Electronic supplementary material Figure ESM 1. Phylogenetic tree of the 135 species of non-human social primate in our sample. Figure ESM 2. Graphical representation of Bonferroni corrected (significance value set to p< 0.00833), univariate PGLS analyses to assess the effect of several socio-ecological variables on home range overlap across non-human social primates. Figure ESM 3. Since in this final analysis we only consider a subset of all species in our sample (those living in mM-mF groups), one could argue we first need to re-establish that the ecological variables (Activity period, Substrate use, Habitat type and Diet) omitted from follow-up analyses on the whole dataset, can also justly be left out from this final test. Bonferroni corrected (significance value set to p< 0.0125), univariate PGLS analyses show that none of these variables significantly affects home range overlap in non-human social primates that live in mM-mF groups, thereby justifying their omission. Table ESM 1. Parameter estimates (B) and significance values as obtained from a PGLS analysis that maximized sample size (n species= 126) by considering only those socio-ecological variables of home range overlap that were found to be significant predictors in previous multi- and univariate PGLS analyses. Variable B SE t-value p Intercept 0.194 0.25 0.78 0.4396 Group size 0.201 0.04 4.67 <0.0001 Defendability -0.124 0.05 -2.44 0.0160 λML= 0.728; R2Adj.= 0.196, F(3,123)= 16.19, p< 0.0001 Table ESM 2. Both the number of males (a, n species= 125), and females (b, n species= 124) in a species’ modal grouping pattern were significantly and positively associated with home range overlap. This provided a first suggestion that collective action problems may arise in either sex. Parameter estimates (B) and significance values as obtained from PGLS analyses. a) Variable Intercept n Males Controlling for: Defendability B SE 0.524 0.26 0.019 0.01 t-value 2.03 3.40 p 0.0445 0.0009 -0.143 0.05 -2.72 0.0074 λML= 0.764; R2Adj.= 0.136, F(3, 122)= 10.76, p< 0.0001 b) Variable Intercept n Females Controlling for: Defendability B SE 0.426 0.25 0.194 0.04 -0.127 0.05 t-value p 1.72 0.0874 4.58 <0.0001 -2.46 0.0155 λML= 0.751; R2Adj.= 0.190, F(3, 121)= 15.41, p< 0.0001 Table ESM 3. Post-hoc PGLS pair-wise comparisons between species living in sM-sF groups (in which a collective action problem among same-sex individuals cannot occur) and mM-mF species characterized by dispersal and philopatry of the larger sex. The analysis shows that only mM-mF species in which the larger sex disperses suffer from increased home range overlap, due to a collective action problem. Parameter estimates (B) and significance values as obtained from PGLS analyses. Multiple comparisons (nspecies= 79) B Intercept 0.479 Social structure: sM-sF mM-mF Dispersal larger sex 0.469 mM-mF Philopatry larger sex 0.082 Controlling for: Defendability -0.067 SE t-value p 0.07 6.84 <0.0001 0.08 0.12 0.06 5.62 <0.0001 0.67 0.5021 -1.13 0.2632 λML= 0.000; R2Adj.= 0.339, F(4, 75)= 14.36, p< 0.0001 Note to Table ESM 4 On the biological validity and practical utility of defendability indices Originally conceived as a quantitative measure of the economic defendability [1] of a ranging area, the Mitani-Rodman D-index was initially used to distinguish between territorial and non-territorial primate species [2, 3]. Based on the assumption that economic defendability in non-volant species depends on an animal’s (or group’s) ability to monitor the boundaries of its range in order to detect potential intruders, the D-index is calculated as the ratio of the average daily path length and the diameter of an (idealised) ranging area: D= d/(4A/π)0.5 , where D is the defendability index, d the average daily path length (km), and (4A/π)0.5 the diameter of a circle with a surface area A (km2) equal to that of the home range. As such, the D-index expresses the number of times an agent (a solitary animal or group) can move across its range within a day, where high values imply the possibility of frequent contact with boundaries at widely dispersed points on the perimeter, and low values indicate that effective defence will be difficult as distant points at the boundary cannot be monitored regularly [2]. In their original study, Mitani and Rodman empirically established that territorial primate species invariably exhibit D-index values ≥ 1.0, and thus that the defence of an area only becomes economically feasible if it can be crossed at least once a day. However, the biological validity of the D-index is not limited to merely making this potentially misleading- dichotomy between territoriality and non-territoriality. Instead, it is far more informative (and true to biological reality) to regard defendability as a continuous variable along an axis of “the monopolizability of space”. Indeed, the extent to which a range can be defended is modelled by the D-index as a monotonically increasing function of the agent’s mobility in relation to its range size requirements. In more recent studies then, the D-index has also successfully been used to e.g. characterise seasonal variation in home range defendability within populations [4, 5], and even as a measure of space use intensity to predict animal parasite loads [6]. The biological validity and utility of the D-index as a continuous measure of economic defendability are thus wellestablished by multiple empirical studies. As an extension of the original defendability index, the Lowen-Dunbar M-index [3] was defined to express economic defendability as a function of the frequency of boundary collisions (similar to the D-index, although now defendability decreases quadratically with range size, rather than linearly), as well as the length of the boundary that needs