Running head: TRUST IN AUTHORITY 1 Trust in Authority Aaron Pacholke Loras College TRUST IN AUTHORITY 2 Abstract Authority generally relies on trust to convince followers to cooperate in its actions and decisions. Where trust fails, authority will use some amount of force to maintain its guidelines. Trust can be measured with approval and willingness to cooperate with authority’s actions. Examples of authority such as the police, employers, and tax officials demonstrate the interplay between fairness, power, character traits, and charisma. Cooperation and approval can be measured relating to authority’s actions. Controversial acts of authority can polarize people over their trust in authority or push populations toward greater extremes of distrust. Studying trust in authority can help prevent lack of approval and cooperation through fairness. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 3 Trust in Authority Discussion Outside of force, trust is the greatest thing that substantiates the power that authorities use to regulate, police, and mandate the lives of others. Trusted authorities are followed willingly while forceful authorities are followed by those who wish to avoid the consequences of not following. Democratic states usually try to rule through trust wherever possible as misbalanced force is thought to evaporate the trust in authority, which this paper will elaborate on. The important questions are what constitutes unbalanced force and what are the repercussions of forceful authority on the level of trust in that authority. I hypothesize that actions seen as overtly forceful in response to protests will have the effect of reducing the trust imparted to that authority by those not involved in the original protests or authority. Bases of Trust There are a few elements that need to be isolated and examined on authority before addressing the nature of its actions in response to protests. The amount of trust bestowed on an authority’s can be through the cooperation offered the authority and the reported approval of the authority’s actions. A distinction should stand between the cooperation and approval of those currently protesting the action of authority and those bystanders observing the protests and authority’s responses. It should also be clarified that protest refers to any action or movement intended to oppose or state objections of the authorities’ actions. This model of authority and protests can include examples from parent/child to government/civilian. Before considering the consequences of authority’s actions, the variables attributing to the approval and cooperation entrusted to authority must be included. Researchers van Dijke, De Cremer, and Mayer cooperated between Universities from the Netherlands to Michigan in TRUST IN AUTHORITY 4 identifying and testing the effects of power and fairness on the level of trust granted to authorities. In surveying Dutch employees, they found that fairness affected the trust in employers that had a high level of power more than those considered to have less power. (van Dijke, De Cremer, and Mayer, 2010) Power can be translated as either the ability to force or the authority’s own strength in character (charisma) in persuading cooperation. Those with a greater amount of power over an individual will have a greater variation of trust impacting their approval probably because of the scale to which that authority can influence the individuals’ lives. It is then the more powerful authorities are more suitable for study when considering the effects of trust on approval. The role of a police officer is an accepted position of power which daily has requires the approval and cooperation of citizens. Australian researchers expanded the current research associated with the public’s views of the police based on the procedural justice/fairness displayed by the police while enforcing laws. Their concern was to research the effect of police fairness on the views held by those handled by law enforcement rather than the wider population which might not have interacted with police at any time. They found that those directly in contact with police behavior, like the greater population not directly influenced that higher perceived fairness from the police resulted in a greater opinion of police legitimacy, hence greater approval and cooperation. (Elliott, Thomas, and Ogloff, 2011). While this study did find that similar changes in views affected fairness, it did not sample observers that did not interact with police or compare the change in views from fair treatment between those handled by police and observers. One possible research idea for study between groups affected and not affected by police actions might reveal a differing level of power attributed to police. The differing trust might impact the amount of variance in trust like that found in the employer/employee study. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 5 (van Dijke, De Cremer, and Mayer, 2010) The differing amounts of exposure to authority could attribute to perceived power and thus affect approval, trust, and cooperation. Cooperation Any authority ultimately asks or demands cooperation from its followers in completing tasks or following the group’s rules. It was earlier purposed that trust in authority could partially be measured through the level of cooperation authority experienced. (De Cremer and Tyler, 2007) Researchers from Tilburg and New York Universities surveyed the effects that trust and procedural fairness have on the level of cooperation received by authorities. An interesting variable they included was the presence of the participant’s voice in the authority’s decisionmaking which actually had a greater effect on cooperation than trust alone. (De Cremer and Tyler, 2007) When participants feel that they contributed to the final decision, they are more likely to cooperate with that decision than just trusting an arbitrary authorities’ decision. This relationship between a participant’s voice in a decision is reinforced in a small study by researchers Van Prooijen, Van Den Bos, and Wilke. The reasoning built upon by said researchers proposed that the more authority listened to participants, the more participants would feel that the outcome would meet their approval from the sense of feedback and respect afforded to participants. (Van Prooijen, Van Den Bos, and Wilke, 2007). Neither previous studies on cooperation examined the approval of authorities actions based on the possible similarity between authorities’ actions and the participant’s own idea of an action. Participant’s proposed actions to a situation could be an interfering variable in studying approval in authorities’ actions unless controlled for with adequately vague questions on the actual actions of authority or else studied independently. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 6 Traits Affecting Trust Two factors should be covered about the view on authority, the authorities’ charisma and the viewpoint’s association within authority. (Fox, Payne, Priest, and Philliber, 1977) Four researchers in 1977 investigated the relationship between approval in an authorities’ actions based on the placement those individuals had within the authority structure within four classes; high command, lower command, worker, and obey class. Their results showed a difference between the approval and cooperation between the highest command and the lowest obey class. The difference in approval was not statistically significant though the obey class was significantly more likely to cooperate with authority than the command class. (Fox, 1977) This clarifies two points; approval and class do not necessarily impact cooperation. In relating this information with previous ideas on authority, it could be that those with lower authority have a more acute response to the power held by those with more authority in negotiating cooperation. A serious implication of this is that those with greater authority are less likely to respect the authority of others such as lawmakers disregarding the laws regulating them. This will be important to remember as we look closer at the tendency to disregard or protest authority. Charisma is a being used as a catch all term for those personality traits attributed to authority which may have some impact on the approval and cooperation with authority. Since these traits, being largly subjective, are mostly attributed to the authority by an observer or participant, there is little real world control over these traits by the authority. De Cremer and den Ouden examined passion as an element of charisma in affecting the perception of procedural fairness. They hypothesized that authorities displaying more passion would impart the impression of being fairer in their dealings. They found that passion had a positive effect on perceived fairness but could also display negative emotions like anger that diminished the TRUST IN AUTHORITY 7 positive impact. (De Cremer and den Ouden, 2009) Compared with authority factors like procedural fairness, trust, association, and power, charismatic factors like passion are more subjective and unaccountable within research. Ruling out factors in Cooperation After considering some of the factors involved in trusting and cooperating with authority, it is time to address the real bones of the issue. Why do individuals protest and reject authority? The answer is partly the interplay between trust and the authorities’ power. (van Dijke, De Cremer, and Mayer, 2010) Cooperation is influenced by the exposure to authority individuals have. (Elliott, Thomas, and Ogloff, 2011) The participant’s voice or sense of influencing an authorities’ decision improves the trust in the outcome. (Van Prooijen, Van Den Bos, and Wilke, 2007) Now these studies have shown some of the mechanisms affecting why people approve and cooperate with authority but not necessarily behind rejecting or protesting authority. Few studies actually go out to find information on the nature of disobedience though a few famous ones like the Milgram and Stanford prison experiments have shown the shocking impact of authority. Following in such infamous footsteps, a lesser known study conducted by Bocchiaro and Zimbardo have participants give increasingly damaging verbal assaults to an actor for failures to learn. Deliberately recording and cataloging outward signs of stress, they found that nine participants who did not stop the verbal assault until the end experienced equivalent stress as the twenty one who stopped at the eleventh question. (Bocchiaro, and Zimbardo, 2010) As the participants displayed equivalent amounts of stress at the same time during the experiment after they split off, it seems like stress has little to do with whether someone follows an authority figure. This counters the common thought that people defy authority because it causes them greater stress than those who would continue to follow even unpleasant orders. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 8 Individual personality traits cannot be ignored when considering responses to authority as there is a scale of behaviors in responding to authority from criminal acts, lawful protests, verbal objections, silent disagreement, and even cooperation. Murphy’s article published in the Springer Science+Business Media Journal addresses personality traits between those that respond differently to authority, a quality called affect intensity. Murphy used two surveys comprising hundreds of Australian tax offenders in measuring their emotional responses to their brush with the Australian tax authorities and their later cooperation with tax laws. The researcher did reconfirm that perceived procedural fairness lessened negative emotions such as anger and frustration which were among those used to measure affect intensity. Fairness was more effective in reducing anger in low affect intensive individuals than with higher intensity against authority. According to the self reported survey, the lower affect intensity individual was barely past statistical significance was more likely to cooperate with authority when treated fairly. (Murphy, 2009) While targeting the response to authority, this survey measured through self reporting on an act of authority that is very not controversial to the majority of the population. Further study should