Emotional Intelligence: An Essential Part of Leadership

advertisement
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE: AN ESSENTIAL PART OF LEADERSHIP EFFECTIVENESS
PROF. SHRADDHA KULKARNI
Assistant Professor, Indira School of Business Studies, Pune, India
ABSTRACT
This paper proposes that emotions play an important role in the leadership process. More precisely, it is proposed that
emotional intelligence, the ability to understand and manage moods and emotions in the self and others, contributes to effective
leadership in organizations. Four major aspects of emotional intelligence: (i) the appraisal and expression of emotion, (ii) the
use of emotion to enhance cognitive processes, (iii) decision making, knowledge about emotions, and (iv)management of
emotions are reviewed. Researcher, subsequently, proposes how emotional intelligence contributes to effective leadership by
focusing on five essential elements of leader effectiveness: (i) development of collective goals and objectives; (ii) instilling in
others an appreciation of the importance of work activities; (iii) generating and maintaining enthusiasm, confidence, optimism,
cooperation, and trust; (iv) encouraging flexibility in decision making & change; and (v) establishing and maintaining a
meaningful identity for an organization.
KEY WORDS: Emotional Intelligence, Leadership Effectiveness
INTRODUCTION
Though extensive research has been carried out on Leadership related topics, there has been debate over the
approaches to leadership in organizational sciences. A wide variety of approaches to leadership has been projected –
researchers have analyzed the styles of leadership, their interpersonal skills and attitudes, how their styles interact with
situational conditions, and how they can make major changes in their organizations (Bass, 1990; Fiedler & House, 1994; Yukl,
1998; Yukl & Van Fleet, 1992).
The importance of leadership is understood by researchers and organizations. Study of different leadership styles and
theories help us analyze the need of leadership execution and influencing the work culture of an organization. Furthermore, the
existing literature denotes the way of how leaders behave in a particular situation, however, the role of emotions in the
leadership process, are often not explicitly considered in the leadership literature, with the notable exception of work on
charisma (e.g. Conger & Kanungo, 1998; Lindholm, 1990). This relative negligence is not astonishing as the organizational
literature has been conquered by a cognitive orientation (Ilgen & Klein, 1989), with emotional state being ignored or being
seen as something that gets in the way of wisdom and effective decision making (Albrow, 1992). Leadership theory and
research have not sufficiently considered how leaders’ moods and emotions influence their effectiveness as leaders. Two initial
studies propose that leaders’ feelings may play an important role in leadership. George and Bettenhausen (1990) found that the
1
extent to which leaders of existing work groups experienced positive moods certainly related to levels of uninterestingly
behavior performed by group members and negatively related to group turnover rates. George (1995) found that work groups
led by sales managers who tended to experience positive moods at work provided higher quality customer service than groups
led by managers who did not tend to experience positive moods at work. While these two studies help to fill a gap in the
leadership literature, in and of themselves, they do not illuminate the role of moods and emotions in the leadership process perse but rather suggest that feelings may be an important factor to consider.
The growing body of literature exploring the role of moods and emotions in human and organizational affairs (e.g.
Fineman, 1993; Forgas, 1995) suggests that, rather than being simply an additional factor to consider, feelings play a much
more central role in the leadership process. The purpose of this paper is to present a framework describing what that role might
be. First, however, it is useful to sample the literature and research findings attesting to the central role of emotions in human
affairs.
EMOTIONS AND LEADERSHIP
The literature concisely defined above is demonstrative of a much wider body of knowledge which suggests that
feelings serve multiple purposes in human affairs. As will be demonstrated below, it is likely that moods play an important role
in leadership. While George and Bettenhausen (1990) and George (1995) investigated some of the prospective helpful
significances of leader’s positive mood, it is likely that a variety of feelings (both emotions and moods) influences leadership
effectiveness. Negative moods, for example, substitute systematic and careful information processing (Sinclair, 1988; Sinclair
& Mark, 1992) and may be beneficial when leaders are dealing with complex problems in which mistakes carry high risk. As
another example, relatively intense negative emotions may correctly redirect a leader’s attention to an issue in need of
