Methods

advertisement
1
Supplementary material
2
Methods
3
Floral arrays
4
Flowers were arranged in a 5 × 4 grid spaced 7 cm apart at the base (5 cm apart at the
5
top), and consisted of 3D-printed 5 cm diameter discs (Makerbot, NY, USA) placed on
6
inverted plastic tubes (8 cm in height), with a coloured circle (the ‘corolla’, printed on
7
laminated waterproof paper: National Geographic Co., USA). The centre of the corolla
8
contained a ‘nectar well’ (diameter: 4mm) and an ‘anther’ (a 25 mm chenille stem:
9
Creatology, China). Flowers were always oriented in the same direction, such that the
10
11
nectar wells faced bees as they entered the arena.
To scent ‘unrewarding’ anthers used in the experiment, we stored artificial anthers
12
overnight in a sealed container of pollen, separated from the pollen by mesh. Treating
13
anthers in this way likely does not make them indistinguishable from rewarding anthers
14
by scent alone, but previous experiments [1] suggest that such treatment is necessary for
15
bees to land on anthers lacking pollen and thus facilitates learning which flowers contain
16
pollen rewards based on the associated floral visual cue.
17
18
Behaviour recorded
19
From the videos we recorded each flower visit made by a bee, including: 1) the colour of
20
the flower (blue or yellow); 2) whether the bee attempted to collect nectar or pollen; and
21
3) whether they were successful at gaining the reward or not (‘rewarded’ or
22
‘unrewarded’).
23
Nectar collection was identified as the bee landing on the ‘corolla’ of the flower
24
and probing the nectar well. We defined nectar visits as ‘rewarding’ when the bee drank
25
the nectar reward (probing the well for >2 seconds; inspection of wells and preliminary
26
observations indicated that bees generally empty the well on their visits, taking 10-30
27
seconds to do so) and as ‘unrewarding’ when she probed a well containing water (bees
28
generally left immediately after doing this). If a bee visited a flower and probed for nectar
29
where she had already emptied the well of its reward (but where there was probably some
30
residue), we excluded this from analysis of training, because it is not clear whether bees
31
experience such visits as rewarding or unrewarding, and because they are a different type
32
of error to visiting the CS-. Across all bees’ visits to collect nectar, less than 1% were re-
33
visits to previously emptied nectar wells.
34
Pollen collection was easily identified as the bee flying directly to the anther and
35
‘scrabbling’ [2,3] at the pollen with her legs in a stereotyped manner, as in [1] (see
36
supplementary video). In packing the pollen onto their corbiculae, bees were also seen
37
extending their proboscis and grooming with their front legs. In our assay, this usually
38
occurred on the flower’s anther or in flight, but occasionally the bee would land to groom
39
elsewhere (on a flower’s corolla or the arena wall). Because bees only collected and
40
searched for pollen from the flowers’ anthers, we defined a ‘visit’ to a flower type as the
41
bee touching the anther with its antennae or legs (either by landing or hovering in front of
42
the top of the anther). If the anther contained pollen and the bee was seen to scrabble with
43
its legs, the visit was recorded as ‘rewarded’. If the anther had no pollen on it and the bee
44
touched it with its antennae or legs the visit was recorded as ‘unrewarded’. If the bee
45
briefly touched the rewarding anther with her antennae or legs but did not scrabble to
46
collect pollen from it then this visit was excluded from analyses, because it was not clear
47
whether the bee had failed to detect the pollen or had detected its presence but decided
48
not to collect it (i.e. whether this visit was perceived as reinforcing or inhibiting to the
49
bee). Unlike when nectar-foraging, pollen-foraging bees did not empty anthers containing
50
pollen on a single visit. Instead, a bee would generally collect pollen from all rewarding
51
anthers, and then return to anthers they had already visited to collect more pollen; these
52
visits were also scored as ‘rewarding’. Across all bees’ visits to collect pollen, 58% were
53
rewarding, 28% were unrewarding and 14% were to rewarding flowers but without the
54
bee collecting pollen.
55
56
Nectar and pollen collection on the artificial flowers can be seen in the
supplementary video.
57
58
Data analysis
59
We carried out all LMMs in R version 2.15.1 (R Development Core Team 2014). LMMs
60
were carried out using the lme() function in nlme package, specifying type III sums of
61
squares and sums contrasts in cases where we included interactions [4]. For all models,
62
maximal models were run initially, and then non-significant interaction terms were
63
removed in a step-wise fashion. In cases of significant interactions, simplified models
64
were re-run to determine the significance of the individual factors in these interactions.
65
To do this, we divided the data into two groups by one of the two-level factors in the
66
significant interaction, and ran two separate analyses in order to determine the
67
significance of the remaining main effects. Results are reported for models with non-
68
significant interaction terms removed.
69
To assess whether bees learned to associate floral colour with nectar and pollen,
70
we compared the proportion of correct flower visits made across trials and treatments. We
71
fitted a linear mixed model (LMM) with the response variable ‘proportion of correct
72
choices made’ and the explanatory variables: trial (a continuous variable between 1 and
73
6); treatment (PYNB or PBNY); reward type (nectar or pollen) and the random factors
74
‘bee’ and ‘colony’(‘Model 1’). As we had two significant interactions (LMM:
75
treatment*reward type: F1, 208 = 5.642; p < 0.05; trial*reward type: F1, 208 = 15.355; p <
76
0.001), we then ran two additional LMMs for each treatment group separately.
