Assessing Adolescents’ Communicative Self-Efficacy To Discuss Controversial Issues: Findings from a Randomized Study of the Word Generation Program Journal: Teachers College Records Manuscript ID: In press Manuscript Featured article Keywords: Communicative self-efficacy, classroom climate, civic education Abstract: Communicative self-efficacy serves as an important link between discussing controversial issues and civic engagement because confidence in one’s discourse skills is important to managing conflicting perspectives and developing solutions to community-based problems. Freely available to schools, Word Generation is a cross-content literacy program that supports teachers in the four main content areas –English language arts, social studies, science, and math— to embed learning of controversial issues through classroom discussions, subjectspecific lessons, and writing. Middle school students (N = 5,870) from highly diverse backgrounds participated in a randomized study of the intervention that was conducted in twelve middle schools located in an urban school district. We analyzed survey data based on students’ self-reported ratings on their communicative self-efficacy, as indicated by confidence to participate in discussions of fifteen different controversial issues related to politics, society, and science. Paired sample t-tests indicate that treatment students reported higher communicative self-efficacy than control students on a set of topics immediately covered prior to testing, but not on the set of topics covered in the previous year. This study informs curriculum developers, policy makers, and educators to consider the importance of incorporating discussions of controversial issues within a framework of subjectspecific instructions. Declines in voting and volunteering among young American adults (18-29 years old) in the last decade affirm the need to help adolescents become civically engaged or participate in activities to address problems in their community (Adler & Googin, 2005; Vercellotti & Matto, 2013). The most recent nationwide report on youth civic engagement indicates that adolescents who frequently discussed controversial issues with peers and families were more likely to become politically engaged and vote in the 2012 elections (CIRCLE, 2013). Communicative self-efficacy serves as an important link between discussing controversial issues and civic engagement because confidence in one’s discourse skills is important to managing conflicting perspectives and developing solutions to community-based problems (Kim & Ball-Rokeach, 2006; Hackler, Ho, & Urquhart-Ross, 1974). Based on a randomized study of the Word Generation program, this study seeks to understand whether a classroom intervention that provides information about and supports discussion of controversial issues can improve adolescents’ communicative self-efficacy. Schools can provide students with opportunities to engage in democratic practices such as debates and political simulations with peers and teachers who hold diverse opinions and beliefs (Author, 2013; Guttmann & Thompson, 1996; McDevitt & Canton-Rosser, 2009; Wray-Lake & Syvertsen, 2011; Verba, Schlozman, & Brady, 1995). Although teachers can take advantage of this diversity of beliefs and values to foster active classroom discussions (Steiner, Brzuzy, Gerdes, & Hurdle, 2003), discussions are rarely emphasized (Hahn, 1991; Hess, 2002; Newmann, 1988; Nystrand, Gamoran, & Carbonaro, 1998). Nystrand and colleagues (1998) evaluated 48 high school social studies classes and found that students participated in an average of only half a minute per class period of classroom discussion, while 63% of the observed class periods lacked explicit support for discussions. Controversial issues can serve as a basis for discussion, which can help students develop stronger commitment to democratic values such as toleration of differences and support for equality (Hess, 2002; Lockwood & Harris, 1985; Torney-Purta, Lehmann, Oswald, & Schulz, 2001). Although there are a number of social studies interventions that use controversial issues as a basis for discussion (Feldman, Pasek, Romer, & Jamieson, 2007; Hess & Posselt, 2002; Venville & Dawson, 2010), a literacy intervention called Word Generation (WG) extends learning of controversial issues by incorporating a comprehensive framework that includes subject-specific instructions in math, English language arts (ELA), science, and social studies. Previous evaluations of the WG program have examined a broad range of research topics including: writing outcomes (Mancilla-Martinez, 2010), vocabulary development (Authors et al., 2012), and differentiated effects for English language learners (Authors et al., under review). This is the first study of the WG program that reports on the communicative self-efficacy aspect of classroom discussion. The purpose of this study is to determine whether participation in the WG program has an impact on students’ communicative self-efficacy to discuss controversial issues. First, we examine the literature on controversial issues discussion in the classroom and students’ development of communicative self-efficacy. Next, we report a study in which we assessed the communicative self-efficacy of adolescents who enrolled in schools randomly assigned to the WG program. Finally, we discuss the implications of our findings for research and practice. ADOLESCENTS’ EXPERIENCE OF PARTICIPATING IN STRUCTURED DISCUSSIONS For the purposes of this study, discussion is defined as a structured form of discourse where participants research a particular issue, adopt a position, and then debate with each other (Maloney & Simon, 2006; Steiner et al., 2003). The key features in structured discussion include: (a) moderation by a facilitator, (b) exploration of differences, and (c) explicit goals of increasing shared meaning and collaboration (Ellinor & Gerard, 1998; Isaacs, 1999; Zorn, Roper, Weaver, & Rigby, 2012). Discussions have been characterized as complex interpersonal activities because participants deliberate on problems that contain a number of possibilities with no definitive solution (Venville & Dawson, 2010). Schools are increasingly concerned about meeting Common Core State Standards (CCSS) that emphasize students’ speaking and listening skills during classroom discussions, in particular to articulating and supporting arguments with evidence (National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, 2010; Swanson, 2013). The Center for Information and Research on Civic Learning and Engagement (CIRCLE) conducts research on civic education in schools and recommends exposing adolescents to classroom debates on controversial issues (CIRCLE, 2013). Adolescents are more likely drawn to discussions that are organized around controversial issues (Hess & Posselt, 2002). Immigration policy and the environmental state of the world are topic questions that have the potential of supporting rich discussions (Harwood & Hahn, 1990). These issues challenge students to consider different