samples extracted from blood. Samples were stored at the Rutgers depository and QC was undertaken by The National Centre for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) using the GAIN QA/QC Software Package (Version 0.7.4). Cases were excluded where call rate was <87% (this threshold was chosen based on the distribution of missingness), gender discrepancy existed between genotype and phenotype data, sample heterozygosity 22 1035697 MSc. Dissertation <32% or a per-family Mendelian error of >2%. SNP QC exclusion criteria was Mendel errors >4, duplicate sample discordance (>1/15), Hardy–Weinberg disequilibrium (P<0.000001), and call rate conditional on minor allele frequency (MAF). In family-based association analysis the major allele can appear over- transmitted, due the errors in calling minor alleles. During QC extra care was taken to examine the relationship between minor allele frequency (MAF) and call rate in association testing. As a result, three criteria were adopted for QC call-rate thresholds: 0.01≤MAF<0.05 with a call rate of ≥99%; 0.05≤MAF<0.10 with a call rate of ≥97%; and 010≥MAF with a call rate of ≥95%. For a complete account of this process see Neale et al. (2008). After this procedure a mild inflation of the major allele remained (222, 089 to 208, 838 SNPs), thus a number of post-QC procedures, described in detail later, was undertaken to correct for this bias. TDT Testing and Covariates Family based associations were tested using a transmission/disequilibrium test (TDT) (Spielman et al., 1993) implemented in PLINK (Purcell et al., 2007). TDT is a method of directly testing associations between disorders and genetic marker in family data through the comparison of transmitted alleles. The test requires at least one parent to be heterozygous for a risk allele, and that the marker allele is in linkage disequilibrium (LD) in the sample population. Generation of the test statistic is based on the frequency of transmission of the risk allele to the affected probands from parents, against transmission of another allele. This technique can be applied to parent-proband trio data and does not require data from other unaffected/affected siblings, making this method well suited to applications involving disorders which may have low-penetrance within families. Unlike other methods for association testing, TDT tests does not suffer artifactual associations as a product of population stratification. The use of principle components to control for population stratification was therefore not required in this analysis. However, some phenotypic differences did exist between collection sites, the largest of these differences being a significantly higher mean age at some sites over others. To control for these differences, participants in the training sample were randomised to subsets stratified by site, so that each subset consisted of an equal numbers of cases from each site. There were no significant differences in mean age or gender between the subsets. As an additional step, age, gender and dummy coded site variables were included as covariates in the TDT analysis. 23 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Pseudo-controls and Imputation Where trio data is available and transmitted/untransmitted alleles are known, up to three potential ‘pseudo-controls’ can be generated at each locus derived from the alternative combinations of parental alleles, based upon the conditional likelihood output from TDT. Although multiple pseudo-controls can be generated, the use of a single pseudo-control derived from both untransmitted alleles have been shown to be most efficient (Cordell and Clayton, 2002), and was the only iteration generated for this analysis. The advantage of a case/pseudo-control design is that it allows parentproband trio data to be to be analysed in a similar way to case-control data, and provides the necessary format to conduct the polygenic analysis. The limitation of this method over a case-control samples is a small reduction in power (Cordell et al., 2004), however the convenience of not requiring a sample of screened control trios makes this approach justified for this initial pilot investigation. Imputation was carried out for ungenotyped SNPs and missing data. Due to limitations of conditional logistic regression this step was necessary to allow standard statistical packages to be used for the analysis. Generation of this data can be performed accurately as SNPs often have high levels of LD. In this study, imputation of missing data in parent-proband trios and generation of the pseudo-controls was conducted prior to the polygenic analysis as part of the ADHD GWA meta-analysis conducted by Neale and colleagues (2010). In summary, HapMap Phase III European CEU and TSI samples were used as reference sets (Thorisson et al., 2005, The International HapMap Comsortium, 2003). Trios were phased in Beagle, then pseudo-controls generated from resulting likelihood estimates of transmitted and non-transmitted alleles, using a haplotype relative risk test (Knapp et al., 1993). Imputation was then conducted for both case and pseudo-controls in Beagle (Browning and Browning, 2009) and tested for accuracy using a logistic regression model in MACH2DAT (Moskvina et al., 2009). Poorly imputed SNPS were excluded from the dataset before analysis. Polygenic Signal Associations of genetic markers with ADHD symptom scores on the PACS were created in the training sample. SNP associations with high symptom scores were split by major and minor alleles and ranked by odds ratio, creating two SNP 24 1035697 MSc. Dissertation lists sorted by association strength. As a polygenic signal comprises of many nominal associations, it will by nature contain many false associations unrelated to the phonotypical trait, on account of the huge number of association tests that have been carried out. As the effect of common genetic variants on risk is very small, as with GWA studies the detection of robust associations is dependent on the power provided by large sample sizes. As sample size increases, more true associations “rise to the top” showing stronger associations with the phenotype, making it increasing likely that they will contribute to the polygenic signal. Therefore, the size of the training sample is key in creating a positive polygenic signal. Following this, within any given data set, the quality of associations included in the polygenic signal can be managed through adoption of inclusion thresholds. At increasingly lenient thresholds a greater number of associations will be included in the polygenic signal but these associations have a greater likelihood of being statistical artefact unrelated to the trait. As a consequence of inclusion these associations will have a net effect of reducing the predictive power of the polygenic signal. Conversely, adopting too stringent a threshold may also reduce predictive power by excluding too many true associations. Therefore, to optimise the number of associations used to generate the polygenic signal, a range of thresholds was tested in the training set using a crossvalidation procedure. Arbitrary thresholds of 0.1%, 1%, 10% and 100% of associations were tested, using equal numbers of major and minor alleles from the ranked SNP lists. For example, associations included in the 1% threshold were comprised of the top 1% of SNPs from the major allele list and the top 1% from the minor allele list. As a consequence of stratifying associations by major and minor alleles, the total number of SNPs included in the 100% threshold was twice the total number of minor alleles, rather than every association detected during TDT. Cross-validation was conducted by dividing the training set into five equal subgroups. Output from TDT from four training subsets were combined to create a polygenic signal based in ADHD trait scores. A linear regression model was