Engagement and Motivational Processes in Reading John T. Guthrie and Susan Lutz Klauda University of Maryland College Park, USA September 2, 2013 To appear in: Afflerbach, Peter. (in press, 2013). Handbook of Individual Differences in Reading. Routledge Publishers, New York. Dr. John T. Guthrie, Jean Mullan Professor of Literacy Emeritus Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology University of Maryland College Park, USA Dr. Susan L. Klauda, Postdoctoral Fellow Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology University of Maryland College Park, USA 1 Abstract We chart the pathways to the attainment of proficiency in reading comprehension from foundation to tertiary levels. Drawing on quantitative and qualitative investigations from many countries and ethnic groups, we identify the qualities of engagement in reading that generate growth in reading achievement. Reading engagement is generated partly from the learners’ motivations, beliefs and values for books and schooling. Interacting dynamically with these motivations, the learner’s language and cognitive processes for reading a variety of materials contribute to engagement. As motivations grow, students gain the will to read. Energy from their will or motivation empowers students to learn the many skills of proficient reading. When engagement is deep and wide, students become self-initiating learners through reading. The chief contributors to engagement are the teacher and classroom. In partnership with parents and others, teachers can use five key teaching practices to build and sustain students’ engagement in reading and writing. Effective practices include: (a) arranging collaboration; (b) providing choices; (c) showing benefits; (d) assuring success; (e) fostering extended engagement. Descriptions and examples of these teaching practices and community supports for reading engagement and achievement are provided. 2 Engagement and Motivational Processes in Reading John T. Guthrie and Susan Lutz Klauda University of Maryland College Park In reading research and educational psychology, engagement and motivation are increasingly prominent issues. This chapter aims to chart the relations among achievement, engagement, motivation and cognitive processes in reading with relevance to classroom contexts and attention to individual differences. Because these constructs all are deeply researched, they can be characterized and connected in various ways. Drawing on well-established research findings, we seek to represent the linkages that hold particular promise for reading educators. In doing so, we use a mid-level of complexity. We steer a middle course between the highest refinement of constructs that is needed for research and the distillation of the constructs to simpler versions of multi-faceted factors which are more accessible for policy and education leaders. Individual differences in motivation and engagement take several forms. First, there are global differences in level of motivation. For the most motivated students, reading is a highly valued, enjoyable activity that is central to their identity. The least motivated students find reading impossibly difficult, distasteful and unimportant in their lives. This variable is positively associated with amount of reading. Students with relatively high intrinsic motivation (reading enjoyment) read three times more than students with relatively low intrinsic motivation Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). Second, different motivations typify different individuals. Some students are high in selfefficacy, believing they are very proficient readers, but do not value reading very much. Other students are keenly devoted to grades and incentives (i.e., have high extrinsic motivation) but are 3 not interested in reading for enjoyment (i.e., have low intrinsic motivation). That is, students vary not only in global level of motivation, but in the profile of their multiple specific motivations. Third, connections between specific motivations and achievement vary among students. For example, self-efficacy correlates with achievement more highly for European American than African American students (McRae, 2011. Despite individual diversity in the strengths and relations of different motivations, there are patterns; motivation links systematically with other reading variables, although exceptions are inevitable. This chapter charts those patterns. General Relations among the Factors of Motivation, Engagement, and Achievement In the ambitious Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, Eccles and Wang (2012) write a commentary entitled “So what is student engagement anyway?” They adopt the definition provided by Skinner & Pitzer (2012) that “engagement is the visible manifestation of motivation” (p. 135) while expressing the traditional view that motivation refers to the internal processes that energize and direct behavior. Emphasizing the importance of distinguishing crisply between constructs, Eccles and Wang embrace the traditional framework that motivation influences behavior, which in turn influences various outcomes, such as learning. In other words, A → B → C, where A=motivation, B=behavior, and C=learning and achievement. Reeve’s (2012) definitions of motivation and engagement agree with those of Eccles and Wang (2012) conceptually, but depart from them operationally. Reeve concurs that “motivation is a force that energizes and directs behavior” (p. 150), and that “engagement refers to the extent of students’ active involvement in a learning activity” (p. 150). He proposes and measures four aspects of engagement, but he does not measure motivation extensively. In his framework, behavioral engagement comprises attention, effort, and persistence in tasks; emotional engagement includes interest and enthusiasm; cognitive engagement involves deep mental processing and self- 4 regulation; and agentic engagement refers to proactive, intentional forms of learning. Although his measures of these four engagement constructs predict achievement well (Reeve, 2012), we believe that the roles of motivation are underrepresented in his system. Predictors and Outcomes of Reading Engagement: A Proposed Heuristic Model Model overview. This model is intended to depict patterns of individual differences in motivation and engagement as they are associated with reading