PROMOTING CONCEPTUAL CHANGE ON STUDENTS’ UNDERSTANDING OF GASES USING INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS DERIVED FROM CONFLICT MAPS Richard R. Jugar Science and Mathematics Education Department College Of Education, University Of San Carlos, Philippines ABSTRACT This study intended to establish the applicability and effectiveness of conflict maps in promoting conceptual change. The study initiates with the identification of the alternative conceptions of secondary school students on the topic of gases. This was followed by the consequent remediation of these identified alternative conceptions using instructional materials developed from conflict maps. Conflict mapping was originally proposed by Tsai (2000) as an approach to enhance science instruction. Using three sections of grade 9 classes as research respondents; two-tiered testing, individual and group interviews, as well as class observations were conducted. Based on pre-testing, the identified prevalent alternative conceptions of the students include (a) the apparent inability of gas to occupy the volume of the container, (b) incompressibility of gases, (c) the increase in particle size of gases when heated, and (d) the settling of confi ned gas particles on the bottom of the container upon cooling. After classroom intervention using the instructional materials developed from conflict maps, analysis of results indicated positive learning gains in all three classes as indicated by the shift of the students’ conception by the majority of the population, from the prevalent alternative conception to the correct scientific conception. Keywords: Conflict Map, Conceptual Change, Instructional Materials, Teaching Strategy, Gases 1. INTRODUCTION The acknowledgement that the learners do not come in the portals of our classroom as an empty vessel ready to be filled in with knowledge is one of the basic tenets of constructivism. Out of this acknowledgement stems the movement that seeks to modify the existent conception of the learner from being incorrect or incomplete into a correct and scientifically accepted conception. This process is known as conceptual change. Over the years, a number of mechanisms have been proposed in an attempt to explain the conceptual change process (Carey 1958; Demastes, Good and Peebles, 1996; Driver 1981; Fensham, Gunstone and White 1994; Mortimer 1995; Niedder and Goldberg 1994; Vosniadou and Ioannides 1998). [11] Relevant Concept 1 Relevant Concept 2 Relevant Concept 3 Critical Event Student Alternative Conception Target Scientific Conception Relevant Concept 4 MIND ENVIRONMENT Discrepant Discrepant Perception Supporting Perception 1 Supporting Perception 2 Figure 1. Schematic Diagram of a Conflict Map as Proposed by Tsai (2000) It is believed that the understanding of the conceptual change process will help educators in coming up with a framework to effectively capture the learning of science concepts, and by extension, could serve as a guide in constructing or developing sound instructional intervention (Vosniadou and Ioannides, 1998). In 2000, Tsai proposed the use of conflict maps as an approach to enhance science instruction. Since its introduction, there were other studies conducted concerning conflict maps. One study investigates the effectiveness of conflict maps and V-shape teaching methods in science conceptual change (Bawaheh et. al., 2010). In another study by Oh (2011), the conflict map was enhanced in light of the Lakatosian methodology. Based on these literatures, conflict mapping was claimed to be useful in instructional material development, lesson planning and implementation, as well as an effective metacognitive tool for learners. Figure 1 shows the schematic diagram of a conflict map. As the figure depicts, a conflict map recognizes both external (environment) and internal (mind) processes associated with learning. As cited in Tsai, the conflict map is actually an extension of Hasweh’s (1986) study from which two conflicts were identified upon disequilibration. The first conflict exists between the learner’s alternative conception and the new conception and the other conflict is between the learner’s alternative conception and the desired scientific conception. The theoretical framework that links conflict mapping to conceptual change is provided by the parallelism of the conflict map’s key components to the conceptual change model or CCM as proposed by Posner, Strike and [12] Hewson in 1982. The conceptual change model enumerates four conditions that should be met in order for the complete conceptual change process to take place, and these conditions are essentially focused on the new conception being introduced. Based on the model, the teacher should ensure that a) there is a significant degree of dissatisfaction with the existing alternative conception, b) the new conception is intelligible, c) the new conception appears initially plausible, and d) the new conception is fruitful or open to new areas of inquiry. The following table presents the parallelism between the conflict map and the conditions of the conceptual change model: Table 1. Parallelism between the conflict map and the conceptual change