then used to test if this four-subset polygenic signal was able to predict ADHD trait scores in the fifth unused training subset. Further iterations were run with different combinations of four subsets being tested on the fifth, until all subgroups had been tested. The mean effect size across all five iterations provides an index of the thresholds’ predictive power for ADHD traits. This cross-validation was conducted for each association threshold (0.1%, 1%, 10% and 100%), following which the best performing threshold was selected for the generation of a polygenic signal using the entire training sample. A linear regression model was then used to apply this 25 1035697 MSc. Dissertation polygenic signal for ADHD to the test set for the prediction of performance on the cognitive variables of RTV, Ce, Oe, and IQ. Major Allele Frequency Family-based association studies show a systematic basis for major allele transmission in genotype samples, due to allele calling bias. Heterozygous alleles are harder to call accurately resulting in more missing data for minor alleles, particularly those with low minor allele frequency (MAF). The resultant over- transmission of major alleles causes artefact in family-based association studies (Hirschhorn and Daly, 2005, Cutler et al., 2001, Gordon et al., 2002), but may be mitigated in case-control samples if the effect on the association statistic is the equal in both groups (Clayton et al., 2005). However, under a case/pseudo-control design, the pseudo-control data is imputed free of calling error, and thus the major-allele over-transmission bias appears in case samples only. If uncorrected, any polygenic signal generated from this data will be dominated by the major allele overtransmission in cases rather than being based on ADHD traits scores. This analysis attempts to address this problem three-fold. Firstly, the IMAGE QC procedures described previously sorted SNPs by MAF and set higher call-rate thresholds for SNPs with the lower MAF (e.g. 0.01≤MAF<0.05, call rate of ≥99%). Adoption of more stringent thresholds for low MAF SNPs should have reduced the proportions of inaccurately called SNPs in the case genotype data. Secondly, the conditional likelihood imputation step carried out for case data, and the generation of pseudo-controls will replace any missing data, including that from poorly called minor alleles, balancing up a proportion of the major allele bias. Thirdly, a conservative approach was adopted during SNP selection for the polygenic score. Top associations in the TDT analysis were ranked and stratified by major and minor alleles, and equal numbers of minor and major alleles then selected for each threshold. This method ensured that associations with major and minor allele contributed equally to the polygenic signal, preventing any remained major allele over-transmission effects from dominating the polygenic signal. 26 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Results Results for the cross-validation in the training set indicated no threshold produced a polygenic signal predictive of ADHD trait scores in the training sample at a p<0.05 level (Figure 1). Variance explained by the signal was less than 0.5% in all cases. The 0.1% threshold was the worst preforming, while the 1%, 10% and 100% thresholds performed similarly. The 10% threshold was marginally more significant than the 1% and 100% thresholds (p=0.17, against p=0.18), and despite the overall non-significance was carried forward to generate the polygenic signal for testing against the training set. However, this signal did not significantly predict cognitive performance for the indexes of Oe, Ce, RTV or IQ (Figure 2). Variance explained by the signal in each case was negligible. 1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 r^2 0.5 p=0.18 p=0.17 1% 10% 0.4 p=0.18 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 p=0.77 0.1% 100% Figure 1. Variance of ADHD symptom scores explained by a polygenic signal derived from four percentage thresholds of major and minor SNP associations included in the signal. No threshold significantly predicted ADHD symptom scores during cross-validation. 27 1035697 MSc. Dissertation 1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 r^2 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 p=0.62 0.1 p=0.83 0 Oe Ce p=0.96 p=0.93 RTV IQ Figure 2. Variance for cognitive variables explained by a polygenic signal for ADHD symptom scores using the best performing threshold of 10% of associated major and minor SNPs. The polygenic signal did not significantly predict scores on any cognitive performance index. Discussion This study was not able to generate a positive polygenic signal for ADHD after testing four different association thresholds in the training set (0.1%, 1%, 10% and 100% of all associations). The best performing threshold of 10% was unable to predict cognitive performance in the test set. The non-significant polygenic predictor, explaining less than 0.5% of the variance for ADHD trait scores in the training set, indicates a lack of power in the training set. The aim of this investigation was to predict cognitive performance using a polygenic signal for ADHD. In doing so, this investigation also aimed to directly test the common genetic variant model for ADHD. As it was not possible to generate a positive polygenic signal in this analysis, this investigation was unable to directly address these aims. It is important to highlight that the failure to detect a polygenic signal for ADHD in this study does not provide evidence for a rare-variant model over the common genetic variant model in ADHD. Nor is it possible to make comment regarding the relationships between those genetic variants conferring risk of ADHD and indexes of cognitive performance. Rather, it implies that methodological limitations is the most likely candidate for the lack of a polygenic signal, and only following improvements to the design might the method of polygenic analysis be in a position to provide in evidence in support of either model. Nevertheless, this pilot study provides a valuable first test of polygenic 28 1035697 MSc. Dissertation analysis methods in ADHD, which will aid evaluation of specific polygenic methodologies for use in subsequent investigations. Power The predictive power of a polygenic score is based upon the strength of the association detected with TDT. Like GWA studies, sample size is critical to produce strong associations given the very small effect size of each individual variant on disease risk. The failure to produce a positive polygenic signal in this study is probably due to a lack of power in the training sample. For contrast, the International Schizophrenia Consortium (Purcell et al., 2009) used a case-control sample of nearly 7000 to produce a polygenic signal explaining around 3% of the variance in schizophrenia. Although the phenotypes are not comparable, as ADHD and schizophrenia may have substantially different genetic aetiologies, it does imply that training sample size may be the primary limitation in this pilot investigation. However, the data from the cross-validation threshold testing procedure remains valuable as it represents the first example of effect sizes for a polygenic signal in ADHD. Using the effect size from the best performing threshold (10%), power calculations were carried out (Figure 3). These indicated that this pilot study had 65% power to generate a positive polygenic predictor of ADHD at a p<0.05 level, with a training set of 464 trios (equivalent to a 928 case-control sample). Extrapolating from this, these calculations predict that 95% power can be achieved at a sample size of 2500, equivalent to 1250 trios using the pseudo-control design. This demonstrates that a polygenic approach in ADHD is feasible using existing ADHD GWA samples and that an immediate future goal should be to repeat this analysis with a larger training sample to attempt to generate a positive polygenic predictor for ADHD. Furthermore, it is known that the pseudo-control design provides less power to detect associations over a case-control design (Cordell et al., 2004). While adoption of this method was justified for this initial pilot investigation, use of casecontrol sample in future could yield increased predictive power in the polygenic signal. Hence, predictions of sample sizes presented here from case/pseudo-control data may be overestimated. 