perspectives, while forcing them to rethink their own stance (Hess & Posselt, 2002). Students are naturally interested in topics that are relevant to their social context (Harwood & Hahn, 1990). A study found that adolescents reported more favorable attitudes towards social studies classrooms that incorporated discussions of controversial issues (Hess, Levstik, & Tyson, 2008). In the likely event that adolescents want to know more about controversial issues, they tend to follow up on current events through the media and discuss political issues with their friends and family (Lee, Shah, & McLeod, 2012). These findings indicate that controversial issues can enhance students’ interest and participation in classroom discussions and extend opportunities for learning through self-initiated activities. We examine teaching strategies and curricula that can help adolescents develop argumentation skills to engage in discourse with peers and teachers. Guided instructions and curriculum material may be required to scaffold students’ learning in regards to skillfully deliberation on controversial issues. Facilitators, such as teachers, can guide students in understanding that competing points of views need to be equally represented (Maloney & Simon, 2006). For example, teachers can limit contributions from outspoken students, so that more hesitant students have opportunities to participate (Harwood & Hahn, 1990). Certain curricula are also designed to provide turn-taking rules and arrangements for students to adopt a specific position and role (Herrenkohl, Palincsar, DeWater, & Kawasaki, 1999). Recent studies indicate that students lacking training in argumentative discourse are more likely to focus on supporting their own positions, while paying little attention to alternative perspectives (Felton, 2004; Felton & Kuhn, 2001; Kuhn & Udell, 2003). Felton (2004) analyzed classroom observation data from a study of middle school students (n = 48) who were randomly assigned to an intervention that taught students to consider different arguments within a controversial topic such as abortion and capital punishment. In comparison with control students who also engaged in classroom debates, treatment students were more likely to use advanced argumentative strategies related to counterarguments and rebuttal. These findings strongly suggest that facilitators and curricula can provide the support required to enable adolescents to productively engage in classroom discussions. COMMUNICATIVE SELF-EFFICACY Based on Bandura's theories on self-efficacy (1986, 1987), communications researchers argue that communicative self-efficacy plays a critical role in developing civic engagement (Kim & Ball-Rokeach, 2006). This concept refers to an individuals’ belief in their confidence level to communicate their own words, thoughts, and feelings to others (Lonie & Rahim, 2010; Milstein, 2005; Zorn et al., 2006). It is important to note that communicative self-efficacy refers to individuals’ beliefs regarding their capabilities to communicate effectively with other, rather than actual capabilities (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Milstein, 2005). Communicative self-efficacy may be determined by a number of factors that include: discussion frequency, topic, and audience. For instance, a person may have high self-efficacy for discussing sports among friends, but low selfefficacy in matters of discussing politics with strangers (Koesten, Miller, & Hummert, 2002; Zorn et al., 2006). Having more frequent discussion opportunities in the classroom can improve communicative self-efficacy (Lin & Liuwood, 1997; Venville & Dawson, 2010). Liu and Littlewood (1997) reports that East Asian college students (n = 2,156) who enrolled in English learning courses with more frequent speaking opportunities gave themselves higher ratings on their confidence and speaking abilities than those enrolled in courses supporting less frequent speaking opportunities. Organizing discussions around controversial topics can also make a difference in participants’ communicative self-efficacy. Zorn and colleagues (2006) analyzed data from a nonrandomized study of adults (n = 59) who participated in focus groups that support discussions of controversial issues. These focus groups consist of a small number of people (usually 6-12 participants) who are brought together by researchers for the purpose of studying their views on particular issues. Facilitators guide the discussions so that participants have equal opportunities to express their opinions (Zorn et al., 2006, 2012). The researchers (Zorn et al., 2006) found significant increases in pre to post-test communicative self-efficacy scores, as indicated by confidence and motivation to engage in public discussions of human biotechnology issues. These findings suggest a possible relationship between participants’ experience of discussing controversial issues and improvements in their communicative self-efficacy. Although these studies identify the promise of discussion in improving individuals’ communicative self-efficacy, several questions remain unanswered. First, most of these communicative self-efficacy studies examine how adults (mean age of 32 years old) experience discussions of controversial issues (Zorn et al., 2006, 2012), but there is much less research on adolescents. With one exception (Venville & Dawson, 2010), the studies do not include control groups (Klosterman & Sadler, 2009; Liu & Littlewood, 1997; Zorn et al., 2006, 2012). Last, these findings are mainly relevant to a non-diverse sample that features a low representation of ethnic minorities including Asians, African Americans, and Latinos (Cross, Taasoobshirazi, Hendricks, & Hickey 2008; Venville & Dawson, 2010; Zorn et al., 2006, 2012). In the next section, we provide an overview of a cross-content literacy intervention implemented in middle schools that organizes vocabulary instructions around discussions of controversial issues. WORD GENERATION PROGRAM The Word Generation (WG) is a cross-content literacy program delivered at the classroom or grade level that instructs students to learn five all-purpose academic words, which are embedded in brief passages covering a different controversial issue each week (Author et al., 2009). The program features controversial topics based on a broad range of political and sciencebased controversies, such as whether the government should fund stem cell research or animal testing. As part of the WG program, teachers in the four main content areas – ELA, social studies, science, and math – present materials related to the controversial issue of the week and explore academic language that is embedded in the curriculum through discourse and writing (Author et al., 2009). For fifteen minutes a day, teachers and students engage in program activities related to vocabulary instruction and classroom discussions. Students have books containing general information about each controversial issue. These books also contain examples of various opinions on a particular issue, which