The Truth About Lies; Judgment About Lying in Adolescents. Valerie Hernandez Southwestern Adventist University In Partial Fulfillment of PSYC 460 Chapter 1 Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to examine adolescents’ judgments about lying to avoid directives from parents or friends regarding moral, personal, and prudential areas (Perkins and Turiel, 2007). Humans are taught to lie at times to not hurt other people’s feelings or to save someone from pain or discomfort. In this study, I will establish how many adolescents believe it is wrong to lie when “covering up” or in general. Theoretical Framework The construct of the moral domain, personal, and prudential domains, is based on a theoretical framework that accounts for the application of moral judgments in various cultural contexts; for extensive discussions see (Turiel, 2002; Turiel & Wainryb, 1994, 2000). In their research on nondisclosure, Smetana et al. (2006) found that adolescents thought they were less obligated to tell parents about personal activities than moral or prudential activities. They also found that older adolescents were more likely than younger ones to accept the legitimacy of nondisclosure and to reject the legitimacy of parental restrictions over personal issues (see also Finkenaurer et al., 2002; Smetana, 2002). The research on nondisclosure has not directly examined adolescents’ judgments about the types of deception defined by Bok (1978/1999, p. 13) as ‘‘messages meant to mislead . . . any intentional message which is stated.’’ This theoretical framework will serve as a basis for conducting research throughout this study. Research Questions and Hypothesis These are questions that will be asked to the sample participants, “Would it be acceptable for the adolescent to lie about the action?; Was it acceptable for the parents or friends to direct the adolescent’s activities? Is lying alright or not alright? I have hypothesized that adolescents would consider lying to “cover up” a misdeed and lying ‘‘in general’’ as wrong, and that adolescents value honesty, but sometimes subordinate it to other goals. (Perkins and Turiel, 2007). Limitations This study will rely on sample participants to be straight forward with their answers and as honest as possible. The sampling process may be difficult to attain 100% accuracy, as one could imagine. But the choice of conducting this study on adolescents regarding relation to peers is due to the closer relationship they entail with each other. Generally, at that age of adolescence, the parent-adolescent relationship weakens which gives them more chance of deception due to fear of consequences. Thus, a large volunteer sample population could result in more accurate responses. Definition of Terms Independent variables - domain, age, and relationship. Dependent variable – Justification. Directives – an authoritative instruction or direction; specific order. This term will be used as a key word throughout the study. Definitions of the moral, personal, and prudential domains have been provided in many places; for summaries, see Nucci, 2001; Smetana, 1988; Turiel, 1983, 1998. Overview of Following Chapters In the next two chapters I will be discussing the literature used to complete the study. Along with the literature review (chapter 2), the methodology (chapter 3) will be explained as thoroughly as possible to show how the study will be done. parts: The methodology consists of four the research design and sampling combined, the data collection method, and the data analysis. Chapter 2 Literature Review The sources I used for this literature review were very descriptive and will help one have a better understanding of the study. One hundred and twenty-eight adolescents (12.1 – 17.3 years) were presented with situations in which an adolescent avoids a directive through deception. The majority of adolescents judged some acts as acceptable, including deception regarding parental directives to engage in moral violations and to restrict personal activities. Other acts of deception were judged as unacceptable, including deception of parents regarding prudential acts, as well as deception of friends in each area. Here is where the research can be analyzed and help the study by recording how the adolescents reacted. By reading this in the article one can note for future studies how effective their study was and base it on my theoretical framework. In addition, lying to conceal a misdeed was negatively evaluated. Most adolescents thought that directives from parents and friends to engage in moral violations or to restrict personal acts were not legitimate, whereas parental directives concerning prudential acts were seen as legitimate (Perkins & Turiel, 2007). We notice here the difference between directives from parents and directives from friends. Justifications for lying in various circumstances have been debated by philosophers. Some of the debates revolve around Kant’s absolutist position that the duty of truthfulness is unconditional, and that all lies are morally prohibited (Bok, 1978/1999). As children, Many have disagreed with Kant because his position does not leave room for exceptions, such as when telling