model Conceptual Change Model Dissatisfaction in the existing alternative conceptions of the learner The new conception must be intelligible Conflict Map Discrepant Perception Target Scientific Concept The target conception must appear initially plausible Critical Event and Relevant Concepts The new conception is fruitful or open to new areas of inquiry Supporting Perceptions In this study, upon taking into account the merits of the conflict map with respect to the Conceptual Change Model, conflict maps were used to develop instructional materials in an attempt to address the identified alternative conceptions of grade 9 students on the particulate nature of gases. Further, the effectiveness and applicability of conflict mapping is judged based on the conceptual gains of the respondents after the conflict-map based classroom intervention has been applied. 2. METHODOLOGY The study was conducted in two private high schools within Cebu City, Philippines. Both schools are Catholic secondary schools with an average class size of 45 students per class. Since three classes (A, B and C) were used in the study, two sections were taken from one school and one section from the other school. Both schools have mixed class gender with heterogeneous grouping with respect to classroom performance, and generally follow the high school chemistry curriculum as prescribed by the Department of Education of the Philippines. Within the identified classes, purposive sampling was done to come up with a list of students for interview and analysis. The selection was based upon the recommendation of the subject teacher, with class performance in their current and previous grading period as basis. For each of the class performance category, one student was chosen per class. Considering that there were three ability groups composed of low, middle and high ability per class, and taking into account that there were three different classes; a total of 9 students composed the focus group, namely 3 students per identified class. A two-tiered 15item test was given to the student respondents followed by individual and group interviews. Classroom observation through a combination of direct and videotaped observations were performed for the duration of the implementation. Prior to the implementation of the study, the teachers of the selected classes were trained in constructing conflict maps. For the analysis of the open ended questions, the following categories and heading was used, as suggested by Abraham et. al. (1994): Sound Understanding (SU): Responses that [13] include all the components of the validated response. Partial Understanding (PU): Responses that included at least one of the components of the validated response, but not all of the components. Partial Understanding with Specific Misconception (SM): Responses that shows understanding of the concept, but also made a statement, which demonstrated a misunderstanding. Specific Misconception (SM): Responses that included illogical or incorrect information. No Understanding (NU): Repeated the question; contained irrelevant information or an unclear response; left the response blank. Based on the results, the students’ responses were thematically grouped to come up with the identified alternative conceptions, upon which the conflict maps were based. The same set of two-tiered test was also used as the post-test followed by another set of in-depth individual and group interviews to account for the changes in the students’ conceptions before and after the instructional intervention. The input of the teacher implementers concerning (a) significant change/s in the way they teach the topic, (b) difficulty in teaching the topic using the instructional materials derived from conflict maps, and (c) changes in the way students interact were also accounted. 3. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 3.1 Students’ Alternative Conceptions on Gases Based on the result of the categorized pretest responses, frequencies of partial understanding (PU), partial understanding with specific misconception (PM), and specific misconceptions were taken together and were considered as the prevalent alternative conception. Responses falling on the category of no understanding (NU) were not taken into consideration since no specific alternative conception may be inferred or identified. Responses on the NU category are those responses that are irrelevant to the concept in question, or the respondents have definitely expressed that they have no idea on how to explain the concept at hand. Most of the identified alternative conceptions were actually common for the three classes. These alternative conceptions were also consistent with the findings of the survey done by Kind (2004), Chiu (2007), and Horton (2007) concerning the alternative conceptions of students on basic chemical and physical ideas. For each of the target concept, it was observed that the number of alternative conceptions that need to be addressed varies from concept to concept. For instance, the student respondents explained the topic on the relationship of temperature and behavior of gas particles in different ways. Using correct conception, the