29 1035697 MSc. Dissertation 95% power: n=2473 predicted 80% power: n=1415 predicted Pilot: effective n=928 (464 trios) Figure 3. Power calculation using the effect size from the best performing polygenic signal for ADHD trait scores in the training sample. This indicates that this pilot study was underpowered, and gives estimates of the training sample required to give 80% and 95% power to generate a predictive polygenic signal for ADHD at the p=0.05 level, assuming case-control design. Under the pseudo-control design, the number of trios required would be half these estimates for case-control designs. Limitations This pilot study suffered some methodological limitations due to the characteristics of the data used in the investigation. However, the outcomes remain valuable in providing evidence for the efficacy of the methodology used and enable this investigation to offer recommendations for methodological improvements for future polygenic follow-up studies. Use of ADHD Symptom Counts to Generate Polygenic Signals Using ADHD symptom counts to generate the polygenic signal may have been a limiting factor in this investigation. The ADHD probands in this sample had a limited range of ADHD trait scores. Possible ADHD trait scores on the PACS range from 0 to 90, although the proband sample had a range of 42-90 (Q1=71; Q3=84). Pseudo-controls were assumed to have a score of 0. It is logical that future studies may wish to examine the predictive power of an ADHD polygenic signal on quantitative trait scores for ADHD symptoms. Such questions may shed light upon the relationships between ADHD risk genes and symptom counts, and may imply 30 1035697 MSc. Dissertation general additive or more precise systemic models of action by the cumulative effect of ADHD risk genes. However, these questions would not have been possible to address with this analysis due to the limited range of trait scores available, and because the pseudo-controls were prescribed an arbitrary trait score of 0. In studies wishing to address questions related to quantitative trait scores, use of a screened case-control sample with a representative range of ADHD symptom scores would be the optimum design. Furthermore, use of ADHD symptom scores to generate the polygenic signal in this analysis created a confound as the strength of the genetic variants associated with ADHD would have been weighted by this score, which displayed both a ceiling effect and a negative skew on the distribution. Additionally, these trait scores are unlikely to be a product of just genetic risk factors; as environmental factors will also meditate the quantity of expressed symptoms. Therefore, a better approach in view of this limitation would have been to use ADHD status as a categorical case-control definition for the TDT. Using ADHD status would have been more robust as it would not influence the association scores by weighting, and is free from confounds not related to genetic liability which may influence trait scores. This method is therefore recommended for subsequent analysis for the generation of the polygenic signal in the training set. For the study of ADHD quantitative trait scores, a viable approach would be to generate the polygenic signal using ADHD status in the training set but then apply this signal to predict ADHD trait scores in the test set, providing a sufficient range of scores is available. Major Allele Bias The IMAGE sample has been shown to have an over-transmission bias for major-alleles, common in family based analysis due to inaccurate minor allele calling. Without correction, this would have resulted in a systematic bias in the polygenic signal for the major allele. To mitigate this potential risk, this study incorporated three procedures to counter the major allele bias: strict QC, imputation and use of equal numbers of major and minor allele within each polygenic signal. However, the frequencies of the major and minor alleles in the sample were similar following imputation (588999 major alleles to 586100 minor alleles). This suggests that the imputation step used as part of the case/pseudo-control design was successful in removing much of the bias, and that the stratification of SNPs by major and minor alleles was unnecessary. Although this study lacked power, the outcomes would recommend imputation through the case/pseudo-control procedure as an effective 31 1035697 MSc. Dissertation counter to major-allele bias. Further, the bias is argued to be less of a problem in case-control samples, providing it is equally present for both cases and controls (Clayton et al., 2005). Therefore, combining imputation with case-control samples should make subsequent polygenic analysis studies resistant to major-allele overtransmission bias without the need to adopt additional correction procedures. Case/Pseudo-control As discussed, screened case-control samples have an advantage over case/pseudo-control samples for polygenic analysis, particularly for addressing research questions regarding the prediction of ADHD trait scores from genetic liability. However, despite some limitations imposed by using pseudo-controls in this data set, power calculations undertaken as part of this investigation imply that this method may be successful at generating a positive polygenic signal with a relatively modest expansion of the training sample (700-1250 trios required). Therefore use of pseudo-controls should still be considered a viable method to utilise existing trio samples for polygenic analysis where screened case-control data is unavailable. Future Directions The first goal of a follow-up investigation is to increase the size of the training set, using screened case-control data, which may help mitigate some of the primary limitations of this pilot study. Analyses are planned using part of the multi-consortium sample used for the largest ADHD GWA to data (Neale et al., 2010). Around 3000 cases and 2500 independent controls are available (of which around 900 cases come from the IMAGE sample). From the power estimates presented here, this should provide an ample training sample for the generation of a positive polygenic score, significantly improving the amount of variance explained and the predictive power of the ADHD signal. If successful in producing a polygenic signal predictive for ADHD in the training sample, this study may then be able to demonstrate that common genetic variants play a role in conferring some ADHD risk. Following successful generation of a polygenic signal for ADHD, the next goal of this extension will be to rerun the analysis to test of the prediction of cognitive performance variables in ADHD. Successful predictions may indicate that some part of the genetic risk for ADHD operates through the genetic liability underlying cognitive abilities. However, it would be necessary to demonstrate that the genetic liability for ADHD operates through the endophenotype, as opposed to it being an independent 32 1035697 MSc. Dissertation epiphenomenon arising from the same common genetic variants (Kendler and Neale, 2010). In light of this, follow up work could look at the mediation of ADHD risk by the variance for cognitive factors, which may in turn offer evidence to validate or falsify various models of potential relationships between the endophenotypes of ADHD and common comorbidities (Rommelse et