achievement. In a review of the contributions of classroom contexts to various engagement-related outcomes, Guthrie, Wigfield & You (2012) proposed the following model: Classroom Contexts → Motivations → Behavioral engagement → Reading achievement. This model concurs substantively with the Eccles and Wang (2012) perspective. Our position, in accord with both Eccles and Wang (2012) and Reeve (2012), is that behavioral engagement refers to active participation as typified by effort, time, and persistence. However, in contrast to Reeve (2012), we believe that motivations such as intrinsic motivation and self-efficacy influence behavioral engagement. For example, when students participate in reading for personal interest (intrinsic motivation) and believe in their capacity (selfefficacy), their behavioral engagement becomes more enthusiastic, confident, and cognitively sophisticated. In this model, and in contrast to Reeve’s (2012) engagement framework, the indicators (measures) of engagement are distinguished from the precursors (antecedents) of engagement. In the heuristic model in Figure 1 we propose that classroom contexts influence multiple motivations and cognitions are all important contributors to engagement, and they generate myriad qualities of behavioral engagement. Behavioral engagement will vary widely in observable effort, time, persistence, enthusiasm, and cognitive involvement that underlie the behaviors. In turn, higher qualities of engagement will generate higher qualities of reading achievement as manifested 5 in fluency, vocabulary, literal comprehension, and reasoning (higher-order comprehension) performance. Behavioral engagement processes in the heuristic model. Our research suggests that engagement may be indicated by positive qualities such as a student’s enthusiasm and devotion of effort that correlate positively with achievement. In contrast, a different form of engagement may consist of active avoidance of reading, including rejecting, evading, minimizing effort, and disconnecting from reading tasks (Guthrie, Klauda & Ho, 2013). We refer to the former, positive aspects of engagement as dedication and the latter, negative aspects as avoidance. This view is similar to that of Skinner, Kindermann, and Furrer (2009) who use the terms engagement and disaffection to represent similarly distinct forms. The advantage of this duality is that the constructs authentically characterize students’ varieties of engagement, and they simultaneously contribute to predicting achievement (Guthrie et al., 2013). Motivations in the heuristic model. Motivations in reading were examined in a detailed review by Schiefele, Schaffner, Moller, and Wigfield (2012). One of their conclusions was that the intrinsic-extrinsic differentiation of motivation is quite legitimate, conceptually and empirically. Intrinsic motivation includes curiosity, or seeking new experiences or information, and involvement, or deep immersion in text. In our proposed heuristic model, we include intrinsic motivation due to its prominent role in predicting acquisition of reading, reading competence, and reading engagement in the forms of effort, time spent, and amount of reading (Guthrie, Wigfield, Metsala, & Cox, 1999). Schiefele and colleagues proposed that self-efficacy and value represent preconditions of intrinsic motivation rather than actual motivations (Schiefele, et. al, 2012). Although conceivable, there is little empirical support for this notion. In our heuristic model we include the motivational construct of self-efficacy, or one’s belief in her capacity to perform 6 reading tasks.. Self-efficacy is conceptually relevant because it energizes behavior and is highly correlated with achievement from grades K-12 , suggesting its integral role in reading development. Similarly, we incorporate the motivation of value in reading, which refers to belief in the importance of reading because it is useful or interesting. Beyond its self-evident relevance, value correlates with reading competence increasingly as students progress through grades K-8 (Ho & Guthrie, 2013). Finally in this heuristic model, we embrace a social construct, which refers to positive behaviors toward peers and sharing favorable beliefs with classmates about reading. This construct is rooted in socialization theories of education that propose that fostering social relationships among students and between teachers and students are central to students’ acquisition of academic dispositions and competencies (Eccles & Wang, 2012). We have not included the widely researched construct of extrinsic motivation due to its typically negative correlations with achievement, nor the constructs from goal theory, such as mastery goals, which have been studied mainly with secondary students (Pintrich, 2010). Cognitive processes in the heuristic model. At the K-3 grade level, reading competencies are usually conceptualized and measured in three forms: letter and simple word recognition, word attack and deliberate decoding, sentence and passage comprehension. To investigate these, researchers often use measures such as the Woodcock-Johnson subtests of letter-word, word attack, passage comprehension and spelling (Foorman, Schatschneider, Eakin, Fletcher, Moats, & Francis, 2006). In addition, Foorman et al. found that primary-level instruction often focused on phonemic awareness, structural analysis of words, vocabulary, and grammar. At the intermediate and secondary levels, more complex self-regulation processes are also at play. In a model highly predictive of school reading for ninth-grade students, factors correlating with comprehension included background knowledge, strategies (such as questioning), word reading, vocabulary and 7 inference (Cromley & Azevedo, 2007). While most factors directly predict comprehension, the effects of strategies on comprehension are often mediated by inference. Because these cognitive systems receive the overwhelming attention of teachers and researchers in reading, we propose that the powers of motivation and engagement variables must be addressed in their typical context of individual differences in reading. The unique benefits of motivation and engagement will be described in a later section, as will classroom contexts that support motivation and engagement. Connections of Reading Engagement, Motivation, Cognitive Processes