Summary of: Penke, L. & Asendorpf, J.B. (2008). Evidence for Conditional Sex Differences in Emotional but Not in Sexual Jealousy at the Automatic Level of Cognitive Processing. European Journal of Personality, 22:3-30. Summary by Haley Andrews, Elizabeth Haralambos, and Andrea Hollingshead For Dr. Mill’s Psyc 310 class, Spring 2012 As we have discussed in class, evolutionary psychological hypotheses suggest that in the case of infidelity, women tend to be more jealous of emotional infidelity, whereas men tend to be more jealous of sexual infidelity. This article reviews two evolutionary hypotheses regarding jealousy towards infidelity, and proposes a further study to combat some of the problems with previous studies on this issue. The authors suggest that inconsistencies lie in the results of previous findings, and they can be resolved when the two hypotheses are evaluated separately and when the underlying cognitive processes are considered. ‘Jealousy’ is a concept in many cultures that, in its broadest meaning, describes affective responses to a real or imagined situation where a personally highly valued possession is threatened to be lost to someone else (e.g. Brochhaus-Enxyklopadie, 1996; Merriam-Webster’s Collegiate Dictionary, 1993). According to Penke and Asendorpf, sex differences in romantic jealousy have been hotly debated, arguably because they serve as a prominent testing ground for an evolutionary psychological approach to sex differences in social cognition and emotion (Penke & Asendorpf, 2008; e.g. Harris, 2003, 2005; Sagarin, 2005). Penke and Asendorpf summarize the classic evolutionary hypothesis: it assumes that men and women react differently to sexual and emotional infidelity because these two types of infidelity posed different adaptive problems. Men are expected to be more jealous to sexual infidelity that women in order to minimize investment in genetically unrelated offspring. Women, on the other hand, are expected to be more jealous to emotional infidelity that men in order to minimize loss of paternal investment in their offspring (Penke & Asendorpf, 2008). In a critique of a previous study by Buss et al., these are sex-typical adaptive problems faced by all mammalian species with internal fertilization and biparental care and Buss et al have assumed that humans have evolved psychological mechanisms that sensitize men to cues of their mate’s sexual infidelity and women to cues of their mate’s emotional infidelity. This focuses on domain specific cognitive modules, in which Buss proposed sex-specific cognitive modules that solved these sex-specific adaptive problems, also known as the “jealousy as a specific ‘innate’ module hypothesis” by Harris, 2000. In the case of this hypothesis, this evolutionary account consists of two independently derived hypotheses, one being sexual infidelity and the other emotional infidelity, and that none of them, one of them, or both of them may be true. The contrasting evolutionary theory by Harris (2000, 2003) proposed a ‘social-cognitive theory of jealousy’, where jealousy is the result of an evolved, but domain-general appraisal mechanism (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). In this module, Harris argues that this mechanism is sensitive to all kinds of threats posed by rivals. In other words, a mate’s sexual or emotional infidelity poses a threat to a relationship, and therefore another variable such as culturally determined gender roles may play a part. The authors go in depth about prior research, offering all possible limitations and interactions that may have an effect on the significance of previous findings to give way to their own study. To test the two evolutionary hypotheses Buss et al., as well as other subsequent studies, used a forced-choice paradigm. In these studies, participants are presented with a hypothetical dilemmas that contrast sexual and emotional infidelity and are forced to choose the more distressing alternative. In this study, there were four sub hypotheses of a sex by type of infidelity interaction: (a) men react more jealous to sexual infidelity that women, (b) women react more jealous to emotional infidelity that men, (c) men react more jealous to sexual infidelity that to emotional infidelity, and (d) women react more jealous to emotional infidelity than to sexual infidelity. According to Harris, a-d should be expected from the specific ‘innate’ model perspective of sexual and emotional jealousy, the two sex differences (a) and (b), and the two within sex differences (c) and (d) (Penke & Asendorpf, 2008). The problem with this study, according to Penke and Asendorpf, is that the forced choice paradigm only tests whether one of the two evolutionary hypotheses or both are supported, not whether both are confirmed. Penke and Asendorpf propose to reconstruct this study for four reasons: (1) there are two different evolutionary hypotheses, (2) confirming both requires a crossover sex type of infidelity interaction, (3) ordinal sex by type of infidelity interactions violate at least on of the two evolutionary hypotheses and (4) sex differences in the forced choice paradigm may be due to particularly strong male sexual jealousy, to particularly strong female emotional jealousy, or both. Thus an evaluation of the two hypotheses requires additional tests that evaluate the sex difference for sexual jealousy and the sex difference for emotional jealousy separately (Penke & Asendorpf, 2008). Additionally, the evolutionary approach has empirically falsifiable hypotheses, whereas the social-cognitive theory does not predict any specific between-sex or within-sex differences in emotional and sexual jealousy. As far as empirical evidence goes, the forced-choice paradigm in the Harris study confounds sex differences in sexual and emotional jealousy preventing strong conclusions because most studies had only two dilemmas to choose from and it gives no information about the cognitive processes that underlie