students can choose to defend during classroom debates. We present an example of the organization of one week’s activities; each week follows a similar sequence (see http://wg.serpmedia.org/ for sample materials). On Monday, the ELA teacher presents a text that starts with a narrative about a girl who was accidentally shot and paralyzed, but for whom stem cell research might offer a cure; the rest of the brief text presents arguments against (religious objections, use of embryos) and in favor of federal funding of stem cell research (scientific and medical advances, global competitiveness). The math teacher provides a lesson that prompts students to calculate how many stem cells are in the human body. On Wednesday, the social studies teacher facilitates debate with the whole class, where students can choose to defend arguments for or against funding stem cell research. Given sufficient time, students also have opportunities to rebut the opposing group’s argument. The science teacher assigns an experiment where students hypothesize and test whether their peers have correct assumptions about stem cells. On Friday, the ELA teacher assigns a writing exercise that prompts students to write a persuasive essay defending their position on the topic. The weekly sequence was developed to provide students with recurrent exposure and opportunities to learn about the controversial topics in various subject-specific contexts (Author et al., 2009). RESEARCH QUESTIONS This study seeks to assess students’ communicative self-efficacy to discuss a particular set of topics that were covered in the WG program, rather than a general self-efficacy to discuss all controversial issues. Given our discussion of the research on using controversial issues to improve communicative self-efficacy, we pose the following research questions (RQ): RQ 1: Did students in the treatment and control group differ in communicative selfefficacy on general topics of interest to adolescents that were not covered in the WG program? RQ 2: Did treatment students report higher communicative self-efficacy on topics that had been discussed as part of the WG program during the school year immediately prior to testing, in comparison with control students? RQ 3: Did treatment students report higher communicative self-efficacy on topics that they had been exposed to as part of the WG program during the school year prior to testing, in comparison with control students? METHOD PARTICIPANTS <INSERT TABLE 1> From Fall 2010 to Spring 2012, middle school students (N = 7,541) participated in a randomized study of the Word Generation (WG) intervention that was conducted in an urban school district located in a West Coast metropolitan area of the United States. Our analytical sample (N = 5,870) was restricted to students who responded to at least three out of the fifteen (20%) survey items analyzed in this study. Table 1 presents grade level contributions from each middle school that participated in the randomized study. Although an equal number of schools were randomly assigned to treatment (n = 6) and control (n = 6) conditions, the sample contains more students who enrolled in treatment (n = 3,518) than control (n = 2,352) schools. Across the three middle school grades, there is a near equal representation of sixth grade (31.5%), seventh grade (32.6%), and eighth grade (35.9%) students in the treatment and control schools. <INSERT TABLE 2> Table 2 indicates that gender of the participants was approximately equally divided: 50.3% males and 49.7% females. The sample consists of students from extremely diverse racial/ethnic backgrounds, which includes: Asians (54.1%), Hispanics (19.3%), and African Americans (6.9%). As is typical in many American urban settings (Wells & Lewis, 2006), a majority of the participants (63.8%) are from low socioeconomic (SES) homes (as indicated by eligibility for the Free and Reduced Lunch program) and 56.8% are English language learners 1. PROCEDURE In the last year of the two-year study, we collected data from the twelve middle schools in the urban school district that participated in a randomized study of the WG intervention. Prior to randomization, state accountability data was used to rank schools on several school-level variables based on the percentage of students identified as ethnic minorities, low-income status, and English language learners, as well as the school’s prior academic achievement. Using propensity score matching, we created composite scores based on the school-level variables and used the composite scores to determine each school’s rankings. Each sequential pair of schools formed a dyad within which randomization occurred. Paired t-tests on each variable used to create the composite scores confirm that treatment and control schools are comparable 2. In the six treatment schools, teachers in the four main content areas (e.g. ELA, social studies, science and math) were required to implement the WG program, whereas teachers in the six control schools did not use the WG or any similar program and provided “business as usual” instruction (Author et al., 2012). In our past evaluations of the WG program, classroom observers gave higher discussion quality ratings in classrooms using the WG schools than classrooms in control schools (Author et al., under review) 3. <INSERT TABLE 3> Students in the WG program encountered a unique set of controversial topics in the first (2010-2011) and second school year (2011-2012) of the randomized study (See Table 3). Since we gave our efficacy survey at the end of the 2011-2012 school year, all students in WG schools had encountered the topics that were taught that year. We refer these topics as “Treatment topics” because all students were exposed to them during the year that we conducted the efficacy survey. The seventh and eighth graders enrolled in the WG program also had experience with the topics covered in the previous year (2010-2011), when they were sixth and seventh grade students. We refer to these topics as “Follow-up topics” because at the time of testing, students had not experienced Word Generation curriculum on those topics for more than a year. Analyzing the Follow-up topics reveals how long any topic-specific increases in self-efficacy last. None of the students in the treatment or control schools encountered the “Control topics”. MEASURES All students who participated in the randomized trial in the year of this study completed survey items that assessed their communicative self-efficacy to discuss controversial issues. Students were asked to respond to the guiding question, “How confident are you in being able to participate in a discussion about the following topics?” and then assessed on a series of 15 topics pertaining to a particular controversial issue (See Table 1 in the Appendix section). For example, students were assessed the question, “How confident are you in being able to participate in a discussion about the topic in regards to should colleges use affirmative action?” and they answered based on a five-point Likert Scale