effect of increasing the temperature of a confined gas will result to the rapid movement of the gas particles. Moreover, smaller particles will tend to move faster compared to the larger particles because of their smaller size. Alternative conceptions of the students however, suggest that the particles of gases actually increase in size resulting in volume expansion. Further, the size of the particles does not affect the movement speed upon heating. In this case, both of these alternative conceptions are pertinent to the affect of temperature on the behavior of gas particles. Since these two alternative conceptions are much related, it is argued that if the learner completely understands the effect of temperature on the behavior of a confined gas, both of these alternative conceptions can actually be corrected. The same is true with the alternative conceptions on a confined gas not fully occupying the entire volume of the container, incompressibility of gases, gas behavior at low temperature, and the concept of gas pressure. [14] The following list summarizes the alternative conceptions that are used as basis in the construction of the conflict maps. These alternative conceptions were identified as common to all three sections that took the pretest. (a) A confined gas does not fully occupy the volume of its container. (b) Gases are not compressible. (c) Gas particles increase in size when heated which results in volume expansion. (d) At low temperature, confined gas particles tend to settle at the bottom of the container. 3.2 Constructed conflict maps and lesson flows Computer Simulation of confined gas molecules For each of the identified alternative conception, a conflict map was constructed and was consequently translated into lesson flows. These lesson flows are rough lesson plans in which the correct timing of implementation of the different components of the conflict map is outlined. Since the lesson flows are simply an outline of the chronological order of events upon implementation, the teacher is relatively free to choose the specific type of class delivery that he deems necessary, effective and appropriate. Figure 2 and 3 respectively present an example of the constructed conflict maps and the consequent lesson flow based on the first identified alternative conception concerning the volume of a gas in a closed container: Molecular Theory of Matter A confined gas does not fully occupy the volume of its container. Diffusion of Gases A confined gas takes the volume and shape of its container. Volume of regular and irregular solids MIND ENVIRONMENT Discrepant Exhibition of containers of different volume Volume comparison between a newly purchased and almost spent portable gas fuel container Figure 2. Conflict Map on Gas Volume [15] Citation of other phenomena or circumstances where diffusion is taking place LESSON FLOW FOR PART 1 The Particulate Property and Fluidity of Gases (Gas Volume) Learning Objectives: Compare and contrast molecule or particle arrangement among solid, liquid and gas; Explain the fluidity and compressibility of gases in relation to its particulate nature; Define diffusion and cite some examples of its occurrence; and Discuss some products and/or materials that utilize the particulate nature and fluidity of gases. Initiating Activity: Conceptual Recall of the State of Matter and Their Basic Attributes Key Points: There are five states of matter (solid, liquid, gas, plasma and BEC) Focus on the definition and/or property of gases as given by the students. Let them come with molecular representations H2O: ice, water and water vapor Highlight the molecular arrangement of gas molecules in a confined container. Lesson Proper Part 1: Fluidity of Gases Key Points Gases are composed of molecules or particles that are in constant motion. Since the particles are in motion, they have the tendency to be dispersed evenly in a confined space. Teacher Presentation 1: Show the students flasks of different volumes. Cover the flasks with corks one at a time. Assume that the cover is airtight; no air goes in or out. (Discrepant event) Question: What is the volume of the air trapped in “this” container? The volume of the confined gas is equal to the volume of its container. An illustration or simulation will be used in this part. (Critical Event) Teacher Presentation 2 (could also be situational description): Open a perfume bottle or body spray on the far end of the classroom. Ask the students on the other end of the classroom if they can detect the fragrance. (Relevant Concept 2) To stress that the particles or molecules of gases are in constant motion, demonstrate the phenomenon of diffusion. (Relevant Concept 1) Group Activity (2-4 students in a group): Thinking Aloud Give a couple of minutes for each of the following questions and solicit responses after the allotted time period. Discuss responses and always relate it to the particulate and fluid nature of gases. Question 1. When you purchase a bottle of butane gas, it is full and weighs more. After using it to cook food for some time, the bottle weighs Concepts Tested Item Number The Particulate Nature of Gases 1-15 Gas Volume 1-6, 9 Gas Temperature 7-13 Gas