use examples of authority’s actions which are more polarizing to help define the distinctions in response from approval to complete resistance. Of particular importance will be reviewing whether fairness still has the effect of increasing cooperation during controversial actions of authority. Controversy Controversial actions of authority are most evident during times of protest when authority’s actions are already being questioned. Moments in history such as during the 1968 democratic convention, the Edmund Pettus Bridge in 1965, and modern controversies surrounding government responses to the Occupy Wall Street movement call for the need to TRUST IN AUTHORITY 9 examine the effect of heavy handed policing in encouraging approval, cooperation and trust. Past protests were controversial in their day and current protests have provided controversial examples of authority to use as well. Supposed police overreaction in the raids on Zuccotti Park, ignoring federal judges protecting protestors’ rights, incarcerating members of the press and city council, enforcing inflated bail on LA protestor’s misdemeanors, excessive use of stun grenades on crowds at Oakland, and multiple account of pepper spraying non violent individuals comprise a bulk of possible examples for an authority reaction survey geared to measure the approval of such actions. Conclusions The study of responses to authority is a critical field considering the implications of obedience and protests. Actions of authorities have shown to effect approval and possible future cooperation through the fairness and the views of their actions. Authorities’ passion and individual’s affect intensity have shown to have only a small impact on trust while authorities’ power and fairness have greater impact. Of particular interest is the controversial level of response when authority figures began to lose approval and cooperation for their action in responding to current displays of disapproval and noncooperation. Applicable conclusions could be drawn to include authority relationships of parents, employers, police, and even nation leaders. Mapping the characteristics of trustworthy responses could improve authorities’ retention of approval and cooperation while still responding to the presence of discontent. Understanding more of how individuals and collective groups respond to acts of authority will further the justification and survival of authority. Acts seen as overly aggressive towards protestors will only hinder the approval, cooperation and trust bestowed on authority. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 10 Method Participants Twenty or more participants will fill eight groups to test the three, two dimensional, variables of feedback, power, and the fairness of the authority. At minimum, the study will include one hundred and sixty adult participants who respond to the ad for paid participation in a product research group. Respondents will be divided into groups randomly except for even distributions of gender and age. Materials This study will require tricking participants into playing a simple real time strategy simulation by speaking into a microphone. The fake strategy game needs only simple graphics since participants will be told that the research is centered on voice recognition and commands, not the participants’ actions. After the fake game, participants will take a survey on their experience seeded with questions on their trust of their “commander”. During testing, participants will be located in a plain room with only the computer monitor and headset equipped with a microphone. The ‘game’ will actually be controlled from a computer in another room by a researcher. A waiting area is needed to greet participants and to act out deciding a commander. One resurcher will also need a pair of weighted dice that land on either red or blue and a false button connected to the microphone headset. Procedure Applicants will enter the experiment one at a time but will encounter another person in the waiting area which they will be lead to believe is another participant, but who is actually a researcher. The two of them will be told the purpose of the experiment is to test out new voice command software on a basic test game platform by an authoritative researcher in a lab coat. Up TRUST IN AUTHORITY 11 to this point, all participants will be given the same cover story and introduced to the same researcher pretending to be a fellow participant. The first difference between groups begins on how the hidden researcher becomes the ‘commander’. To control the fairness of the commander’s authority, the commander will be chosen arbitrarily over half the participants and seemingly randomly in the other half. In reality, the question of commander is never in doubt as the participants will always be turned down. At this point, the participant is separated from the commander as they are lead to separate rooms depositing the participant in the game monitor room with the microphone headset. Half of the commanders will seemingly be selected at random by having both commander and participant select either red or blue and then having a the researcher pull out a single set of dice with three sides red and three sides blue. The researcher will actually have a pair of dice pocketed and pull out the one weighted for the color the commander chose. The other half of participants will see the researcher greet the commander with some level of familiarity from some unrelated acquaintance and later selected them with bias for the role of greater responsibility over the participant. When alone with the researcher in the game room, all participants will be told that they are to command their forces in the game by speaking normally into the headset’s microphone and that the commander will give orders through the headset. Everyone will be told that they are free to play however they want by following or ignoring any of the commander’s orders. While in reality, participants will get paid the same amount for their participation no matter what, half of the participants will be told that the commander will be able to deduct some of the participant’s pay for any reason, increasing the perceived power of the commander’s authority. