Dispersion, Spatial Behaviour and Burrows of the ghost crabs Ocypode cordimana (Fabricius) and Ocypode ceratophthalma (Pallas), (Decapoda, Brachyura, Ocypodidae) By: Louise Raggett Abstract Acknowledgments I am particularly grateful to Martyn Drabik-Hamshare, to whom this studies’ data collection was a combined effort (see Drabik-Hamshare, 2010). I would also like to thank my supervisor John Allen for the opportunity to go to Bird Island and conduct this study and for his support during the preparation of this document. Further thanks go to Dr. Colin Little whose enlightenment in the habits and ecology of Ocypode, helped me decide upon this studies focus. Abbreviations H.W.M. High water mark C.S.R. Complete spatial randomness Contents 1. Introduction.......................................................................................................................................... 8 1.1 Dispersion and ecological significance ........................................................................................... 8 1.2 Environmental factors .................................................................................................................. 10 1.3 Population density ....................................................................................................................... 10 1.3.1 Seasonal variations in population ......................................................................................... 10 1.3.2 Temperature effects.............................................................................................................. 11 1.3.3 Human disturbance ............................................................................................................... 11 1.4 Ocypode zonal distribution .......................................................................................................... 11 1.4.1 Beach exposure ..................................................................................................................... 12 1.4.2 Sympatric species .................................................................................................................. 13 1.4.3 Distribution and crab size ...................................................................................................... 13 1.5 Ocypode burrows: function and distribution of morphotypes .................................................... 14 1.5.1 Zonal distribution and burrow morphotype ......................................................................... 14 1.6 Social and behavioural implications of Ocypode Burrows ........................................................... 16 1.6.1 Interactions- agonistic and non-agonistic ............................................................................. 16 1.6.2 Aggregative behaviour .......................................................................................................... 17 1.6.3 Female centred competition ................................................................................................. 18 1.6.4 Use of burrows by reproductively active males .................................................................... 18 1.6.5 Pyramid building as sematectonic signals ............................................................................ 19 1.6.6 Pyramid building in enforcing male territories ..................................................................... 21 1.6.7 Visual, acoustic and vibrational communication .................................................................. 21 1.7 Summary ...................................................................................................................................... 23 2. Aims of Study...................................................................................................................................... 24 3. Method ............................................................................................................................................... 24 3.1 Study site ...................................................................................................................................... 24 3.2 Decapod species found at the site ............................................................................................... 25 3.3 Choice of beach for sampling ....................................................................................................... 29 3.4 Mapping burrows in a ‘point pattern’ .......................................................................................... 32 3.5 Data collection for transects of Beach A ...................................................................................... 32 3.6 Mapping of Nearest Neighbours for transects 2,3 and 4 ............................................................. 35 3.7 Activity data collection ................................................................................................................. 38 3.7 Observations of adult O. ceratophthalma ................................................................................... 38 4. Analysis of Data .................................................................................................................................. 40 4.1 Population distribution and density ............................................................................................. 40 4.1.1 Population density ................................................................................................................ 41 4.1.2 Crab activity........................................................................................................................... 41 4.2 Dispersion pattern of Ocypode burrows ...................................................................................... 41 4.2.1 Frequency distributions among quadrats ............................................................................. 41 4.2.2 Nearest Neighbour Analysis .................................................................................................. 43 4.3 The relationship between crab size and distance to neighbours ................................................ 44 4.3.1 Size of burrow effect on nearest neighbour ......................................................................... 44 4.4 Dispersion of different juveniles and adults ............................................................................ 45 5. Results ................................................................................................................................................ 45 5.1 Beach morphology ....................................................................................................................... 45 5.2 Population Distribution ................................................................................................................ 47 5.3 Density of burrows ....................................................................................................................... 49 5.4 Population estimate ..................................................................................................................... 50 5.5 Zonal distribution ......................................................................................................................... 51 5.6 Burrows types............................................................................................................................... 52 5.7 Crab activity.................................................................................................................................. 54 5.8 Dispersion pattern: using frequencies in quadrats ...................................................................... 56 5.9 Dispersion of Ocypode burrows: using nearest neighbour distances .......................................... 58 5.10 Dispersion pattern of different size categories and mature males ........................................... 59 6. Discussion .............................................................................................. Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.1 Differences between O. cordimana and O. ceratopthalma ............ Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.3. Distribution of burrows .................................................................. Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.3.1 Variation of burrow density along the beach .......................... Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.3.1 Distribution of different burrow sizes ...................................... Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.3.2 Distribution of juvenile versus adult burrows .......................... Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.3.3 Variation up the beach ............................................................. Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.3.4 Burrow morphology ................................................................. Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.4 Dispersion of burrows ..................................................................... Error! Bookmark not defined. 6.4.1 Burrow defence ........................................................................ Error! Bookmark not defined. 