1 An Approach to Evaluating the Interaction Between Academic Performance Issues and Problem Behavior Displayed by School-Aged Children Definitions and References Kelly M. Schieltz, PhD, BCBA-D, LBA Licensed Psychologist October 21, 2014 Procedures Functional Analysis (FA or BFA) “An analysis of the purposes (functions) of problem behavior, wherein antecedents and consequences representing those in the person’s natural routines are arranged within an experimental design so that their separate effects on problem behavior can be observed and measured; typically consists of four conditions: three test conditions—contingent attention, contingent escape, and alone—and a control condition in which problem behavior is expected to be low because reinforcement is freely available and no demands are placed on the person.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) “An analysis in terms of behavioral functions (effects of responses); alternatively, an analysis in terms of functional relations (e.g., the production of pupillary constriction by light might be discussed as a pupillary reflex, but a functional analysis deals with it as a transition from one point to another on a continuous mathematical function relating pupillary diameter to light intensity).” (Catania, 1998) References Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197-209. (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 320, 1982). Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Northup, J., Cigrand, K., & Asmus, J. (1992). Brief functional assessment techniques to evaluate aberrant behavior in an outpatient setting: A summary of 79 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 713-721. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147-185. Brief Experimental Analysis of Academic Behavior (BEA) Reference Daly, E. J., III, Witt, J. C., Martens, B. K., & Dool, E. J. (1997). A model for conducting a functional analysis of academic performance problems. School Psychology Review, 26, 554-574. 2 Concurrent Operants (Choice) Assessment (CO) “Involves two or more schedules of reinforcement (e.g., FR, VR, FI, VI) that are simultaneously available. Each alternative is associated with a separate schedule or reinforcement and the organism is free to distribute behavior to the schedules.” (Pierce & Cheney, 2008) Reference Harding, J. W., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Cooper, L. J., Asmus, J., Mlela, K., & Muller, J. (1999). An analysis of choice making in the assessment of young children with severe behavior problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 63-82. Preference Assessment (PA) “When several schedules of reinforcement are available concurrently, one alternative may be chosen more frequently than others. When this occurs, we say that the organism shows a preference for that alternative.” (Pierce & Cheney, 2008) References Fisher, W. W., & Mazur, J. E. (1997). Basic and applied research on choice responding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 387-410. Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491-498. Concepts Positive Reinforcement “Occurs when a behavior is followed immediately by the presentation of a stimulus that increases the future frequency of the behavior in similar conditions.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) “The response-produced presentation of positive reinforcers (or the increase or maintenance of responding resulting from this operation). Reinforcers are stimuli (e.g., food); reinforcement is an operation (e.g., presentation of food given a response) or process. The operation reinforces responses, not organisms; organisms are sometimes said to be rewarded, but this term often implies effects of stimuli other than reinforcing effects. Earlier in its history, reinforcement was also applied to presentation of the US in respondent conditioning, but that usage is now unusual. A stimulus is a positive reinforcer if its presentation increases responding that produces it.” (Catania, 1998) 3 Negative Reinforcement “A stimulus whose termination (or reduction in intensity) functions as reinforcement.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) “The response-produced termination of negative reinforcers (or the increase or maintenance of responding resulting from this operation). Reinforcers are stimuli (e.g., food); reinforcement is an operation (e.g., presentation of food given a response) or process. The operation reinforces responses, not organisms; organisms are sometimes said to be rewarded, but this term often implies effects of stimuli other than reinforcing effects. Earlier in its history, reinforcement was also applied to presentation of the US in respondent conditioning, but that usage is now unusual. A stimulus is a negative reinforcer if its removal increases responding that terminates or postpones it.” (Catania, 1998) Motivating Operations (MOs) “An environmental variable that (a) alters (increases or decreases) the reinforcing or punishing effectiveness of some stimulus, object, or event; and (b) alters (increases or decreases) the current frequency of all behavior that has been reinforced or punished by that stimulus, object, or event.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) References Gewirtz, J. L., & Baer, D. M. (1958a). Deprivation and satiation of social reinforcers as drive conditions. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 57, 165-172. Gewirtz, J. L., & Baer, D. M. (1958b). The effect of brief social deprivation on behaviors for a social reinforcer. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 56, 49-56. Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407-414. Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149-155. Michael, J. (1983). Evocative and repertoire-altering effects of an environmental event. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2, 19-21. Michael, J. (1988). Establishing operations and the mand. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 6, 3-9. Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191-206. Michael, J. (2000). Implications and refinements of the establishing operation concept. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 401-410. Abolishing Operations (AOs) “A motivating operation that decreases the reinforcing effectiveness of a stimulus, object, or event. For example, the reinforcing effectiveness of food is abolished as a result of food ingestion.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) 4 Establishing Operations (EOs) “A motivating operation that establishes (increases) the effectiveness of some stimulus, object, or event as a reinforcer. For example, food deprivation establishes food as an effective reinforcer.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) “Formally, an establishing operation is defined as any change in the environment that alters the effectiveness of some stimulus or event as reinforcement and simultaneously alters the momentary frequency of the behavior that has been followed by that reinforcement. Thus, an establishing operation has two major effects: it increases the momentary effectiveness of reinforcers supporting operant behavior; and it increases the momentary probability of operants that have produced such reinforcement. For example, the most common establishing operation is deprivation for primary reinforcement. This procedure has two effects. First, food becomes an effective reinforcer for any operant that produces it. Second, behavior that has previously resulted in getting food becomes more likely.” (Pierce & Cheney, 2008) Behavioral Economic Theory “The use if economic concepts (price, substitute commodity, etc.) and principles (e.g., marginal utility) to predict, control, and analyze the behavior of organisms in choice situations.” (Pierce & Cheney, 2008) “Behavioral economics…provides a bridge between prospect theory and the study of steady-state operant behavior.” (Francisco, Madden, & Borrero, 2009) References Green, L., & Freed, D. E. (1993). The substitutability of reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 141-158. Hursh, S. R., Madden, G. J., Spiga, R., DeLeon, I. G., & Francisco, M. T. (2013). The translational utility of behavioral economics: The experimental analysis of consumption and choice. In G. J. Madden, W. V. Dube, T. D. Hackenberg, G. P. Hanley, & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis: Volume 2, Translating Principles into Practice (pp. 191-224). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Francisco, M. T., Madden, G. J., & Borrero, J. (2009). Behavioral economics: Principles, procedures, and utility for applied behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst Today, 10, 277-294. Madden, G. J., Bickel, W. K., & Jacobs, E. A. (2000). Three predictions of the economic concept of unit price: In a choice context. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 73, 45-64. 5 Complements “Reinforcers are consumed at a constant ratio (e.g., one tortilla chip to 5 grams guacamole, one left shoe to one right shoe). A complementary relation between reinforcers is demonstrated if the price of one reinforcer increases (e.g., the price of guacamole doubles) and consumption of both reinforcers (chips and guacamole) decreases, despite no increase in the price of chips. Such a decrease reveals the tendency of the two commodities to be consumed in a constant ratio.” (Hursh, Madden, Spiga, DeLeon, & Francisco, 2013) “Two reinforcers that are classified as complements are those that are typically purchased and consumed together. For example, food and water, toothbrushes and toothpaste, and left and right shoes tend to be purchased and “consumed” together. Importantly, if consumption of one reinforcer (e.g., potato chips) declines when its price is increased, then consumption of a complement (e.g., chip dip) will also decline even though the latter has not been subject to a price increase.” (Francisco, Madden, & Borrero, 2009) Substitutes “A substitute reinforcer, as the name implies, is one that is functionally similar and is readily traded for the other reinforcer. Common examples of substitutes include nickels and dimes, staples and paper clips, etc. When the price of one reinforcer increases, its consumption will decrease more if a substitute is available.” (Francisco, Madden, & Borrero, 2009) Extinction “The discontinuing of a reinforcement of a previously reinforced behavior (i.e., responses no longer produce reinforcement); the primary effect is a decrease in the frequency of the behavior until it reaches a prereinforced level or ultimately ceases to occur.” (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007) “In operant behavior, discontinuing the reinforcement of responding (or the reduction in responding that it produces). In negative reinforcement (escape and avoidance), extinction has often referred to the discontinuation of aversive stimuli, although the term applies more appropriately to discontinuing the consequences of responding, so that aversive stimuli occur but responses no longer prevent them.” (Catania, 1998) Other References Cited Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Pierce, W. D. & Cheney, C. D. (2008). Behavior analysis and learning (4th ed.). New York, NY: Psychology Press.