the truth is in conflict with avoiding harm to persons or preventing injustices. A hypothetical example used to illustrate that deception is sometimes justified is when a murderer asks where his intended victim has gone. In a real-life context, people often lied in order to save others from Nazi concentration camps during World War II. Research in the medical field shows that deception is judged as justified in some contexts involving the prevention of harm (Freeman, Rathore, Weinfurt, Schulman, & Sulmasy, 1999). In response to hypothetical situations, a majority of physicians judged deception of an insurance company to be acceptable when it is the only means of preventing harm to patients (to obtain treatment otherwise unavailable). Psychological research too, demonstrates that children, adolescents, and adults do not evaluate all lies in the same way. It has been found that children and adults rate (Peterson, Peterson, & Seeto, 1983) or judge (Bussey, 1999) selfishly motivated lies as worse than so-called ‘‘white lies’’ intended to spare the feelings of others and lies to benefit another or prevent harm. It has also been found that Chinese children (more so than Canadians) negatively rated telling the truth about carrying out a good deed (Lee, Cameron, Xu, Fu, & Board, 1997). Similarly, college students (Lindskold & Walters, 1983) rate lies for personal gain as wrong and lies to prevent harm or embarrassment to others as acceptable. These and other studies (Kahn & Turiel, 1988; Nucci, 1981) show that children and adolescents judge acts of deception to be wrong in certain circumstances, but acceptable when telling the truth would result in hurting the feelings of others. There is also research showing that college students tell lies to both strangers and those close to them, including parents (DePaulo & Kashy, 1998.) The finding that young people deceive their parents should not be surprising. It is generally acknowledged that particularly during adolescence conflicts occur with parents and that adolescents deceive parents about a number of matters they consider within their personal jurisdiction. Chapter 3 Research Design & Sampling The methodology for this study is adopted in part by a study done by Perkins and Turiel (2007). The participants in this study consisted of 100 adolescents, (50 males, 50 females), from public schools in Dallas. The sample students were chosen by recruitment going to different classes at the middle school and asking the students if they would like to be a part of the study. Most of the students were interested with a result of 89% volunteer rate. The students needed parental consent to participate in this study, so permission slips were given to each volunteer student. age. Age group ranged from 11-14 years of Race and ethnicity was diverse and varied from 40% Whites, 30% Hispanics, 20% Blacks and 10% Asians. Judgments about deception in different contexts were assessed through an interview that included hypothetical situations in which adolescents and parents or adolescents and friends disagreed over two moral, two personal, and two prudential activities. The classifications were based on criteria used in a large body of previous research (Nucci, 2001; Smetana, 2002; Turiel, 1983, 1998). Data Collection Method In order to provide participants with instances of deception that would be treated as meaningful, the stories described an adolescent who defies the directive and lies about it. Participants’ judgments concerning two questions were assessed. First, would it be acceptable for the adolescent to lie about the action? Second, was it acceptable for the parents or friends to direct the adolescent’s activities? Evaluations of lying for personal gain were also assessed by presenting a story that depicted an adolescent who deceives parents or friends to cover up a misdeed. They were also presented with the general question, ‘‘Is lying alright or not alright?’’ In each story, the adolescent wants to engage in an activity, the parents/friends object to the activity, and the protagonist continues to engage in the activity and lies about what he or she is doing. The stories classified as moral involved fairness (parents/friends object to an adolescent wanting to befriend someone of another race) and harm (parents/friends want the adolescent to fight with another student who is teasing him/her). The stories classified as personal involved choices of relationships (parents/friends object to the adolescent dating someone they do not like) and leisure time (the adolescent wants to join a club and the parents/friends think it is a waste of time). The stories classified as prudential involved potential harm to the self-regarding academic welfare (parents/friends object to an adolescent’s choice not to finish her homework) and physical welfare (parents/friends object to the adolescent’s choice to ride a motorcycle). One additional story, presented after the other stories, depicted an adolescent who lies to parents or friends to cover up a misdeed: in anger the adolescent breaks the parents’ or friends’ cellular phone and tells her parents/friends she does not know who did it (because of time constraints, 8 participants were not given this story). This story (in the moral domain) was designed to present a situation about lying that might not be perceived as in conflict with other goals in the domains of personal jurisdiction or prudence. At the end of the interview, participants were posed with the following question: ‘‘In general, do you think lying is alright or not alright?’’ All interviews were tape-recorded and transcribed. The order of the presentation of the initial six stories was counterbalanced. Each participant was presented with either all seven stories with the parent – adolescent relationship or all seven stories with friendships. In three stories, the protagonist is female and in three the protagonist is male. In the story about covering up a misdeed, the protagonist is female. Based on previous research (KeltikangasJarvinen & Lindeman, 1997), the gender of the protagonist was not expected to have an impact on judgments about lying. The stories and questions were aimed at examining evaluations and judgments about lying in contexts where adolescents defy directives from parents or friends. Therefore, the questions focused on lying (rather than defiance or noncompliance) and the legitimacy of the directives. Participants were asked the following questions regarding the acts of deception: Is it all right or not all right for x (the adolescent) to do the action in secret and not tell his/her parents/friends? In each story, this question directly followed the depiction of the adolescent who deliberately lied about his or her activities. Almost all participants’ responses indicated that they understood the question to bear on continuing the activity and lying about it. A few stated that nondisclosure without lying would be acceptable; they were then asked to evaluate an adolescent who lies about the act. Justifications were elicited by asking, why or why not? In order to ascertain how participants viewed the legitimacy of directives, they were asked: Is it all right or not all right that the parents/friends expect him to do (not to do) this? Why or Why not? (Perkins and Turiel, 2007). This data collection method was chosen because it has proven to work before therefore not needing to conduct a pilot study. Proposed Data Analysis After the study is conducted, responses will be analyzed by means of a log-linear approach in which models are generated that predict the response on a specified dependent variable as a function of various combinations of the specified independent variables. References Bok, S. (1999). Lying: Moral choice in public and private life. New York: Vintage Books. (Original work published in 1978) Bussey, K. (1999). Children’s categorization and evaluation of different types of lies and truths. Child Development, 70, 1338 – 1347 Finkenauer, C., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Meeus, W. (2002).Keeping secrets from parents: Advantages and disadvantages of secrecy in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 123 – 136. Freeman, V. G., Rathore, S. S., Weinfurt, K. P., Schulman, K.A., & Sulmasy, D. P. (1999). Lying for patients: Physician deception of third party payers. The Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 2263 – 2270. Kahn, P. H., & Turiel, E. (1988). Children’s conceptions of trust in the context of social expectations. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 34, 403 – 419. Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., & Lindeman, M. (1997). Evaluation of theft, lying and fighting in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 467 – 481. Lee, K., Cameron, C. A., Xu, F., Fu, G., & Board, J. (1997). Chinese and Canadian children’s evaluations of lying and truth telling: Similarities and differences in the context of pro- and antisocial behaviors. Child Development, 68, 924 – 934. Lindskold, S., & Walters, P. S. (1983). Categories for the acceptability of lies. The Journal of Social Psychology, 120, 129 – 136. Nucci, L. P. (1981). Conceptions of personal issues: A domain distinct from moral or societal concepts. Child Development, 52, 114 – 121. Perkins, S. A., & Turiel, E. (2007). To lie or not to lie: To whom and under what circumstances. Child Development. 78(2), 609-621. DOI:10.1111/j.14678624.2007.01017.x Peterson, C. C., Peterson, J. L., & Seeto, D. (1983). Developmental changes in ideas about lying. Child Development, 54, 1529 – 1535. Smetana, J. G. (2002). Culture, autonomy, and personal jurisdiction in adolescent-parent relationships. In H. W. Reese & R. Kail (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 29, pp. 51 – 87). New York: Academic Press. Turiel,E.(1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Turiel,E.(1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th Ed., Vol. 3, N. Eisenberg, (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development, pp. 863 – 932). New York: Wiley. Turiel, E., & Wainryb, C. (1998). Concepts of freedoms and rights in a traditional hierarchically organized society. British Journal of Developmental Psychology., 16, 375 – 395. Turiel, E., & Wainryb, C. (2000). Social life in cultures: Judgments, conflicts, and subversion. Child Development, 71, 250 – 256.