and Achievement Engagement and achievement (Path A). At all levels of schooling, engagement has been correlated with reading achievement. This proposition is depicted as Path A in the heuristic model (Figure 1). For primary students, behavioral engagement as indicated by teachers’ observations of students’ active participation, enthusiasm for reading, and effort in overcoming difficulties predicts cognitive reading competencies, both within a grade level and across grades (Hughes, Lou, Kwok, & Loyd, 2008). Among elementary students, behavioral engagement in the form of students’ amount of reading predicted standardized reading comprehension scores highly. (DeNaeghel, Vankeer, Vansteenskiste & Roesseel, 2012). Using a similar measure of behavioral engagement, Guthrie et. al. (1999) showed that a nationally representative sample of secondary students’ reading engagement predicted standardized reading achievement when socioeconomic status was controlled. In a meta-analysis of students from K-12, yet another indicator of behavioral engagement in reading consisting of print exposure was shown to predict reading comprehension substantially Print exposure also predicted word recognition and oral language skillsfor younger children (Mol & Bus, 2011). Most broadly, the 2009 Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) survey of 15year-olds in more than 40 countries showed that reading engagement predicted reading-literacy 8 achievement substantially (OECD, 2010). Not only was engagement nearly equal to reading strategies in its prediction of achievement, but engagement mediated a substantial amount of the effect of socioeconomic status and gender on achievement. That is, when lower income students were engaged readers, their achievement nearly matched the achievement of higher income students, and reading engagement nearly closed the gender gap between the boys and girls in most countries (OECD, 2010). It is abundantly evident that many indicators manifest the association of behavioral engagement and reading achievement across a variety of cultures and social conditions. This is good news for education policy because engagement can be increased in classrooms by teachers who use engagement-generating instructional practices, as described later in this chapter. Furthermore, it is interesting that individual differences in engagement were so strongly associated with achievement that the apparent influence on achievement of another form of individual difference (variation in socioeconomic background) diminished. This finding suggests that it may be particularly powerful for teachers of students from weaker socioeconomic backgrounds to systematically implement practices known to foster engagement. Motivations and engagement (Path B). In the heuristic model, we propose that the effects of motivation on engagement are especially important; that is, motivation does not automatically or magically translate into increased achievement. Rather, in line with our general frame, Motivation →Engagement→Achievement, we suspect that motivation exerts its power to increase achievement through increasing students’ amount of cognitively intense, personally significant reading activity; motivation fuels sustained, self-regulated reading activity, of which one consequence is relatively high reading test scores or teacher-awarded grades. Empirically, the pathway is well supported. For elementary students, Becker, McElvany, and Kortenbruck (2010) showed that intrinsic motivation linked stronglywith students’ behavioral engagement in the form 9 of amount of reading (as rated by students and parents). For middle-schoolers, Guthrie, et. al. (2013) found that self-reported motivations of intrinsic motivation, self-efficacy, value, and prosocial behaviors were positively correlated with positive behavioral engagement, termed dedication, whereas motivations of perceived difficulty, devaluing and antisocial behaviors were correlated with avoidance. Cognitive processes and engagement (Path C). It is equally plausible that students who possess an abundance of well-formed reading competencies might be more behaviorally engaged readers than those who do not. It is difficult to conceive of a student investing high amounts of time and persistence in reading if the student does not have an established cognitive system for reading. Although struggling readers may often work assiduously, they are rarely voracious consumers of text. Empirical studies confirm the strength of Path C in the heuristic model. For instance, among primary students, teachers’ ratings of students’ behavioral engagement in the form of active, enthusiastic participation in learning was well predicted by students’ reading achievement on standardized tests in the same time period (Hughes et. al., 2008). Highly engaged readers were high achievers, and vice versa. In this case the cognitive process of reading and reading achievement were quite similar. More studies at the primary level should be directed to the relations of specific cognitive processes and engagement. At the elementary level, students who were capable of deeply comprehending and integrating multiple texts were likely to be frequent readers of lengthy books (Guthrie, et. al., 1999). This association of cognitive competence and behavioral engagement was highly significant even when individual difference factors of background knowledge, socioeconomic status and motivation were controlled statistically. In sum, cognitive competencies in reading – themselves another aspect of individual differences in reading 10 -- are connected to reading behavioral engagement according to diverse measures of both reading engagement and achievement, suggesting that Path C is well established. Unique contributions of motivation and cognition to engagement. In the previous section, we suggested that reading motivations and cognitive processes are associated with reading engagement; they may in fact be considered sources of individual differences in engagement. However, the evidence was limited to simple correlations. Thus it is possible that because cognitively able students are also motivated, the correlation of motivation and engagement may be an artifact of the power of cognition. This issue is crucial, because if motivation is an artifact, we need not emphasize it in education, although it may be a valuable