Child Engagement: An Important Factor in Child Learning What is Engagement? Engagement refers to the amount of time a child is actively participating in an activity in a contextually appropriate manner (Bailey & Wolery, 1992; McWilliam & Bailey, 1992, 1995; McWilliam, Trivette, & Dunst, 1995; Risley & Twardosz, 1976). Contextually based learning refers to learning that takes place in everyday, real-life settings and activities that provide a child the basis for acquiring functionally meaningful and culturally relevant behavior. Contextually based learning is learning that occurs in the same place where the behavior that is learned is typically used. The following are examples of child engagement in typical activities: An infant sitting in a high chair who is eating bites of cereal placed on the high chair tray, A child who is pointing to a picture in a book during a shared reading activity, A child in a sand box who is shoveling sand into a bucket, Two or more children involved in peer-mediated interactions, and Children on a playground involved in some type of play activity. Engagement is typically manifested in terms of intense involvement in an activity, persistence in completing a task, intense concentration, fascination with people or materials, and absorption in play. Why is Engagement Important? Engagement is important because unless children are actively involved in different kinds of learning opportunities, there is little or no opportunity to practice existing skills or to learn new skills nor is there an opportunity for adults to use systematic instructional practices to support child learning. In contrast, when children are actively involved in activities that are contextually appropriate, not only do children have the opportunity to practice existing skills and learn new skills, but adults have the opportunity to interact with the children in ways that encourage and support the children’s learning. Figure 1 shows an example of one way in which this process works. The foundation of the model is interest-based learning opportunities. The way in which children’s interests (choices, preferences, etc.) function as a factor (Bronfenbrenner, 1993; Wachs, 2000) that influences learning and development can be explained as follows: Persons, objects, and events that are interesting to children are what captures and maintains their attention (Fogel, 1997), encourages them to interact with people and objects (Rusher, Cross, & Ware, 1995), and promotes participation in social and nonsocial activities (Göncü, Tuermer, Jain, & Johnson, 1999). Interest-based playing, interaction, and participation provide a starting point for children’s learning through their engagement (McWilliam & Ware, 1994) in everyday activities. When children are actively engaged in everyday activities, they have the opportunity to practice existing 1 skills, perfect emerging skills, and acquire new competence (Farver, 1999). Activities that afford children opportunities to express competence are ones that are more likely to encourage and support child learning and exploration (Wachs, 1979). As a result of exploration, children come to learn the relationship between their behavior and its consequences, thereby enhancing and strengthening their sense of mastery (MacTurk & Morgan, 1995). A sense of mastery, in turn, reinforces existing and promotes new interests, encouraging yet further engagement with people and materials. How do Recommended Practices Impact Engagement? The majority of the child-level DEC Recommended Practices either explicitly or implicitly emphasize child engagement as the context for both child learning opportunities and for adults to use instructional practices to promote child learning in the activities. An example from the Instruction Recommended Practices that explicitly focuses on child engagement is “Practitioners use explicit feedback and consequences to increase child engagement, play, and skills.” Another example from the Instruction Recommended Practices that implicitly focuses on engagement is “Practitioners plan for and provide the level of support, accommodations, and adaptations needed for the child to access, participate, and learn within and across activities and routines” where engagement is implied by the opportunities for children to participate fully in activities. Figure 1 also shows how the appropriate use of Recommended Practices influences children’s engagement in learning activities. What is the Research Evidence? Research indicates that higher levels of child engagement with adults, peers, and materials during the early childhood years are related to the learning of children both with and without disabilities (e.g., Blair, Fox, & Lentin, 2010; Duda, Dunlap, Fox, Lentini, & Clarke, 2004; Fantuzzo, Sekino, & Cohen, 2004; Greenwood, Carta, & Dawson, 2000; McWilliam & Bailey, 1992, 1995; Palermo, Hanish, Martin, Fabes, & Reiser, 2007; Strain, Danko, & Kohler, 1995;Williford, Vick Whittaker, Vitiello, & Downer, 2013). Research also demonstrates that among children at-risk for poor outcomes there is a link between engagement and children’s learning. Hart and Risley (1995), for example, 2 documented the cumulative effect of high levels of engagement, multiple learning opportunities, and positive consequences on children’s language learning. Focusing on increasing the level of children’s engagement, therefore, is a useful outcome for the ECTA work with States that is likely to enhance the learning of children with disabilities. References Bailey, D. B., Jr., & Wolery, M. (1992). Teaching infants and preschoolers with disabilities (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill. Blair, K.-S. C., Fox, L., & Lentini, R. (2010). Use of positive behavior support to address the challenging behavior of young children within a community early childhood program. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 30(2), 68-79. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Duda, M. A., Dunlap, G., Fox, L., Lentini, R., & Clarke, S. (2004). An experimental evaluation of positive behavior support in a community preschool program. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 24, 143-168. Fantuzzo, J., Sekino, Y., & Cohen, H. (2004). An examination of the contributions of interactive peer play to salient classroom competencies for urban Head Start children. Psychology in the Schools, 41(3), 323-336. Farver, J. A. M. (1999). Activity setting analysis: A model for examining the role of culture in development. In A. Göncü (Ed.), Children's engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives (pp. 99-127). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Fogel, A. (1997). Information, creativity, and culture. In C. Dent-Read & P. ZukowGoldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development: Ecological approaches to organism--environment systems (pp. 413-443). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Göncü, A., Tuermer, U., Jain, J., & Johnson, D. (1999). Children's play as cultural activity. In A. Göncü (Ed.), Children's engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives (pp. 148-170). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., & Dawson, H. (2000). Ecobehavioral assessment systems software (EBASS):A system for observation in education settings. In T. Thompson, D. Felce & F. J. Symons (Eds.), Behavioral observation: Technology and applications in developmental disabilities (pp. 229-251). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes. MacTurk, R. H., & Morgan, G. A. (Eds.). (1995). Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations, and applications. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 3 McWilliam, R. A., & Bailey, D. B. (1992). Promoting engagement and mastery. In D. B. Bailey, Jr. & M. Wolery (Eds.), Teaching infants and preschoolers with disabilities. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. McWilliam, R. A., & Bailey, D. B. (1995). Effects of classroom social structure and disability on engagement. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 15, 123147. McWilliam, R. A., & Ware, W. B. (1994). The reliability of observations of young children's engagement: An application of generalizability theory. Journal of Early Intervention, 18, 34-47. McWilliam, R. A., Trivette, C. M., & Dunst, C. J. (1985). Behavior engagement as a measure of the efficacy of early intervention. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 5, 59-71. Palermo, F., Hanish, L. D., Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., & Reiser, M. (2007). Preschoolers' academic readiness: What role does the teacher-child relationship play? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 407-422. Risley, T. R., & Twardosz, S. (1976). The preschool as a setting for behavioral intervention. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of behavior modification and behavior therapy (pp. 453-474). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Rusher, A. S., Cross, D. R., & Ware, A. M. (1995). Infant and toddler play: Assessment of exploratory style and development level. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 297-315. Strain, P. S., Danko, C. D., & Kohler, F. (1995). Activity engagement and social interaction development in young children with autism: An examination of "free" intervention effects. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 3, 108-123. Wachs, T. D. (1979). Proximal experience and early cognitive-intellectual development: The physical environment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 25, 3-41. Wachs, T. D. (2000). Necessary but not sufficient: The respective roles of single and multiple influences on individual development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Williford, A. P., Vick Whittaker, J. E., Vitiello, V. E., & Downer, J. T. (2013). Children's engagement within the preschool classroom and their development of selfregulation. Early Education & Development, 24(2), 162-187 4