1. Introduction Ghost crabs of the genus Ocypode are the most widespread members of Ocypodidae, occurring on all major oceans, (Daumer et al, 1963; George and Knott, 1965, Mclachlan and Brown, 2006). They are a dominant feature of sub-tropical and tropical sandy beaches, (Barrass, 1963; Taylor, 1968; Chan et al, 2006), and mostly appear to have crepuscular and nocturnal activity, (Cott, 1930; Hagasaka, 1935; Barrass, 1963; Hughes, 1966; Hill and Hunter, 1973; Shuchman and Warburg, 1978; Strachan et al, 1999). Ocypode are primarily predacious, (Gibson and Hill, 1947; Hughes, 1966; Wolcott, 1978; Strachan et al, 1999), but are also scavengers, and deposit feeders, (Hagasaka, 1935; Hughes, 1966; Jones, 1972; Trott, 1988; Strachan et al, 1999). Due to their abundance, relative size, extensive bioturbation activities and omnivory, Ocypode perform a key function in sandy shore ecosystems, (Wolcott, 1978; Chan et al, 2006; Tureli et al, 2009). As all Ocypode individuals burrow regardless of size or sex, (De, 2005), they are ideal organisms to study dispersion and space related behaviours, (Lighter, 1977). 1.1 Dispersion and ecological significance Studying the dispersion of organisms has considerable, wide-ranging ecological significance, (Moore and Chapman 1986; Dale 1999; Southwood and Henderson, 2000). The dispersion pattern of a population is defined here as the spatial distribution of individuals within their population’s geographical range at any one point in time, (Browns and Orians, 1970). In a continuum of dispersion patterns, three categories are generally recognised. Dispersion may be Regular, where individuals are more evenly spaced than expected by chance, aggregated, where individuals are closer than expected by chance, and random, when there is an equal probability of any individual occupying any point in space irrespective of the position of others, (Southwood and Henderson, 2000; Fowler et al, 1998). However, simply identifying a deviation from random is of little ecological interest, (Dale, 1999). Instead, the discernment of the proximate and ultimate factors behind the observed spatial behaviour is the major focus of most studies. Dispersion is scale-dependant, (Dale, 1999) and is a function of both environmental factors, such as resource availability, which dictate where animals are distributed at a large scale perspective, and behavioural factors, which dictate the spacing of conspecifics usually at a finer scale, (Pearson, 1949, Dale, 1999, Fero and Moore, 2008). The latter include factors such as social units, (Brown and Orians, 1970), species specific individual distances, (Hediger, 1955), agonistic interactions such as territorial behaviour, (Maher and Lott, 1995), and social dominance, (Hemerijic, 2000), which are in turn impacted by environmental factors, (Fero and Moore, 2008). In crustaceans, many studies focus upon either environmental or behavioural impacts, (e.g. Shuchman and Warburg, 1978 versus Lighter, 1977), however both should be considered, (Fero and Moore, 2008) . Studies on Ocypode in general have been relatively few compared to their relatives the fiddler crab in the genus Uca, (Popper et al, 2001), and many gaps in our knowledge remain. 1.2 Environmental factors Studying the spatial distribution of Ocypode indirectly through their burrows requires an understanding of their ecology and the environmental factors which affect them, (Lighter, 1977; Barrass, 1967, Alberto and Fontoura, 1999; Turra et al, 2005). As most species inhabit heterogeneous habitats, from a large-scale perspective an aggregated dispersion pattern is most common, (Moore and Chapman, 1986), especially on coastal shores where there are strong environmental gradients, (Benson, 2002). Previous studies provide only a limited picture of what principal factors determine density, zonal distribution and burrowing behaviour of Ocypode. 1.3 Population density The population densities of Ocypode species are variable, with large differences observed even between neighbouring populations, (Frey and Mayou, 1971; Chakrabati, 1981). However, identifying the primary factors determining densities is challenging due to the dynamic nature of beaches, (Jarmillo et al, 2000), and their multiple confounding factors (Quijon et al, 2000). Little is known about the impact of resource availability, (Lucrezi et al, 2008), but high cannibalism rates appear to reduce juvenile density, (Cowles, 1908; Williams, 1965). 1.3.1 Seasonal variations in population Recruitment, (Strachan et al, 1999), winter dispersal (Tureil et al, 2009) and growth (Wolcott, 1978) affect seasonal variations dramatically. Beach type and exposure appear to impact density, but not consistently intraspecifically, (Jones,1972; Jarmillo and Mclachlan, 1993; Dugan and Hubbard, 1996; Chan et al, 2006). 1.3.2 Temperature effects Temperature tends to dictate ghost crab burrowing activity, (Shuchman and Warberg, 1878), which generally occurs between temperatures of 16 and 30˚C, (Christoff, 1986; Weinstein, 1998). 1.3.3 Human disturbance Many studies suggest human disturbance from pedestrians and vehicles is a predominant factor affecting density of different Ocypode species, (Hughes, 1966; Frey and Mayou, 1971; Chakrabati, 1981; Barros, 2001; Blankesteyn, 2006; Maccarone and Matthews, 2007; Moss and Mcphee, 2006; Neves and Benvenuti, 2006; Schlacher et al, 2007; Lucrezi et al, 2008; Hobbs et al, 2008; Brook et al, 2009; Magalhaes et al, 2009; Yong and Lim, 2009). These results suggest Ocypode species would therefore make good biological indicators, although densities can increase around food refuse, (Hill and Hunter, 1973; Steiner and Leatherman, 1981), while hot weather and short term human trampling can lead to temporarily plugged and imperceptible burrows, reducing burrow counts erroneously (Lucrezi et al, 2009). Tidedominated beaches also have too many confounding factors impacting density as they are particularly heterogeneous habitats, (Turra et al, 2005). 1.4 Ocypode zonal distribution Ghost crabs usually burrow in distinct zones on beaches. Most studies suggest Ocypode classically burrow in just above the H.W.M., (Grubb, 1971; Jones, 1972; Hartnoll, 1975; Burggren and McMahon, 1988), contrary to the belief of De, (2005). For example, O. ceratophthalma, (Barrass, 1963; Hughes, 1966; Jones, 1972), O. urvillei, (Burggren and McMahon, 1988), O. saratan, (Clayton, 2001), O. macrocera, ,(Burggren et al, 1988), O. cursor, (Tureli et al, 2009), O. ryderi (Vannini, 1980b), and O. platytarsis, (Naidu, 1951). Some species have adapted to supralittoral areas, (O. aegyptiaca,, (Magnus, 1960), O. gaudichaudii, (Quijon et al, 2001), O. jousseaumei, (Clayton, 2005) whilst others even extend into sandy soils inland, e.g. O. cordimana, (Horch, 1975; Stoddart, 1984), O. kuhlii, (Macnae and Kalk, 1962; Jones, 1972), O. Africana, (Strachan et al, 1999), O. albicans, (Pearse et al, 1942) and O. quadrata, (Borradaile, 1903; Pearse et al, 1942; Hill and Hunter, 1973; Strachan et al, 1999; Alberto and Fontoura, 1999; Clayton, 2005). However there are intraspecifc differences in Ocypode zonal distributions as well as density, occurring spatially, temporally and between different sexes and ages. At this coarse scale, (Brown and Orians, 1970), dispersion appears to be a reflection of burrowing conditions and crab physiology rather than social interactions. 1.4.1 Beach exposure Beach exposure appears to affect zonal distribution, (Jarmillo et al, 2000). Indeed during storms, zonal distributions can shift landwards, (Alberto and Fontoura, 1999; Neves and Benvenuti, 2006; Hobbs et al, 2008). For intertidal species, distribution may be determined by exposure times between tides, as distributions often shift in sync with annual tidal changes (Barrass, 1963; Jones, 1972). Hughes, (1966) suggests for O. ceratophthalma, these changes are indirect, reflecting shifts in sandpacking, suggesting crab distribution is primarily determined by suitable burrowing substrate. Contrary to Takahasi (1935), Ocypode burrows do not reach the water table to renew respiratory water like most fiddler crabs, (Chakrabati, 1981; Strachan et al, 1999; Chan et al, 2006). Instead crabs regular moisten gills in the sea, (Williams, 1965), and can absorb capillary moisture from sand via setae found on their abdominal segment, (Hatnoll, 1973) and third and fourth ambulatory legs, (Wolcott, 1976). O. cursor at least, appear to be sensitive to very specific moisture levels, (Warburg and Shuchman, 1978) and studies have found distribution is often largely determined by the water gradient, (Lucrezi et al, 2009), with shifts closer to the sea observed during summer, (Shuchman and Warburg, 1978; Lucrezi et al, 2009; Tureli et al, 2009). This is a contrast to temporal stability observed in species adapted to dry sand areas such as O. gaudichaudii, (Quijon et al, 2001). Many studies indicate that density decreases away from the sea, (Hill and Hunter, 1973; Tureli et al, 2009) and it appears this may be linked to increasing height from the water table and decreased capillary water, (Turra et al, 2005). 1.4.2 Sympatric species The impact of sympatric species has never been examined, perhaps due to the great variation in zonation compared to rocky shores. However, both O. cursor and O. laevis appear to be competitively displaced by the larger O. africana, (Strachan et al, 1999) and O. cordimana, (Fellows, 1975) respectively. 1.4.3 Distribution and crab size There is a clear trend that as crabs increase in size, their distribution migrates landwards, as found in O. quadrata, (Duncan, 1986; Frey and Mayou, 1970; Hill and Hunter, 1973, Fisher and Tevesz, 1979; Turra et al, 2005), O. ceratophthalma, (Hayasaka, 1935; Chakrabati, 1981, Chan et al, 2006), and O. cursor, (Shuchman and Warburg, 1978; Strachan et al, 1999; Tureli et al, 2009). As juveniles have proportionately smaller gills, (Chakrabati, 1981) and loose water faster than adults, (Eshky, 1985), this may be a function of physiological competence, (Fisher and Tevesz, 1979; Chakrabati, 1981; Chan et al, 2006). However, some studies have observed no such trend, even within the same species, (Barrass, 1963; Fellows, 1975; Maccorone and Matthews, 2007; Seike and Nara, 2008; Yong and Lim, 2009). This suggests the relationship between physiology and distribution is not clearcut. (see Drabik-Hamshare, 2010 for review). 