contributor to a configuration of individual differences. Regrettably, most motivation theories do not test whether their motivational variables influence achievement in the context of relevant cognitions. For this discussion, we propose that motivation and cognition each have distinctive, unique effects on achievement. At all grade levels investigators have examined whether individual differences in motivation and cognitive reading competencies have distinct effects on achievement. For elementary grade students, Becker, et. al. (2010) showed that students’ intrinsic motivation increased their behavioral engagement, as measured by amount of reading, when prior reading achievement and extrinsic motivation were controlled. The association of motivation and engagement was quite strong.. Cohering with this finding, Guthrie et. al. (1999) found that elementary students’ behavioral engagement in the form of reading amount was predicted by intrinsic motivation even when previous achievement and background knowledge were statistically controlled. Whatever their achievement level, more intrinsically motivated students were more likely to read widely and deeply than less intrinsically motivated ones. 11 For middle school students, Jang, Kim, and Reeve (2012) found that behavioral engagement as represented by students’ self-ratings of their active involvement in learning was predicted by their perceived autonomy, a construct closely related to intrinsic motivation. The association was sustained when grades from a range of classes were controlled. Among secondary students, Guthrie et. al. (1999) reported that the motivation of interest in English/Language Arts increased behavioral engagement as measured by reading amount when standardized reading achievement and socioeconomic status were controlled. According to Jang (2008), college students who concentrated intensely understood unfamiliar, difficult text more deeply than students who were less behaviorally engaged. In this investigation students were randomly assigned to a treatment that would increase their engagement vs. a treatment not expected to influence engagement. Thus prior cognitive reading competencies were controlled experimentally rather than through multiple regression procedures. Several investigators have reported that motivation variables of task value or self-reported ‘grit’ predict reading achievement even when strong cognitive variables such as IQ and the use of self-regulated strategies are statistically controlled (Braten, Ferguson, Anmarkrud, & Stromso, 2010). Finally, for primary students, Hughes, Luo, and Kwok (2008) found in a three-year longitudinal study that motivation influenced students’ engagement even when their level of reading performance was controlled statistically. Higher performing students were more engaged based on teacher ratings than lower performing students, irrespective of their motivation level. In interpreting this study, we consider the authors’ measure of the quality of teacher-student relationships to reflect students’ motivation levels, in line with our conception of social motivation. This assumption is warranted, moreover, from extensive evidence that many qualities of the teacher-student relationship are positively connected to classroom literacy engagement (Pianta, 12 Hamre, & Allen, 2012). Thus, it appears in the Hughes at el. study that both motivation and reading cognition had effects on students’ engagement levels. Altogether, however, evidence of the unique associations of individual differences in motivation and cognition with engagement for primary students is weaker than other grade levels, and more direct tests of their unique effects are needed. Motivation and engagement spur growth of achievement over time. To this point, connections among reading engagement, motivation and achievement, as shown in the heuristic model, have been made within discreet acts in a limited time period.. However, motivations also energize achievement growth over time. Students who are highly motivated in the fall of an academic year will surpass their less motivated peers the following spring even when they begin at the same achievement level. It is reasonable to question how this occurs, and research has not explored this dynamic fully. Research reviewed here shows, however, that motivation increases behavioral engagement, including time, effort, concentration and a high volume of reading. In addition, being a highly engaged reader is associated with proficient reading. Reading proficiency depends on automaticity of many cognitive processes including decoding print to language, use of background knowledge for comprehension, and rapid inferencing at local and global levels (Kintsch, & Kintsch, 2005). Automaticity is known to accelerate from frequency of meaningful activity. Consequently, we believe it is reasonable to suggest that motivation and engagement build achievement growth by fostering automaticity of processes fundamental to reading expertise. Evidence that reading motivation fuels achievement growth is plentiful. Creating a growth model over three years of middle school, Retelsdorf, Koller, and Moller (2011) showed that reading comprehension (reasoning with text) in year 3 was predicted by the motivations of interest 13 and curiosity in year 1 when reading comprehension in year 1 was controlled. Interest spurred comprehension growth irrespective of prior comprehension levels. The more basic cognitive process of decoding speed showed the same result. Working with elementary students, Taboada, Tonks, Wigfield, and Guthrie (2009) reported that internal reading motivation based on teachers’ ratings predicted growth of achievement over three months according to two reading measures, a standardized comprehension test and a task requiring integrating information from and reasoning with multiple texts. Because the students’ motivations were inferred from teacher ratings of enthusiasm, interests, and voluntary reading, it is more consistent with our current distinction of motivation and engagement to refer to the teachers’ ratings as a measure of behavioral engagement rather than internal motivation. Accepting that reformulation, the finding is that behavioral engagement predicted reading growth in two achievement measures. To describe the strength of these motivation effects, Guthrie, Hoa, Wigfield, Tonks, Humenick, and