EEG correlates of spatial working memory deficits in ADHD: vigilance, encoding and maintenance. A. Lenartowicz12, A. Delorme345, P.D. Walshaw12, A.L. Cho12, R. M. Bilder12, J. McGough12, J.T. McCracken12, S. Makeig3, S.K. Loo12 Channel vs. IC Space Analyses While independent component analysis (ICA) of EEG data has been increasing in prevalence within the literature, the unique assumptions of ICA warrant further treatment. It is notable that the ICA decomposition results do not use any knowledge of the locations of the scalp electrodes. They find a best-fitting purely statistical model. For these reasons, we present here additional analyses to evaluate how the IC model presented in our primary analyses relates to effects at individual channels. Mid-Occipital Cluster vs. Channel Effects. To better understand how to interpret the IC model with respect to the overlying scalp channel data, we performed two additional analyses. First we assessed how much variance in selected scalp channel signals was accounted for by the projection of the ICs in the medial occipital cluster. Here we evaluated the mean variance across electrode channels Iz, O1 and O2, the three scalp channels showing strongest group main effects. The ICs accounted for 29.0% (SE=2.6) of TD time course variance and for 25.5% (SE=2.8) ADHD time course variance. While a significant contribution, these results demonstrate the expected breadth of cortical contributions to individual scalp electrode channels. In a second analysis, we evaluated to what extent the clustered ICs captured the effects that were present in the overlying channel data. For the average of the Iz, O1, and O2 channel time series, we performed the same repeated-measure ANOVA (with GROUP and LOAD as factors) as on the cluster ICs, first on the raw data and, second, after subtracting from these channel signals the projections of the mid-occipital cluster ICs. If the cluster ICs account for a significant amount of the effect, then removing their contribution from the channel data should decrease the F statistic for each effect. The results are presented in Fig. S1a. For the effects of LOAD and GROUP, we observed a decrease in the magnitude of the effect after subtracting the mid-occipital cluster contribution from the channel data (FLOAD(1,69) = 8.6 vs. 2.72; FGROUP(1,69) = 9.5 vs. 7.7). The decrease indicates that the mid-occipital cluster indeed accounted for part of the effect in the channel data. Since the F statistic remained significant, however, we inferred that other source clusters also accounted for some of these effects. We therefore in addition removed the contributions of the bilateral occipital and mid-parietal clusters, and then also the bilateral parietal and mid-central clusters. The first step eliminated the effect of LOAD (FLOAD(1,69) = 0.3), indicating that lateral occipital and superior occipital sources accounted for the remainder of its variance. Similarly, these two steps reduced the effect of GROUP (from FGROUP(1,69) = 7.2 to 5.1). However, the GROUP effect was still significant, suggesting that it had contributions from other sources. However, unlike in the IC data, the interaction between GROUP and LOAD was not significant in the channel data, F(1,69) < 1, indicating that the effect was masked in the scalp signals and was unmasked only by ICA source separation and clustering. Figure S1. The contributions of the midoccipital and mid-frontal IC clusters to the overlying electrode channel data were evaluated by comparing F statistics for effects of interest before and after the removal of cluster ICs from the channel data. For an occipital channel mean (Iz, O1, O2) (a), the F statistics for the group (green dashed) and load (brown solid trace) effects on encodingperiod alpha band ERD were attenuated but not eliminated by removal of contributions from the mid-occipital cluster. The effects were reduced or eliminated by further removing either the mid-parietal or the lateral-occipital cluster ICs, consistent with the observed multiple source contributions to the channel data. Though group by load interaction (dotted blue trace) was not significant in the channel data, in the midoccipital IC source cluster itself (not shown) it was significant. (b) For a frontal channel mean (Fz, F3, F4), the F statistics for the group and load effects on maintenance theta were considerably reduced after removing the contributions of the mid-frontal cluster, consistent with this cluster accounting for the majority of the theta-band effects observed in the supervening channel data. Mid-Frontal Cluster vs. Channel Effects. As for the mid-occipital cluster, we also assessed the effects of the mid-frontal IC cluster contribution to overlying scalp channel data. First we assessed how much variance in the most strongly projected scalp channel time series, here electrode channel Fz, was accounted for by projections of the clustered IC processes. For electrode channel Fz, the mid-frontal cluster ICs accounted for 30.4% (SE=2.0) of time course variance in TD and for 34.4% (SE=2.2) time course variance in ADHD participants. Next, as for the mid-occipital cluster, we performed the repeated-measure ANOVA (with GROUP and LOAD as factors) on the average time series across frontocentral scalp channels Fz, F3 and F4, first on the raw data and, second, after subtracting from these channels the variance accounted for by the projected activities of the mid-frontal cluster ICs. The results are presented in Fig. S1b. For the LOAD and GROUP effects, after subtracting the midfrontal cluster contribution from the channel data we observed a decrease in effect magnitude (FLOAD(1,69) = 0.9 vs. 0.4; FGROUP(1,69) = 2.1 vs. 0.2). The F statistics were small and not significant indicating that these effects were largely masked in the raw signal data and unmasked by ICA source separation. Discussion The percent variance accounted for (pvaf) in the maximally-projected scalp channel data by the clusters of interest confirms that the data recorded at scalp electrodes represent additive mixtures of source signals. As shown in Figure 2, for instance, the “occipital” cluster scalp maps (mid-, left-, right-occipital, parietal) clearly overlap, and this is congruent with an apportioning of the scalp channel variance among sources and source clusters. We show that the significant group differences are only present for one of the occipital clusters -- this is also congruent with expecting weakened group effects at individual scalp electrodes. In particular, signals (and thus, signal variance) at each occipital electrode mixes effects from the mid-occipital ICs (which showed group effects) and from lateral occipital ICs (which did not show group effects). The effects of additive mixtures of source signals were also present in the frontocentral scalp channels, in which significant effects were not reliable.