1.5 Ocypode burrows: function and distribution of morphotypes Several factors that impact burrow morphology and thus burrowing behaviour should be considered in the context of Ocypode distribution. Burrowing is typical of macroinvertebrates inhabiting soft sediments, (Katrak et al, 2008) and is essential to Ocypode survival, (Wolcott, 1984). Burrows provide several functions, (Berti et al 2008; Tureil et al, 2009) including a refuge from predators, (mainly seabirds), (Cowles, 1908), and a “homebase”, (Browns and Orians, 1970) which feeding, breeding and territorial behaviour orientate around, (Tureil et al, 2009). As Ocypode crabs are ‘quasi-terrestial’, their gills must remain moist (De, 2005) and thus burrows also provide a refuge from surface temperatures, (Chan et al, 2006), a supply of moisture, (Barrass, 1963), and a plugged air column to breathe during tidal submersion, (De, 2005). 1.5.1 Zonal distribution and burrow morphotype Ocypode burrow shape varies intraspecifically and studies have found distinct zonal distributions of different burrow ‘morphotypes’. Although morphotypes vary among populations and beach types, (e.g. Chakrabati, 1981), generally it is found that there is a landward transition from shallow, vertical unbranched tubes to increasingly deeper, sloping, Y shaped burrows, representing the letters IJUY, (Frey and Mayou, 1970; Allen and Curran, 1972; Hill and Hunter, 1973; Shuchman and Warburg, 1978; Vanini, 1980a, Chakrabati, 1981; Chan et al, 2006). Similar transitions have been seen in other burrowing crustaceans, (Perez-Chi et al, 2005; Li et al, 2008; Berti et al, 2008), although other Ocypodidae such as Uca species tend to have consistently, simple ‘J’ shaped burrows, (Koretsky et al, 2002; Lim, 2006). Increased depth may reflect a changing watertable, (Strachan et al, 1999; Tureli et al, 2009) or sediment layer level, (Borradaile, 1903; Barrass, 1963), whilst burrow slope is likely to be a function of crab weight, substrate consistency, (Fellows, 1966), and beach slope, (Duncan, 1986). This transition in increasing complexity often coincides with an increase in crab size, (Frey and Mayou, 1970; Hill and Hunter, 1973; Lighter, 1977; Chakrabati, 1981; Chan et al, 2006). It is thought that juveniles usually dig simple, shallow tubes as their burrows are less permanent as they must often wet their gills at sea, and as they grow larger and become more terrestrial, burrows are more permanent, allowing deeper, complex burrows with added functions in the form of chambers and secondary branches, (Chakrabati, 1981; Chan et al, 2005) which may be associated with copulation, (Milne and Milne, 1946), and/or provide a refuge against up-wash, (Chakrabati, 1981) or predators, (Cowles, 1908). Indeed the temporary burrows of other decapods are often shallow, single tubes, (Braithewait and Talbot, 1972), compared to the complex galleries of species with permanent burrows, (Green, 2004). However, it appears that both the size of the crab and their sub-environment, affect burrow shape, as size increases are not always correlated with this landward burrow shape transition, (Seike and Nara, 2008), and often sharp transitions occur between zones rather than sizes, (Hill and Hunter, 1973; Chakrabati, 1981). Irrespective of age, burrowing behaviour may be affected by temperature, grain size and packing, slope and sand moisture, (Chakrabati, 1981). Interestingly, the suggestion that ghost crabs subject to tidal action maintain pressure equilibrium between external hydrodynamics and internal capillarity by making their burrow larger, with added branches and curves during a single diel tidal cycle, contradicts the findings of previous studies drastically, (De, 2005). 1.6 Social and behavioural implications of Ocypode Burrows The social behaviour of Ocypode has not been studied in great detail, excepting perhaps the behaviour of O. saratan males, O. ceratophthalma and O. laevis, (Linsenmair, 1963; Lighter, 1977). Many studies are observational and this is associated with the difficulty in observing the behaviour of these crabs without disturbing them, (Clayton, 2005). The general focus has been on Ocypode communication and mate choice which have implications for spacing. Although all Ocypode individuals require a burrow for their survival, (Lighter, 1977), burrows may be either transitory or permanent and maintained, and many ‘wanderers’ can temporarily lack a shelter, (Wolcott, 1978; Strachan et al, 1999). 1.6.1 Interactions- agonistic and non-agonistic Generally non-agonistic interactions appear minimal, (Hughes, 1966; Lighter, 1977), whereas defensive or agonistic interactions are typical of communal decapods. Many wandering crabs actively compete with residents for burrows and digging space, e.g. in O. ceratophthalma, (Jones, 1972; Hughes, 1973; Lighter, 1977; Brooke, 1981), O. gaudichaudii, (Schober and Christy, 1993), O. laevis, (Lighter, 1977), O. saratan, (Linsenmair, 1967), O. jousseamei, O. rotundata and O. platyarus, (Clayton, 2005). Interactions are generally ritualised, (Schone, 1968), like many in the genus Uca, (Altevogt, 1957; Crane, 1958). Lighter (1977) found that as crabs developed, not only did their sand disposal behaviour get more complex, but so did their defensive behaviours, with added behavioural repertoires, ‘pushing’ and ‘grappling’ with chelae in O. ceratophthalma and ‘pushaside’, in O. laevis, being added to ‘chase’ and ‘threat’ behaviour observed in juveniles. 1.6.2 Aggregative behaviour Wandering crabs often aggregate at the water’s edge before seeking a burrow, (Vannini, 1976; Wolcott, 1978; Eshky, 1985; Trott, 1988; Strachan et al, 1999; Tureli et al, 2009). This is possibly to forage an unidentified food source, (Strachan et al, 1999) but parallels the seemingly aimless wandering stage exhibited by soldier crabs, (Cameron, 1966). This behaviour has clear social implications, and despite aggregating, individuals maintain individual distances between conspecifics, (Clayton, 2005), similar to that seen during foraging behaviour, (Wolcot, 1978). Such behavioural spacing appears, however, to be overridden when food sources are concentrated, (Hughes, 1966; Lighter, 1977). Burrows appear to be maintained if they are not naturally damaged often, (Hughes, 1966; Vannini, 1980a). For example O. ceratophthalma individuals subject to tidal action and burrow collapse tend to have temporary burrows (Hughes, 1966), whereas supratidal individuals maintain burrows for at least 10 days, (Hughes, 1966). It is not known whether temporary residency has an effect on social interactions, but from studies on other crabs, (Schembri, 1981), reduced agonistic interactions seem likely and is supported by the observation that few competitive interactions are apparent in O. quadrata, (Clayton, 2005). These have temporary burrows and prefer to takeover abandoned burrows, (Fimpel, 1975). In fiddler crabs, long lived burrows can lead to complex social interactions, for example neighbour recognition, (Detto et al, 2006) and territorial coalition, (Backwell and Jennions, 2004). 1.6.3 Female centred competition Some behavioural studies focus their attention on sexually mature males, which show strong burrow defence behaviour, (Linsenmair, 1963). Male Ocypode individuals encounter receptive females unpredictably in time and space due to their highly mobile nature and infrequent matings, (Christy, 1987). Females also do not appear to require a suitable incubation burrow that they cannot provide easily for themselves, unlike most Uca females, (Hughes, 1966). Therefore Ocypode males have evolved ‘female centred competition’, a mating strategy where males attract and defend females, (Christy, 1987). This is rare in decapods and found only in the family Ocypodidae, (Wada, 1981, 1983, 1984; Christy et al, 2001). 1.6.4 Use of burrows by reproductively active males The basis of reproductive activities is the burrow, from which the male ‘advertises’ himself to females, (Christy, 1987) and in which copulation usually occurs, (Netto et al, 2007). These burrows are not part of the spectrum of burrow shapes described in 1.1.5, but spiral tightly in the direction of the handedness of the crab, as a function of digging posture (Barrass, 1963), and differ from the spiral burrows of Uca dug as predator defences, (Basan and Frey, 1977). Some studies appeared to have overlooked these inter-sexual differences, (Barrass, 1963; Hughes, 1966) or concluded no sex differences were apparent due to low female sample sizes, (Chakrabati, 1981; Chan et al, 2005). However, it seems these burrows are dug and defended by males of O. ceratophthalama,(Farrow, 1961; Fellows 1966; Lighter, 1977; Hughes, 1973), O. saratan, (Linsenmair, 1967; Eshky, 1985), O. kuhlii, (Jones, 1972), O. jousseaumei, (Clayton, 2005) and O. gaudichaudii, (Schoeber and Christy, 1993). Some species such as O. occidentalis and O. quadrata have been observed in surface matings, (Hughes, 1973) and do not have spiral burrows. Although copulations have remarkably never been witnessed, (Clayton, 2005), males and females with post-copulation evidence have been found, (Linsenmair, 1967). The androgenic gland is responsible for eliciting spiral burrow construction behaviour in O. ceratophthalama, (Lighter, 1977) and due to its cyclic activity, (Thampy and John, 1970; Payen, 1972), such behaviour appears to be synchronised so that it peaks when tides are lowest (Lighter, 1977). Although juveniles often dispose excavated sand in fan shapes, (thought to decrease burrow conspicuousness), (Hughes, 1966; Jones, 1972; Clayton, 2005) and females and nonreproductively active males in loose, inconspicuous piles, (Lighter, 1977, Hughes, 1973), these active males construct “pyramid” structures. 