immediate attention (Frigda, 1988).
Through the discussion, it is apparent that it is not too difficult to construct scenarios in which leaders would be well
served by the experience of a variety of types of moods and emotions. Moreover, one can also construct scenarios in which a
leader’s effectiveness may be hampered by the experience of certain moods and emotions. Leaders who experience anger
frequently may have a difficult time building good relationships with followers and producing their trust (Jones & George,
1998). Likewise, a leader who frequently experiences positive moods on the job may fail to notice and attend to performance
shortfalls that are less than apparent.
Hence, this inquiry into the role of emotions in leadership is not bent on determining the ‘right’ or ‘effective’ moods
and emotions that facilitate leadership effectiveness. Leaders are obviously human beings with the full range of moods and
emotions potentially available to them. Both positive and negative moods and emotions serve numerous functions in people’s
lives. Likewise, both positive and negative moods and emotions can sometimes be the cause of human dysfunctions.
2
This paper does seek to explore, however, whether effective leaders possess certain emotional capabilities just as they may
possess certain cognitive capabilities (Bass, 1990; Kirkpatrick & Locke, 1991; Yukl, 1998). Moods and emotions play an
extensive role in thought processes and behavior (Bower, 1981; Bower & Cohen, 1982; Clark & Isen, 1982; Forgas, 1995;
George & Brief, 1992; Isen & Baron, 1991; Isen & Shalker, 1982; Isen et al., 1978; Leventhal & Tomarken, 1986; Rosenhan et
al., 1981; Teasdale & Fogarty, 1979) and the same moods and emotions can result in both improved or impaired effectiveness
depending upon multiple factors including the index of effectiveness (a quick,
heuristic-based response vs. a careful
consideration of alternatives, Salovey et al., 1993; Sinclair & Mark, 1992). Moreover, research proposes that people can and do
take steps to manage their own and others’ moods and emotions (Mayer et al., 1991; Salovey & Mayer, 1989–90). Might it be
that some leaders have superior mood/emotion capabilities which allow them to use and benefit from the variety of feelings
they experience on the job? Might it also be that these capabilities enable leaders to influence, and develop effective
interpersonal relationships with, their followers? Interpersonal relationships are loaded with moods and emotions as is effective
social influence. These mood/emotion capabilities have been addressed by emotional intelligence theory and research.
The following discusses the emotional intelligence and the theory and research which support its role in human
affairs. It is demonstrated, how emotional intelligence may be a key contributor to leadership effectiveness and outline how
different aspects of emotional intelligence facilitate the varied activities central to effective leadership. While emotional
intelligence has been linked previously to specific leader behaviors (Megerian & Sosik, 1996), this paper adopts a broader
approach and explores the multitude of ways in which emotional intelligence may contribute to leadership effectiveness.
Furthermore, it is pointed out that earlier leadership approaches, and in particular the trait approach, also have described certain
leadership skills or traits that may either be subsumed under or may partially overlap with emotional intelligence (for reviews,
see Bass, 1990; Yukl, 1998).
Moreover, while the term ‘emotional intelligence’ has been coined relatively recently, it bears some resemblance and
partially overlaps with earlier concepts such as social intelligence (Legree, 1995; Sternberg & Smith, 1985; Wong et al., 1995).
However, as Mayer, Salovey and Caruso (in press) suggest, emotional intelligence is theoretically preferable to earlier
constructs such as social intelligence because it is more focused the results.
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
Emotional intelligence includes internal, private feelings that influence functioning which may not necessarily be
linked to social skills and also focuses exclusively on emotional skills rather than confounding them with social or political
knowledge (Mayer et al., in press). Hence, emotional intelligence captures capabilities and skills in the emotion domain to a
greater extent than prior constructs.
Emotional intelligence is ‘the ability to perceive emotions, to access and generate emotions so as to assist thought, to
understand emotions and emotional knowledge, and to reflectively regulate emotions so as to promote emotional and
intellectual growth’ (Mayer & Salovey, 1997: 5). Prior to continuing, it should be pointed out that the term ‘emotional’ in
3
emotional intelligence is used broadly to refer to moods as well as emotions. So as to be consistent with the emotional
intelligence literature, in the remainder of this paper, ‘emotions’ will be used to refer to both emotions and moods.
