Expressing inheritance When psychologists explore the extent to which a characteristic (e.g., intelligence, schizophrenia) is inherited, they use various different techniques. The concordance rate expresses the extent to which two measures are in agreement. For example, suppose that 20 twin pairs are studied in which at least one twin has developed schizophrenia. If there are 18 pairs in which both twins have schizophrenia, then the concordance rate is 18/20 or 90%, indicating a very high level of concordance. We can take things further if we compare concordance rates in monozygotic (identical) twins and dizygotic (non-identical or fraternal) twins. Monozygotic twins share 100% of their genes, whereas dizygotic twins share only 50%. If genetic factors are important, we would expect monozygotic twins to have a higher concordance rate than dizygotic twins. If genetic factors don’t play a role, then there should be no difference in concordance rate between the two types of twins. We can use information from concordance rates to work out heritability. This provides an estimate of the extent to which genetic factors contribute to individual differences in some characteristic within a given population. Heritability of 1.0 would mean that all the variance or variation within a given population was due to genetic factors, and heritability of 0.0 would mean that genetic factors were totally irrelevant. We can use information about IQ from pairs of monozygotic and dizygotic twins in a similar way. In essence, if individual differences in intelligence depend to some extent on genetic factors, then identical twins should resemble each other more in IQ than should fraternal twins. If environment is all important, then identical twins should be no more similar to each other in IQ than fraternal twins. In either case, the outcome of taking account of the relevant information is an estimate of heritability of intelligence.