Attitude Measurement Slide 1 Attitude measures are common in many marketing surveys. The goal of this lecture is to define what we mean by an attitude and show different ways in which attitudes may be measured. Slide 2 This cartoon is meant to reinforce the notion that measuring people’s attitudes is a dicey proposition. At best, we hope that the answers that they provide to our attitude questions are somewhat reflective of the way they think. At worst, the answers that they give us, especially if the questions are ill conceived, can be quite squirrelly. Slide 3 (No Audio) Slide 4 The key notion here, in terms of the definition of attitude, is that it’s enduring, and therefore reasonably stable. If I ask you today about your attitude toward this course, and then I ask you next week about your attitude toward this course, your answers should be fairly consistent. Slide 5 Attitudes are hypothetical constructs in the sense that marketing researchers assume that such constructs exist. By assuming they exist and by measuring them, marketers seemingly are better able to predict people’s subsequent behaviors. Here, the definition of hypothetical constructs suggests that they are not observable directly; they must be measured by indirect means. In the case of attitudes, those indirect means are either verbal expression or overt behaviors. We can infer somebody’s attitudes about something—based on what they say or what they indicate on a questionnaire—or by how they act. Slide 6 The problem with attitudes as a hypothetical construct relates to causality. Marketers assume that attitudes are predictive of people’s future behaviors. The psychological literature also suggests that behaviors tend to influence people’s attitudes. For example, my requiring you to attend class or to work through these online lectures regularly may subsequently induce more favorably feelings towards the class because you have to justify to yourself that you repeatedly attend. Seemingly, attitudes are causes of behaviors, but behaviors also are causes of attitudes. Because a thing cannot be both a cause and an effect of the same other thing, attitudes are problematic from a scientific perspective. That said, as one of my finance professors at Purdue used to say, “In the kingdom of the blind, the one eyed man is king.” This maxim suggests that although attitudes are imperfect indicators of people’s subsequent behaviors, they provide one of the better tools to forecast those behaviors. As one of the better tools, they’re used by researchers. Slide 7 Psychologists and marketers think of attitudes algebraically and the next several slides will show what I mean by that phrase. Page | 1 Slide 8 Marketers assume that attitudes have three components. One component is the affective component, which refers to people’s feelings or emotions towards an object. Slide 9 The second component of attitudes is the cognitive component, which sometimes is referred to as neutral cognitions. The cognitive component entails people’s beliefs or knowledge about the world independent about how they feel about those beliefs. For example, I’ve certain beliefs about the fuel efficiency of my car, and I’ve different beliefs about the number of channels I receive through my DirecTV and Tivo configuration. (Love that MLB package.) Cognitions are merely the facts of the world as people understand them. Slide 10 The third component of attitudes is the behavioral component, which addresses issues about predispositions to act in a certain way, or people’s intentions to act a certain way, or people’s expectations about acting a certain way. These intentions or expectations can be about things as simple as ice cream or things as complex as a home or automobile. Slide 11 For those of you who prefer graphic representation, this slide summarizes what I’ve just said. An attitude, which is a hypothetical construct, is comprised of three sub-constructs; affect, the way we feel about something; cognition, what we believe about that thing; and behavioral intentions, what we intend to do about that thing. At the bottom of the slide, there are different indicators for these three sub-constructs. Those indicators are what we measure with our questionnaire items. Slide 12 Some of you may recall the hierarchy-of-effects model from a class on consumer behavior or a class on advertising. This model assumes that people move from one state to the next in sequence. First, people become aware that a product exists and that awareness can be measured, either through aided or unaided recall. After they become aware, then they learn about the attributes of that product—those neutral cognitions or factual knowledge. Once they learn about that product, then they begin to form feelings towards it; hence, the affect component. Then, they begin to like or dislike that product, which may become their first choice or part of the set of products they might consider on their next purchase occasion. The next step is purchase intentions. If people know of a product, know something about it, and have some feelings about it, then they may intend to purchase it on their next purchase occasion. Finally, people tend to act on those intentions, or at least that’s what this model forecasts. Note that the three components of attitudes are represented by the middle three boxes in the figure: knowledge, affect, and intentions. Slide 13 This hierarchy-of-affects model assumes that people drop out along the way, and that’s what is illustrated in this slide. Marketers can think about all users of a product, people who are aware of a brand, people who prefer that brand, people who last bought that brand, and people who are satisfied with that brand. This sequence is hierarchical. People first become aware, then they become knowledgeable, then they have certain feelings, and then they have intentions to buy. They may have purchased the brand the previous time and they—based on purchase Page | 2 experience—tend to purchase it on the next occasion. The reason I present this hierarchy is because I want to stress the importance of measuring all three components of attitudes. It’s insufficient to measure awareness only, although without awareness people can’t have opinions on facts about the brand or feelings towards the brand or intentions to buy the brand. Marketing researchers can measure people’s knowledge about a brand and that’s critical because sometimes people are misinformed. Researchers can measure people’s liking of a brand because that may be reflective of their subsequent behaviors. Researchers can measure people’s intentions to purchase a brand because the closer the measure to people’s behaviors, the more predictive that measure should be of those behaviors. Clearly, the name of the game for marketing researchers is to predict the behaviors of potential customers accurately. Slide 14 Although many theories you learned in other marketing classes are relevant to marketing research, they are relevant especially for designing attitudinal measures. Here’s an example: store loyalty may be conceived