Emotional intelligence essentially describes the ability to effectively join emotions and reasoning, using emotions to
facilitate reasoning and reasoning intelligently about emotions (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). In other words, emotional
intelligence taps into the extent to which people’s cognitive capabilities are informed by emotions and the extent to which
emotions are cognitively managed. Additionally, it should be pointed out that emotional intelligence is distinct from
predispositions to experience certain kinds of emotions captured by the personality traits of positive and negative affectivity
(George, 1996; Tellegen, 1985).
There are at least four major aspects of emotional intelligence: The appraisal and expression of emotion, the use of
emotion to enhance cognitive processes and decision making, knowledge about emotions, and management of emotions;
Appraisal and expression of emotion: The ability to perceive emotions in oneself and others as well as in objects, art, stories,
music, and other stimuli; Use of emotion to enhance cognitive processes and decision making: The ability to generate, use, and
feel emotion as necessary to communicate feelings or employ them in other cognitive processes; Knowledge about emotions:
The ability to understand emotional information, to understand how emotions combine and progress through relationship
transitions, and to appreciate such emotional meanings; Management of emotions: The ability to be open to feelings, and to
modulate them in oneself and others so as to promote personal understanding and growth. This discussion drawn from the work
of Mayer, Salovey, and their colleagues (e.g. Mayer & Salovey, 1993, 1995, 1997; Mayer et al., 1990; Salovey & Mayer,
1989–90, 1994; Salovey et al., 1993, 1995).
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND EFFECTIVE LEADERSHIP
While emotional intelligence can lead to enhanced functioning in diverse aspects of life such as achievement and close
relationships (Goleman, 1995; Salovey & Mayer, 1989–90), it has been proposed that it may play a particularly important role
in leadership effectiveness. To clarify this role, the researcher proposes how the four aspects of emotional intelligence
described above – appraisal and expression of emotion, use of emotion to enhance cognitive processing and decision making,
knowledge about emotions, and management of emotions – contribute to effective leadership.
In order to explore the implications of emotional intelligence for effective leadership, it is necessary to identify the
fundamental nature of effective leadership. This is no easy task given the plethora of leadership theories, approaches, and
empirical findings. Fortunately, several recent syntheses of the leadership literature have been offered which are consistent in
terms of their descriptions of effective leadership. Based on the syntheses of Yukl (1998), Locke (1991), and Conger and
Kanungo (1998), as well as the larger leadership literature, specific elements of leadership effectiveness can be identified. Note
that, while no specific theory of leadership is entailed in these elements, the elements themselves have roots in a variety of
theoretical traditions. As described by these authors (i.e. Conger & Kanungo, 1998; Locke, 1991; Yukl, 1998), effective
4
leadership contains the following essential features: Development of a collective sense of goals and objectives and how to go
about achieving them; Instilling in others knowledge and appreciation of the importance of work activities and behaviors;
Generating and maintaining excitement, enthusiasm, confidence, and optimism in an organization as well as cooperation and
trust; Encouraging flexibility in decision making and change; Establishing and maintaining a meaningful identity for an
organization.
The goals and objectives considered here are major, all-embracing goals that are commonly referred to as the leader’s
vision for the organization (e.g. Conger & Kanungo, 1998; Locke, 1991). Emotional intelligence may contribute to leaders
developing a compelling vision for their groups or organizations in a number of ways. First, leaders may use their emotions to
enhance their information processing of the challenges, threats, issues, and opportunities facing their organizations. Leaders are
often faced with a large amount of information characterized by uncertainty and ambiguity; out of this information, they need
to chart a course for their groups or organizations.
In terms of the AIM model (Forgas, 1995); leaders are likely to engage in substantive processing as they seek to
determine the direction for their organizations. They are dealing with complex information with high uncertainty and the desire
to be accurate. AIM model suggests that current affective state is likely to influence judgments resulting from substantive
processing through the mechanism of affect priming.
