Running Head: LITERATURE REVIEW: EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY Literature Review: Relations between Caregivers’ and Children’s Empathy, Emotion Recognition, and Anger Bias July 17, 2014 Jordan Barnada Stevie N. Grassetti, M.A. Julie A. Hubbard, Ph.D. University of Delaware Psychology Department EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW 1 Literature Review: Relations between Caregivers’ and Children’s Empathy, Emotion Recognition, and Anger Bias Empathy is defined as “…the capacity to experience the emotions of another and/or…the capacity to comprehend the emotions of another” (Jolliffe & Farrington, 2006, p. 589). The ability to recognize emotions in others may be an important component of empathy. To date, though, little is known about how children develop skills needed for emotion recognition and empathy or whether emotion recognition skills and empathy are both conceptually and empirically related constructs. The overarching aim of the current study was to take a first step toward addressing these gaps by examining concurrent measures of empathy and emotion recognition in caregivers and their children. More specifically, the first goal was to examine relations between caregivers’ and children’s emotion recognition and empathic abilities to explore the possibility that these skills are intergenerationally transmitted. The second goal was to investigate whether caregivers and children show similar biases when they make emotion recognition errors, specifically if they are similar in their tendency to be biased toward perceiving anger and/or other hostile emotions. Finally, the third goal was to explore whether emotion recognition skills, emotion recognition bias, and empathy for others are empirically related in both caregivers and children. Research on the association between caregivers’ and children’s emotion recognition skills is limited and equivocal. Many previous studies have found that children fare better emotionally and socially when their caregivers show clear emotional expression and when their caregivers can recognize their emotions (Dunsmore, Her, Halberstadt, & Perez-Rivera, 2009). De Los Reyes, Lerner, Thomas, Daruwala, and Goepel (2013) explored the implications of emotional recognition and reported a low non-significant correlation between caregiver and adolescent EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW 2 performance on an emotion recognition task (r = .021, p = 0.14). In addition, they found that the lower caregivers and their children score on emotion recognition tasks, the greater discrepancies and conflicts they have in their relationship. Research on relations between caregivers’ and children’s empathic abilities is also unclear. Van Lissa, Hawk, de Wied, Koot, and van Lier (2014) found that caregiver and adolescent perspective taking are related, but only between mothers and daughters, not mothers and sons. Moreover, Farrant, Devine, Maybery, and Fletcher (2004) found an indirect relationship between caregiver and child empathy such that high levels of maternal cognitive empathy were associated with child cognitive empathy skills but only because mothers high in maternal cognitive empathy also engage in positive parenting in which they encourage their children to take others’ perspectives. Paradoxically, three studies found very small, nonsignificant correlations and therefore no evidence of intergenerational transmission of empathic abilities and perspective taking (Oguz & Akyol, 2008 (p values ranged from -.16 to .19); Strayer & Roberts, 1989 (r = -.20 for mothers, r = <.15 for fathers); 2004 (r = .07 for mothers, r = -.13 to .12 for father). They concluded children’s empathy is not directly related to caregivers’ empathy in that it may be only one of many factors that influence empathy in children (Strayer & Roberts, 1989). Due to such inconsistent findings, the question of whether empathy is intergenerationally transmitted remains unclear. In regards to the second goal of the current study, there is no research on whether caregivers and children show similar biases in their emotion recognition errors. However, a literature does exist on the intergenerational transmission of hostile attributional biases, and this literature may be relevant to the question at hand. Hostile attributional bias is when people interpret ambiguous situations or stimuli as angry/hostile without information that would EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW 3 uncover the true intentions of the situation or stimuli (Dodge, 1980). Overall, the literature is equivocal about whether hostile attribution biases are intergenerationally transmitted. Three existing studies support intergenerational transmission. Specifically, Werner (2012) and Nelson, Mitchell, and Yang (2008) found that mothers who tend to make hostile attributions also had children who displayed hostile attribution biases with their peers, although a similar correlation was not found between fathers and their children. A gender difference also emerged in a study by MacBrayer, Milich, and Hundley (2003) in which hostile attributions were intergenerationally transmitted between mothers and daughters but not mothers and sons. However, Halligan, Cooper, Healy, and Murray (2007) found no significant correlations between caregivers’ and children’s hostile attribution biases, suggesting that more research needs to be done on both the intergenerational transmission of this construct and on the broader construct of biases in emotion recognition errors. Our third goal is to examine if emotion recognition, anger bias, and empathy are related constructs in both children and caregivers. For children, previous literature has found correlations between emotion recognition and empathy. For example, Carr and Lutjemeier (2005) found a moderate positive relationship between the constructs in that children who could accurately recognize fearful emotions via facial expressions also demonstrated higher empathic abilities. Belacchi and Farina (2012) also found a correlation between empathy and emotion comprehension in children. They concluded that “…emotion recognition is a prerequisite for empathy”, and that the better children are at perspective taking, the better they are at understanding emotions (Belacchi & Farina, 2012). In regards to adults, the literature is sparser, with much of the focus of emotion recognition and empathy on their roles in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Golan, Baron-Cohen, Hill, and Golan (2006) found