the sex differences in choices. As an alternative to the forced-choice paradigm, other studies used the separate evaluation of sexual and emotional infidelity to make sure that jealousy was not confounded. Using negative emotional ratings to evaluate sex differences in sexual and emotional infidelity, however, failed to confirm the expected sex by type of infidelity interaction. Few other studies looked at the cognitive processing of infidelity cues, in which a reaction time was measured between the presentation of the forced-choice dilemma and the decision. These modules were seemingly consisted with evolutionary hypotheses for emotional jealousy, but not for sexual jealous. Thus, the insignificant findings coupled with the small amount of research on cognitive processes gives way to Penke and Asendorpf’s present study. The aim of their study was to test the two evolutionary hypotheses with both the forcedchoice method and continuous emotion ratings for many dilemmas in a sample of sexually experienced young adults. To approach the underlying cognitive processes, Penke and Asendorpf studied the automatic of the responses in the forced-choice task in two conditions: cognitive constraint condition and deliberation condition. In the cognitive constraint condition, participants were instructed to rely on their spontaneous preferences, while simultaneously remembers a sequence of numbers. In the deliberation condition, participants were instructed to take their time, vividly imagining scenarios as to make a careful decision. In this case decisions, emotion ratings, and response times were all recorded. This study was thus designed to evaluate previous studies’ designs separately with respect to the two evolutionary hypotheses when processing infidelity cues (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). Additionally, three tasks were presented in the same order for all participants to make a comparison of results for different methods possible within the same sample. First, participants completed the forced-choice under cognitive constraint (strongest influence). Second, they completed emotion ratings with vivid imagination (intermediate influence). And third, they completed forced-choice with deliberation and vivid imagination (weakest influence). Penke and Asendorpf expected (1) the classic sex difference in the forced-choice paradigm under both conditions, particularly under cognitive constraint, (2) that women rate emotional infidelity situations more negatively than men, but that men do not rate sexual infidelity situations more negatively than women and (3) that women process emotional infidelity cues faster than men, but that men do not process sexual infidelity cues faster than women (Penke & Asendorpf. 2008). The authors also looked at extraneous factors of romantic relationship status and education on these effects. The study was comprised of 284 participants (71 unmarried couples and 142 singles, 141 men and 143 women, age M = 23.7 years, SD = 2.7) who were recruited for a study on “Love, Sexuality, and Personality” by flyers and postings in a number of public places with the incentive of sixteen euro and personal feedback in return for participation. The study began with the forced-choice with cognitive constraint, and participants were instructed to evaluate situations that might occur in a relationship when you are distracted by another task. Participants were presented with a six digit series that they were asked to keep in mind, presented with descriptions that might occur in a relationship, and then asked to recall the six digits. They were asked to spontaneously decide each time which of the two presented dilemmas upset or distressed them more, as well as remember the digits. They were given 14 different pairs of situations that included six infidelity dilemmas adapted from the Buss et al. (1992, 1999) and eight distractors, and the order was identical for all participants. The decision, reaction time between the presentation of all 14 dilemmas and the participant’s decision, and the correctness of the remembered number were all recorded. In the next portion of the study, continuous emotion ratings, participants were instructed to report without being distracted what they felt in six situations. They were asked to imagine the situations vividly and realistically and then rate the situations for the amount of anger, anxiety, jealousy, and humiliation on a five-point scale from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely’. They were presented in a fixed alternating order of an event of emotional or sexual infidelity. Ratings and reaction times were recorded for each individual rating. Lastly, participants completed the forced-choice with deliberation portion of the study. They were once again presented with the six pairs of situations and asked to choose which one would distress or upset them more. In this category, participants were instructed to take their time to imagine the situation as vividly and realistically as possible to make a decision according to their respective feelings. The same six jealousy dilemmas used in the cognitive load condition were presented in the same fixed order, and the decisions and reaction times for each response were recorded. In the forced-choice under cognitive constraint condition, 16% of the participants correctly remembered the six-digit number for all six dilemmas, 61% made two or more errors and two participants failed every time to remember the number. A series of t tests were used to analyze sex differences in the preference scores for emotional infidelity relative to sexual infidelity. The test revealed a significant, moderately large effect for all choices of all participants, confirming that more women (77%) than men (63%) judged emotional infidelity as more distressing than sexual infidelity (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). Through a sex x education ANOVA indicated that the sex difference