Research linking positive moods to creativity suggests that when leaders are in positive moods they may be more
creative (Isen et al., 1987) and, hence, more likely to come up with a compelling vision that contrasts with existing conditions.
For example, people in positive moods have been found to be more integrative, use broader categories, and approach problems
and categorization more flexibly (Isen & Baron, 1991; Isen & Daubman, 1984; Isen et al., 1985; Murray et al., 1990). Creating
a compelling vision for an organization can be an exercise in creativity, positive thinking, and flexibility and such an exercise
will be facilitated by positive moods (Isen et al., 1985; Murray et al., 1990). Leaders who are high on emotional intelligence
will be better able to take advantage of and use their positive moods and emotions to envision major improvements in their
organizations’ functioning.
Leaders high on emotional intelligence also are likely to have knowledge about the fact that their positive moods may
cause them to be overly optimistic. Hence, in order to ensure that they are being realistic and appropriately critical, they may be
more likely to revisit their judgments when in a more neutral or negative mood to ensure a careful consideration of all the
issues involved. Such leaders also are likely to be better able to repair negative moods arising from any number of sources that
may limit flexibility and creativity, and, more generally, use meta-mood processes to manage their moods and emotions in
functional ways (Mayer et al., 1991).
Importantly, leaders need not only to come up with a compelling vision, but also to effectively communicate it
throughout the organization in such a way that it does come to be shared and is ‘collective’. By accurately appraising how their
followers currently feel, relying on their knowledge of emotions to understand why they feel this way, and influencing
5
followers’ emotions so that they are receptive to and supportive of the leader’s goals or objectives for the organization and
proposed ways to achieve them, leaders may help to ensure that their vision is shared or collective. For example, a leader who
is high on emotional intelligence may act on emotional knowledge which suggests that followers are more likely to experience
positive emotions and be supportive of the leader’s goals and objectives when the leader expresses confidence in followers and
serves to elevate their levels of self-efficacy (Gardner & Avolio, 1998).
LIMITATION OF THE PRESENT STUDY
This study is a review of the established literature and the discussion can’t be backed with empirical testing. Clearly,
what is needed now is empirical research which tests the ideas proposed in this paper. Given the complexities of the issues
involved, both qualitative and quantitative methodologies hold promise for exploring the ways in which emotional intelligence
may contribute to leader effectiveness, as theorized in this paper. Meaningful quantitative investigations could take place in
both field and laboratory settings as well as through the use of management simulations. Additionally, given the stage of
development of theorizing and research on emotional intelligence, there are several measures of emotional intelligence that
have been developed and could be used to measure the emotional intelligence levels of research participants (e.g. Mayer et al.,
1997; Salovey et al., 1995).
CONCLUSIONS
The present analysis recommends that, at a minimum, emotions and emotional intelligence are well-intentioned of
deliberation in the leadership domain. Emotional intelligence has the probable to contribute to effective leadership in manifold
ways, some of which have been irradiated in this paper. The special relevance to leadership revolves around the fact that
leadership is an emotion-laden process, both from a leader and a follower perspective. A caveat concerning the current analysis
is that it has focused primarily on leaders and it has been argued that leadership theory and research would benefit from
consideration of a more follower-centered approach (e.g. Meindl, 1990, 1993; Meindl et al., 1985). In this regard, the study of
emotional intelligence and leadership would benefit from the consideration of emotional intelligence in followers and its
effects on the leadership process. Additionally, and from a symbolic interactionism perspective, it would be interesting to
explore how interactions between leaders and followers result in the creation and management of emotions in a work setting.
To conclude, investigating how leaders’ capabilities in the emotion domain or their emotional intelligence contribute to their
effectiveness certainly seems worthy of future empirical research and theorizing. Hopefully, the current analyses will provide
researchers with some support in this regard, this paper eventually, tries to denote that the study of leadership effectiveness has
a significance in organizational research and emotional aspect of a leader need to be given due prominence.
REFERENCES