1.6.5 Pyramid building as sematectonic signals Ocypode construct ‘pyramid’ structures which appear to act as sematectonic signals, (Wilson, 1975). These pyramidal structures are similar to those built by many males of other species, as they play no role in the care for eggs and young, (Linsenmair, 1973). These include the pillars, (Christy, 1988a), chimneys, (Wada and Murata, 2000), mudballs, (Burford et al, 2001), hoods, (Zucker 1981; Clayton, 1998), shelters, (Zucker, 1974), and barricades, (Wada, 1994), built by 17 Uca species, (Burford et al, 2001). Similar to many of Uca sand structures, (Christy, 1988a, 1988b, 2001; Muramatsu, 2010), the pyramids function as highly developed forms of signals for mate attraction, obviating the need for an active display, (Wilson, 2000). These signals are only for long distance attraction, (Linsenmair, 1967), possible only in such flat, featureless habitats as beaches and tidal flats, (Kitaura et al, 2002; Takedo, 2005). Pyramids appear to be analogous to the long distance signal ‘hoods’ constructed by male Uca musica, (Christy et al, 2001) and may similarly attract females though both landmark orientation behaviour, (Herrnkind, 1983) and optical conspicuousness (Zeil and Ak-Mutairi, 1996). Pyramids may be ‘honest’ signals signifying male quality as males do appear to aim for the largest pyramids which are clearly energetically costly, (Linsenmair, 1967) however the signal strength of such ‘displays’, (krebs and Davis, 1993), would have to have little withinmale variation compared to inter-male variation and evidence that females select males with large pyramids, to increase reproductive fitness would have to be shown, (Burford et al, 2001). Pyramids are also important ‘sign-stimuli’ in colony formation by stimulating other reproductively active males to dig pyramids close by. They also enforce other males to construct their own spiral/pyramid complexes at a minimum 134cm distance apart, acting as ‘petrified display signals’, and allowing males to maintain active territories, (Linsenmair, 1967). This may be why pyramids are often built earlier than the rest of the population during daylight hours, allowing visual signals to be strong, (Hughes, 1966). Indeed a nocturnal population of O. saratan appeared to lose the signal function of pyramids, (Linsenmair, 1967). 1.6.6 Pyramid building in enforcing male territories The role of pyramids in enforcing male territories and maintaining neighbour distances is similar to the sand structures of many other Uca species, (Oliveira et al, 1998; Muramatsu, 2010) as well as the mounds built by Ilyopax pingi, (Wada et al, 1994). Whether the pyramids reduce inter-neighbour aggression like many large structures, (Zucker, 1974; Clayton, 1987; Wada, 1994) is unclear, but displacement by dominant intruders does occur. Interestingly these conflicts and displacements are nearly always between crabs of the same handedness, (Linsenmair, 1967), of which there is usually a 1:1 ratio in Ocypode, (Barrass, 1963; Linsenmair, 1967; Eshky, 1985; Strachan et al, 1999). Unless there are large size discrepancies, the owner also usually wins, (Linsenmair, 1967) probably due to a prior residency effect, found in many conflicts between burrowing crustaceans, (Peeke et al, 1995; Jennions and Backwell, 1996; Takahashi et al, 2001). 1.6.7 Visual, acoustic and vibrational communication Other than the structural signals built by males, Ocypode crabs exhibit various acoustic, vibrational and visual behaviours which appear to be intraspecific signals, (Clayton, 2008, Horsch and Salmon, 1969). The function of these have not been conclusively identified, (Popper et al, 2001), partly due to the inability to elicit the natural behaviours during manipulations, (Clayton, 2005). Several behaviours are clearly agonistic, one being the universal ‘threat’ posture taken up by ghost crabs during defensive interactions, (Hughes, 1966; Lighter, 1977; Brooke, 1981; Kuriharaetal, 1989). The carapace is tilted vertically, with the merus held sideways, and the chelae vertically, (Lighter, 1977), characteristic of semi-terrestrial crabs, (Schone, 1968). Brooke, (1981) suggests this is classic threat behaviour, where size is accentuated, much like hair raising seen in mammals. Acoustic and or vibrational signals have had comparatively less research in decapods in general, (Popper et al, 2001). Sounds produced by Ocypode species are species specific and include ‘rapping’, where both chelae drum the ground, (Horch and Salmon, 1969), ‘stridulation’, where stridulatory teeth on the major chelae are rubbed against the crab’s plectrum, (Clayton, 2005) and ‘ tamping’, where sand in patted using chelae and inwardly flexed dactylii, (Warburg and Shuchman, 1979). Many studies tentatively suggest these signals are used by crabs to interact with neighbours, (Alcock, 1892; Cott, 1929; Crane, 1941; Hughes, 1966; Horch and Salmon, 1972;Vannini, 1980a). Clayton (2005) suggests rapping in O. jousseamei is a signal to advertise the crabs territorial presence and increases indiscriminately at the sighting of any specie or sex. Such ‘advertising signals’ are in fact common, in insects, (Dumontier, 1963) and anurans, (Wells, 1977). Stridulation is a close range signal, quickly attenuating due to its higher frequency and is thought to be associated with inter-male burrow defence, (Clayton, 2005). Some studies suggest claw waving and acoustic/ vibrational signals are produced mainly by males, with peaked activity coinciding with lunar cycles suggesting they play a role in female attraction, (Barrass, 1969; Imafuku et al, 2001; Popper et al, 2001). Waving is a widespread signal in Uca to attract females, Imafuku et al, 2001), and has also been observed in O. ryderii, (Vannini, 1976, 1980a), O. stimpsoni, (Wada, 1978; Imafuku et al, 2001), O. gaudichaudii, (Wright, 1968) and O. platytarsus, (Clayton, 2001, 2008). Also similar to certain Uca species, (Altevogt, 1959; Salmon and Stout, 1962; Müller, 1989), these are combined with auditory ‘rapping’ in O. stimpsoni, (Clayton, 2008). O. platytarsus have perhaps the most developed displays involving complex rapping with stridulation and even dances, (Clayton, 2008). Ocypode populations which lack pyramids appear to utilise acoustic signals more frequently , e.g. O. stimpsoni, O. macrocera and O. platytarsus, (Clayton, 2001; Imafuku et al, 2001), whilst species observed in surface matings display little burrow advertisement signalling at all, (Horch and Salmon, 1969). 1.7 Summary Although recent studies are increasing our understanding, (Clayton, 2008), more research is clearly required to understand these communicative behaviours before their impact on conspecifics spacing in Ocypode can be understood, as has been done extensively in many Uca species, (Crane, 1966; Salmon and Atsaides, 1968; Latruffe et al, 1999). The same is true for the impact of environmental factors as discussed above. A good starting point is to identify a population’s dispersion pattern at different spatial scales, (Dale, 1999) as this indicates whether potential negative interactions are occurring between conspecifics (Connell, 1962; MacArthur and Connell, 1966) and whether the environment is homogenous or heterogeneous, (Brown and Orians, 1970; Piou and Feller, 2009). 2. Aims of Study Despite recent interest in their potential as biological indicator species, there is a general lack of knowledge on the ecological and social factors implicated in the distribution and abundance of these organisms. The main aim of this study was therefore to examine the dispersion pattern of active ghost crab burrows found on a sandy beach on Bird Island, in the Seychelles, to gain an insight into both their social nature and the impact of the environment upon their burrows and burrowing behaviour. In association, this study also aimed to investigate whether densities and morphotypes of burrows varied on the beach, and as the size of a crab is likely to have both social and physiological implications, the impact of crab size, determined indirectly from burrow diameter, upon spacing, dispersion and burrow morphology were also examined. Burrows were indistinguishable between two overlapping species, O. ceratophthalma and O. cordimana, and therefore similar to many Ocypode studies, the study aims were not specific to a particular species. Consequently the daytime activities of each species were recorded to identify any species-specific ecological differences. 3. Method 3.1 Study site This study was carried out on a low lying, reef derived sand cay in the Seychelles, figure 1, called Bird Island, (previously known as Île aux Vaches), currently 1.4km2 in size. The climate is seasonal tropical with a mean temperature of 28°C, (Walsh, 1984). The study took place over a period of nine days at the beginning of the NW monsoon period where most rainfall occurs. Humans only began to have a significant impact on the island in the early 20 th century when bird guano extraction from Bird’s sooty turn colony began, shortly followed by a coconut plantation, (Feare, 1979). Today the island is a managed conservation resort with a small permanent human population, (Brook et al, 2009). Figure 1 Central Seychelles showing position of Bird Island in the north. (from Hill et al, 2002). 