Littles (2007) reported growth of reading comprehension grade equivalence scores for fourth graders. Holding prior achievement constant, students who were highly interested in reading in the fall gained 3 years in reading grade equivalent in the 3 months until winter. Moreover, students who increased in interest and involvement during the same 3 months gained 1 year of reading grade equivalence. Notably, students with low interest showed no reading comprehension growth in the same period, which indicates an achievement decline relative to their improving peers. These findings confirm that the benefits of motivation for achievement growth are not a mere marginal luxury. Reading motivation may stand as the strongest psychological variable influencing achievement. Classroom Practices and Contexts Influence Motivations (Path D) 14 Arranging collaboration fosters prosocial goals. Collaborative activity in the classroom is often viewed as a driver of literacy engagement. Our conception of this construct is grounded in activity theory (Leontiev, 1981; Scribner & Cole, 1981) and its extension to classrooms by Guttierez and Lee (2009). When the more expert teacher and less expert student collaborate in culturally valued literate practices, students acquire the literate action scenarios (Scribner & Cole, 1981). Successful student uptake of literacy practices generates increased academic proficiencies. An equally abundant research literature related to collaboration examines teacher-student relationships that support students’ social and emotional well-being during learning (Martin & Dowson, 2009). Furthermore, favorable relationships with teachers and peers foster active participation in school activities among secondary school students (Juvoven, Espinoza, & Knifsend, 2009). In the absence of these personal connections, students’ engagement declines, leading disproportionately to disengagement (Griffiths, Lilles, Furlong, & Sidhwa, 2009) and dropping out (Rumberger, 2011). Thus teacher-student collaboration and emotional relationships are expected to influence students’ academic and reading engagement directly without necessarily being mediated through a motivational construct. Examples of arranging collaboration. Reading is often a solo, silent activity. In contrast, students are social creatures. Most simply, teachers can harness students’ desires to interact by fostering reading as a social pursuit. Teachers can ask students of any age to read together. For the youngest, this may be partners reading aloud simultaneously. For older elementary aged students, partner reading can consist of individuals reading a passage silently and sharing the most salient points. Partners can each write a challenging question for the other to answer, and the outcomes can be discussed. If partners in upper elementary school write a summary of a chapter, they can then compare which summary elements in their chapters are common and which ones are distinct. 15 Such partnerships can be short and simple, consisting of 5 minutes of reading and one minute of sharing. They can become as long and complex as the students can successfully perform. Student-led discussion groups are exciting for class members. If they are well-organized, they can increase the depth of students’ reading comprehension. In CORI for grade 7, teachers use an adaptation of the ‘collaborative reasoning’ approach (Chinn, Anderson, & Waggoner, 2001). After every member of a group reads a complex text for 10-15 minutes silently, the discussion begins. One team member states a key point; the next person adds a new, relevant point; each person contributes new information. At the end, the last person states a summary of what the members said, and the group discusses whether the summary was complete or should be extended. To be successful, teachers must set a few rules for groups regarding such aspects as the number of students per group, turn-taking, active listening, full participation of all members, and accountability for staying on task (which, for instance, could be demonstrated through writing or explaining the final synthesis). Providing choices and relevance increases intrinsic motivation. Grounded in selfdetermination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2009), autonomy support in the classroom refers to shared control between the teacher and students. When teachers afford students input and choices in their learning, their interests and participation increase (Zhou & Deci, 2009), which enhances achievement (Reeve, 2012). Experimental studies have shown that ‘intrinsic framing’ is stronger than ‘extrinsic framing’ for facilitating conceptual learning from text (Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Soenens, &Matos, 2005). In these experiments, emphasizing personal interest or relevance (intrinsic framing) rather than task proficiency (extrinsic framing) as a basis for reading consistently leads to deeper text comprehension among young adolescents. Classroom autonomy support has been shown to increase students’ intrinsic motivation (enjoyment and enthusiasm) as 16 well as engagement (active participation and sustained work) reciprocally at multiple grade levels (Skinner & Belmont, 1993). In our model, we use both choices and relevance to represent autonomy support. Examples of providing choice and relevance. Giving choices in the classroom is often misunderstood. It is not merely a trip to the media center to select books or extended free reading with no instruction. We encourage many mini-choices during instruction. Teachers can give students a mini-choice of which character to focus on in a story. They read about all characters while becoming an expert on one. In a book on civil war history, students can choose which battles to learn about most deeply. Then in class discussion, they learn about all battles in the war and their relations to each other. Choice of texts is important, but choice of task also can be powerful. Suppose a teacher has taught students to make predictions and also to write their own questions about a text before reading. In learning about the early explorers, students can be given a choice of whether to write two questions related to an explorer’s goals or personality, or make two predictions about the outcome of an expedition described in text. Later the teacher may ask students to use the opposite strategy. While having the opportunity for input, students nevertheless cover the reading curriculum comprehensively. Turning to the importance of providing relevance, consider that texts that are remote from students’ experience and knowledge base are usually boring. Teachers can foster interest in any text, however, by increasing its relevance through related media. A brief, 5-minute video depicting a lion hunting a gazelle in the Serengeti Plain will pique students’ interest in reading a text on predation. With newfound interest, students will read more avidly about predation and survival in various biomes. 