Albrow, M. Sine ira et studio – or do organizations have feelings? Organization Studies, 1992, 13, 313–29.
6

Alvesson, M. Leadership as social integrative action: A study of a computer consultancy company. Organization Studies, 1992,
13, 185–209.

Apfel, R.J. & Sifneos, P.E. Alexithymia: Concept and measurement. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 1979, 32, 180–90.

Aronson, E. & Linder, D. Gain and loss of esteem as determinants of interpersonal attractiveness. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 1965, 1, 156–71.

Ashforth, B.E. & Humphrey, R.H. Emotion in the workplace: A reappraisal.

Human Relations, 1995, 48, 97–125.

Bass, B.M. Bass and Stogdill’s handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and managerial applications (3rd ed.). New York:
Free Press, 1990.

Batson, C.D. Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology. Vol. 20. New York: Academic

Press, 1987, pp. 65–122.

Bower, G.H. Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 1981, 36, 129–48.

Bower, G.H. Mood congruity of social judgments. In J.P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments. Elmsford, NY:
Pergamon Press, 1991, pp. 31–53.

Bower, G.H. & Cohen, P.R. Emotional influences in memory and thinking: Data and theory. In M.S. Clark and S.T. Fiske
(Eds), Affect and cognition: The Seventeenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum, 1982, pp. 291–331.

Buck, R. Individual differences in nonverbal sending accuracy and electrodermal responding: The externalizing–internalizing
dimension. In R. Rosenthal

(Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication: Individual differences. Cambridge,

MA: Olegeshlager, Gunn, & Hain, 1979, pp. 140–70.

Buck, R. The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press, 1984.

Campbell, R.J., Kagan, N.I. & Krathwohl, D.R. The development and validation of a scale to measure affective sensitivity
(empathy). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1971, 18, 407–12.

Clark, M.S. & Isen, A.M. Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In A. Hastorf and
A.M. Isen (Eds), Cognitive social psychology. New York: Elsevier, 1982, pp. 73–108.

Clark, M.S. & Waddell, B.A. Effects of moods on thoughts about helping, attraction and information acquisition. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 1983, 46, 31–5.

Clore, G.L. & Parrott, G. Moods and their vicissitudes: Thoughts and feelings as information. In J.P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion
and social judgments. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press, 1991, pp. 107–23.

Conger, J.A. & Kanungo, R.N. Charismatic leadership in organizations.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1998.

Cunningham, M.R., Steinberg, J. & Grev, R. Wanting to and having to help: Separate motivations for positive mood and guiltinduced helping. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 1980, 38, 181–92.
7

Damasio, A.R. Descartes’ error. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons, 1994.Easterbrook, J.A. The effects of emotion on cue
utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 183–200.

Ekman, P. Cross culture studies of facial expression. In P. Ekman (Ed.) Darwinand facial expression: A century of research in
review. New York: Academic

Press, 1973, pp. 169–222.

Emmons, R.A. & Colby, P.M. Emotional conflict and well-being: Relation to perceived availability, daily utilization, and
observer reports of social support.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1995, 68, 947–59.

Epstein, S. Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook ofpersonality theory and research. New York:
Guilford Press, 1990, pp.165–91.

Epstein, S. & Meier, P. Constructive thinking: A broad coping variable withspecific components. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1989,57, 332–50.

Fiedler, K. On the task, the measures and the mood in research on affect andsocial cognition. In J.P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and
social judgments. Elmsford,NY: Pergamon Press, 1991, pp. 83–104.

Fiedler, F.E. & House, R.J. Leadership theory and research: A report of progress.In C.L. Cooper and I.T. Robertson (Eds), Key
reviews in managerial psychology.New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994, pp. 97–116.