77
To determine whether bees visited different flowers in the test phase between the
78
two treatments and for the two different rewards, we initially ran a model with the
79
response variable ‘number of visits in the first 10 visits’ and the same explanatory
80
variables as ‘Model 1’ except excluding ‘trial’ and including ‘flower colour’ (blue,
81
yellow, orange or purple). As this model showed that the colour of flower the bee visited
82
differed both depending on whether they were searching for nectar or pollen and between
83
the two treatment groups, via a 3-way interaction (treatment*reward*colour interaction:
84
F3, 126 = 92.332; p < 0.0001), we proceeded to address the two treatment groups separately
85
in two further models.
86
These two models showed that within each treatment group, the colour of flower a
87
bee visited differed when she was searching for nectar versus pollen (LMM of PBNY
88
data: reward*flower colour interaction: F3, 63 = 36.739; p < 0.0001; LMM of PYNB data:
89
F3, 63 = 58.198; p < 0.0001). To better understand these interactive effects and because we
90
were interested in specifically which colours bees chose over others when searching for
91
each reward type, we then carried out four individual models for each reward type within
92
each treatment. All of these models showed that bees did not visit colours equally: PBNY,
93
nectar: F3, 27 = 34.054; p < 0.0001; PBNY, pollen: F3, 27 = 11.869; p < 0.0001; PYNB,
94
nectar: F3, 27 = 62.947; p < 0.0001; PYNB, pollen: F3, 27 = 16.964; p < 0.0001). We used
95
pair-wise comparisons between flower colour factors to determine where these
96
differences lay.
To determine whether short-term “reward specialists” (bees that collected only
97
98
nectar or pollen from blue flowers) differed in their learning and test performance vs.
99
bees that collected both nectar and pollen, we compared the proportion of correct visits
100
for either nectar or pollen (for nectar- and pollen-“specialists”, respectively) to bees that
101
had collected both rewards. We used LMMs as described above but included only
102
explanatory variables ‘trial’ and ‘behaviour’ (specialist or short-term “generalist”) and the
103
random factor ‘bee’.
104
105
106
107
Colourimetry
108
We measured the reflectance spectra of the four flower colours used in the experiment
109
and irradiance at the array using a single beam UV-VIS spectrophotometer (Ocean
110
Optics, Dunedin, FL, U.S.A.) (Figure S1). The spectrophotometer was connected by
111
means of a USB cable to a computer running SpectraSuite software (Ocean Optics). We
112
then plotted the reflectance spectra into bee colour space taking into account the
113
photoreceptor spectral sensitivities of B. impatiens [5] and irradiance, using AVICOL v.6
114
(AVICOL: A program to analyse spectrometric data; free program available from the
115
author at dodogomez@yahoo.fr) (Figure S2). These analyses confirmed that for bees, the
116
human-purple target was more similar to the human-blue target, and the human-orange
117
target was more similar to the human-yellow target (for chromatic contrasts see Table
118
S1).
119
120
121
Figure S1: Reflectance spectra of the four colours used in the experiment. Colours refer
122
to human visual perception.
123
124
Table S1: Chromatic contrasts of the four colour stimuli used relative to each other. All
125
data are in hexagon units, and are calculated as described elsewhere [6].
Comparison
Chromatic contrast
yellow-orange
0.102
yellow-blue
0.234
yellow-purple
0.286
orange-blue
0.334
orange-purple
0.389
blue-purple
0.078
126
127
Results
128
129
Comparison between foragers that collected one vs. two reward types
In the test phase, PBNY-trained bees that had collected both rewards during
130
131
training made significantly more errors during the test phase than bees that foraged for
132
pollen alone, as they visited yellow and orange flowers more frequently for pollen (Mann
133
Whitney U-Tests: orange: n=10,5, U=0, p < 0.005; yellow: n=10,5, U=0, p < 0.005).
134
PYNB-trained bees that either collected only nectar or both rewards did not differ in their
135
test performance, making the same number of attempts to collect nectar from yellow and
136
orange flowers (Mann Whitney U-Tests: orange: n=10,8, U=22.50, p = 0.102; yellow:
137
n=10,8, U=29.50, p = 0.312).
138
139
References
140
141
1.
Muth, F., Papaj, D. R. & Leonard, A. In revision at Animal Behaviour Bees
remember flowers for more than one reason: Pollen mediates associative learning.
142
143
144
2.
Buchmann, S. L. 1983 Buzz pollination in angiosperms. In Handbook of
Experimental Pollination Biology (eds C. E. Jones & R. J. Little), pp. 73–113. Van
Nostrand Reinhold Company.
145
3.
Thorp, R. 2000 The collection of pollen by bees. Plant Syst. Evol. 222, 211–233.
146
147
148
4.
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., & Sarkar, D. (2014). nlme: linear and nonlinear
mixed effects models. R package version 3.1–117.URL: http://cran. r-project.
org/web/packages/nlme/index. html.
149
150
151
5.
Chittka, L. 1992 The colour hexagon: A chromaticity diagram based on
photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J.
Comp. Physiol. A 170, 533–543. (doi: 10.1007/BF00199331)
152
153
154
6.
Spaethe, J., Tautz, J. & Chittka, L. 2001 Visual constraints in foraging
bumblebees: flower size and color affect search time and flight behavior. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 3898–3903. (doi:10.1073/pnas.071053098)
155
Download