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 12 The other half will be reminded that they will be paid the same no matter how they play, decreasing the perception of the commander’s authority. The final variable between groups is the availability of participants to talk back to the commander. Half of the groups will have a button which they will press to speak to the commander during the game. Despite the ability to speak with the commander, they will not change the commander’s orders. The other half will not be able to speak back to the commander. The researcher will leave the room while the fake game plays out as the participant decides how much or little they cooperate with the commander’s instructions. The commander will make the same orders in all groups while the participant instructs the game through the microphone. The participant’s cooperation will be measured from how often and well the commander’s orders are followed. To recap on variables, there are three conditions which are different between the eight groups. Fairness of the authorities’ position will be modified by having participants perceive either a random or biased selection of the role of commander. The power of the authority is changed from the control the commander is said to have over the participant’s reward for participating in the study. The perceived amount of feedback will be from the ability to talk to the authority even though it will not change the demands authority places on the participant. Cooperation will be measured as how often participants follow the authority’s orders. Participants’ trust in the authority will be measured from survey questions hidden in a performance questionnaire on their experience with the voice commands. Results A strong positive correlation was found between the measures used to determine trust in the commander and the participant’s performance in cooperating with the authority’s commands, TRUST IN AUTHORITY 13 p~.8, s=.001. Among the eight groups, the one that had the authority chosen with bias, had power over the participant and deaf to the participant’s feedback produced the least cooperation and trust with an estimated range of 10-20%. The group that perceived the authority having been chosen fairly, was open to feedback, and had less power over participant received the most cooperation and reported trustworthiness at an estimated range of 80-90%. There were significant correlations from all variables affecting power, fairness, and feedback with the measures of trust and cooperation ranging from .23 to .85. When the data is put through a scatterplot, the points resemble four groups set on a rising slope with the highest ranked group on trust and cooperation being the one with feedback, fairness, and less power. The two larger groups are in line with the extreme low end of the spectrum and describe the three groups that had two of the qualities of the most trustworthy group and three of the groups that only shared one quality with the most trustworthy group. The scatterplot backs up the strong correlations seen between variables and measures. Discussion Authorities that are arbitrary, deaf to feedback, and powerful are more likely to be viewed as less trustworthy and inspire less cooperation than those which seem fairer, listen to feedback, and are less powerful. The data supported this hypothesis through a strong correlation between trust, cooperation, and the factors of power, feedback, and fairness. Participants were most trusting and cooperative when authority was responsive to feedback, chosen fairly, and had less direct power over the participant. This means that authority is least trusted and followed when it becomes too unfair, deaf to protests, and powerful. An important thing to note is that the three factors were more influential together than they were separately; meaning that a trustworthy authority is more likely to have two or more TRUST IN AUTHORITY 14 positive qualities than just one or none. It is also possible that extreme cases of one factor, such as power, may be enough to overcome the effects of the other two qualities. For example, protestors may feel so threatened by the active power of riot police that they forget that they have been treated fairly and listened to beforehand. Having identified these qualities of authority, it will be important to map them more closely in further research. TRUST IN AUTHORITY 15 References Bocchiaro, P., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2010). Defying unjust authority: An exploratory study. Current Psychology: Research & Reviews, 29(2), 155-170. doi:10.1007/s12144-0109080-z De Cremer, D., & den Ouden, N. (2009). 'When passion breeds justice': Procedural fairness effects as a function of authority's passion. European Journal Of Social Psychology, 39(3), 384-400. doi:10.1002/ejsp.529 De Cremer, D., & Tyler, T. R. (2007). The effects of trust in authority and procedural fairness on cooperation. Journal Of Applied Psychology, 92(3), 639-649. doi:10.1037/00219010.92.3.639 Elliott, I., Thomas, S. M., & Ogloff, J. P. (2011). Procedural justice in contacts with the police: Testing a relational model of authority in a mixed methods study. Psychology, Public Policy, And Law, 17(4), 592-610. doi:10.1037/a0024212 Fox, W. S., Payne, D. E., Priest, T. B., & Philliber, W. W. (1977). Authority position, legitimacy of authority structure, and acquiescence to authority. Social Forces, 55(4), 966-973. doi:10.2307/2577566 Murphy, K. (2009). Procedural justice and affect intensity: Understanding reactions to regulatory authorities. Social Justice Research, 22(1), 1-30. doi:10.1007/s11211-008-0086-8 van Dijke, M., De Cremer, D., & Mayer, D. M. (2010). The role of authority power in explaining procedural fairness effects. Journal Of Applied Psychology, 95(3), 488-502. doi:10.1037/a0018921 TRUST IN AUTHORITY Van Prooijen, J., Van Den Bos, K., & Wilke, H. M. (2007). Procedural justice in authority relations: The strength of outcome dependence influences people's reactions to voice. European Journal Of Social Psychology, 37(6), 1286-1297. doi:10.1002/ejsp.435 16