Fineman, S. (Ed.) Emotion in organizations. London: Sage, 1993.

Ford, M.E. & Tisak, M.S. A further search for social intelligence. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 1983, 75, 196–206.

Forgas, J.P. Affect in social judgments and decisions: A multi-process model. In

M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental and social psychology, Vol. 25.

San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1992a, pp. 227–75.

Forgas, J.P. On bad mood and peculiar people: Affect and person typicality in impression formation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,1992b, 62, 863–75.

Forgas, J.P. On making sense of odd couples: Mood effects on the perception of mismatched relationships. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,1993, 19, 59–71.

Forgas, J.P. Sad and guilty? Affective influences on the explanation of conflict episodes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1994, 66, 56–68.

Forgas, J.P. Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model. Psychological Bulletin,1995, 117, 39–66.

Forgas, J.P. & Bower, G.H. Mood effects on person perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1987,
53, 53–60.

Forgas, J.P., Bower, G.H. & Krantz, S.E. The influence of mood on perceptions of social interactions. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 1984,20, 497–513.

George, J.M. State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1991, 76,
299–307.

George, J.M. Leader positive mood and group performance: The case of custom service. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
1995, 25, 778–94.
8

George, J.M. Trait and state affect. In K.R. Murphy (Ed.), Individual differences and behavior in organizations. San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass, 1996, pp. 145–71.

George, J.M. & Bettenhausen, K. Understanding prosocial behavior, sales performance, and turnover: A group level analysis in
a service context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1990, 75, 698–709.

George, J.M. & Brief, A.P. Feeling good – doing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work – organizational spontaneity
relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 1992, 112, 310–29.

George, J.M. & Brief, A.P. Motivational agendas in the workplace: The effects of feelings on focus of attention and work
motivation. In B.M. Staw and

L.L. Cummings (Eds), Research in organizational behavior, Vol. 18. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1996, pp. 75–109.

Gerstner, C.R. & Day, D.V. Meta-analytic review of leader–member exchange theory: Correlates and construct issues. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 1997, 82, 827–44.

Goleman, D. Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books, 1995.

Graen, G.B. & Uhl-Bien, M. Relationship-based approach to leadership: Development of leader–member exchange (LMX)
theory over 25 years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 1995, 6, 219–47.

Harris, S.G. & Sutton, R.I. Functions of parting ceremonies in dying organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 1986,
29, 5–30.

Hochschild, A.R. The managed heart: The commercialization of human feeling.

Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, 1983.

House, R.J. Leadership in the twenty-first century. In A. Howard (Ed.), The changing nature of work. San Francisco: JosseyBass, 1995, pp. 411–555.

Ilgen, D.R. & Klein, H.J. Organizational behavior. In M.R. Rosenzweig & L.W. Porter (Eds), Annual Review of Psychology,
Vol. 40. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews, 1989, pp. 327–51.

Isen, A.M., Shalker, T.E., Clark, M. & Karp, L. Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 1–12.

Isenberg, D.J. How senior managers think. Harvard Business Review, 1984, November–December, 81–90.

Jones, G.R. & George, J.M. The experience and evolution of trust: Implications for cooperation and teamwork. Academy of
Management Review, 1998, 23, 531–46.

Kanter, R.M. The change masters. New York: Simon & Schuster, 1983.

Katz, I.M. & Campbell, J.D. Ambivalence over emotional expression and wellbeing: Nomothetic and idiographic tests of the
stress-buffering hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1994, 67, 513–24.

Katz, L. & Epstein, S. Constructive thinking and coping with laboratory induced stress. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1991, 61, 789–800.

Kavanagh, D.J. & Bower, G.H. Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and sadness on perceived capabilities. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 1985, 9, 507–25.

Kessler, R.C., Price, R.H. & Wortman, C.B. Social factors in psychopathology:

Stress, social support, and coping processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 1985, 36, 531–72.
9

King, L.A. & Emmons, R.A. Conflict over emotional expression: Psychological and physical correlates. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1990, 58, 864–77.

