CHAPTER I WHAT IS PSYCHOLINGUISTICS? The Domain of Psycholinguistic Inquiry Linguistics is the discipline that describes the structure of language, including its grammar, sound system, and vocabulary. The field of psycholinguistics, or the psychology of language, is concerned with discovering the psychological processes by which humans acquire and use language. Conventionally, psycholinguistics addresses three major concerns (Clark & Clark, 1977; Tanenhaus, 1989): 1. Comprehension: How people understand spoken and written language. This is a broad area of investigation that involves scrutiny of the comprehension process at many levels, including investigation of how speech signals are interpreted by listeners (speech perception), how the meanings of words are determined (lexical access), how the grammatical structure of sentences is analyzed to obtain larger units of meaning (sentence processing), and how longer conversations or texts are appropriately formulated and evaluated (discourse). Concerns specifically relevant to how written language is processed are also part of this domain. 2. Speech production: How people produce language. The chapters that follow suggest that it is somewhat easier to study comprehension than production; we can use controlled language stimuli and then analyze patterns of accuracy and error, response time, and other behaviors to arrive at an estimate of how listeners process language. However, it is more difficult to gain insight into how concepts are put into linguistic form; the process is largely hidden from observation, and speakers’ verbal expressions, even in response to rather controlled eliciting stimuli, vary considerably. We learn most about the probable nature of the speech production process from speakers’ mistakes (speech errors or false starts) and from’ breaks in the ongoing rhythm of connected speech (hesitation and pausal phenomena, or speech disfluencies). 3. Acquisition: How people learn language. The major focus in this domain has been on how children acquire a first language (developmental psycholinguistics) What is a language? Human language is characterized by its hierarchical structure. By this we mean that the message is divisible into smaller units of analysis. The child’s utterance is a sentence that contains smaller discrete elements such as words and sounds, and these can be recombined to make other utterances (for example, “I won’t eat tofu. I hate it.”). All human languages are characterized by such structural properties. Conversely, it is difficult to analyze the “substructure” of infant or animal cries. Although some substructure may appear to exist in the bee’s dance and the bird’s replication of human speech, such messages lack the infinite creativity of human language. Competent language users are able to produce and understand a virtually unlimited number of well-formed sentences in their language. And all human languages express the full range of speakers’ experiences, even imaginary ones. Such is not the case (as far as we can tell) with animal languages. The parakeet is not free to discuss the weather with you, nor can he even paraphrase his message and say, “I’m a pretty bird,” or “You’re not a bird.” Although the bee is adept at directing its fellow bees to nectar, it is incapable of warning them that an irate homeowner is coming after them with a can of insecticide. It may (or may not) possess such knowledge, but it lacks a sufficiently rich system of symbols and rules for their combination to allow transmission of a large variety of concepts. The structural properties of any language include rules for using it properly; thus language is a rule-governed system of behavior. There is no right or wrong way to bark or cry (though some versions may be more annoying than others). Conversely, the rules of English specify that the child in our example may not say, "Tofu I like not, eat and it won’t I.” English, like other languages, has conventions for knowing what words must be included and for ordering these words in sentences. Distinguishing Between Language and Speech Although some authors say that language is sound (Dinneen, 1967) or that the medium of language is sound (Bolinger & Sears, 1981), this is not necessarily true. Most of the world’s languages are spoken or oral, and for most individuals speaking precedes and is of greater importance than reading or writing. However, some human languages are signed or gestural. These languages, of which American Sign Language (ASL) is one example, have the same basic linguistic features found in oral human languages. Thus, like spoken languages, they are rule-governed, arbitrary systems of communication with hierarchical substructuring that are capable of infinite creativity and spontaneously acquired by infants exposed to them. Levels of Language Analysis A psycholinguist who wishes to understand how a sentence such as How do people communicate with one another? is processed must first acknowledge that understanding it depends on several smaller tasks: • The sounds of the message must be isolated and recognized. • The words must be identified and associated with their meanings. • The gramma’icalstructure of the message must be analyzed sufficiently to de termine the roles played by each word. • The resulting interpretation of the message must be evaluated in light of past experience and the current context. Only then can the utterance be considered “understood.” Linguists, philosophers, and psychologists have long appreciated that language is a complex system that can be considered at multiple levels of analysis. Every human language may be analyzed in terms of its phonology (sound