17 In science, hands on activities, such as looking at a leaf through a magnifying glass, or observing life in an aquarium will focus, sustain and deepen reading about biology. In history, brief videos of historical enactments can launch a class discussion of politics (such as those related to slavery) and foster penetrating analysis of related texts (such as the emancipation proclamation). Offering students choices within texts and providing real world activities related to texts generate intrinsic motivation for reading. Emphasizing the importance of reading increases students’ valuing. Drawing on expectancy-value theory (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002), evidence shows that there is a significant association between teachers’ expectations for students and students’ growth in reading achievement. As students internalize the expectations of significant others for a specific domain such as English, their value for that domain and selections of courses in that domain increase (Durik, Vida, & Eccles, 2006). Several studies have reported the benefits of providing a ‘valuing rationale’ for reading an uninteresting text. In two studies, for example, the valuing rationale described how information in the text would provide immediate professional benefit for prospective teachers. Groups receiving a valuing rationale showed enhanced behavioral engagement (close attention to reading) and increased conceptual comprehension of information text compared to no-rationale groups (Jang, 2008). In another study, asking students to find usefulness and applicability in text increased students’ comprehension in both laboratory and classroom settings compared to control conditions (Hulleman, Godes, Hendricks, & Harackiewicz, 2010). We use the phrase ‘emphasizing importance’ to represent the instructional practice of providing a valuing rationale. Examples of emphasizing importance. For older students in secondary school, teachers can use the very technique employed in several studies. Teachers can request students to write a 18 statement explaining how a text will benefit them in some way. The text may improve their competence as a professional; it may be crucial to performing well in the next course; it may afford them a memorable literary experience that influences their understanding of themselves For younger adolescents in CORI, we foster recognition of the importance of text through concrete activities. For instance, in a day, students view a brief video, record their observations, read extensively on the same topic, and share their learning with a partner. After 60 minutes the teacher asks students to reflect on which activity they learned the most from. Inevitably they realize that reading the text was most informative. Teachers emphasize the importance of text in other ways, too. After reading a complex text, they ask students to identify the sentences that most helped them understand, and explain why. After a debate, teachers ask students to show the textual sources of their best points. Gradually students realize that texts help them to speak, write, and explain new things to their friends. Broader, more general discussion of which texts were most informative, interesting, or provocative also will enrich the perceptions that text is valuable. Assuring success increases self-efficacy. From the viewpoint of Bandura’s social cognitive theory, competence support during reading instruction has been investigated experimentally (Schunk & Mullen, 2009). Studies of competence support in the form of providing feedback on progress and helping students set realistic goals in a specific reading task increases self-efficacy for the particular academic domain of the activity (Schunk & Zimmerman, 2007). Competence support may also appear in the form of using content domain texts, such as science trade books, that are readily decodable and enable students to derive meaning related to their observations and knowledge of the world while learning reading skills (Guthrie, McRae, & Klauda, 2007). Examples of assuring success. An obvious but profound act is to provide students text they can read successfully, or, in other words, that address individual differences in reading proficiency. 19 No motivation is more debilitating than dread of difficulty. When a low achieving student faces a daunting text, she will take steps to avoid it within a few minutes. The alternative is humiliation, which she has learned to minimize. Although comprehensive academic standards such as the Common Core state that students should read on-grade or more challenging texts, not all students should begin there. To build self-confidence, effective teachers locate texts that students can read aloud fluently and understand literally. Instruction can then be devoted to deeper comprehension with manageable text. Gaining proficiency and self-efficacy occur together. Foster extended engagement. The practices of motivation support are designed to increase engaged reading. The point of affording choice, for example, is not to teach students how to make good choices, although that may be necessary. The point is to motivate more and deeper reading. If the time required to make a choice consumes 20 minutes out of a 30 minute learning activity, very little reading was accomplished. In a 30 minute time slot, 3 minutes should be given for the choice and 27 minutes for the reading. The cost of time taken for the motivational activity is justified only if it increases the total time spent reading within an instructional unit. If 10 minutes is given to discussion on one day, the activity was successful only if it generates more than 10 additional minutes reading within a few days. Because learning from