that adults with Autism Spectrum EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW 4 Disorder have trouble with both recognizing emotions and the ability to empathize, which they propose shows that the two constructs are related. More generally, Konrath, Corneille, Bushman, and Luminet (2014) state that emotion recognition and empathy must be related because individuals need to be able to read others’ emotions in order to empathize. In their study, they found that the more empathic an adult was, the better he or she was at recognizing emotions (Konrath, et al., 2014). In the current study, examining caregivers’ and children’s self-reports of empathy and performances on an emotion recognition task may help to us further understand whether emotion recognition and empathy are related constructs. To recap, the first goal of the current study was to examine relations between caregivers’ and children’s emotion recognition and empathic abilities to explore the possibility that these skills are intergenerationally transmitted. We hypothesized significant positive relations between caregivers’ and children’s emotion recognition and empathy. The second goal was to investigate whether caregivers and children show similar biases toward mis-identifying other emotions as anger. We predicted significant positive relations between caregivers’ and children’s anger biases. Finally, the third goal was to explore whether emotion recognition skills, anger bias, and empathy for others are related in both caregivers and children. Based on the literature, we predicted that emotion recognition skills and empathy would be positively correlated with each other in both caregivers and children while those two constructs would be negatively correlated with anger bias. The current study is marked by a number of noteworthy strengths. As reviewed above, much of the literature in this area is weak and equivocal, and the investigation is designed to clarify findings in several ways. First, the study includes measures of emotion recognition and empathy in both caregivers and children in one study, allowing for a thorough examination of the EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW relations between these constructs within both child samples and adult samples, as well as an exploration of the intergenerational transmission of emotion recognition and empathy. Second, the investigation assesses emotion recognition through a well-validated performance-based task (the Reading the Eyes of the Mind task). Finally, this study is the first to include a measure of anger bias alongside measures of emotion recognition and empathy in both caregivers and children, allowing for an exploration of the intergenerational transmission of this bias as well as an examination of how this bias relates to caregivers’ and children’s levels of empathy. 5 EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW 6 References Belacchi, C. & Farina, E. (2012). Feeling and thinking of others: Affective and cognitive empathy and emotion comprehension in prosocial/hostile preschoolers. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 150-165. Carr, M. B. & Lutjemeier, J. A. (2005). The relation of facial affect recognition and empathy to delinquency in youth offenders. Adolescence, 40, 601-619. De Los Reyes, A., Lerner, M. D., Thomas, S. A., Daruwala, S., & Goepel, K. (2013). Discrepancies between parent and adolescent beliefs about daily life topics and performance on an emotion recognition task. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 971-982. Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162-170. Dunsmore, J. C., Her, P., Halberstadt, A. G., & Perez-Rivera, M. B. (2009). Parents’ beliefs about emotions and children’s recognition of parents’ emotions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 121-140. Farrant, B. M., Devine, T. A. J., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2011). Empathy, perspective taking, and prosocial behaviour: The importance of parenting practices. Infant and Child Development, 21, 175-188. Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., & Golan, Y. (2006). The “Reading the Mind in the Films” task: Complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Social Neuroscience, 1, 111-123. Halligan, S. L., Cooper, P. J., Healy, S. J., & Murray, L. (2007). The attribution of hostile intent in mothers, fathers, and their children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 594- EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW 7 604. Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2006). Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 589-611. Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2006). Examining the relationship between low empathy and bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 540-550. Konrath, S., Corneille, O., Bushman, B. J., & Luminet, O. (2014). The relationship between narcissistic exploitativeness, dispositional empathy, and emotion recognition abilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38, 129-143. MacBrayer, E. K., Milich, R., & Hundley, M. (2003). Attributional biases in aggressive children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 698-708. Nelson, D. A., Mitchell, C., & Yang, C. (2008). Intent attributions and aggression: A study of children and their parents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 793-806. Oguz, V. & Akyol, A. K. (2008). Perspective-taking skills of 6-year old children: Preschool attendance and mothers’ and fathers’ education and empathic skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107, 481-493. Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1989). Children’s empathy and role taking: Child and parental factors, and relations to prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 227–239. Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (2004). Children’s anger, emotional expressiveness, and empathy: Relations with parents’ empathy, emotional expressiveness, and parenting practices. Social Development, 13, 229-254. Van Lissa, C. J., Hawk, S. T., de Wied, M., Koot, H. M., & van Lier, P. (2014). The longitudinal interplay of affective and cognitive empathy within and between adolescents and EMOTION RECOGNITION AND EMPATHY: LITERATURE REVIEW mothers. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1219-1225. Werner, N. E. (2012). Do hostile attribution biases in children and parents predict relationally aggressive behavior? The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 173, 221-245. 8