was moderated by the educational level of the participants. The less educated participants demonstrated a stronger sex difference, where as the better educated participants showed a small sex difference. Sex differences in the reaction times were analyzed separately for sexual and emotional infidelity choices. It was found that, decisions for sexual infidelity were as fast as decisions for emotional jealousy and men and women did not differ in their decision time. However, the sex difference was different for sexual and emotional infidelity. Later t tests showed that women were faster than men when they chose emotional infidelity, where as an opposite inclination for sexual infidelity was not significant (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). In the emotion ratings condition there was a significant, moderately large main effect of sex, in that women rated the infidelity situations more negatively than men. It was then suggested to analyze the sex effects separately for each infidelity type. Post hoc t tests showed a significant effect of sex for ratings of jealousy, anxiety, and humiliation but not for anger. It was revealed that women reported overall more negative emotions for both sexual and emotional infidelity, with a slightly larger effect size for emotional jealousy. It was also found that the education effect was mainly driven by stronger emotional jealousy of less educated women. In regards to the sex differences in the reaction times, the reaction time data for the emotion ratings did not show any significant effects of sex, type of infidelity, type of emotion, education or their interactions (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). In the forced choice with deliberation condition, a significant, moderately large effect confirmed that more women than men judged emotional infidelity as more distressing than sexual infidelity. A sex x education ANOVA showed that this sex differenced was moderated by the participant’s educational level. This indicated that the less educated participants showed a strong sex difference where as better-educated participants showed only a small, marginally significant sex difference (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). In regards to reaction times the sex difference for emotional infidelity was close to being significant. Penke and Asendorpf (2008) state that: The results for the two forced-choice tasks suggest consistently that the sex difference for the classic forced-choice task is due to fast, spontaneous decisions, rather than due to long deliberation and that it is more pronounced in less educated participants (18). The present study brought to light three main findings, two of which confirmed the hypotheses derived from previous literature on sexual and emotional jealousy. The first finding that women are more likely than men to pick emotional infidelity over sexual jealousy as the more distressing choice when they were forced to decide, seems to be the result of spontaneous, automatic reactions, not controlled deliberation (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). Secondly, the results for response measures insinuate that the sex difference in the forced-choice condition is almost only driven by sex differences in emotional jealousy. Women reported more negative emotions than men for both types of infidelity, with a slightly significant stronger sex difference for emotional infidelity. It was also found that women chose emotional infidelity faster than men in both forced-choice tasks, where as the tendency of men to choose sexual infidelity was not significant (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). Lastly, attained education level turned out to be a strong moderator of the sex difference in sexual versus emotional jealousy. Lower educated participants showed a large sex difference where as the higher educated showed a small difference. The continuous emotion ratings presented that this relative effect was due to a strong sex difference for emotional jealousy among the lower educated participants (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). In regards to sexual jealousy, the early evolutionary discussions of jealous centered on male sexual jealous as an evolved adaptation to minimize cuckoldry. But, the evidence that human males get more jealous to a mates sexual infidelity than human females is mixed. A sex difference such as this was not significantly confirmed in the present study or by the 10 similar studies reviewed by Sagarin (2005). The reactive time measure signified that while in all four cases, men chose sexual infidelity faster and processes cues for it faster than women, all differences failed to reach statistical significance (Penke &Asendorpf, 2008). It is thought that the emotion-rating task was affected by the general sex difference that women experience emotions more intensely than men, in the context of relationship infidelity. Penke and Asendorpf (2008) stated that: Men have indeed evolved a higher sensitivity for sexual infidelity, this tendency seems to interact with so many other factors that the resulting sex difference is minimal, at least in the forced-choice and rating paradigms (21). In contrast, the results for emotional jealousy consistently showed that women react more jealous to emotional infidelity than men. In the emotion ratings and reaction times of forcedchoice decisions under cognitive load, women chose emotional infidelity as the more distressing alternative faster than men. The finding that there is a weak sex difference for sexual jealousy, suggest that the robust sex difference in the forced-choice paradigm is mainly driven by a sex difference in emotional jealousy. The studies reviewed by Sagarin (2005) and the reaction time studies by Schiitzwohl (2004, 2005) are also consistent with this view. To address environmental influences regarding cognitive processing, the effect of educational level on sex differences in