of (1) not at all, (2) a little, (3) somewhat, (4) very, and (5) extremely. Eleven survey items were based on the topics that were covered in the WG program. The other four items also assessed topics of national interest to youth (Kettering Foundation, 2011) that were not covered in the WG program. ANALYSIS PLAN The research questions posed in this study are answered by two-sample mean comparison t -tests to compare differences between students in the treatment and control groups on the average communicative self-efficacy scores of the Control topics (Research Question 1), Treatment topics (Research Question 2), and Follow-Up topics (Research Question 3). Students responding to at least 75% of the controversial issue items corresponding to the Control, Treatment, and Follow-Up topics were included in our analysis of average scores. We also used differences in the average scores of students in the treatment and control groups to calculate overall effect sizes. For the purposes of providing additional information, we also report the communicative self-efficacy scores for each controversial issue item. RESULTS Table 4 presents means and standard deviations of students’ communicative self-efficacy scores on each topic item, as well as average scores in the Control, Treatment, and Follow-Up topics. <INSERT TABLE 4> RQ 1: Did students in the treatment and control group differ in communicative self-efficacy on general topics of interest to adolescents that were not covered in the WG program? The average communicative self-efficacy scores for the Control topics were virtually identical between treatment (M = 2.92, SD = 0.79) and control (M = 2.92, SD = 0.78) students. No differences were found between treatment and control students for the Control topics. Interestingly, we find that control students (M = 2.83, SD = 1.21) reported higher communicative self-efficacy than treatment students (M = 2.75, SD = 1.19) on one of the four items in the Control topics (i.e. “Should schools have a vocational track?”). No differences in communicative self-efficacy scores were found between treatment and control students on the other three items. RQ 2: Did treatment students report higher communicative self-efficacy on topics that had been discussed as part of the WG program during the school year immediately prior to testing, in comparison with control students? Our results indicate that treatment students reported higher communicative self-efficacy than control students on Treatment topics, which suggests that participating in the WG program led to improvements in students’ confidence to discuss topics that were immediately covered prior to testing. Based on t-test analyses, the difference of 0.10 in average scores on the Treatment topics between treatment (M = 2.61, SD = 0.82) and control (M = 2.51, SD = 0.75) students was statistically significant, t (5,814) = -2.67, p < 0.001. We used this difference in average scores to calculate the average treatment effect (d = 0.13). For the purposes of analyzing controversial issues corresponding to the Treatment topics, treatment students report higher communicative self-efficacy than control students on four out of the six items in the Treatment topics. For example, treatment students (M = 2.43, SD = 1.34) report more confidence than control students (M = 2.30, SD = 1.31) to participate in a discussion about whether the government should allow animal testing, t (5,825) = -3.51, p < 0.001. RQ 3: Did treatment students report higher communicative self-efficacy on topics that they had been exposed to as part of the WG program during the school year prior to testing, in comparison with control students? Similar to our results in RQ 1, treatment and control students had virtually identical average communicative self-efficacy scores on Follow-Up topics. These findings suggest that participating in the WG program did not lead to improvements on students’ confidence to discuss Follow-Up topics. Control students reported higher communicative self-efficacy than treatment students on two topic items. For instance, control students reported more confidence about engaging in discussions on whether Creation should be taught in school and the difference of 0.12 was found to be statistically significant, t (3,992) = -3.37, p < 0.001. In contrast, treatment students reported higher communicative self-efficacy on only one topic (i.e. “Should the government fund stem cell research?”). DISCUSSION The results from this evaluation suggest that curricular emphasis on supporting regular discussions of controversial issues can be effective in improving students’ communicative selfefficacy. We note, however, that program effects on students’ communicative self-efficacy were limited to the topics discussed as part of the WG program during the school year immediately prior to testing. These findings are congruent with past studies that found improvements in participants’ communicative self-efficacy when they participated in structured discussions of controversial issues (Liu & Liuwood, 1997). Although past studies suggest that adults can benefit from discussions of controversial issues (Zorn et al., 2006, 2012), the current study extends these findings to adolescents. In addition to displaying confidence to discuss socioscientific topics such as stem cell research, students in the WG program reported more confidence to discuss social issues based on the moral implications of death penalty and rap music. Although controversial issues can serve as the basis for active discussions among adolescents, careful facilitation and curricular support may be needed to effectively build students’ communicative self-efficacy to discuss these issues with their peers. We consider possible explanations of why the WG program may be effective in improving students’ communicative self-efficacy to discuss specific controversial issues. Gist and colleagues (1991) suggests that structured discussions provide students with opportunities to observe similar-aged peers discuss and work towards a collective focus on developing a solution. In addition, teachers can facilitate discussions so that adolescents have opportunities to speak in a structured environment that aims to give respectful treatment to the speaker. These specific discussion features can contribute to participants having positive beliefs in their ability to engage in discussions (Zorn et al., 2006). Another explanation is that the WG program provides subjectspecific instructions that can help build students’ knowledge of controversial issues. Past studies indicate that students learn argumentation skills more effectively when these instructions are combined with subject specific lessons (Angeli & Valanides, 2009, Means & Voss, 1996; Terenzinin, Springer, Pascarella, & Nora, 1995; Tsui, 1999). Means and Voss (1996) found that knowledge is related to performance during debate, such as the number and types of reasons generated by participants. Last, the WG program provides students with opportunities to reflect on different arguments