as an attitude or as a behavior. When we think of it as an attitude, we typically measure store loyalty in terms of multiple store attributes, such as people’s attitudes towards merchandising displays, assortment of goods, and value for the money. However, we also can conceive of store loyalty in strictly behavioral terms, such as the frequency that people visit a store. For example, if I tend to shop regularly at Dillard’s—if I went there last week and the week before and the week before—then I’m also considered store loyal strictly based on my behaviors; my attitudes towards Dillard’s are not considered. Hence, marketing researchers who want to measure store loyalty could conceive of it in two very different ways and design two very different types of measures. If conceived as an attitude, then the appropriate measures entail people’s impressions about a store’s attributes; if conceived as a behavior, then the appropriate measures entail the frequency with which people visit that store. Slide 15 I’d like to take this notion of theory a bit further. In the next several slides, I’ll discuss the notion of conceptual and operational definitions. Before I can talk about a conceptual definition, I must define concept, which is a generalized idea about a class of objects, attributes, occurrences, or processes. Clearly, concept is a mental construct rather than a physical construct. Slide 16 As a conceptual definition relates to how we conceive of a concept, an operational definition is more mechanical. It specifies what the researcher must measure. Operational definitions are really measurement definitions, and conceptual definitions are really theoretical definitions. Slide 17 To be certain that it’s clear what I mean by an operational definition, here’s an example that’s unrelated to attitudes: Who should be included as a household member? You may think that’s a relatively straightforward notion, but you can see there are many issues about who should and who shouldn’t count as a household member for census-taking purposes. Slide 18 Here’s an example of conceptual and operational definitions that are more related to attitudes. The example here is media skepticism, which is a hypothetical psychological construct that marketing researchers measure. Here, media skepticism is defined as the degree to which Page | 3 people are skeptical about the reality presented by mass media. Seemingly, skepticism varies among people: from people who are mildly skeptical and accept most of what they see and hear, to people who completely discount and disbelieve whatever is presented in mass media. Slide 19 Armed with this conceptual definition of media skepticism, what type of question might a marketing researcher include on a questionnaire? Here’s an example of one possibility: Please tell me how true each statement is about the media. Is it very true, not very true, or not at all true? Then here are the three statements: (1) The program was not very accurate in its portrayal of the problem. (2) Most of the story was staged for entertainment purposes. (3) The presentation was slanted and unfair. This example shows that the construct media skepticism requires multiple items to be measured. It’s not the type of construct that can be assessed with one simple question, such as “How skeptical are you about the accuracy of what’s portrayed in the media?” People don’t tend to respond well to those types of global questions, any more than they could indicate their impression of a store by answering a single question, such as “What do you think the image of Dillard’s is?” When researchers try to assess store image, they explore many issues, such as value for the dollar, quality of the merchandising display, et cetera. Note from these examples that marketing researchers often are required to develop multi-item measures when they explore attitudes in particular and hypothetical constructs in general. Slide 20 This slide shows a single-item conceptual versus operational definition, which relates to favorability towards a brand and a predisposition to purchase it on subsequent occasions. Slide 21 As this slide summarizes, specifying the conceptual and operational definitions for attitude are the first two steps in developing sound attitude measures. Once we have those measures, we can then use them to collect data and then we can analyze the quality of those measures to assess the underlying construct. I’ll talk more about steps 3, 4, and 5 in subsequent lectures. Slide 22 (No Audio) Slide 23 How can a marketing researcher assess people’s attitudes? A researcher can ask people in one of four ways: (1) To rank a set of competing objects, indicating which they prefer most to which they prefer least (2) To rate one or more objects on a set of characteristics Page | 4 (3) To sort several objects on a characteristic, which is somewhat like ranking but it requires a physical rearrangement of things. (4) To provide their choices: first preferred, second preferred, and third preferred choice. Slide 24 Ranking tasks are best performed over a small number of objects. Respondents asked to rank 25 different makes of automobiles from their most preferred to their least preferred probably could indicate—with some degree of reliability—their most preferred, their second most preferred, and their least preferred. Any rankings between the top two and bottom one are likely to be unreliable. To make those in-between rankings reliable, researchers typically limit respondents to ranking only a few things; as a rule of thumb, let’s say roughly a half dozen. Ranking more than a half dozen things will result in unreliable rankings for other than the most and least preferred things. Slide 25 Researchers can ask respondents to rate many more things than they rank because when rating something, the respondents need only to consider one thing at a time. Slide 26 Sorting, to a large extent, is similar to ranking. The advantage to sorting—as a mechanical task—is that we can ask people to sort many more objects than we can ask them to rank. Thus, sorts tend to be more reliable than ranking for a large number of objects. Slide 27 Although it may not seem so initially, choice tasks are a type of attitude measurement because we can infer people’s attitudes from their choices. Marketers assume that the brand chosen as most preferred is the one about which the respondent has the most positive attitudes and the brand selected as least preferred is the one about which the respondent has the least favorable attitudes. Slide 28 To recap, the goal of this lecture was to first define attitudes, in terms of their three components: affect, cognitive, and behavioral intentions. The second goal was to stress the importance of theory in designing sound attitude measurements. The third goal was to define both conceptual and operational definitions, show how they differ and show how they relate to one another. The final goal was to indicate different ways that marketing researchers use to measure attitudes: ranking, rating, sorting, or choice approaches. Page | 5