3.2 Decapod species found at the site Four species of Decapods were observed on the island during the study, colour plate 1. Two individuals of Geograpsus grayi (purple nipper) were observed in the islands centre and Coenobita rugosa hermit crabs were also noticed at the tops of beaches. On the beaches, only two ghost crabs were observed; Ocypode ceratophthalma, found up to 20m inland; and surprisingly Ocypode cordimana, which rarely extends from its inland habitat in to beach areas, (Taylor 1968, 1971). O. cordimana colouration varied considerably, from a dark purple to a pale buff morph, as found by Grubb, (1971) on Aldabra. Ocypode juveniles were undistinguishable, being without pigment or horns until maturity, and were highly cryptic on the white sands. The low species diversity on the beaches is made up for by the large ghost crab population sizes, observed across the Seychelles islands, (Taylor, 1971). Colour plate 1 a) Ocypode cordimana, Purple morph, b) O. cordimana buff morph, c) Ocypode ceratophthalma, d)juvenile ghost crab of unknown species, e) Geographus grayi, f) Coenobita rugosa. (a, d-f taken by author/co-worker whilst b and c were taken by Chapman also on Bird Island: http://planetchapman.net/main.php/v/2008/Seychelles/Bird/DSC_9697.JPG.html) a) b) c) d) e) f) 3.3 Choice of beach for sampling Although only one beach could be effectively sampled, all beaches on the island were observed. Large differences in morphology and crab distribution were noticed, table 1. Beach A was chosen for the study, having many burrows to sample, few tourists, even width and a sheltered location, figure 2. According to Feare, (1979), there is a cycle of erosion and at the time study this would have meant beach A in particular had net deposition. However annually, net erosion is occurring at a rapid rate at that side of the island, (Pers. Comm. with island staff). Figure 2: Arial view of bird island and its beaches (appear blue in photo so are outlined in black for clarity). Beach A was selected for the site of study (shown in yellow). Table 1: Initial observations of crab activity on different beaches illustrated in Figure 1 . A. 16m wide, large numbers of crab burrows of different sizes with even coverage across beach although higher numbers on left side. B. Too windy (burrows filled with sand). Very wide foreshore. Similar crab burrows patterns to A. C. 10m wide (narrower to the East). Burrow numbers much lower, seemingly as wind stronger. Slope also too steep for burrows to be permanent. D. 8m wide, even steeper. Choked with dead trees suggesting recent erosion inland- made data collection impossible. Interestingly many crabs, but only large individuals with few small burrows evident. E. Shallow, 11-14m wide. No crabs were seen and few burrows except at beach top, (mostly small to medium sized). Few scattered pyramids. F. Shallow, 18m wide. Large berm near top where slope was downwards towards vegetation. Long line of pyramids just above swash limit and a few on berm. Many males were visable building, although few juveniles were seen. Beach was good to sample however unfortunately too far from site of stay. G. 80m wide. Start of bird colony. No burrows for first 25m, only found near second berm away from bird colony and towards foreshore. Few mounds just above swash limit. The further from the sea was a clear pattern of increasing burrow size, but at very low densities. H. Very flat 100-175m wide area until berm then sharp drop to sea. Flat devoid of burrows, either due to bird colony or distance from sea. I. 60m wide with few burrows. Mainly small with the occasional large. Bird colony on beach so sampling impossible. J. 15-20m wide (by this time high-tide). Considerable throwing patterns evident around burrows and many large/small burrows. 3.4 Mapping burrows in a ‘point pattern’ Using photographs to map burrow positions (Heywood and Edwards, 1961; Henson, 1961) was not feasible. Mapping burrows in a ‘point pattern’ gave the best opportunity for detailed analysis of spatial distribution, (Diggle, 2003; Illian et al, 2008). The method enables one to distinguish between the two ‘negative/positive’ forms of aggregated dispersion, (Dale, 1999). Such data analyses form an entire subsector of statistics, used extensively in phytosociological studies, (Dale, 1999, Kenneth and Looney, 1985). Beach A was chosen for sampling. Similar studies have used a single large transect, (Lighter, 1977). However four replicate transits of beach A were thought necessary to compensate for local beach variation. 3.5 Data collection for transects of Beach A Transect width was chosen as 10m so that few nearest neighbours would be outside the transect. Transects were plotted with a compass and strings perpendicular to the sea at equal distances along the beach.. These began 1.5 m from the vegetation and continued until the HWM was reached, figure 3. This was as very few burrows were dug in the foreshore and these were all temporary. Figure 3: Diagram of beach and the different zones. Four 10m wide transects placed at intervals of 205m across beach (approximately 680m in length) in the foreshore. Red box shows approximate area when activity of crabs was measured. The first transect was divided into and mapped in 1m2 quadrats and x,y coordinates for each burrow were noted. The largest and smallest diameters of entrances were measured with vernier callipers as an indirect measurement of resident crab carapace length; a strong, linear relationship consistently found in Ocypode including in O. ceratophthalma, (Fellows, 1966; Lighter, 1977; Chan et al, 2006) and O. cordimana, (Moss and Mcphee, 2006). Direction of burrow entrance, steepness, (Vertical, sloped or coiled) were also recorded. Emergence holes, (Barrass, 1963) and abandoned burrows were precluded. Distinction between burrows of O. cordimana and O. ceratophthalma, was difficult as expected (Seike and Nara, 2008). Further notes such as presence of oval shaped mouth and pyramid were recorded and beach characteristics including berms, vegetation and beach slope . The first transect was fully mapped following but did not span the entire zone due to mapping time constraints. Figure 4: Diagram showing the method for transect 1 where contiguous quadrats were mapped. As for all transects it was positioned in the foreshore of the beach 1.5m from vegetation and ending at the upper limit of the swash. This was done three times. Nearest neighbours outside the transect area were counted to negate the problem of delimited area of study (Dale 199; Sinclair 1985) There is a general recognition that Ocypode burrows are inhabited by one individual, (Milne and Milne, 1946; Wolcott, 1978; Quijón et al, 2001) so density was first estimated for each zone (berm or flat) by counting the number of active burrows, a method used frequently in ghost crab studies, (Barros, 2001; Lucrezi et al, 2008; Yong and Lim, 2009). Randomly selecting a sample of burrows using numbered flags, (Pielou, 1969) was found to be unfeasible. Therefore, using a calculator, random coordinates were chosen by finding an X and a Y coordinate along the transect’s length and width. From this point the nearest ‘reference’ burrow was found and the distance between measured to calculate its coordinates, figure 5, a method known as T-square sampling, (Waite, 2000). The distance and compass direction to five nearest neighbours were measured, 5 being so that different spatial scales could be measured, (Thompson, 1956; Dale, 1999 Using the distances and direction from the reference burrow, the coordinates of the neighbouring burrows could be calculated using triangulation, figure 4. This method was independently designed for this study however is in fact used similarly by botanists, (Dale, 1999) although accuracy has been found to be somewhat low, compared to the method used to plot reference burrows, (Mosby, 1959). So that a better comparison could be made with transect 1, this same method was applied to the mapped data (random selection of reference burrows and their neighbours), although using a buffer zone of 1m. This set of data will be referred to as transect 1a. 3.6 Mapping of Nearest Neighbours for transects 2,3 and 4 The three replicates transects were sampled using a ‘short-cut’ nearest neighbour technique (Diggle, 2003) and mapped by triangulation, Figure 5. Figure 5: Mapping of transects 2, 3 and 4. a) Position on beach, b) Up close detailed view. B is the 10m string plotted across the transect at a randomly found height, in which a random width, C, can be found. A is the nearest burrow to C, and P the distance between. The coordinates (X, Y) for burrow A are thus (X)= C and (Y)= B +/-P. Trigonometry was used to calculate the coordinates (x,y) for the five nearest neighbouring burrows such as E, using; A’s coordinates, direction to neighbours, α (measured as the degrees off N), and the distance, Q, to A. B C a) b) Figure 6: Calculation of coordinates, (x,y) for neighbouring burrows, (examples). The corresponding orientation of the right angle triangle for each neighbour meant different equations were needed (see table 2 for entire list) depending on the number of degrees of N. A) y= Y-Q(COS(α+64)) x= X-Q(SIN(α+64)) B) y= Y+Q(SIN(α-26)) x= X-Q(COS (α-26)) C) y= Y+Q x= X D) y= Y+Q(COS (α-116)) x= X+Q(SIN(α-116)) Table 2: Trigonomic equations used to calculate neighbouring burrow coordinates, (x,y), where Y and X are the coordinates for the reference burrow, and α is the direction to the neighbouring burrow, measured in degrees off North (N). Degree’s off N to neighbouring burrow 0° 0° - 25° 26° - 115° 116° 117° - 205° 206° 207° - 295° 296° 297° - 360° y X Y-Q(SIN26) Y-Q(COS(α+64) Y+Q(SIN(α-26)) Y+Q Y+Q(COS (α-116)) Y Y-Q(SIN(α-206)) Y-Q Y-Q(COS(α-296)) X-Q(COS(26)) X-Q(SIN(α+64)) X-Q(COS (α-26)) X X+Q(SIN(α-116)) X+Q X+Q(COS(α-206)) X X-Q(SIN(α-296)) 3.7 Activity data collection A section of beach A, (red boxed area in figure 3), was patrolled every hour in a total of 9 slots throughout the day, from 9:15 to 12: 15 and 15:15 to 18:15. The number of adult O. cordimana and O. ceratophthalma and undetermined juvenile ghost crabs were noted in three areas: the backshore, the foreshore and the transition zone where vegetation grows in a sandy area for approximately 20m inshore. The same walk was made through each area, in the same time period. Throughout the study this area was being pruned of Casuarina bushes, an activity that appeared to expose large amounts of detrital food for O. ceratophthalma but not O. cordimana. 