emotional jealousy was found in all three conditions. This suggests that emotional jealousy is already conditional to education-related environment factors on the automatic level of processing. Outline A. Introduction 1. Debate over sex differences in romantic jealousy a. Classic evolutionary hypothesis: men and women react differently to sexual and emotional infidelity because these two types of infidelity posed different adaptive problems aa. Men are expected to be more jealous of sexual infidelity than women in order to minimize investment in genetically unrelated offspring bb. Women are expected to be more jealous of emotional infidelity than men in order to minimize loss of parental investment in their offspring cc. ‘Jealousy as a specific ‘innate’ module hypothesis” (Harris, 2000): this evolutionary account consists of two independently derived hypotheses, one for sexual and one for emotional infidelity, where one, none, or both may be true b. “Social cognitive theory of jealousy”: jealousy is the result of an evolved, but domain general appraisal mechanism; this mechanism is sensitive to all kinds of threats posed by rivals aa. jealousy is aroused when a rival outdoes someone in domains that are particularly important to the self bb. this theory does not expect universal sex differences in sexual and emotional jealousy cc. they may or may not exist, depending on culturally determined gender roles 2. Closer look at the two evolutionary hypotheses a. both are weak tests of evolutionary hypotheses because they confound sex differences in sexual jealousy and emotional jealousy b. four cases of a sex by type of infidelity aa. men react more jealous to sexual infidelity than women bb. women react more jealous to emotional infidelity than men cc. men react more jealous to sexual infidelity than to emotional infidelity dd. women react more jealous to emotional infidelity than to sexual infidelity c. forced-choice only tests whether one of the two evolutionary hypotheses or both are supported; it does not test whether both hypotheses are confirmed d. social-cognitive theory does not predict any specific between-sex or withinsex differences in emotional or sexual jealousy 3. The necessity to study cognitive processes a. it is the aim of evolutionary psychology to study the design of cognitive modules as the mediating mechanisms between evolution and adaptive behavior b. this requires and integration of the study of ultimate evolved functions and proximate cognitive processes c. spontaneity versus deliberation in terms of making a decision to cues of emotional and sexual infidelity is important, as well as the extent to which emotional versus sexual infidelity violate gender roles within a specific culture 4. Empirical evidence a. Forced-choice paradigm b. Separate ratings of sexual and emotional infidelity c. Cognitive processing of infidelity cues B. The present study 1. aim was to test the two evolutionary hypotheses using a combination of a. forced-choice method b. continuous emotion ratings 2. cognitive processes a. automaticity of responses in the forced-choice task b. decision rating 3. hypotheses a. the classic sex difference in the forced-choice paradigm under both conditions, particularly under cognitive constraint b. women rate emotional infidelity situations more negatively than men, but that men do not rate sexual infidelity situations more negatively than women c. women do not rate emotional infidelity cues faster than men, but that men do not process sexual infidelity cues faster than women C. Method 1. Sample a. German native speakers between 20-30 years of age b. Recruited in a large city by flyers and postings in various public places 2. Design a. forced dilemmas with cognitive constraint and the instruction to respond spontaneously b. continuous emotion ratings with the instruction to vividly imagine each situation c. deliberate forced-choice dilemmas with the instruction to vividly imagine each alternative and take enough time for the decision D. Results 1. Data recording and screening a. The order of sexual versus emotional infidelity within each of the six dilemmas was counterbalanced. b. Responses were recorded such that decisions for sexual and emotional infidelity were always coded as 0 and 1. 2. Consistency Across Dilemmas a. All three conditions were found to consistent across the dilemmas and situations. d. b. Individual log transformed reaction times formed sufficiently reliable scales. 3. Forced-choice under cognitive constraint a. First task: 16 % of the participants correctly remembered the six-digit number for all six dilemmas, 61% made two or more errors and two participants always failed to correctly remember the number. b. For all choices of all participants, a significant, moderately large effect confirmed that more women (77%) than men (63%) judged emotional infidelity as more distressing than sexual infidelity. c. Sex and education interactions d. Sex differences in the reaction times 4. Emotion Ratings a. There was a significant, moderately large main effect of sex, with women rating the infidelity situations overall more negatively than men. b. Post hoc t tests showed a significant effect of sex for ratings of jealousy, anxiety, and humiliation but not for anger. c. Women reported overall more negative emotions for both sexual and emotional infidelity, with a slightly larger effect size for emotional jealousy. d. Post hoc t tests showed that for the less educated participants, the sex difference for sexual infidelity was small and not significant, but the sex difference for emotional infidelity was significant and fairly large. i. The education effect was mainly driven by stronger emotional jealousy of less educated women. 5. Forced-choice with deliberation a. A significant, moderately large effect confirmed that more women than men judged emotional infidelity as more distressing than sexual infidelity. 