within a controversial issue, which helps them understand the argumentative structure behind discourse practice (Felton, 2004; Kuhn & Udell, 2007). Students may develop more confidence in using advanced argumentation skills when they are directed to consider the “opponent’s assertions and the use of strategies to influence them” (Kuhn & Udell, 2007, p. 91). We acknowledge these findings as possible explanations of why the WG program may be effective in helping students develop communicative self-efficacy. Results from this study also indicate that treatment and control students did not differ in their communicative self-efficacy on Control and Follow-Up topics. Whereas no difference was expected for Control topics, the lack of difference on Follow-up topics indicates that the rise in topical self-efficacy is of limited duration. Perhaps this finding reflects students’ need for ongoing involvement with issues to maintain the knowledge structures that support communicative self-efficacy (Cooper & Sweller, 1987). Controversial issues evolve rapidly and require sustained attention to current events through the media (Lin & Salwen, 1997). It is worth noting that the WG curriculum is not very intensive; each controversial topic gets at most 60-80 minutes of classroom attention. Extensions or improvements to the WG curriculum that includes media and technology integration (Feldman et al., 2007), explicit critical-thinking instructions (Angeli & Valanides, 2009), and support to engage parents in facilitating discussions at home (Bergeron, Weemuth, Rhodes, & Rudenga, 1996) might extend treatment effects. STRENGTHS, LIMITATIONS AND SUGGESTIONS The strength of the study is the large-scale randomized design, which allows for rigorous comparisons between students who participated in the WG program and those who did not. According to past research (Fan, 2001), the tests used in this study can be considered statistically powerful because of the large sample size that was analyzed. Also, the study extends previous findings with its focus on mostly low-income adolescents from diverse ethnic backgrounds. Although these design features enhance the study’s importance, we acknowledge certain limitations. First, the study relies on students’ self-reported confidence to engage in discussions of controversial issues; we have not captured their actual performance during classroom discussions of controversial issues. It is certainly possible that a student who reported having higher confidence to discuss controversial issues will be actively engaged during classroom discussions. The same student, however, is equally likely to have a weak performance during classroom discussion. Past studies on self-efficacy indicate that overconfident individuals may inflate their perceived performance levels, which results in decreased attention to understanding the topics (Powers, 1991; Vancouver, Thompson, & Williams, 2001). Second, we did not administer a pre-test survey, which could have provided baseline information on how students perceived their own communication competencies prior to the randomized study. Last, it cannot be inferred at this point whether differences in students’ communicative self-efficacy may be attributed to other factors such as value orientation, prior knowledge, and conceptual understanding (Ennis, 1998). Although the results of this study provide support for the hypothesized effects, the specific processes that produced these effects require additional analyses. The results from our study also indicate the need for more research in the following areas that relate to examining adolescents’ communicative self-efficacy. In understanding how students participate in these discussions, past studies indicate the need to understand how communicative self-efficacy interacts with other factors that relate to students’ prior knowledge, conceptual understandings, and epistemological beliefs regarding controversial issues (Nussbaum & Bendixen, 2003; Sadler, Chambers, & Zeidler, 2004; von Aufschnaiter, Erduran, Osborne, & Simon, 2008). With the use of student interviews and video footage of classroom discussions, we could also answer qualitative questions about how students respond to specific discourse strategies, topics, and formats used in discussions (Maloney & Simon, 2006). Future studies should also consider longitudinal development of communicative self-efficacy, which informs how these skills may translate into future political behavior. Expanding research in these critical areas is important to developing comprehensive assessments and teaching tools that will help schools effectively use controversial issues as a basis for classroom instructions and discussions. The purpose of this study was to understand whether a literacy intervention that organizes classroom discussions around controversial issues has the potential of influencing students’ communicative self-efficacy. Findings from the study suggest that students who enroll in this intervention are more likely to exhibit confidence in their abilities to engage in discussions of controversial issues that are taught and discussed as part of the program. Despite these positive findings, program effectiveness might well be improved by spending more time on each topic, adding opportunities to revisit certain issues, and/or extending deliberation to out-of-school contexts (e.g. home, online, and after-school programs). Nonetheless, the implications of this study highlight the importance of combining frequent discussion opportunities of controversial issues with instruction in subject-specific content. In facilitating discussions of controversial issues, teachers face the daunting challenge of not only ensuring that students maintain respect towards each other, but also making sure that competing points of views are equally represented (Halpern, 1998; Hess, 2002). The goal of this exercise is not to encourage students to take a dogmatic stand on a particular issue, but to help them develop confidence about their skills to consider multiple sides of an issue as informed and critical consumers of information. FOOTNOTES 1 Preliminary analyses (results not shown in the tables) indicate that students’ communicative self-efficacy did not vary substantially across key demographic features (e.g. race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, gender); thus, providing justification of not controlling for these demographic features (Ammentorp, Sabroe, Kofoed, & Mainz, 2007; Finlay, Maughan, & Webster, 1998; Tankova, Dakovska, & Koev, 2004). We also do not account for nesting within schools in our analysis given the small number of schools (n = 12) that participated in the study. The intra-class coefficient (ICC ) was found to be 0.006, which indicates no considerable differences with respect to the proportion of between-school variance. 