3.7 Observations of adult O. ceratophthalma At night, adult (fully horned and pigmented) O. ceratophthalma came out of their burrows en masse and moved into the transitory zone between the beach and inland area to forage . Densities appeared particularly high, (5 crabs m-2), possibly due to Casuarina pruning activities which appeared to unearth food for the crabs. Individuals, away from their burrows, could be caught with fishing nets, and handedness and sex ratios examined, (growth and sex differences were examined by Drabik-Hamshare, 2010). Capture occurred in the inland area where activity area was measured, figure 3, and although not truly random, crabs were chased until caught so as to ensure no bias towards slower crabs. No juvenile Ocypode were found in this area. Marking carapaces allowed re-caught individuals to be identified; although the population was so open and re-caught individuals too few for a mark, release, recapture analysis. Crabs sex, carapace and horn colours were noted and horns, large and small chelipeds and carapace width and diameter were measured using vernier callipers, figure 7, for around 12 individuals each night. Figure 7: Example of how author measured crab chelae. Measurements of horns were from the top of the eye to horn tip and carapace width was measured from the widest points and length was measured from between the crabs eyes. 4. Analysis of Data The analysis of data collected from burrows held three assumptions; that ‘a single burrow represents the position of a single crab’, ‘burrow diameter represents the carapace length of inhabitants’, and that ‘burrows represent the position of crabs active such that a sample of the population frozen in space and time’. These are considered valid for reasons given in previous sections. Transects usually took two days to cover, and mapping (once formulae had been corrected) revealed few errors suggesting new holes were seldom made or lost during the timescale of the data collection. Many holes were partly narrowed in one dimension so that the widest diameter was used to indicate size of carapace (Area=(Widest burrow diameter/ 2) 2 x π) as used by Barrass, (1963). 4.1 Population distribution1 and density The size distribution of burrows in each transect zone was plotted using burrow diameter. Any individuals sampled twice were excluded. The numbers of juveniles and adults between transects were analysed using a chi square test of independence. O. ceratopthalama and O. cordimana generally reach sexual maturity is 33mm (Haley, 1973; Lighter, 1977), and 30mm, (Jackson et al, 1991) in carapace length respectively. Therefore burrows less than 30mm wide were considered to be dug by non-sexually mature ‘juveniles’ and those 30mm or greater by mature ‘adults’. As in reality, burrow size is not accurate to the mm, ( Fellows, 1966; Lighter, 1977; Chan et al, 2006), these represent only approximate estimations. 1 The term ‘population’ is used in the results section, however it is important to remember this study examined two populations collectively due to lack of discrimination between O. cordimana burrows and O. ceratophthalma burrows and that this term is therefore somewhat redundant. 4.1.1 Population density Density was estimated from the number of active burrows across the beach. Although dispersion patterns were examined, no account was taken of environmental gradients across beaches, such as in moisture, (Shuchman and Warburg, 1978), exposure, (Quijon et al, 2001) and sand packing, (Hughes, 1966). A kruskal-wallace test was used to compare the medians of densities recorded in specified beach zones, found to be non-normally distributed with a kolmogorov-smirnov test, (P<0.05). The number of ghost crabs on Beach A was then estimated using the average. 4.1.2 Crab activity Chi squared test goodness of fit tests were used on analyses examining the frequencies of crabs. Tests were conducted using a Yates correction and only carried out when expected values were 5 or greater. 4.2 Dispersion pattern of Ocypode burrows Evans, (1952) suggests that different tests for non-randomness can under certain circumstances give conflicting results, such as those described by Grieg-Smith, (1983). Therefore more than one test of non-randomness should always be used, with further tests then carried out as needed, (Ludwig and Reynolds, 1988). 4.2.1 Frequency distributions among quadrats One can compare the frequency distribution of burrows amongst quadrats to that expected of a Poisson distribution, i.e. the null hypothesis, complete spatial randomness, (CSR). If random, the mean to variance ratio should approximate one, (1 ≈ S2 / ). Significant deviations suggesting non-randomness, (the alternate hypothesis), were tested by calculating the index of dispersion ID, which is approximately distributed as 2. The critical limits used were, 0.95 and 0.05, and degrees of freedom, n-1, (Southwood and Henderson, 2000) Where: is the number of events at i= 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 burrows 1m-2 f is frequency and n is the sample size is the mean SE is the standard error The second method is to calculate expected frequencies according to a Poisson distribution and use a Chi square goodness of fit to test for a significant deviation from observed (O) distributions. A Poisson is particularly applicable if the density per quadrat is low as was found in this study, (Kenneth and Looney, 1985). The chi square value was calculated using: Where expected frequencies (E) = For transect 1, complete mapping allows such analyses to be carried out on contiguous quadrats, (Dale, 1999). Such an analysis is very valuable to a study on a point pattern, (Dale, 1999; Diggle, 2005) and the area fully mapped in the other transects was calculated by plotting circles centred on each primary burrow with the radius stretching as far as the fifth nearest neighbour. Caution must be taken however as individuals rather than quadrats were selected randomly and gaps are likely to be under-sampled if burrows were severely aggregated. Therefore transect 1a was compared with 1 for differences which would suggest this method had a bias. 4.2.2 Nearest Neighbour Analysis To analyse the degree of a spatial pattern at different scales, one can plot mean square variance against quadrat (block) size to identify scales where aggregation or regularity is occurring, (Kenneth and Kershaw, 1973; Dale, 1999). Only in transect 1 would such an ANOVA be possible however. Ideally such analyses should be used on multiple samples to see if the observed pattern was a chance occurrence, (Kenneth and Looney, 1985). Nearest neighbour analyses form a useful alternative, commonly used in studies short of time and manpower, (Clark and Evans, 1954; Dale, 1999) and are particularly useful to studies upon burrowing animals, (Hairston, 1959). Whether consistencies with the above analyses occur can also be examined, (Southwood and Henderson, 2000). For this study Thompson’s statistic, was used, (Thompson, 1956), an extension of Clark and Evans, (1954). This enables the dispersion pattern to be perceived over a larger area, as well as more accurately, (Southwood and Henderson, 2000). This analyses was valid as the density was measured directly for the entire transect and the requirements of “individuals in a continuum”, (Pielou, 1969) was considered fulfilled. Due to small sample sizes, (>30), the correction factor suggested by Clark and Evans, (1954) was used, (n-1). 4.3 The relationship between crab size and distance to neighbours Space can be viewed as a resource and if limited become the object of competitive interactions, (Lighter, 1977). A regular dispersion would indicate that Ocypode individuals are maintaining a distance between each other, due to negative interactions such as territorial behaviour. If the distance between borrows depends on the size of the burrows in question, this might indicate …….a despotic ideal distribution REFERNCE? 