text socially is powerful, well-organized discussion builds deeper and longer reading capacities. This criterion applies to each motivation practice, including support for choice, relevance, success, importance and collaboration. Motivation activities should accelerate amount and depth of reading by 200%-500%. That is how the heuristic model works. Classroom contexts energize motivations (Path D); motivations energize engagement behaviors (Path B); and engagement fuels achievement (Path A). Quantity of engaged reading is a vitally important individual difference variable, with some students much 20 higher than others. When classroom contexts foster it for everyone, the associated factor of achievement is likely to increase for all students. Discussion of the Heuristic Model and Its Implications Limitations. A substantial issue related to the multiple pathways and potential pathways in the heuristic model remains to be addressed. It is possible to draw additional connective ties within the model to achievement. For example, motivation may directly increase achievement; that is, it is possible that engagement does not mediate all of the effects of motivation on achievement (Becker, 2010). Further, the relationships may all be reciprocal; in other words, each arrow could be bidirectional. Space does not allow us to describe or defend the presence or absence of every possible path in a completely saturated version of the heuristic model. However, we argue that there is evidence favoring each of the lettered paths in the model. Due to space limits, we have not presented evidence supporting Path E which links Classroom instruction to cognitive processes in reading. Many other reviews and chapters address this vital link. Furthermore, space does not permit the presentation of the substantial body of experimental data that more strongly suggest causal links in the model. A single, accurate model enjoying the concurrence of all behavioral scientists does not yet exist. In its absence, we propose that under typical conditions of measurement and modeling, each of the paths A-E in the current model can be empirically justified. Implications. Following the heuristic model, classroom teaching should be designed to include a fusion of motivation and cognitive support for reading engagement, from the primary grades through higher education. Without motivation, children cannot become engaged readers who devote the time and manifest the self-regulation needed to become proficient readers. A cognitively capable reader is only half a reader. Dependent on engagement, reading expertise 21 emerges only when both the cognitive and motivational halves of reading are deliberately nurtured by teachers and administrators. To build literacy, classroom excellence can be fostered by following this foundation for richer literacy learning experiences for all students. Although students vary dramatically in their reading motivation and engagement, classroom contexts can raise the aspirations and commitments of all. 22 References Becker, M., McElvany, N., & Kortenbruck, M. (2010). Intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation aspredictors of reading literacy: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 773-786. Braten, I., Ferguson, L., Anmarkrud, O., & Stromso, H. (2013). Prediction of learning and comprehension when adolescents read multiple texts: the roles of word-level processing, strategic approach and reading motivation. Reading & Writing, 26, 321-348. Chinn, C.A., Anderson, R.C., & Waggoner, M.A. (2001). Patterns of discourse in two kinds of literature discussion. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(4), 378–411. Cromley, J., & Azevedo, R. (2007). Testing and refining the direct and indirect inferential mediation model of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 311325. DeNaeghel, J., Van Keer, H., Vansteenkiste, M., & Rosseel, Y. (2012). The relation between elementary students’ recreational and academic reading motivation, reading frequency, engagement and comprehension: A self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1006-1021. Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and ability beliefs as predictors of high school literacy choices: A developmental analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 382-393. Eccles, J., & Wang, M. (2012). Part I Commentary: So what is student engagement anyway? In S. Christensen, A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 133-149). New York: Springer Science. 23 Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,109–132. Foorman, B., Schatschneider, C., Eakin, M., Fletcher, J., Moats, L., & Francis, D. (2006). The impact of instructional practices in Grades 1 and 2 on reading and spelling achievement in high poverty schools. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 1-29. Griffiths, A., Lilles, E., & Furlong, M. (2009). The relations of adolescent student engagement with troubling and high-risk behaviors. In K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 563-585). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Guthrie, J. T., Hoa, A. L. W., Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., Humenick, N. M., & Littles, E. (2007). Reading motivation and reading comprehension growth in the later elementary years. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 282-313. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., & You, W. (2012). Instructional contexts for engagement and achievement in reading. In S. Christensen, A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 601-634). New York: Springer Science. Guthrie, J. T., Klauda, S. L., & Ho, A. (2013). Modeling the relationships among reading instruction, motivation, engagement, and achievement for adolescents. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 9-26. Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A. C., & Klauda, S. L. (2007). Contributions of Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction to knowledge about interventions for motivations in reading. Educational Psychologist, 42, 237-250. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Metsala, J. L., & Cox, K. E. (1999). Motivational and cognitive predictors of text comprehension and reading amount. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(3), 231-257. 