al., 2011). Examining the relationships between ADHD, comorbidities and cognitive endophenotypes in this way will specifically help researchers to understand the pathways from genetic risk variants to cognitive impairments and other behaviours. An alternative progression may be to explore if different parts of the polygenic signal for ADHD are predictive of cognitive outcomes. For example, as familial influence of IQ and RTV in ADHD has been shown to be largely independent (Wood et al., 2011), it might be predicted that different parts of the signal (i.e. a different profile of risk associations) would be predictive of different cognitive outcomes. These developments may be valuable to identify regions of interest or novel candidates for candidate gene studies in relation to genes underlying the ADHD linked cognitive endophenotypes, without the requirement to obtain larger GWA samples with associated cognitive performance data. Conclusions In conclusion, this pilot investigation was unsuccessful in generating a significant polygenic predictor of ADHD using a training set of 464 parent-proband trios and matched pseudo-controls. However, this study yielded useful effect sizes, allowing power calculations to be carried out which indicated that the generation of a polygenic signal for ADHD appears to be feasible using currently available ADHD GWA data. Further, this study trialled a number of methodologies for use in polygenic analysis, allowing recommendations for future study design to be made. Firstly, strict QC in conjunction with imputation for missing case data was found to be successful as correcting for the major-allele over transmission bias commonly found in family based association testing. Secondly, use of an ADHD status score, over ADHD trait scores, would have been the better technique to adopt during TDT to generate the polygenic signal, as it would have been more robust in relation to non-genetic confounds influencing the expression of symptoms. Thirdly, case-control data might improve power and may mitigate problems with major allele over-transmission, as well as offering the potential to examine relationships between genetic risk and ADHD trait scores, if the range of scores is representative. However, it has been demonstrated here that for specific investigations not related to ADHD trait scores, 33 1035697 MSc. Dissertation with methodological improvements, the case/pseudo-control design may upscale effectively and would remain useful where case-control data in unavailable. The goal of planned future investigations is to produce a polygenic signal for ADHD using a large case-control sample, which can then be applied to predict cognitive performance. If successful, progressions of this design may look at mediation of ADHD risk by the variance associated with cognitive variables or isolation of specific parts of the polygenic signal that are associated with difference cognitive performance indicators. 34 1035697 MSc. Dissertation References Andreou, P., Neale, B. M., Chen, W., Christiansen, H., Gabriels, I., Heise, A., Meidad, S., Muller, U. C., Uebel, H., Banaschewski, T., Manor, I., Oades, R., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Sham, P., Steinhausen, H. C., Asherson, P. & Kuntsi, J. 2007. Reaction time performance in ADHD: improvement under fast-incentive condition and familial effects. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1703-15. Apa 2000. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-IV-TR, Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association. Apa. 2010. DSM-5: Options Being Considered for ADHD [Online]. Arlington, Va.: American Psychiatric Association. Available: http://www.dsm5.org/Proposed%20Revision%20Attachments/APA%20Option s%20for%20ADHD.pdf [Accessed 28/07/2011]. Applegate, B., Lahey, B. B., Hart, E. L., Biederman, J., Hynd, G. W., Barkley, R. A., Ollendick, T., Frick, P. J., Greenhill, L., Mcburnett, K., Newcorn, J. H., Kerdyk, L., Garfinkel, B., Waldman, I. & Shaffer, D. 1997. Validity of the age-of-onset criterion for ADHD: a report from the DSM-IV field trials. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 36, 1211-21. Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L. & Fletcher, K. 2002. The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 279-289. Barkley, R. A., Smith, K. M., Fischer, M. & Navia, B. 2006. An examination of the behavioral and neuropsychological correlates of three ADHD candidate gene polymorphisms (DRD4 7+, DBH TaqI A2, and DAT1 40 bp VNTR) in hyperactive and normal children followed to adulthood. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 141B, 487-498. Bauermeister, J. J., Shrout, P. E., Ramirez, R., Bravo, M., Alegria, M., MartinezTaboas, A., Chavez, L., Rubio-Stipec, M., Garcia, P., Ribera, J. C. & Canino, G. 2007. ADHD correlates, comorbidity, and impairment in community and treated samples of children and adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 35, 883-98. Bell, A. S. 2011. A Critical Review of ADHD Diagnostic Criteria: What to Address in the DSM-V. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15, 3-10. Biederman, J. 2003. Pharmacotherapy for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) decreases the risk for substance abuse: findings from a longitudinal follow-up of youths with and without ADHD. J Clin Psychiatry, 64 Suppl 11, 38. Biederman, J. 2004. Impact of comorbidity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry, 65 Suppl 3, 3-7. Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K. & Tsuang, M. T. 1991. EVIDENCE OF FAMILIAL ASSOCIATION BETWEEN ATTENTION-DEFICIT DISORDER AND MAJOR AFFECTIVE-DISORDERS. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 633-642. Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Monuteaux, M. C., Bober, M. & Cadogen, E. 2004. Gender effects on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, revisited. Biol Psychiatry, 55, 692-700. Biederman, J., Kim, J. W., Doyle, A. E., Mick, E., Fagerness, J., Smoller, J. W. & Faraone, S. V. 2008. Sexually Dimorphic Effects of Four Genes (COMT, SLC6A2, MAOA, SLC6A4) in Genetic Associations of ADHD: A Preliminary Study. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B, 1511-1518. Bradshaw, N. J., Christie, S., Soares, D. C., Carlyle, B. C., Porteous, D. J. & Millar, J. K. 2009. NDE1 and NDEL1: multimerisation, alternate splicing and DISC1 interaction. Neuroscience Letters, 449, 228-33. 35 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Brookes, K., Xu, X., Chen, W., Zhou, K., Neale, B., Lowe, N., Aneey, R., Franke, B., Gill, M., Ebstein, R., Buitelaar, J., Sham, P., Campbell, D., Knight, J., Andreou, P., Altink, M., Arnold, R., Boer, F., Buschgens, C., Butler, L., Christiansen, H., Feldman, L., Fleischman, K., Fliers, E., Howe-Forbes, R., Goldfarb, A., Heise, A., Gabriels, I., Korn-Lubetzki, I., Marco, R., Medad, S., Minderaa, R., Mulas, F., Muller, U., Mulligan, A., Rabin, K., Rommelse, N., Sethna, V., Sorohan, J., Uebel, H., Psychogiou, L., Weeks, A., Barrett, R., Craig, I., Banaschewski, T., Sonuga-Barke, E., Eisenberg, J., Kuntsi, J., Manor, I., Mcguffin, P., Miranda, A., D Oades, R., Plomin, R., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Sergeant, J., Steinhausen, H. C., Taylor, E., Thompson, M., Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P. & Johansson, L. 2006. The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: association signals in DRD4, DAT1 and 16 other genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 934-953. Browning, B. L. & Browning, S. R. 2009. A Unified Approach to Genotype Imputation and Haplotype-Phase Inference for Large Data Sets of Trios and Unrelated Individuals. American Journal of Human Genetics, 84, 210-223. Chen, W., Zhou, K., Sham, P., Franke, B., Kuntsi, J., Campbell, D., Fleischman, K., Knight, J., Andreou, P., Arnold, R., Altink, M., Boer, F., Boholst, M. J., Buschgens, C., Butler, L., Christiansen, H., Fliers, E., Howe-Forbes, R., Gabriels, I., Heise, A., Korn-Lubetzki, I., Marco, R., Medad, S., Minderaa, R., Muller, U. C., Mulligan, A., Psychogiou, L., Rommelse, N., Sethna, V., Uebel, H., Mcguffin, P., Plomin, R., Banaschewski, T., Buitelaar, J., Ebstein, R., Eisenberg, J., Gill, M., Manor, I., Miranda, A., Mulas, F., Oades, R. D., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Sergeant, J., Sonuga-Barke, E., Steinhausen, H. C., Taylor, E., Thompson, M., Faraone, S. V. & Asherson, P. 2008. DSMIV combined type ADHD shows familial association with sibling trait scores: a sampling strategy for QTL linkage. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 147B, 1450-60. Clayton, D. G., Walker, N. M., Smyth, D. J., Pask, R., Cooper, J. D., Maier, L. M., Smink, L. J., Lam, A. C., Ovington, N. R., Stevens, H. E., Nutland, S., Howson, J. M., Faham, M., Moorhead, M., Jones, H. B., Falkowski, M., Hardenbol, P., Willis, T. D. & Todd, J. A. 2005. Population structure, differential bias and genomic control in a large-scale, case-control association study. Nat Genet, 37, 1243-6. Cordell, H. J., Barratt, B. J. & Clayton, D. G. 2004. Case/pseudocontrol analysis in genetic association studies: A unified framework for detection of genotype and haplotype associations, gene-gene and gene-environment interactions, and parent-of-origin effects. Genetic Epidemiology, 26, 167-85. Cordell, H. J. & Clayton, D. G. 2002. A unified stepwise regression procedure for evaluating the relative effects of polymorphisms within a gene using case/control or family data: application to HLA in type 1 diabetes. American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 124-41. Corvin, A., Craddock, N. & Sullivan, P. F. 2010. Genome-wide association studies: a primer. Psychological Medicine, 40, 1063-1077. Cutler, D. J., Zwick, M. E., Carrasquillo, M. M., Yohn, C. T., Tobin, K. P., Kashuk, C., Mathews, D. J., Shah, N. A., Eichler, E. E., Warrington, J. A. & Chakravarti, A. 2001. High-throughput variation detection and genotyping using microarrays. Genome Res, 11, 1913-25. Daly, M. J., Weiss, L. A., Shen, Y. P., Korn, J. M., Arking, D. E., Miller, D. T., Fossdal, R., Saemundsen, E., Stefansson, H., Ferreira, M. a. R., Green, T., Platt, O. S., Ruderfer, D. M., Walsh, C. A., Altshuler, D., Chakravarti, A., Tanzi, R. E., Stefansson, K., Santangelo, S. L., Gusella, J. F., Sklar, P., Wu, B. & Consortium, A. 2008. Association between microdeletion and microduplication at 16p11.2 and autism. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 667-675. 36 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Dempster, E. L., Mill, J., Craig, I. W. & Collier, D. A. 2006. The quantification of COMT mRNA in post mortem cerebellum tissue: diagnosis, genotype, methylation and expression. Bmc Medical Genetics, 7, -. Doyle, A. E., Faraone, S. V., Seidman, L. J., Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Waldman, I. D., Pennington, B. F., Peart, J. & Biederman, J. 2005. Are endophenotypes based on measures of executive functions useful for molecular genetic studies of ADHD? J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 46, 774-803. Dudbridge, F. & Gusnanto, A. 2008. Estimation of significance thresholds for genomewide association scans. Genetic Epidemiology, 32, 227-234. Elia, J. & Devoto, M. 2007. ADHD genetics: 2007 update. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 9, 434-9. Elia, J., Gai, X., Xie, H. M., Perin, J. C., Geiger, E., Glessner, J. T., D'arcy, M., Deberardinis, R., Frackelton, E., Kim, C., Lantieri, F., Muganga, B. M., Wang, L., Takeda, T., Rappaport, E. F., Grant, S. F. A., Berrettini, W., Devoto, M., Shaikh, T. H., Hakonarson, H. & White, P. S. 2010. Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 637-646. Faraone, S. V. & Biederman, J. 2005. What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 9, 384-91. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Mennin, D., Wozniak, J. & Spencer, T. 1997. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with bipolar disorder: a familial subtype? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 36, 1378-87; discussion 138790. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J. & Monuteaux, M. C. 2000. Toward guidelines for pedigree selection in genetic studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Genetic Epidemiology, 18, 1-16. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J. & Monuteaux, M. C. 2001. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with bipolar disorder in girls: further evidence for a familial subtype? J Affect Disord, 64, 19-26. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Mick, E., Murray, K., Petty, C., Adamson, J. J. & Monuteaux, M. C. 2006. Diagnosing adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Are late onset and subthreshold diagnoses valid? American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1720-1729. Faraone, S. V., Lasky-Su, J., Biederman, J., Laird, N., Tsuang, M., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W. & Lange, C. 2007. Evidence for an association of the dopamine D5 receptor gene on age at onset of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Annals of Human Genetics, 71, 648-659. Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A. & Sklar, P. 2005. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry, 57, 1313-23. Franke, B., Neale, B. M. & Faraone, S. V. 2009. Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Human Genetics, 126, 13-50. Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I. C., Rasmussen, P., Kadesjo, B., Soderstrom, H., Rastam, M., Johnson, M., Rothenberger, A. & Niklasson, L. 2004. Co-existing disorders in ADHD -- implications for diagnosis and intervention. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 13 Suppl 1, I80-92. Gizer, I. R., Ficks, C. & Waldman, I. D. 2009. Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Human Genetics, 126, 51-90. Glessner, J. T., Wang, K., Cai, G., Korvatska, O., Kim, C. E., Wood, S., Zhang, H., Estes, A., Brune, C. W., Bradfield, J. P., Imielinski, M., Frackelton, E. C., Reichert, J., Crawford, E. L., Munson, J., Sleiman, P. M., Chiavacci, R., Annaiah, K., Thomas, K., Hou, C., Glaberson, W., Flory, J., Otieno, F., Garris, M., Soorya, L., Klei, L., Piven, J., Meyer, K. J., Anagnostou, E., Sakurai, T., Game, R. M., Rudd, D. S., Zurawiecki, D., Mcdougle, C. J., Davis, L. K., Miller, J., Posey, D. J., Michaels, S., Kolevzon, A., Silverman, J. M., Bernier, R., Levy, S. E., Schultz, R. T., Dawson, G., Owley, T., Mcmahon, W. M., 37 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Wassink, T. H., Sweeney, J. A., Nurnberger, J. I., Coon, H., Sutcliffe, J. S., Minshew, N. J., Grant, S. F., Bucan, M., Cook, E. H., Buxbaum, J. D., Devlin, B., Schellenberg, G. D. & Hakonarson, H. 2009. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature, 459, 569-73. Gordon, D., Finch, S. J., Nothnagel, M. & Ott, J. 2002. Power and sample size calculations for case-control genetic association tests when errors are present: application to single nucleotide polymorphisms. Hum Hered, 54, 2233. Gottesman, Ii & Gould, T. D. 2003. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry, 160, 636-45. Haslam, N., Williams, B., Prior, M., Haslam, R., Graetz, B. & Sawyer, M. 2006. The latent structure of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a taxometric analysis. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 639-647. Hirschhorn, J. N. & Daly, M. J. 2005. Genome-wide association studies for common diseases and complex traits. Nat Rev Genet, 6, 95-108. Ingason, A., Rujescu, D., Cichon, S., Sigurdsson, E., Sigmundsson, T., Pietilainen, O. P., Buizer-Voskamp, J. E., Strengman, E., Francks, C., Muglia, P., Gylfason, A., Gustafsson, O., Olason, P. I., Steinberg, S., Hansen, T., Jakobsen, K. D., Rasmussen, H. B., Giegling, I., Moller, H. J., Hartmann, A., Crombie, C., Fraser, G., Walker, N., Lonnqvist, J., Suvisaari, J., TuulioHenriksson, A., Bramon, E., Kiemeney, L. A., Franke, B., Murray, R., Vassos, E., Toulopoulou, T., Muhleisen, T. W., Tosato, S., Ruggeri, M., Djurovic, S., Andreassen, O. A., Zhang, Z., Werge, T., Ophoff, R. A., Rietschel, M., Nothen, M. M., Petursson, H., Stefansson, H., Peltonen, L., Collier, D., Stefansson, K. & St Clair, D. M. 2011. Copy number variations of chromosome 16p13.1 region associated with schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry, 16, 17-25. International Schizophrenia Consortium 2008. Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications increase risk of schizophrenia. Nature, 455, 237-41. Itsara, A., Cooper, G. M., Baker, C., Girirajan, S., Li, J., Absher, D., Krauss, R. M., Myers, R. M., Ridker, P. M., Chasman, D. I., Mefford, H., Ying, P., Nickerson, D. A. & Eichler, E. E. 2009. Population analysis of large copy number variants and hotspots of human genetic disease. American Journal of Human Genetics, 84, 148-61. Jensen, P. S., Martin, D. & Cantwell, D. P. 1997. Comorbidity in ADHD: Implications for research, practice, and DSM-V. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1065-1079. Johnson, K. A., Wiersema, J. R. & Kuntsi, J. 2009. What would Karl Popper say? Are current psychological theories of ADHD falsifiable? Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 15. Kalscheuer, V. M., Fitzpatrick, D., Tommerup, N., Bugge, M., Niebuhr, E., Neumann, L. M., Tzschach, A., Shoichet, S. A., Menzel, C., Erdogan, F., Arkesteijn, G., Ropers, H. H. & Ullmann, R. 2007. Mutations in autism susceptibility candidate 2 (AUTS2) in patients with mental retardation. Human Genetics, 121, 501-9. Karam, R. G., Bau, C. H. D., Salgado, C. a. I., Kalil, K. L. S., Victor, M. M., Sousa, N. O., Vitola, E. S., Picon, F. A., Zeni, G. D., Rohde, L. A., Belmonte-De-Abreu, P. & Grevet, E. H. 2009. Late-onset ADHD in adults: Milder, but still dysfunctional. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 697-701. Kebir, O., Tabbane, K., Sengupta, S. & Joober, R. 2009. Candidate genes and neuropsychological phenotypes in children with ADHD: review of association studies. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 34, 88-101. Kendler, K. S. & Neale, M. C. 2010. Endophenotype: a conceptual analysis. Mol Psychiatry, 15, 789-97. Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Barkley, R., Biederman, J., Conners, C. K., Demler, O., Faraone, S. V., Greenhill, L. L., Howes, M. J., Secnik, K., Spencer, T., Ustun, T. B., Walters, E. E. & Zaslavsky, A. M. 2006. The prevalence and correlates 38 1035697 MSc. Dissertation of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 716-723. Klein, C., Wendling, K., Huettner, P., Ruder, H. & Peper, M. 2006. Intra-subject variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry, 60, 108897. Knapp, M., Seuchter, S. A. & Baur, M. P. 1993. The haplotype-relative-risk (HRR) method for analysis of association in nuclear families. American Journal of Human Genetics, 52, 1085-93. Kuntsi, J., Andreou, P., Ma, J., Borger, N. A. & Van Der Meere, J. J. 2005. Testing assumptions for endophenotype studies in ADHD: reliability and validity of tasks in a general population sample. Bmc Psychiatry, 5, 40. Kuntsi, J., Eley, T. C., Taylor, A., Hughes, C., Asherson, P., Caspi, A. & Moffitt, T. E. 2004. Co-occurrence of ADHD and low IQ has genetic origins. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 124B, 41-47. Kuntsi, J., Neale, B. M., Chen, W., Faraone, S. V. & Asherson, P. 2006a. The IMAGE project: methodological issues for the molecular genetic analysis of ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2, 27. Kuntsi, J., Rogers, H., Swinard, G., Borger, N., Van Der Meere, J., Rijsdijk, F. & Asherson, P. 2006b. Reaction time inhibition, working memory and 'delay aversion' performance: genetic influences and their interpretation. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1613-1624. Kuntsi, J., Wood, A. C., Rijsdijk, F., Johnson, K. A., Andreou, P., Albrecht, B., AriasVasquez, A., Buitelaar, J. K., Mcloughlin, G., Rommelse, N. N. J., Sergeant, J. A., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Uebel, H., Van Der Meere, J. J., Banaschewski, T., Gill, M., Manor, I., Miranda, A., Mulas, F., Oades, R. D., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Steinhausen, H. C., Faraone, S. V. & Asherson, P. 2010. Separation of Cognitive Impairments in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Into 2 Familial Factors. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 11591167. Kuntsi, J., Wood, A. C., Van Der Meere, J. & Asherson, P. 2009. Why cognitive performance in ADHD may not reveal true potential: Findings from a large population-based sample. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 570-579. Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Mcburnett, K., Biederman, J., Greenhill, L., Hynd, G. W., Barkley, R. A., Newcorn, J., Jensen, P., Richters, J. & Et Al. 1994. DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Am J Psychiatry, 151, 1673-85. Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Lee, S. S. & Willcutt, E. 2005. Instability of the DSM-IV Subtypes of ADHD from preschool through elementary school. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 62, 896-902. Lasky-Su, J., Anney, R. J. L., Neale, B. M., Franke, B., Zhou, K., Maller, J. B., Vasquez, A. A., Chen, W., Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Banaschewski, T., Ebstein, R., Gill, M., Miranda, A., Mulas, F., Oades, R. D., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Sergeant, J., Sonuga-Barke, E., Steinhausen, H. C., Taylor, E., Daly, M., Laird, N., Lange, C. & Faraone, S. V. 2008. GenomeWide Association Scan of the Time to Onset of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B, 1355-1358. Lesch, K. P., Timmesfeld, N., Renner, T. J., Halperin, R., Roser, C., Nguyen, T. T., Craig, D. W., Romanos, J., Heine, M., Meyer, J., Freitag, C., Warnke, A., Romanos, M., Schafer, H., Walitza, S., Reif, A., Stephan, D. A. & Jacob, C. 2008. Molecular genetics of adult ADHD: converging evidence from genomewide association and extended pedigree linkage studies. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 1573-1585. Levy, F., Hay, D. A., Mcstephen, M., Wood, C. & Waldman, I. 1997. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 36, 737-44. 39 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Lucki, I. 1998. The spectrum of behaviors influenced by serotonin. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 151-162. Mccarthy, S. E., Makarov, V., Kirov, G., Addington, A. M., Mcclellan, J., Yoon, S., Perkins, D. O., Dickel, D. E., Kusenda, M., Krastoshevsky, O., Krause, V., Kumar, R. A., Grozeva, D., Malhotra, D., Walsh, T., Zackai, E. H., Kaplan, P., Ganesh, J., Krantz, I. D., Spinner, N. B., Roccanova, P., Bhandari, A., Pavon, K., Lakshmi, B., Leotta, A., Kendall, J., Lee, Y. H., Vacic, V., Gary, S., Iakoucheva, L. M., Crow, T. J., Christian, S. L., Lieberman, J. A., Stroup, T. S., Lehtimaki, T., Puura, K., Haldeman-Englert, C., Pearl, J., Goodell, M., Willour, V. L., Derosse, P., Steele, J., Kassem, L., Wolff, J., Chitkara, N., Mcmahon, F. J., Malhotra, A. K., Potash, J. B., Schulze, T. G., Nothen, M. M., Cichon, S., Rietschel, M., Leibenluft, E., Kustanovich, V., Lajonchere, C. M., Sutcliffe, J. S., Skuse, D., Gill, M., Gallagher, L., Mendell, N. R., Craddock, N., Owen, M. J., O'donovan, M. C., Shaikh, T. H., Susser, E., Delisi, L. E., Sullivan, P. F., Deutsch, C. K., Rapoport, J., Levy, D. L., King, M. C. & Sebat, J. 2009. Microduplications of 16p11.2 are associated with schizophrenia. Nat Genet, 41, 1223-7. Mcloughlin, G., Albrecht, B., Banaschewski, T., Rothenberger, A., Brandeis, D., Asherson, P. & Kuntsi, J. 2010. Electrophysiological evidence for abnormal preparatory states and inhibitory processing in adult ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6, -. Mcloughlin, G., Ronald, A., Kuntsi, J., Asherson, P. & Plomin, R. 2007. Genetic support for the dual nature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Substantial genetic overlap between the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive components. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 999-1008. Merwood, A. & Asherson, P. in press. ADHD. A lifespan genetic perspective. Advances in Mental Health and Intellectual Disabilities, 5, 38-46. Mick, E., Todorov, A., Smalley, S., Hu, X., Loo, S., Todd, R. D., Biederman, J., Byrne, D., Dechairo, B., Guiney, A., Mccracken, J., Mcgough, J., Nelson, S. F., Reiersen, A. M., Wilens, T. E., Wozniak, J., Neale, B. M. & Faraone, S. V. 2010. Family-based genome-wide association scan of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 49, 898905 e3. Moskvina, V., Craddock, N., Holmans, P., Nikolov, I., Pahwa, J. S., Green, E., Owen, M. J. & O'donovan, M. C. 2009. Gene-wide analyses of genome-wide association data sets: evidence for multiple common risk alleles for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder and for overlap in genetic risk. Mol Psychiatry, 14, 252-60. Murphy, K. & Barkley, R. A. 1996. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults: Comorbidities and adaptive impairments. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 393-401. National Institutes of Health 2000. National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference Statement: diagnosis and treatment of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 39, 182-93. Neale, B. M., Lasky-Su, J., Anney, R., Franke, B., Zhou, K. X., Maller, J. B., Vasquez, A. A., Asherson, P., Chen, W., Banasehewski, T., Buitelaar, J., Ebstein, R., Gill, M., Miranda, A., Oades, R. D., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Sergeant, J., Steinhausen, H. C., Sonuga-Barke, E., Mulas, F., Taylor, E., Laird, N., Lange, C., Daly, M. & Faraone, S. V. 2008. Genome-Wide Association Scan of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B, 13371344. Neale, B. M., Medland, S. E., Ripke, S., Asherson, P., Franke, B., Lesch, K. P., Faraone, S. V., Nguyen, T. T., Schafer, H., Holmans, P., Daly, M., Steinhausen, H. C., Freitag, C., Reif, A., Renner, T. J., Romanos, M., Romanos, J., Walitza, S., Warnke, A., Meyer, J., Palmason, H., Buitelaar, J., 40 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Vasquez, A. A., Lambregts-Rommelse, N., Gill, M., Anney, R. J. L., Langely, K., O'donovan, M., Williams, N., Owen, M., Thapar, A., Kent, L., Sergeant, J., Roeyers, H., Mick, E., Biederman, J., Doyle, A., Smalley, S., Loo, S., Hakonarson, H., Elia, J., Todorov, A., Miranda, A., Mulas, F., Ebstein, R. P., Rothenberger, A., Banaschewski, T., Oades, R. D., Sonuga-Barke, E., Mcgough, J., Nisenbaum, L., Middleton, F., Hu, X. L., Nelson, S. & Subg, P. G. C. A. 2010. Meta-Analysis of Genome-Wide Association Studies of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 884-897. Palmason, H., Moser, D., Sigmund, J., Vogler, C., Hanig, S., Schneider, A., Seitz, C., Marcus, A., Meyer, J. & Freitag, C. M. 2010. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder phenotype is influenced by a functional catechol-Omethyltransferase variant. Journal of Neural Transmission, 117, 259-267. Polanczyk, G., De Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J. & Rohde, L. A. 2007. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 942-948. Polderman, T. J., Gosso, M. F., Posthuma, D., Van Beijsterveldt, T. C., Heutink, P., Verhulst, F. C. & Boomsma, D. I. 2006. A longitudinal twin study on IQ, executive functioning, and attention problems during childhood and early adolescence. Acta Neurol Belg, 106, 191-207. Purcell, S., Neale, B., Todd-Brown, K., Thomas, L., Ferreira, M. A., Bender, D., Maller, J., Sklar, P., De Bakker, P. I., Daly, M. J. & Sham, P. C. 2007. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, 559-75. Purcell, S. M., Wray, N. R., Stone, J. L., Visscher, P. M., O'donovan, M. C., Sullivan, P. F., Sklar, P., Ruderfer, D. M., Mcquillin, A., Morris, D. W., O'dushlaine, C. T., Corvin, A., Holmans, P. A., Macgregor, S., Gurling, H., Blackwood, D. H. R., Craddock, N. J., Gill, M., Hultman, C. M., Kirov, G. K., Lichtenstein, P., Muir, W. J., Owen, M. J., Pato, C. N., Scolnick, E. M., St Clair, D., Williams, N. M., Georgieva, L., Nikolov, I., Norton, N., Williams, H., Toncheva, D., Milanova, V., Thelander, E. F., Sullivan, P., Kenny, E., Quinn, E. M., Choudhury, K., Datta, S., Pimm, J., Thirumalai, S., Puri, V., Krasucki, R., Lawrence, J., Quested, D., Bass, N., Crombie, C., Fraser, G., Kuan, S. L., Walker, N., Mcghee, K. A., Pickard, B., Malloy, P., Maclean, A. W., Van Beck, M., Pato, M. T., Medeiros, H., Middleton, F., Carvalho, C., Morley, C., Fanous, A., Conti, D., Knowles, J. A., Ferreira, C. P., Macedo, A., Azevedo, M. H., Kirby, A. N., Ferreira, M. a. R., Daly, M. J., Chambert, K., Kuruvilla, F., Gabriel, S. B., Ardlie, K. & Moran, J. L. 2009. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature (London), 460, 748-752. Rommelse, N. N. 2008. Endophenotypes in the genetic research of ADHD over the last decade: have they lived up to their expectations? Expert Rev Neurother, 8, 1425-9. Rommelse, N. N. J., Altink, M. E., Fliers, E. A., Martin, N. C., Buschgens, C. J. M., Hartman, C. A., Buitelaar, J. K., Faraone, S. V., Sergeant, J. A. & Oosterlaan, J. 2009. Comorbid Problems in ADHD: Degree of Association, Shared Endophenotypes, and Formation of Distinct Subtypes. Implications for a Future DSM. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 793-804. Rommelse, N. N. J., Geurts, H. M., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J. K. & Hartman, C. A. 2011. A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1363-1396. Roohi, J., Montagna, C., Tegay, D. H., Palmer, L. E., Devincent, C., Pomeroy, J. C., Christian, S. L., Nowak, N. & Hatchwell, E. 2009. Disruption of contactin 4 in three subjects with autism spectrum disorder. J Med Genet, 46, 176-82. 41 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Scahill, L. & Schwab-Stone, M. 2000. Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9, 541-+. Sklar, P., Smoller, J. W., Fan, J., Ferreira, M. a. R., Perlis, R. H., Chambert, K., Nimgaonkar, V. L., Mcqueen, M. B., Faraone, S. V., Kirby, A., De Bakker, P. I. W., Ogdie, M. N., Thase, M. E., Sachs, G. S., Todd-Brown, K., Gabriel, S. B., Sougnez, C., Gates, C., Blumenstiel, B., Defelice, M., Ardlie, K. G., Franklin, J., Muir, W. J., Mcghee, K. A., Macintyre, D. J., Mclean, A., Vanbeck, M., Mcquillin, A., Bass, N. J., Robinson, M., Lawrence, J., Anjorin, A., Curtis, D., Scolnick, E. M., Daly, M. J., Blackwood, D. H., Gurling, H. M. & Purcell, S. M. 2008. Whole-genome association study of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 558-569. Smalley, S. L., Kustanovich, V., Minassian, S. L., Stone, J. L., Ogdie, M. N., Mcgough, J. J., Mccracken, J. T., Macphie, I. L., Francks, C., Fisher, S. E., Cantor, R. M., Monaco, A. P. & Nelson, S. F. 2002. Genetic linkage of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on chromosome 16p13, in a region implicated in autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 959-963. Spielman, R. S., Mcginnis, R. E. & Ewens, W. J. 1993. Transmission test for linkage disequilibrium: the insulin gene region and insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus (IDDM). American Journal of Human Genetics, 52, 506-16. Stefansson, H., Rujescu, D., Cichon, S., Pietilainen, O. P., Ingason, A., Steinberg, S., Fossdal, R., Sigurdsson, E., Sigmundsson, T., Buizer-Voskamp, J. E., Hansen, T., Jakobsen, K. D., Muglia, P., Francks, C., Matthews, P. M., Gylfason, A., Halldorsson, B. V., Gudbjartsson, D., Thorgeirsson, T. E., Sigurdsson, A., Jonasdottir, A., Bjornsson, A., Mattiasdottir, S., Blondal, T., Haraldsson, M., Magnusdottir, B. B., Giegling, I., Moller, H. J., Hartmann, A., Shianna, K. V., Ge, D., Need, A. C., Crombie, C., Fraser, G., Walker, N., Lonnqvist, J., Suvisaari, J., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Paunio, T., Toulopoulou, T., Bramon, E., Di