system), morphology (rules for word formation), lexicon (vocabulary), syntax (rules for combining words into grammatically acceptable sequences), semantics (conventions for deriving the meanings of words and sentences), and pragmatics (rules for appropriate social use and interpretation of language in context). Linguists strive to develop descriptions of a language that capture its characteristics at each of these levels. Psycholinguists, in turn, seek to determine whether these levels or units of analysis are represented in the actual process of producing or understanding various forms of language. Some call this endeavor the search for the psychological reality of linguistic descriptions. We may more readily appreciate the complex interaction of these systems by considering efforts to get by in a language we do not know. Anyone who has attempted to function in a foreign culture using only an English—foreign language dictionary knows the frustrating limitations of a list of words and their meanings. Below we will examine some of the many specific abilities that underlie competent use of language. As we do, it may become evident that, although using and understanding language is a relatively quick and easy task for most of us, many aspects of our linguistic knowledge are subconscious in nature. Phonology The words of a language are divisible into sound sequences, and part of language knowledge is an understanding of the particular sounds used in a language, as well as the rules for their combination. The study of how the sounds of a language are physically enunciated is known as articulatoiy phonetics and will be explored in Chapter 3. A great number of speech sounds are available to the world’s languages. The distinctive sounds used in a language are its phonemes. Phonemes are contrastive; changing from one to another within a word produces either a change in meaning or a nonword. Most of us are familiar with the experience of trying to learn a second language whose phonemic inventory (sounds used in the language) differs from our own. Because we can find minimal pairs such as pit and bit, which differ only in respect to/pt and /b/and have different meanings, we know that/pt and /b/ are different phonemes. But consider the English words pot and spot. More force is used when we say pot, and the resulting puff of air is called aspiration. Linguists write the sound in this word as [p]. The /p/ of spot is produced without aspiration—it is unaspirated. Yet this sound contrast cannot be used to mark a difference in meaning in English, where the two sounds are separate allophones (variants of the same phoneme) rather than separate phonemes. Sequences of Sounds: Phonotactics: it deals with how sounds is arranged to join syllable and word. The phonological system of a language also includes rules for the interpretation of prosody or intonation and stress patterns. The Lexicon and Semantics Semantic is the study of word meaning and the ways in which words are related to one another in our mental lexicon. It also includes the study of sentential meaning in contemporary linguistics. Morphology: The study of Word Formation The study of how word is formed. There are two morphemes; a) free morpheme and b) bound morpheme. Syntax: Combining Words to Form Sentences. Pragmatics and Discourse Pragmatics determines our choice of wording and our interpretation of language in different situations. Much of our language use involves discourse, usually defined as verbal or written interactions longer than single utterances. Beyond the level of understanding single words or sentences, we more typically need to evaluate what we hear or read within a particular context and on the basis of our prior knowledge—we are thus able to make inferences. The situational setting or context of spoken messages is often crucial to their successful interpretation. “Success,” in this sense, is taken as the hearer’s full appreciation of the speaker’s intent. Metalinguistic Capacity: The Ability to Analyze Our Own Language Metalinguistic means, literally, “language about language.” It is often difficult to ex~plain our knowledge about language. Our talents for speaking well and understanding competently are aspects of our linguistic knowledge, whereas our ability to reflect upon our language— our understanding of how we do these things—represents an aspect of our metalinguistic knowledge. In this book we ask metalinguistic questions such as “How do I understand the meaning of word?” “How do I find words when I want to talk about things?” “Are some words easier or harder for people to understand?” THE ACQUISITION OF LANGUAGE BY CHILDREN The rapidity with which children acquire language has fascinated scholars and parents for thousands of years. Although questions about the nature of child language learning and the methods of study used have varied, they embrace many recurring themes. The nature— nurture controversy maintains a robust presence among child language researchers. Some (often called nativists) side with nature and maintain that language is basically innate, that children are born with a special, unique human talent that can extrapolate the grammar of a language without overt instruction or correction. Others stress the role of nurture and claim that adults teach language to children by using special kinds of simplified language with them and providing them with feedback when they have used the language well or poorly. An important component of Chomsky’s early writings (1957, 1965) was its emphasis on the role of linguistic theory and the limits of behaviorist psychology in explaining the acquisition of language by children.