Pressure 3, 9, 14-15 less since most of the content is already spent. In this case, how do you compare the volume of gas inside a newly purchased bottle and a used one? (Supporting Perception 1) Question 2. Ask each group to cite a phenomenon where diffusion of gases is taking place e.g. the exchange of oxygen and carbon dioxide in the [16] lugs, spreading of aroma of food etc. (Supporting Perception 2) single designation since these items assess more than a single concept. Lesson Closure Table 2. Questionnaire Item Distribution of Basic Concepts on Gases Summarize the Key points of the Topic A. Gases are made up of the particles or molecules that are in constant motion. B. Spaces exist in between these molecules. C. Gases are fluid – they follow the volume and shape of their container. Diffusion is one of the proofs that gas molecules are in constant motion. It is the movement of gas molecule from an area of high concentration to an area of low concentration that enables a confined gas to occupy the entire volume of the container. Figure 3. Lesson Flow Since there were four main alternative conceptions that were identified, a total of four conflict maps as well as four lesson flows were constructed that is specific for each identified alternative conception. [Actual conflict maps and lesson flows quoted in this research are available from the authors upon request.] 3.3 Students’ Conceptions After Instructional Intervention The posttest was conducted as the fifth part of the implementation phase. Further input on the Kinetic Molecular Theory (KMT) of gases was also discussed. In this part, the concepts on the particulate nature of gases, gas temperature, gas pressure and gas volume, which were taught during the first four parts were actually used to establish the postulates that compose the KMT. Table 2 shows the item distribution of the basic concepts on gases that were assessed with respect to the questionnaire given to the students. Some of the items have more than a Based on the result of the posttest, all three classes registered a positive learning gain for all items after the topic on gases has been taught using the instructional materials based on conflict maps. Several test items registered 100% for sound understanding, which implies that all students in the class were able to correctly explain the concept in question. The common item that consistently displayed a 100% SU gain for all classes is item #12. This item asks the students to describe the movement speed of gas particles when the temperature is increased. Classes A and B both got 100% SU gain for item #1. This item focuses on the volume of a confined gas. Despite not getting a 100% SU gain for item #1, Class C was able to register an 84% SU gain that can be considered as positive. Class A also got 100% SU gain in item #5 that relates the confined volume of a gas to the mole concept. In the same item, Classes B and C got a positive SU gain of 98% and 70% respectively. Despite the positive percentage of SU gains for all items, there were some items that registered less than 15% positive SU gains. Item #11 for Class B only registered 13% positive SU gain. This question was on the volume expansion of different gases. Item #7 for Class C only registered 9% positive SU gain. This item focused on the behavior of gases at low temperature. Item #2, which focuses on the volume of two different gases in a single confined container registered a mere 11% of positive SU gains for Class C. These low positive [17] SU gains clearly suggest that despite the intervention, some of the students still adhere to their preconceptions and alternative conceptions. Items #2, #4 and36 deal with volume of two different gases confined in the same container. Comparing with the positive SU gains of items 1 and 5, it clearly shows that the students’ conception have significantly changed from their preconception that gases do not fully occupy the volume of the container to the correct conception that the volume of a confined gas is dictated by the volume of the container. However, despite having this correct conception, most students cannot give the respective volume of the individual gases confined in the same container. The students’ inability to give the correct volume seems to stem from the fact that when applied to gases, the scenario tends to violate the whole part postulate, which states that the sum of the parts is equal to the whole. The tendency of the students’ reasoning is to utilize the idea of ‘sharing’ the container volume between two gases. Focusing on the SU gains of after the instructional intervention, the new conceptions of majority of the respondents concerning gases can be summarized as follows: (a) the volume of a confined gas is dictated by the volume of its container, (b) the relatively large distance in between gas molecules accounts for the compressibility of gases, (c) as the temperature increases, the movement speed of the gas particles also increases with smaller molecules tend to move faster than large molecules at the same temperature, and (d) at low temperature, molecules of a confined gas still occupy the entire volume of the container but move at a relatively low speed. 