6. Forced- choice task summary a. The sex difference for the classic forced-choice task is due to fast, spontaneous decisions, rather than due to long deliberation and that it is more pronounced in less educated participants. 7. Relationship Effects a. All participants had at least once experienced a committed, sexual relationship that lasted at least 1 month. b. Neither the total number of committed sexual relationships the participants had so far, nor the duration of the current relationships of the paired participants were significantly correlated with any of the jealousy measures. E. Discussion 1. Three main findings; two confirmed the hypotheses derived from previous literature a. Women are more likely than men to choose emotional infidelity over sexual jealousy as the more distressing alternative when they are forced to make a choice b. The sex difference in the forced-choice paradigm is almost exclusively driven by sex differences in emotional jealousy. Women reported more negative emotions than men for both sexual and emotional infidelity. c. Attained education level turned out to be a strong moderator of the sex difference in sexual versus emotional jealousy. 2. Sexual Jealousy a. Early evolutionary discussions of jealousy focused on male sexual jealousy as an evolved adaptation to minimize cuckoldry. The evidence that human males react more jealous to a mate’s sexual infidelity than human females is mixed. b. Men have evolved a high sensitivity for sexual infidelity, this tendency seems to interact with so many other factors that the resulting sex differences is minimal. 3. Emotional Jealousy a. The results for emotional jealousy consistently suggested that women react more jealous to emotional infidelity than men. 4. Environmental Influences a. Germany can be regarded as having a high degree of gender equality. b. Concerning cognitive processing, the effect of educational level on sex differences in emotional jealousy was found in all three conditions. 5. Strengths and limitations of the present study a. Strengths i. Systematic variation of the automaticity of the jealousy responses ii. Greater reliability of the jealousy assessments iii. the recording of the response times iiii. Validity checks b. Limitations: Only one culture was studied Critical Review Items (a) Interesting points 1. Interesting point: Although it was not the main concern of the researchers, education played an important role in some of the findings. Penke and Asendorpf found larger sex differences for their lower educated participants that were mainly driven by stronger emotional jealousy of less educated women. 2. The fixed order of the three conditions in the present study is very important because asking participants for spontaneous decisions after they have made deliberate ones on the same scenarios could lead to skewed results. 3. The study aims to benefit evolutionary psychology by spending more effort of the specific description and empirical testing of psychological mechanisms. The authors suggest that their study is a step in the right direction, and they hope it will inspire evolutionary psychologists interested in this and other domains of psychological functioning to move from the ultimate expectations to the detailed study of proximate cognitive mechanisms. (b) Weaknesses 1. The present study only included young adults, who might weigh emotional versus sexual infidelity more strongly according to their early developmental environments, than older adults, who might increasingly weigh them according to their own experiences 2. The findings are limited to the fact that one culture was studied. Only native German participants were included in this study. Therefore, it does not allow researchers to provide strong support got the adaptive plasticity of human jealousy. 3. The dilemmas provided in each condition of the study were hypothetical situations. Penke and Asendorpf note that higher-level cognitive processes might be necessary to anticipate the threat of sexual infidelity from more subtle cues that the straightforward descriptions of sexual infidelity acts used in the study. The alternative of drawing from memory rather than description, however, produces bias. Test Questions 1. The aim of the present study was to a. disprove previous findings b. to test the two evolutionary hypotheses using the forced choice method c. to test the two evolutionary hypotheses using continuous emotion ratings d. a combination of b and c 2. To test the two evolutionary hypotheses, researchers have used a forced-choice paradigm. What is the purpose of the forced-choice paradigm? a. participants rate how they feel about dilemmas b. participants take their time to reflect on whether they are more jealous of emotional or sexual jealousy c. participants are confronted with dilemmas that contrast hypothetical situations of emotional and sexual infidelity, and they choose the more distressing. 3. What was one of the differences between the forced-choice with cognitive constraint condition and the forced-choice with deliberation condition? a. the hypothetical situations were different in each condition b. the forced-choice with cognitive constraint condition included a six digit series that participants were instructed to recall once they finished reading through the dilemmas c. the forced-choice with deliberation condition asked participants to rate their emotions of anger, anxiety, jealousy, and humiliation on a 5-point scale in addition to reading the dilemmas 4. The classic evolutionary hypothesis assumes that men and women react differently to sexual and emotional infidelity because these two types of infidelity posed different adaptive problems. (T/F) 5. All participants in the study were not in a serious committed relationship. (T/F) 6. Participants in the forced-choice with deliberation condition were presented with hypothetical situations of emotional and sexual infidelity for the first time. (T/F) Answer Key 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. d c b T F F