2 Treatment and control schools are comparable across all school-level variables, which suggests that the randomization process was successful. In our analysis of the second year in the randomized study, we acknowledge significant differences in academic achievement –as indicated by CST ELA, vocabulary, and reading comprehension scores – between WG and control schools. This finding suggests that a year of participating in the WG program helped improve students’ academic skills. 3 The author (under review) compared classroom discussion quality in schools that participated in a randomized study of the WG program. Observers attended training sessions that instructed on using standardized codes for teacher and conversational moves. Up to three observers were assigned to rate classrooms in treatment and control schools. Participating teachers were randomized and a schedule was created identifying specific observation periods to be conducted on a given day. The discussion quality rating was developed to account for several measures including: the number of students participating, frequency of teacher initiating open-ended question, and students’ intellectual contributions. In 168 observations conducted in content-area classrooms across treatment (n = 80) and control schools (n = 88), observers gave higher classroom discussion quality rating on classrooms using the WG program, in comparison with those in the control schools. REFERENCES Ammentorp, J., Sabroe, S., Kofoed, P.E., & Mainz, J. (2007). The effect of training in communication skills on medical doctors’ and nurses’ self-efficacy: a randomized controlled trial. Patient education and counseling, 66(3), 270-277. Adler, R. P., & Goggin, J. (2005). What do we mean by “civic engagement”? Journal of Transformative Education, 3, 236–253. Angeli, C., & Valanides, N. (2009). Instructional effects on critical thinking: Performance on illdefined issues. Learning and Instruction, 19, 322–334. Author. (2013). Citizenship education in American schools and its role in developing civic engagement: A review of the research. Educational Review. Author et al. (2009). Generating knowledge of academic language among urban middle school students. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2(4), 325-344. Author et al. (2012). Language proficiency, home-language status, and English vocabulary development: A longitudinal follow-up of the Word Generation program. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition (15), 437-451. Author et al. (Under review). Word Generation randomized trial: Discussion mediates the impact of program treatment on academic word learning. Reading Research Quarterly. Author et al. (Under review). Differential effects of a systematic vocabulary intervention on adolescent language minority students with varying levels of English proficiency. International Journal of Bilingualism. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman. Bergeron, B. S., Weemuth, S., Rhodes, M., & Rudenga, E. A. (1996). Language development and thematic instruction: supporting young learners at risk. Childhood Education, 72(3), 141145. Button, M., & Ryfe, D. (2005). What can we learn from the practice of deliberative democracy? In J. Gastil & P. Levine (Eds.), The deliberative democracy handbook. San Francisco: Josey-Bass. Center for Information and Research on Civic Learning & Engagement (CIRCLE) (2013). All together now: Collaboration and innovation for youth engagement. Boston, MA: Tufts University. Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(4), 347. Cross, D., Taasoobshirazi, G., Hendricks, S., & Hickey, D. T. (2008). Argumentation: A strategy for improving achievement and revealing scientific identities. International Journal of Science Education, 30(6), 837-861. Davies, S. R. (2009). Doing dialogue: Genre and flexibility in public engagement with science. Science as Culture, 18(4), 397-416. Ellinor, L., & Gerard, G. (1998). Dialogue: Rediscover the transforming power of conversation. New York: J. Wiley & Sons. Ennis, R. H. (1989). Critical thinking and subject specificity: Clarification and needed research. Educational Researcher, 18(3), 4-10. Fan, X. (2001). Parental involvement and students' academic achievement: A growth modeling analysis. The Journal of Experimental Education, 70(1), 27-61. Feldman, L., Pasek, J., Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2007). Identifying best practices in civic education: Lessons from the Student Voices program. American Journal of Education, 114(1), 75-100. Felton, M. K. (2004). The development of discourse strategies in adolescent argumentation. Cognitive Development, 19(1), 35-52. Felton, M., & Kuhn, D. (2001). The development of argumentive discourse skill. Discourse Processes, 32(2-3), 135–153. Finlay, I., Maughan, T., & Webster, D. (1998). A randomized controlled study of portfolio learning in undergraduate cancer education. Medical Education, 32, 172-176. Gist, M. E., Stevens, C. K., & Bavetta, A. G. (1991). Effects of self-efficacy and post-training intervention on the acquisition and maintenance of complex interpersonal skills. Personnel Psychology, 44(4), 837-861. Guttmann, A., & Thompson, D. F. (1996). Democracy and disagreement. Harvard: Harvard University Press. Hackler, J. C., Ho, K.-Y., & Urquhart-Ross, C. (1974). The willingness to intervene: Differing community characteristics. Social Problems, 21(3), 328–344. Hahn, C. L. (1991). Controversial issues in social studies. In J. Shaver (Ed.), Handbook of research on social studies teaching and learning (pp. 470-480). New York, NY: MacMillan Reference Books. Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Disposition, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53(4), 449. Harwood, A. M., & Hahn, C. L. (1990). Controversial issues in the classroom. Bloomington, IN: ERIC Clearinghouse for Social Studies/Social Science Education. Herrenkohl, L. R., Palincsar, A. S., DeWater, L. S., & Kawasaki, K. (1999). Developing scientific communities in classrooms: A sociocognitive approach. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8(3-4), 451-493. Hess, D. E. (2002). Discussing controversial public issues in secondary social studies classrooms: Learning from skilled teachers. Theory & Research in Social Education, 30(1), 10-41. Hess, D., & Posselt, J. (2002). How high school students experience and learn from the discussion of controversial public issues. Journal of Curriculum and Supervision, 17(4), 283-314. Isaacs, W. (2008). Dialogue: The art of thinking together. London: Random House Digital, Inc. Kettering Foundation. (2011). Naming and framing difficult issues to make sound decisions. Dayton, OH: Charles F. Kettering Foundation. Kim, Y.-C., & Ball-Rokeach, S. J. (2006). Community storytelling network, neighborhood context, and civic engagement: A multilevel approach. Human Communication Research, 32(4), 411–439. Kim, J., Wyatt, R. O., & Katz, E. (1999). News, talk, opinion, participation: The part played by conversation in deliberative democracy. Political Communication, 16(4), 361-385. Klosterman, M. L., & Sadler, T. D. (2010). Multi‐level Assessment of Scientific Content Knowledge Gains Associated with Socioscientific Issues‐based Instruction. International Journal of Science Education, 32(8), 1017-1043. Koesten, J., Miller, K. I., & Hummert, M. L. (2002). Family communication, self-efficacy, and white female adolescents' risk behavior. The Journal of Family Communication, 2(1), 7-27. Kuhn, D., & Udell, W. (2003). The development of argument skills. Child Development, 74(5), 1245– 1260. Kuhn, D., & Udell, W. (2007). Coordinating own and other perspectives in argument. Thinking & Reasoning, 13(2), 90-104. Lee, N.