24 Gutiérrez, K., & Lee, C. D. (2009). Robust informal learning environments for youth from non-dominant groups: Implications for literacy learning in formal schooling. In L. M. Morrow, R. Rueda, & D. Lapp (Eds.), Handbook of research on literacy and diversity (pp. 216-232). New York: Guilford Press. Ho, A. N., & Guthrie, J. T. (2013). Patterns of association among multiple motivations and aspects of achievement in reading. Reading Psychology, 34, 101-147. Hughes, J., Lou, W., Kwok, O., & Loyd, L. (2008). Teacher-student support effortful, engagement, and achievement: a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 114. Hulleman, C. S., Godes, O., Hendricks, B. L., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Enhancing interest and performance with a utility value intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 880-895. Jang, H. (2008). Supporting students’ motivation, engagement, and learning during an uninteresting activity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 798-811. Jang, H., Kim, E., & Reeve, J. (2012). Longitudinal test of self-determination theory’s motivation mediation model in a naturally occurring classroom context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1175-1188. Juvonen, J., Espinoza, G. & Knifsend, C. (2009). The role of peer relationships in student academic and extracurricular engagement. In K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 387-403). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Kintsch, W., & Kintsch, E. (2005). Comprehension. In S. G. Paris & S. A. Stahl (Eds.), Children's reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 71-92). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 25 Leontiev, A.A. (1981). Psychology and the Language Learning Process. Oxford: Pergamon. McRae, A. (2011). Teacher competence support for reading in middle school. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, Md. Martin, A.J., & Dowson, M. (2009). Interpersonal relationships, motivation, engagement, and achievement: Yields for theory, current issues, and educational practice. Review of Educational Research, 79, 327-365. Mol, S., & Bus, A. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 267-296. OECD (2010). PISA 2009 Results: Learning to Learn – Student Engagement, Strategies and Practices (Volume III) http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264083943-en Pianta, R., Hamre, B., Allen, J. (2012). Teacher-students relationships and engagement; Conceptualizing, measuring and improving the capacity of classroom interactions. In S. Christensen, A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 365-386). New York: Springer Science. Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 544-555. Reeve, J. (2012). A self-determination theory perspective on student engagement. In S. Christensen, A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 149-173). New York: Springer Science. Retelsdorf, J., Koller, O., & Moller, J. (2011). On the effects of motivation on reading performance growth in secondary school. Learning and Instruction, 21, 550-559. 26 Rumberger, R. (2011). Dropping out: Why students quit school and what can be done about it. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). Promoting self-determined school engagement: Motivation, learning, and well-being. In K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 171-195). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Schiefele, U., Schaffner, E., Moller, J., & Wigfield, A. (2012). Dimensions of reading motivation and their relation to reading behavior and competence. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, 427-463. Schunk, D. & Mullen, C. (2009). Self-efficacy as an engaged learner. In K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 291-237). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Schunk, D., & Zimmerman, B. (2007). Influencing children’s self-efficacy and self-regulation of reading and writing through modeling. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 23: 7-25. Scribner, S. & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85 , 571–581. Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. J. (2009). A motivational perspective on engagement and disaffection: Conceptualization and assessment of children’s behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69, 493-525. 27 Skinner, E. & Pitzer, J. (2012). Developmental dynamics of student engagement, coping and everyday resilience. In S. Christensen, A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 21-45). New York: Springer Science. Taboada, A., Tonks, S., Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. (2009). Effects of motivational and cognitive variables on reading comprehension. Reading & Writing, 22, 85-106. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., & Matos, L. (2005). Examining the motivational impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing and autonomy-supportive versus internally controlling communication style on early adolescents’ academic achievement. Child Development, 76, 483-501. Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J.T. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for reading to the amount and breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 420-432. Zhou, M., Ma, W. J., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The importance of autonomy for rural Chinese children’s motivation for learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 492-498. 28 Figure 1 In the “Indicators” box with Reading Achievement (below), can you spell out “Task performance”? Reading Engagement: Heuristic Model with Indicators and Components of Achievement, Engagement, Motivation and Cognition Indicators: Self-report Teacher rating Indicators: Student report Observer rating Teacher selfreport Video recording D Motivation in Reading Components: B Intrinsic Efficacy Value Social Classroom Instruction and Teaching E Self-report Teacher rate Observation Diaries Indicators: Test scores Grades Task performance A Reading Achievement Indicators: Behavioral Engagement In Reading Self-report Task perform. Components: Components: Effort Enthusiasm Persistence Self-regulation Reasoning Literal Fluency Vocabulary Components: Choice and relevance Success Importance Collaboration Volume of reading Indicators: Cognition in Reading C Components: Word rec. Fluency Literal Reasoning 29