McCall, M.W. & Kaplan, R.E. Whatever it takes: Decision makers at work. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1985.

Mandler, G. Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley, 1975.

Mayer, J.D. How mood influences cognition. In N.E. Sharkey (Ed.), Advances in cognitive science, Vol. 1. Chichester: Ellis
Horwood, 1986, pp. 290–314.

Mayer, J.D. & Geher, G. Emotional intelligence and the identification of emotion. Intelligence, 1996, 22, 89–113.

Mayer, J.D. & Salovey, P. The intelligence of emotional intelligence. Intelligence, 1993, 17, 433–42.

Mayer, J.D. & Salovey, P. Emotional intelligence and the construction and regulation of feelings. Applied and Preventive
Psychology, 1995, 4, 197–208.

Mayer, J.D. & Salovey, P. What is emotional intelligence: Implications for educators. In P. Salovey and D. Sluyter (Eds),
Emotional development, emotional literacy, and emotional intelligence. New York: Basic Books,

1997, pp. 3–31.

Mayer, J. D., DiPaolo, M. & Salovey, P. Perceiving affective content in ambiguous visual stimuli: A component of emotional
intelligence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1990, 54, 772–81.

Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P. & Caruso, D.R. Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale. New Caanan, CT: Unpublished manuscript,
1997.

Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P. & Caruso, D. Competing models of emotional intelligence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of
human intelligence. New York: Cambridge, in press.

Salovey, P., Mayer, J.D., Goldman, S.L., Turvey, C. & Palfai, T.P. Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring
emotional intelligence using th trait meta-mood scale. In J.W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association, 1995, pp. 125–54.

Schwarz, N. Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E.T. Higgins and R.M.
Sorrentino (Eds), Handboo of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, Vol. 2. New York: Guilford Press,
1990, pp. 527–61.

Schwarz, N. & Bless, H. Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In J.P.
Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press, 1991, pp. 55–71

Singer, J.A. & Salovey, P. Mood and memory: Evaluating the network theory of affect Clinical Psychology Review, 1988, 8,
211–51.

Sternberg, R.L. & Smith, C. Social intelligence and decoding skills in nonverbal communication. Social Cognition, 1985, 3,
168–92.

Taylor, G.J. Alexithymia: Concept, measurement, and implications for treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1984, 141,
725–32.

Teasdale, J.D. & Fogarty, S.J. Differential effect of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic
memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1979, 88, 248–57.
10

Tellegen, A. Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In
A.H. Tuma and J.D. Maser (Eds), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1985, pp. 681–706.

Thayer-Singer, M. Psychological dimensions in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 1977, 28, 13–27.

Thoits, P. Social support as coping assistance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1986, 54, 416–23.

Trice, H.M. & Beyer, J.M. The cultures of work organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1993.

Van Maanen, J. & Kunda, G. ‘Real feelings’: Emotional expression and organizational culture. In B.M. Staw and L.L.
Cummings (Eds), Research in Organizational Behavior, Vol. 11. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1989, pp. 43–103.
Walker, R.E. & Foley, J.M. Social intelligence: Its history and measurement Psychological Reports, 1973, 33, 839–64.

Wasielewski, P.L. The emotional basis of charisma. Symbolic Interactionism, 1985, 8, 207–22.

Wispe, L.G. The distinction between sympathy and empathy: To call forth a concept, a word is needed. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1986, 50, 314–21.

Wong, C.T., Day, J.D., Maxwell, S.E. & Meara, N.M. A multitrait-multimethod study of academic and social intelligence in
college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1995, 87, 117–33.

Yukl, G. Leadership in organizations (4th ed.) Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1998.

Yukl, G. & Van Fleet, D.D. Theory and research on leadership in organizations. In M.D. Dunnette and L.M. Hough (Eds),
Handbook of Industrial an Organizational Psychology, Vol. 3. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1992, pp. 147–
97.
11
Download