4. CONCLUSION Students possess alternative conceptions on the basic concepts of gases. These alternative conceptions include the basic concepts of the particle nature of matter (gases), the volume of a confined gas, the compressibility of gases, the behavior of gases at high and low temperature, and gas volume expansion. Specifically, the identified alternative conceptions were the following: (a) a confined gas does not fully occupy the volume of its container, (b) gases are not compressible, (c) gas particles increase in size when heated which results to volume expansion and (d) at low temperature, confined gas particles tend to settle at the bottom of the container. Further, the observed alternative conceptions of students on gases were consistent with the literatures of Kind (2004), Chiu (2007) and Horton (2007) based on their surveys of alternative conceptions on basic chemical systems. Furthermore, the identified alternative conceptions of the students may be grouped whenever these alternative conceptions are closely related. For each alternative conception or group of related alternative conceptions, a single conflict map may be developed. In order for the developed conflict to be effectively used in the actual classroom setting, a lesson flow may be constructed to serve as a guide for the timely implementation of the components as well as the key concepts to be taught. Since the lesson flow is not as structured as a detailed lesson plan, teachers are free to use their style of lesson execution and delivery so long as all the components are taken and seamlessly integrated. As noted, majority of the students were able to change their alternative conceptions to the correct scientific conception after being taught using the instructional materials based on conflict maps. This was evidenced by the positive SU (sound understanding) gains observed in all items of the posttest. Some of the items were even able to register 100% SU gains that translate to all students in the class being able to demonstrate a sound understanding in that particular item. Considering the results of the posttest and consequent interviews, the use of conflict maps in the development of [18] instructional material to promote conceptual change has been shown to be both effective and applicable. Fensham, P., Gunstone, R., White, R. (1994). The Content of Science: A constructivist approach to its teaching and learning. London, Falmer Press References Freyberg, P., Osborne, R., (1996). Learning in Science- Assumptions About Teaching and Learning, pp. 83-90. Auckland: Heinemann Education Abraham, M. R., Williamson, V. M., & Westbrook, S. L. (1994). A cross-age study of the understanding of five chemistry concepts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(2), 147-165 Ali Khalid, A. B., Ahmad Nurulazam, M. Z., & Ghazali, M. (2010). The effectiveness of conflict maps and the V-shape teaching method in science conceptual change among eighth-grade students in Jordan. International Education Studies, 3(1), 96-108. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/8216956 67?accountid=50192 Carey, S. (1991). Knowledge Acquisition: Enrichment or Conceptual Chang? The Epigenesis of Mind pp. 257-291. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates Chiu, M., (2007). A national Survey of Students’ conceptions of Chemistry I Taiwan. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 29, No. 4, p. 421-452, doi : 10/1080095006901072964 Demastes, S., Good, R., Peebles, P. (1993). Patterns of Conceptual Change in Evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, Vol. 33 pp. 407-431 Driver, R., (1981). Pupil’s Alternative Frameworks in Science. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 3, No. 1, pp. 93-101, doi: 0140-5284/81/0301 0093 Hwang, B. (1995). Students’ Conceptual Representations of Gas Volume in Relation to the Particulate Model of Matter. Retrieved September 20, 2010, from EBSCO Database. Kind, V., (2004). Beyond Appearances: Students’ Misconceptions About Basic Chemical Ideas, 2nd Edition: Durham University Press Merritt, J. Shwartz, Y., Rogat, A. & Krajcik, J. Middle school students’ development of the particle model of matter. A paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association of Research in Science Teaching, April, 2007, New Orleans, LA Mortimer, E., (1995). Conceptual Change of Conceptual Profile Change? Journal of Science Education, Vol. 4, pp. 267-285 Niedderer, H., Goldberg, F. (1994). An Individual Student’s Learning Process in Electric Circuit. Paper presented at NARST 1994 Annual Meeting. Anaheim, CA Oh, J. (2011). Using an Enhanced Conflict Map in the Classroom (Photoelectric Effect) Based on Lakatosian Heuristic Principle Strategies. International Journal Of Science And Mathematics Education, 9(5), 1135-1166. Posner, G.J., Strike, K.A., Hewson, P.W. and Gerrtzog, W.A. (1982). Accommodation of a [19] scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Journal of Science Education, Vol. 6 pp. 221-227 Science Education, Vol. 22, No. 3 pp. 295-302, doi: 0950-0693 Tsai, C. (2000). Enhancing Science Teaching: the use of ‘conflict map’. International Journal of [20]