-J., Shah, D. V., & McLeod, J. M. (2012). Processes of political socialization: A Communication mediation approach to youth civic engagement. Communication Research, 40(5), 669–697. Lin, C. A., & Salwen, M. B. (1997). Predicting the spiral of silence on a controversial public issue. Howard Journal of Communications, 8(1), 129-141. Liu, N.F., & Littlewood, W. (1997). Why do many students appear reluctant to participate in classroom learning discourse? System, 25(3), 371-384. Lockwood, A. L., & Harris, D. E. (1985). Reasoning with democratic values: Ethical problems in United States history. New York: Teachers College Press. Long, S., & Long, R. (1975). Controversy in the classroom: Student viewpoint and educational outcome. Teaching Political Science, 2, 275-299. Lonie, J. M., & Rahim, H. (2010). Does the addition of writing into a pharmacy communication skills course significantly impact student communicative learning outcomes? A pilot study. Journal of Pharmacy Practice, 23(6), 525-530. Maloney, J., & Simon, S. (2006). Mapping children’s discussions of evidence in science to assess collaboration and argumentation. International Journal of Science Education, 28(15), 1817-1841. Martocchio, J. J., & Webster, J. (1992). Effects of feedback and cognitive playfulness on performance in microcomputer software training. Personnel Psychology, 45(3), 553-578. Mancilla-Martinez, J. (2010). Word meanings matter: Cultivating english vocabulary knowledge in fifth grade spanish-speaking language minority learners. TESOL Quarterly, 44(4), 669-699. Means, M. L., & Voss, J. F. (1996). Who reasons well? Two Studies of informal reasoning among children of different grade, ability, and knowledge levels. Cognition and Instruction, 14(2), 139– 178. McDevitt, M. J., & Caton-Rosser, M. S. (2009). Deliberative barbarians: Reconciling the civic and the agonistic in democratic education. InterActions: UCLA Journal of Education and Information Studies, 5(2). Milstein, T. (2005). Transformation abroad: Sojourning and the perceived enhancement of self-efficacy. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29(2), 217-238. National Governors Association Center for Best Practices. (2010). Common Core State Standards (English language arts and literacy in history/social studies, science, and technical subjects). Washington D.C.: National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State. Newmann, F. M. (1988). Higher order thinking in high school social studies: An analysis of classrooms, teachers, students, and leadership. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin, National Center on Effective Secondary Schools. Nussbaum, M. E., & Bendixen, L. D. (2003). Approaching and avoiding arguments: The role of epistemological beliefs, need for cognition, and extraverted personality traits. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28(4), 573-595. Nystrand, M., Gamoran, A., & Carbonaro, W. (1998). Towards an ecology of learning: The case of classroom discourse and its effects on writing in high school English and social studies. University at Albany: Center on English Learning & Achievement. Powers, W. T. (1991). Commentary on Bandura's" human agency.". American Psychology, 46, 152-153. Sadler, T. D., Chambers, F. W., & Zeidler, D. L. (2004). Student conceptualizations of the nature of science in response to a socioscientific issue. International Journal of Science Education, 26(4), 387-409. Steiner, S., Brzuzy, S., Gerdes, K., & Hurdle, D. (2003). Using structured controversy to teach diversity content and cultural competence. Journal of Teaching in Social Work, 23(1-2), 55-71. Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Marti, C. N. (2006). A meta-analytic review of obesity prevention programs for children and adolescents: The skinny on interventions that work. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 667. Swanson, K. (2013). Teaching the common core speaking and listening standards: Strategies and digital tools. United Kingdom: Routledge. Tankova, T., Dakovska, G., & Koev, D. (2004). Education and quality of life in diabetic patients. Patient education and counseling, 53(3), 285-290. Terenzini, P. T., Springer, L., Pascarella, E. T., & Nora, A. (1995). Influences affecting the development of students’ critical thinking skills. Research in Higher Education, 36, 23– 39. Torney-Purta, J., Lehmann, R., Oswald, H., & Schulz, W. (2001). Citizenship and education in twenty-eight countries: Civic knowledge and engagement at age fourteen. Amsterdam: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement. Tsui, L. (1999). Courses and instruction affecting critical thinking. Research in Higher Education, 40(2), 185-200. Vancouver, J. B., Thompson, C. M., & Williams, A. A. (2001). The changing signs in the relationships among self-efficacy, personal goals, and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(4), 605-620. Venville, G. J., & Dawson, V. M. (2010). The impact of a classroom intervention on grade 10 students' argumentation skills, informal reasoning, and conceptual understanding of science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47(8), 952-977. Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (1995). Voice and equality: Civic voluntarism in American politics. Harvard: Harvard University Press. Vercellotti, T., & Matto, E. (2013). The role of media use in the classroom and at home in improving political knowledge. Presented at the 2013 APSA Teaching and Learning Conference, Long Beach, CA. von Aufschnaiter, C., Erduran, S., Osborne, J., & Simon, S. (2008). Arguing to learn and learning to argue: Case studies of how students' argumentation relates to their scientific knowledge. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45(1), 101-131. Wells, J., & Lewis, L. (2006). Internet Access in US Public Schools and Classrooms: 1994-2005. Highlights. NCES 2007-020. National Center for Education Statistics. Wray‐Lake, L., & Syvertsen, A. K. (2011). The developmental roots of social responsibility in childhood and adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2011(134), 11-25. Zorn, T. E., Roper, J., Broadfoot, K., & Kay Weaver, C. (2006). Focus groups as sites of influential interaction: Building communicative self-efficacy and effecting attitudinal change in discussing controversial topics. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 34(2), 115-140. Zorn, T. E., Roper, J., Weaver, C. K., & Rigby, C. (2012). Influence in science dialogue: Individual attitude changes as a result of dialogue between laypersons and scientists. Public Understanding of Science, 21(7), 848-864. TABLES Table 1. Grade Level Contributions by Schools Organized by Treatment Condition 6th Graders 7th Graders 8th Graders All Grades Schools n % of school n % of school n % of school N % of sample by condition 155 56 76 256 -107 650 43.9% 48.3% 17.7% 36.9% -31.3% 27.6% 59 21 117 212 214 111 734 16.7% 18.1% 27.3% 30.5% 51.2% 32.5% 31.2% 139 39 236 226 204 124 968 39.4% 33.6% 55.0% 32.6% 48.8% 36.3% 41.2% 353 116 429 694 418 342 2,352 15.0% 4.9% 18.2% 29.5% 17.8% 14.5% 40.1% Word Generation Schools Apple Arbola Flower Square Hemlock Moon Palm Total 371 279 118 62 87 281 1,198 35.0% 36.4% 40.8% 36.9% 25.0% 31.7% 34.1% 352 239 72 61 135 323 1,182 33.2% 31.2% 24.9% 36.3% 38.8% 36.5% 33.6% 338 248 99 45 126 282 1,138 31.9% 32.4% 34.3% 26.8% 36.2% 31.8% 32.3% 1,061 766 289 168 348 886 3,518 30.2% 21.8% 8.2% 4.8% 9.9% 25.2% 59.9% All Schools 1,848 31.5% 2,106 35.9% 5,870 100.0% Control Schools Duffie Oak Evergreen Honeysuckle Maple Rosemary Vineland Total 1,916 32.6% Note. Analytic sample based on students who responded to at least 3 out of 15 survey items. Table 2. Demographics of the Analytical Sample by Treatment Condition Control Schools Female Special Education Free and Reduced Lunch Eligible English Language Learner Asian African-American Hispanic White Other Total Students Word Generation Schools n % n % 968 175 1,344 1,219 1,115 146 397 110 60 2,352 50.0% 9.0% 69.5% 63.0% 57.6% 7.6% 20.5% 5.6% 3.1% 40.1% 1,595 261 1,944 1,710 1,673 209 597 332 160 3,518 49.5% 8.1% 60.4% 53.1% 51.9% 6.5% 18.5% 10.3% 4.9% 59.9% Note. Analytic sample based on students who responded to at least 3 out of 15 survey items. All Schools N % 2,563 49.7% 436 8.5% 3,288 63.8% 2,929 56.8% 2,788 54.1% 355 6.9% 994 19.3% 442 8.6% 220 4.3% 5,870 100.0% Table 3. Overview of the Control, Treatment, and Follow-Up Topics Sixth Graders Seventh Graders Eighth Graders WG Control # of Schools Schools Students Instructed Instructed Tested WG Control # of Schools Schools Students Instructed Instructed Tested WG Control # of Schools Schools Students Instructed Instructed Tested Treatment Topics Should athletes be allowed to use steroids? Should there be more strict dress codes at schools? Is the death penalty justified? Does rap music have a negative impact on students? What should be done about global warming? Should the government allow animal testing? Y Y Y Y Y Y N N N N N N 1,825 1,842 1,825 1,827 1,811 1,836 Y Y Y Y Y Y N N N N N N 1,901 1,907 1,904 1,907 1,892 1,906 Y Y Y Y Y Y N N N N N N 2,085 2,093 2,082 2,085 2,073 2,085 Follow-Up Topics Should colleges use Affirmative Action? Should the government fund stem cell research? Should Creation be taught in schools? Should English be the official language of the U.S.? Is nuclear power a danger to society N N N N N N N N N N 1,820 1,822 1,833 1,819 1,825 Y Y Y Y Y N N N N N 1,902 1,912 1,906 1,907 1,901 Y Y Y Y Y N N N N N 2,081 2,091 2,092 2,087 2,079 Control Topics Should schools protect students from cyber-bullying? Should schools be a place for debates? Should secret wiretapping be legal? Should schools have a vocational track? N N N N N N N N 1,837 1,815 1,810 1,837 N N N N N N N N 1,909 1,899 1,895 1,914 N N N N N N N N 2,086 2,078 2,063 2,098 Note. WG = Word Generation; Y= Yes, N = No. Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations of Students' Communicative Self-Efficacy to Participate in Discussions of Control, Treatment, and Follow-Up Topics with Estimated Effect Size Control Schools Topic Questions Word Generation Schools All Schools t-test Effect Size (Cohen's d) n M SD n M SD N M SD Difference Sig Research Question # 1 Should schools protect students from cyberbullying? Should schools be a place for debates? Should secret wiretapping be legal? Should schools have a vocational track? Average Score of Control Topics 2,344 2,334 2,322 2,314 2,332 3.92 2.57 2.36 2.83 2.92 1.22 1.21 1.30 1.21 0.79 3,505 3,498 3,470 3,454 3,489 3.92 2.64 2.36 2.75 2.92 1.21 1.21 1.27 1.19 0.78 5,849 5,832 5,792 5,768 5,821 3.92 2.61 2.36 2.78 2.92 1.21 1.21 1.28 1.20 0.79 0.00 + 0.08 0.00 - 0.08 0.00 ** ns Research Question # 2 Should athletes be allowed to use steroids? Should there be more strict dress codes at schools? Is the death penalty justified? Does rap music have a negative impact on students? What should be done about global warming? Should the government allow animal testing? Average Score of Treatment Topics 2,327 2,343 2,323 2,328 2,315 2,332 2,324 1.99 2.02 2.65 2.37 3.70 2.30 2.51 1.28 1.23 1.29 1.29 1.27 1.31 0.75 3,484 3,499 3,488 3,491 3,461 3,495 3,492 2.07 2.16 2.74 2.50 3.75 2.43 2.61 1.31 1.29 1.30 1.29 1.24 1.34 0.82 5,811 5,842 5,811 5,819 5,776 5,827 5,816 2.04 2.11 2.71 2.45 3.73 2.38 2.57 1.30 1.27 1.30 1.29 1.25 1.32 0.80 + 0.08 + 0.14 + 0.09 + 0.13 + 0.05 + 0.13 + 0.10 Research Question # 3 Should colleges use Affirmative Action? Should the government fund stem cell research? Should Creation be taught in school? Should English be the official language of the U.S.? Is nuclear power a danger to society? Average Score of Follow-Up Topics 1,686 1,693 1,687 1,692 1,682 1,690 2.70 2.80 3.14 3.15 3.18 2.99 1.07 1.11 1.17 1.37 1.36 0.76 2,301 2,313 2,307 2,308 2,298 2,306 2.71 2.98 3.02 3.03 3.21 2.99 1.04 1.15 1.21 1.36 1.28 0.77 3,987 4,006 3,994 4,000 3,980 3,996 2.70 2.90 3.07 3.08 3.20 2.99 1.05 1.14 1.20 1.36 1.31 0.77 + 0.01 + 0.18 - 0.12 - 0.12 + 0.03 0.00 *** *** *** *** *** + 0.13 *** *** ** ns Note. Students were given the prompt "How confident are you in being able to participate in a discussion about the following topics?" and followed by a series of controversial topics. Answers based on a 5-point Likert scale of (1) "not at all" to (5) "extremely" confident in being able to participate in such discussion; Analytic sample based on students who responded to at least 3 out of 15 survey items; Sample used to analyze Follow-Up Topics restricted to only seventh and eighth graders; Averages for Control, Treatment and Follow-Up Topics only includes students who responded to at least 75% of the corresponding items; ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. 33 APPENDIX Survey Items Assessing Students' Communicative Self-Efficacy in Discussions of Controversial Issues How confident are you in being able to participate in a discussion about the following topics? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Should colleges use Affirmative Action? Should the government fund stem cell research? Should Creation be taught in schools? Should schools protect students from cyber-bullying? Should English be the official language of the U.S.? Should there be more strict dress codes at schools? Should schools be a place for debates? Should secret wiretapping be legal? Is the death penalty justified? Should athletes be allowed to use steroids? Does rap music have a negative impact on students? Is nuclear power a danger to society? What should be done about global warming? Should schools have a vocational track? Should the government allow animal testing? Not at all A Little Somewhat Very Extremely (1) (2) (3) (4) (5)