4.3.1 Size of burrow effect on nearest neighbour For each transect, regressions were conducted on size of burrow and distance to nearest neighbours. A kolmogorov-smirnov test found the data to be non-normal, (P<0.05). From a variety of transformations, a log transformation yielded the smallest K-S value and the largest probability that data does not deviate from a normal distribution. This result was therefore used. However, given that two transects had skewed as opposed to normal distributions, results should be treated with caution. To take into account the impact of neighbour’s size, a second regression was done on average size of a burrow and nearest neighbour and the distance between them. Although again the residuals of two transects were non-normally distributed, according to a K-S test, (P>0.05), transformations did not improve normality and were subsequently not used. Both regressions were also conducted on the collated data from transects 2, 3 and 4 as these had similar densities and dispersions. Finally the mean distance to the first three nearest neighbours was found for juveniles and adults and compared using a student’s t-test to see if adults have statistically greater individual spacing even if nearest neighbour distances did not increase linearly with size of crab. 4.4 Dispersion of different juveniles and adults A final analysis was also conducted separately on the dispersion of small, (>2.0cm), medium, (2.0 to 3.99cm) and large individuals, (3.0 to 3.99cm). This was achieved using the mapped transect and comparing their distribution amongst quadrats to a Poisson distribution using a goodness of fit. Data from all transects were also combined in a test for ‘segregation’, (Pielou, 1961), between juveniles and adults. The procedure used is usually one to test for association between two species and in this case compared the observed frequencies of nearest neighbor pairings of juveniles and adults to the expected when the null hypothesis of independence is assumed, (Waite, 2000), in a typical chi square contingency table. Pielou’s coefficient of segregation was also found (S), where 1 indicates complete segregation between juveniles and adults, 0 un-segregated and -1 that pairings between species/types occur in isolated pairs (full association): 5. Results 5.1 Beach morphology The beach was generally divided into zones of berms which are deposits of beach material forming the active shoreline at high tide, or by storms. These are separated by flats, although there were differences in the position and width of these, as well as differences in height from sea level, Figure 5.1. The high tide mark was indicated by the third berm, however the HTSM, and thus the upper intertidal zone was unknown, but likely to be indicated by the second berm, (Short, 1996). The width of the foreshore varied with the tides and along the beach, between 4 and 10m, and the backshore varied between 35.5 and 38.5m. Figure 5.1: Cross sections, (a), and aerial view of transects, (b), showing slopes and berms. Transects 2, 3 and 4 were measured up to the HWM (the highest point of the swash zone or foreshore, as shown by a steeper slope and blue). a) Transect 1 did not reach the HWM HWM and maximum limit of swash Transect 4 Transect 3 Transect 2 Transect 1 b) HWM Transect 1 HWM Transect 2 HWM Transect 3 HWM Transect 4 Distance from top of transects/beach (m) 5.2 Population Distribution The nearest neighbor method recorded around half of the total transect areas and burrows, suggesting results should amply represent the total transect, table 5.1. The population distribution of burrows between transects are not identical but do show similarities, (figure 5.2). Although transect 1 lacks data for crabs near the H.W.M., which according to a linear relationship found by Drabik-Hamshare, (2010) is likely to have mostly juveniles, a bimodal shape is apparent in each transect. A large peak is observed in the number of crabs approximating 1-2cm in carapace length, (which tended to be unpigmented) and a smaller, less consistent peak in mature crabs approximating 3-5cm. The proportion of non-sexually mature crabs and sexually mature crabs, table 5.1, was significantly different between transects, (X2cal= 21.31 > X2tab=16.27, P=0.001, d.f= 3, Chi square test of independence). Table 5.1 Percentage of transects sampled and juvenile/ adult ratio % of burrows sampled % transects mapped Juvenile/ adult ratio Transect 1 100% 100% 2.8 Transect 2 47% 55% 1.3 Transect 3 59% 46% 1.6 Transect 4 53% 56% 4.0 Figure 5.2 Relative population distributions of Ocypode crabs (O. ceratopthalama and O. cordimana combined) determined indirectly from maximum burrow diameters Frequency (%) Maximum burrow diameter (cm) 5.3 Density of burrows Overall, density appears to increase along the beach, figure 5.3. This is very slight for the first three transects, from 0.60 to 0.67 crab m-2, but sharply increases at transect 1 at 0.95 crab m-2, table 5.2. Although density was not measured here up until the HWM, higher densities were consistent along the first 20m that were measured. The use of differing methods necessitates caution when comparing densities, however despite perhaps more detailed burrow counts in the first transect, (counts per quadrat rather than counts per zone), higher densities did seem apparent in the field on transect 1 and burrows were easy enough to spot in both methods. Table 5.2 Active burrow (crab ) density of both Ocypode ceratophthalma and Ocypode cordimana on four transects Transect No. burrows Density area (number of burrows Inside transect Outside transect* (m2) per m2) Transect 1 200 190 0 0.950 Transect 2 385 91 33 0.665 Transect 3 355 113 21 0.647 Transect 4 360 101 33 0.597 * Neighbours found outside transect Figure 5.3 Changes in density of Ocypode burrows along the approximately 680m beach 5.4 Population estimate The use of burrows has been shown to be an accurate estimation of density and population of Ocypode crabs, (Warren, 1990; Barrass, 1963; Buffer and Bird 2002; Barros, 2001). If the average backshore width is used to estimate the area covered by the population, (24820m2), then the estimated population size is 17,740 Ocypode individuals based on the average density of 0.7418 burrows m-2. However the standard error is high, (±3946 crabs, P=0.05) and unlikely to be a good estimate, given the change in density along the beach. Overall it is clear however that on beach A alone, the ghost crabs numbered in their thousands, although it is unclear if one species predominated. A daily mark capture release on foraging adult O. ceratophthalma failed to provide a estimate of their numbers due to high immigration and emigration rates and of a total of 80 caught, only 7 were re-catches, (see Drabik-Hamshare, 2010). 5.5 Zonal distribution Densities changes were therefore examined in the mapped transect, (1) figure 5.4. No clear increase or decrease along the beaches cross-section is observed, although density was highest either side of the upper berm. The density in different zones (berms and flats) also did not consistently increase or decrease towards the sea, with different patterns seen between transects. The variation within zones was also larger than that between zones, figure 5.5. A kruskal-wallis test accordingly found no significance between the median densities of each zone, (X2cal= 2.78< X2tab= 9.49, P=0.05), although as zones were at different heights and distance from the sea their affect on abundance may have been confounded. Figure 5.4 Density of Ocypode crabs per m2 across beach cross section to illustrate zonal distribution H.W.M. Figure 5.5 Differences in median density of Ocypode burrows between beach zones and interquartile range 5.6 Burrows types Burrow type did not appear to be associated with a given distance from the sea as expected, figure 5.6, with large variation found between transects. Vertical burrows were the only type found at <22m from the H.W.M. 9% of burrows were coiled, but only 80% of these appeared to be dug by mature crabs (>30mm burrow diameter). The ratio of right or left turning coiled burrows did not differ significantly from 1:1, (X2=0.681, <0.05, chi square goodness of fit), similarly to the 1:1 ratio found in caught individuals, (See Drabik-Hamshare, 2010). Sloping burrows were dug both by juveniles (60%) and mature crabs, (40%) whilst vertical burrows were mostly dug by small juveniles, (84% >2cm in width). Figure 5.6 Frequencies of burrow types at different distances from the H.T.M expressed as fractions of total with standard deviation showing large variation between transects Frequency of burrows (expressed as fraction of total) Coiled burrows Sloped burrows Vertical burrows Distance from H.T.M 5.7 Crab activity Overall twice as many mature O. ceratophthalma than O. cordimana were observed active during the day, (see table 5.3 for significant daily differences), with unidentified juveniles always seen in low numbers, mostly due to their crypsis and tendency to remain near burrows. Juveniles appeared to be more diurnal than adults, peaking around 3:00. Mature Ocypode were mostly nocturnal; O. cordimana activity peaked later than mature O. ceratophthalma, around dusk, and at night was seen in large numbers particularly inland. O. ceratophthalma activity gradually peaked earlier from 16:15 to 14:15 in both the beach and the transition where their burrows did not reach, possibly relating to the shifting high-tide, (see appendix 7.7 for daily totals and tidal changes). Very few juveniles or mature O. cordimana were observed in the transition zone, figures 5.8 and 5.9, although many O. cordimana were observed there later at night. In O. ceratophthalma however, no significant difference in numbers were observed between the two zones2, figure 5.8, and the overall difference between O. cordimana and O. ceratophthalma was not significant on the beach, figure 5.7, (X2= 0.0105, P<0.05, chi square goodness of fit). Daily variations appeared to be affected by weather as cloudy days coincided with peak activity. The 3 rd day was a full moon; and unusually low numbers of mature Ocypode in the evening occurred although this was likely a factor of heavy rain. Table 5.3 Total recorded crabs each day and significant difference between using Chi squared test for goodness of fit Day 1 2 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 Day 6 Day 7 Chi square goodness of fit only possible on data collected 4:15 onwards due to low expected frequencies, (>5), however at times tested, P<0.05, (see appendix, 7.10). Total Observed O. ceratophthalma 81 O. cordimana 46 Expected same for both 63.5 species (1:1 ratio) X2 Chi Square 9.65** 126 112 119 42 23 32.5 132 39 85.5 128 50 89.0 206 76 141.0 144 52 98 0.83 5.57* 50.58*** 34.19*** 59.93*** 43.18*** *P=0.05, **P=0.01, ***P=0.001 Figure 5.7 Average activity recorded during the day for O. ceratophthalma and O. cordimana on the beach Figure 5.8 Average activity recorded during the day for O. ceratophthalma and O. cordimana in the transition zone Figure 5.9 Average activity recorded during the day for juvenile Ocypode in both the beach and transition zone 859 398 628.5 - 5.8 