Forti, M., Murray, R., Ruggeri, M., Vassos, E., Tosato, S., Walshe, M., Li, T., Vasilescu, C., Muhleisen, T. W., Wang, A. G., Ullum, H., Djurovic, S., Melle, I., Olesen, J., Kiemeney, L. A., Franke, B., Sabatti, C., Freimer, N. B., Gulcher, J. R., Thorsteinsdottir, U., Kong, A., Andreassen, O. A., Ophoff, R. A., Georgi, A., Rietschel, M., Werge, T., Petursson, H., Goldstein, D. B., Nothen, M. M., Peltonen, L., Collier, D. A., St Clair, D. & Stefansson, K. 2008. Large recurrent microdeletions associated with schizophrenia. Nature, 455, 232-6. Swanson, J. M., Kinsbourne, M., Nigg, J., Lanphear, B., Stefanatos, G. A., Volkow, N., Taylor, E., Casey, B. J., Castellanos, F. X. & Wadhwa, P. D. 2007. Etiologic subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Brain imaging, molecular genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 39-59. Taylor, E., Schachar, R., Thorley, G. & Wieselberg, M. 1986. Conduct disorder and hyperactivity: I. Separation of hyperactivity and antisocial conduct in British child psychiatric patients. Br J Psychiatry, 149, 760-7. The International Hapmap Comsortium 2003. The International HapMap Project. Nature, 426, 789-96. Thorisson, G. A., Smith, A. V., Krishnan, L. & Stein, L. D. 2005. The International HapMap Project Web site. Genome Res, 15, 1592-3. Tye, C., Mcloughlin, G., Kuntsi, J. & Asherson, P. 2011. Electrophysiological markers of genetic risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Expert Reviews in Molecular Medicine, 13. Uebel, H., Albrecht, B., Asherson, P., Borger, N. A., Butler, L., Chen, W., Christiansen, H., Heise, A., Kuntsi, J., Schafer, U., Andreou, P., Manor, I., Marco, R., Miranda, A., Mulligan, A., Oades, R. D., Van Der Meere, J., Faraone, S. V., Rothenberger, A. & Banaschewski, T. 2010. Performance variability, impulsivity errors and the impact of incentives as genderindependent endophenotypes for ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 51, 2108. 42 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Uhl, G. R., Drgon, T., Liu, Q. R., Johnson, C., Walther, D., Komiyama, T., Harano, M., Sekine, Y., Inada, T., Ozaki, N., Iyo, M., Iwata, N., Yamada, M., Sora, I., Chen, C. K., Liu, H. C., Ujike, H. & Lin, S. K. 2008. Genome-wide association for methamphetamine dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 345355. Valo, S. & Tannock, R. 2010. Diagnostic instability of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes: effects of informant source, instrumentation, and methods for combining symptom reports. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol, 39, 749-60. Venero, J. L., Machado, A. & Cano, J. 1991. Turnover of Dopamine and Serotonin and Their Metabolites in the Striatum of Aged Rats. Journal of Neurochemistry, 56, 1940-1948. Weiss, G., Hechtman, L., Milroy, T. & Perlman, T. 1985. Psychiatric Status of Hyperactives as Adults - a Controlled Prospective 15-Year Follow-up of 63 Hyperactive-Children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24, 211-220. Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V. & Pennington, B. F. 2005. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry, 57, 1336-46. Williams, N. M., Zaharieva, I., Martin, A., Langley, K., Mantripragada, K., Fossdal, R., Stefansson, H., Stefansson, K., Magnusson, P., Gudmundsson, O. O., Gustafsson, O., Holmans, P., Owen, M. J., O'donovan, M. & Thapar, A. 2010. Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet, 376, 1401-1408. Wood, A. C., Asherson, P., Van Der Meere, J. J. & Kuntsi, J. 2010. Separation of genetic influences on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and reaction time performance from those on IQ. Psychological Medicine, 40, 1027-37. Wood, A. C. & Neale, M. C. 2010. Twin Studies and Their Implications for Molecular Genetic Studies: Endophenotypes Integrate Quantitative and Molecular Genetics in ADHD Research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 874-883. Wood, A. C., Rijsdijk, F., Johnson, K. A., Andreou, P., Albrecht, B., Arias-Vasquez, A., Buitelaar, J. K., Mcloughlin, G., Rommelse, N. N. J., Sergeant, J. A., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Uebel, H., Van Der Meere, J. J., Banaschewski, T., Gill, M., Manor, I., Miranda, A., Mulas, F., Oades, R. D., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Steinhausen, H. C., Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P. & Kuntsi, J. 2011. The relationship between ADHD and key cognitive phenotypes is not mediated by shared familial effects with IQ. Psychological Medicine, 41, 861871. Zhou, K. X., Dempfle, A., Arcos-Burgos, M., Bakker, S. C., Banaschewski, T., Biederman, J., Buitelaar, J., Castellanos, F. X., Doyle, A., Ebstein, R. P., Ekholm, J., Forabosco, P., Franke, B., Freitag, C., Friedel, S., Gill, M., Hebebrand, J., Hinney, A., Jacob, C., Lesch, K. P., Loo, S. K., Lopera, F., Mccracken, J. T., Mcgough, J. J., Meyer, J., Mick, E., Miranda, A., Muenkel, M., Mulas, F., Nelson, S. F., Nguyen, T. T., Oades, R. D., Ogdie, M. N., Palacio, J. D., Pineda, D., Reif, A., Renner, T. J., Roeyers, H., Romanos, M., Rothenberger, A., Schafer, H., Sergeant, J., Sinke, R. J., Smalley, S. L., Sonuga-Barke, E., Steinhausen, H. C., Van Der Meulen, E., Walitza, S., Warnke, A., Lewis, C. M., Faraone, S. V. & Asherson, P. 2008. Meta-Analysis of Genome-Wide Linkage Scans of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B, 1392-1398. 43 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Appendix Appendix 1 – List of Full Gene Names CDH13 - cadherin 13 CHMP7 - CHMP family member 7 COMT - catechol-O-methyltransferase DAT1/SLC6A3 - solute carrier family 6 (neurotransmitter transporter, dopamine), member 3 DISC1 - disrupted in schizophrenia 1 DRD4 - dopamine receptor D4 DRD5 - dopamine receptor D5 HTR2A - 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) receptor 2A HTT/SLC6A4 - solute carrier family 6 (neurotransmitter transporter, serotonin), member 4 LOXL2 – lysyl oxidase-like 2 NDE1 - nudE nuclear distribution gene E homolog 1 (A. nidulans SHFM1 - split hand/foot malformation type 1 SLC9A9 - solute carrier family 9 (sodium/hydrogen exchanger), member 9 SNAP25 - synaptosomal-associated protein, 25kDa TNFRSF10A - tumor necrosis factor receptor superfamily, member 10a TNFRSF10D – tumor necrosis factor receptor superfamily, member 10d, decoy with truncated death domain 44 1035697 MSc. Dissertation Appendix 2 – List of Abbreviations 5HT – serotonin ADHD - Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder APA - American Psychiatric Association Ce - commission errors CNV - copy number variants DA – dopamine DSM-5 - Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition. DSM-III - Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition. DSM-IV - Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition. GWA – genome-wide association ICD-10 - International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Revision. IMAGE - International Multi-Centre ADHD Gene project IQ – intelligence quotient LD - linkage disequilibrium MAF - minor allele frequency MRT – mean reaction time NA – noradrenaline Oe - omission errors PACS - Parental Account of Childhood Symptom QC – quality control RT – reaction time RTV – reaction time variability i.e. standard deviation of reaction time SDQ - Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire SNP - single-nucleotide polymorphism TDT - transmission disequilibrium test WAIS - Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, fourth edition WISC - Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children, Third Edition 45