Dispersion pattern: using frequencies in quadrats Transects maps are shown in colour plate 5.10. Maps f) and g) show the quadrats fully mapped using the nearest neighbor method. The densities calculated from these quadrat samples were found to be very different to the actual densities measured, Table 5.4. This is the opposite to the expected inflation of densities under the identified potential bias of quadrats selection. As a different result is concluded when the mapped data from transect 1 is sampled and mapped quadrats found under the nearest neighbor method, (transect 1a); that the quadrat data is indeed biased, even if not in the way expected seems likely. This combined with the slight error in plotting coordinates by triangulation of angles, (Mosby, 1969), means that only transect 1 where contiguous quadrats were mapped is considered reliable although the others are nonetheless indicated below. Although the use of nearest neighbour data mapping by triangulation to extract quadrat frequency data potentially forms a way of quickly collecting data for a usually laborious point pattern analysis, it seems that due to non random quadrat selection, this method may only prove useful in allowing a map of samples to be constructed, 5.10 a) to e). Similar to Pielou’s (1959) hypothetical dispersion pattern, the measured dispersion patterns differ between the two dispersion methods, demonstrating the need for a nearest neighbor analysis of dispersion. Even so transect 1 which is considered reliable is indicated by the index of dispersion, table 5.5, and the Poisson goodness of fit, table 5.6, (in bold), to be significantly regular, (P=0.05 and P<0.005 respectively), and the deviation from CSR is demonstrated seen in figure 5.10a. Table 5.4 Number of both Ocypode ceratophthalma and Ocypode cordimana burrows in quadrats Transect 1 1a 2 3 4 No. of burrows (in quadrat sample) 190 55 78 65 88 Number of fully mapped 1m2 quadrats Inside Outside transect transect 200 0 47 1 159 50 139 18 179 53 sample density (burrows 1m -2) 1.146 0.373 0.414 0.379 Actual density (burrows 1m -2) 0.950 0.950 0.665 0.647 0.597 Table 5.5 Index of dispersion for Ocypode burrows across four transects. Transe ct Variance (S2) Mean ( ) 1 1.143 0.95 Varianc e to mean ratio (S2 / ) 1.203 Index of Degree dispersio s of n (ID) freedo m (n1)* 239.47 189 tabulate tabulate d 2 when p= 0.05 d 2 when p= 0.95 232.91 167.36 Regular 1a 1.000 1.146 0.872 41 47 64.00 32.27 Random 2 0.422 0.277 1.523 284.80 208 219.91 156.37 Regular 3 0.334 0.411 0.812 141.34 156 205.78 144.49 Clumped 4 0.425 0.361 1.178 273.19 231 268.53 197.74 Regular * Note, n is the total no. of samples (quadrats) whether inside or outside desingated quadrat areas. Distributi on Table 5.6 Goodness of fit test for observed burrow distribution among quadrats compared to that expected under a Poisson distribution. Transect 1 Transect 1a Transect 2 Transect 3 Transect 4 Degrees of freedom (categories) 2 1 1 1 1 No. of quadrats 200 55 78 65 88 Calculated 10.5690 1.41642 0.40928 1.20023 0.890838 2 Probability level (p) 0.005 0.234 0.522 0.273 0.345 Distribution Regular Random Random Random Random Figure 5. 10 Frequency distribution of burrows in 1m2 quadrats (dark gray bars) compared to expected following a Poisson (or random) distribution, (light gray line). Frequency of quadrats a) Transect 1 c) Transect 3 b) Transect 2 d) Transect 4 4 Number of burrows in 1m2 quadrats 5.9 Dispersion of Ocypode burrows: using nearest neighbour distances Maps of the nearest neighbor clusters are shown in 5.10, a) to e). The nearest neighbor analysis, shows that dispersion did differ at different scales. At a fine scale, of the first nearest neighbor distance (average 82cm), the burrows were significantly regular, in agreement with the results above (transect 1), suggesting the quadrat size chosen was the correct size for the density of burrows encountered, (Dale, 1999). At a greater scale, of the second nearest neighbor, on average 119cm away, and of the third nearest neighbor, on average 147cm away, the burrows tend towards a significantly clumped distribution, table 5.7. Table 5.7: Distribution based on extension of Clark and Evans, (1954) nearest neighbour analysis, (Thompson, 1956) Transect 1a Transect 2 Transect 3 Transect 4 Scale of 1st nearest neighbour Scale of 2nd nearest neighbour Scale of 3rd nearest neighbour R1 Critical Distribution R2 Critical Distribution R3 Critical Distribution limits limits limits 1.235 >1.209, Regular 1.167 >1.145, Regular 1.115 <1.118 Random P=0.05 P=0.05 >0.882, p=0.05 1.254 >1.224, Regular 0.800 <0.845, Clumped 0.794 <0.845, Clumped P=0.05 P=0.05 P=0.01 1.333 >1.275, Regular 0.816 <0.848, Clumped 0.821 <0.845, Clumped P=0.05 P=0.05 P=0.01 1.363 >1.275, Regular 0.749 <0.749, Clumped 0.749 <0.845, Clumped P=0.05 P=0.01 P=0.01 5.10 Dispersion pattern of different size categories and mature males Although burrows collectively were dispersed regularly, when separate size categories were examined, the distribution of small burrows in quadrats (>2.0cm) did not significantly deviate from expected assuming CSR, whilst burrows 3.0cm or larger did significantly deviate from CSR with more burrows clumping than expected, table 5.8 and figure 5.10b. The observed frequencies of nearest neighbour pairings between juveniles and adults were fewer than the expected frequencies under the null hypothesis of independence, table 5.9, suggesting segregation between juveniles and adults, (S=0.722). However this difference was not significant, (X2cal= 0.722 < X2tab = 3.84, P=0.05) and therefore the null hypothesis was accepted. Table 5.8 Dispersion pattern of burrows in three size categories using Poisson goodness of fit test Degrees of No. of Calculated X2 Probability level Dispersion freedom burrows small 1 106 1.819 0.177 Random (>2.0cm) medium (2.0 1 37 4.0382 0.044 Clumped to 3.99 cm) large (>4.00 1 47 8.1194 0.004 Clumped cm) Table 5.9 Goodness of fit test for segregation between juveniles and adults (S= Pielou’s coefficient of segregation). A= observed Expected chi square relative freq assuming A and B juveniles occur independently of each B= other adults AA 41 38.54 0.0555 A 0.602151 AB 15 17.46 0.6865 B 0.397849 BA 23 25.46 0.4708 A' 0.688172 BB 14 11.54 0.1854 B' 0.311828 Total 93 93 1.3982 S= 0.722 Unfortunately only 2-3 of the burrows sampled in trasnsects 2-4 and 13 out of 190 burrows on transect 1 were coiled. Therefore a separate analysis of the dispersion of these was not possible, despite the potentially clumped distribition of half the burrows, occurring in one area near the H.W.M on transect 1, figure 5.10. However as some coiled burrows, (section 5.6), and many oval shaped burrows were dug by crabs smaller than 30mm, using these featrues to identify ‘copulation’ burrows does not seem reliable. Evidence of a nearby pyramid is likely to be the best indicator of sexually mature males, however identifying the associated burrow is difficult due to the distance between. Nearby burrows to pyramids are nonetheless shown in green, figure 5.10. Figure 5.10 Position of coiled burrows (in red) amongst the rest of the population, (blue) 5.13 Impact of burrow and therefore crab size upon distance to nearest neighbours Despite the finding that crabs were digging burrows in a regular distribution, suggesting negative interactions, crab size, (measured as burrow diameter) did not appear to have an effect on individual spacing, table 5.9. This lack of correlation was clear, figure 1.10, and despite the invalidation of normality in transects 1 and 4, it is believed this result is reliable considering the clear lack of correlation depicted in plotted data, figure 1.10. Table 5.9 Effect of log burrow size on log average distance to three nearest neighbours Transect 1 Transect 2 Transect 3 Transect 4 Combined Regression results Normality plot **F1,22= 0.45, P=0.510 *F1,19= 0.12, P=0.736 *F1,22= 0.00, P=0.944 **F1,22= 0.84, P=0.368 *F1,91=0.17, P=0.685 skewed K-S test results log burrow log av. Distance to diameter NN P>0.150 P>0.150 normal P<0.010 P>0.150 normal P<0.010 P>0.150 skewed P<0.030 P>0.150 normal p<0.010 P>0.150 Figure 5.10 Example of lack of correlation between log burrow size and log average distance to nearest neighbours, (see appendix for other transects) Even if the size of the nearest neighbour was taken into account, no effect of size of burrows was found upon distance between them, table 5.9. Although two transects showed higher F values, (transects 3 and 4), they each suggested opposing correlations, figure 5.12. Lastly, when burrows were divided into immature and mature sizes, no difference in the mean distance to the first three nearest neighbours were found, figure 5.11. The mean nearest neighbour distances of juveniles and adults are shown in figure 5.12. No statistical difference was found, (T= -0.390, P=0.70, d.f=91). Table 5.10 effect of combined burrow size of two neighbouring burrows on the distance between them Regression results Assumptions of normality met K-S test results log average burrow Distance to NN diameters Transect 1 F1,22= 0.48, P=0.494 normal P=0.444 Transect 2 F1,19= 0.06, P=0.811 skewed P<0.010 Transect 3 F1,22= 3.31, P=0.082 normal P<0.010 Transect 4 F1,22= 4.45, P=0.075 normal P<0.030 Combined* F1,91=0.01, P=0.918 skewed P=>0.150 *only transects 2,3 and 4 with similar densities and dispersion patterns P>0.150 P=0.047 P<0.010 P=0.132 P<0.010 Figure 5.11 Interval Plots of mean distance to nearest neighbour for burrows dug my mature and immature crabs (burrows >30mm and 30mm or greater, respectively). 95% CI for the Mean Mean distance to nearest neighbour, (cm) 140 130 120 110 100 immature mature Figure 5.11 Combined burrow size of nearest neighbours against the distance between them.