to be monitored to obtain effective defence against intruders. In addition, the M-index allows for multiple, independently moving foraging parties, as might occur in species with high fission-fusion dynamics. It is defined as: M= N(sv/d2) , where M is the defendability index (or “fractional Monitoring rate”), N the number of independently moving foraging parties, s the detection distance or length of the home range boundary that can be monitored during each visit (km), v the daily path length (km), and d2 the squared diameter of a circle with surface area equal to that of the home range (km2). Thus defined, economic defendability scales with the proportion of the boundary that each independent sub-group can effectively monitor every time it reaches the border of the home range. Both the D- and M-index, therefore, reflect a monotonically increasing propensity to defend an area as it becomes easier to patrol. Lowen and Dunbar, moreover, pointed out that both indices are strongly rooted in the kinetic theory of ideal gases [3]. A recent and independent review on the use of ideal gas models to analyse animal encounters concluded that this framework continues to provide a useful approximation of biological reality [7]. Although the M-index has some theoretically desirable properties over the D-index, the two indices are conceptually very similar and were indeed highly interrelated in the original Lowen and Dunbar study (log-transformed values from Tables 2 and 3 were strongly correlated [rPearson= 0.886, nspecies= 49, p< 0.0001]). Unfortunately, however, the two additional parameters required by the Mindex (N and s) have never explicitly been quantified for any primate species. Moreover, the implicit assumptions that foraging parties move truly independently, and are equally capable of effective range defence (not all sub-groups may be sufficiently large or contain members of the appropriate age-sex class to keep intruders out) severely undermine the practical value of the M-index. Despite its theoretical refinements then, in practice the M-index may not really provide an improved estimate of economic defendability, as it is simply not possible yet to correctly parameterise it based on empirical data. This stands in contrast to the proven usefulness and analytical power of the readily calculated D-index. For this reason, we based our analyses and conclusions in the main body of our paper on the D-index, rather than the M-index. As we show below, however, our results and conclusions are very robust against the choice of the measure of economic defendability used. In tables ESM 4.1 - 4.3, we offer results from the analyses presented in Tables 1 - 3, after substituting the D-index by a “biologically most plausible guess” at the value of the M-index for each species in our sample. In a first trial, we parameterised the M-index as suggested in the original paper [3], assuming a value of s= 0.05, and N=1 (as even in fission-fusion taxa, effective defence is only possible if a sufficiently large number of individuals of the appropriate age-sex class are within a single party). A strong correlation existed within our dataset between log-transformed values of the D-index and this variant of the M-index (rPearson= 0.891, nspecies= 126, p< 0.0001). Parameter estimates and associated standard errors were virtually identical in all analyses, and predictor variables identified as significant were always the same as in our original analyses. In a second trial, we attempted to add more biological realism to the M-index by doubling the value of s for species living in open habitats as well as those with loud inter-group vocalisations [primary source: 8] and, in addition, doubling the value of N for species with high fission-fusion dynamics. Again, the correlation between log-transformed D- and M-index values was very high (rPearson= 0.898, nspecies= 126, p< 0.0001). Results from these analyses are presented in Tables ESM 4.1 - 4.3, and are fully in line with the original analyses and conclusions. No noticeable improvements in model performance were detected (ΔAIC’s with the original model were always less than 2). We conclude that, regardless of the index used to model economic defendability, social variables always accounted for a significant (and previously ignored) proportion of the variation in home range overlap across the primate taxon. Thus, our findings provide evidence for the interpretation that primate territory economics are strongly affected by a collective action problem, more so than by range defendability. This implies that non-human social primates perform suboptimal in a cooperative task, as has also been described for human groups [9]. Literature cited 1. Brown J.L. 1964 The evolution of diversity in avian territorial systems. The Wilson Bulletin 76(2), 160-169. 2. Mitani J.C., Rodman P.S. 1979 Territoriality: The relation of ranging pattern and home range size to defendability, with an analysis of territoriality among primate species. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5(3), 241-251. 3. Lowen C., Dunbar R.I.M. 1994 Territory size and defendability in primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 35(5), 347-354. 4. Buzzard P.J. 2006 Ranging patterns in relation to seasonality and frugivory among Cercopithecus campbelli, C. petaurista, and C. diana in the Taï Forest. International Journal of Primatology 27(2), 559-573. (doi:10.1007/s10764-0069028-1). 5. Bartlett T.Q. 2009 Seasonal home range use and defendability in white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Khao Yai National Park, Thailand. In The Gibbons (eds. Lappan S., Whittaker D.), pp. 265-275, Springer Science+Business Media. 6. Nunn C.L., Dokey A.T.-W. 2006 Ranging patterns and parasitism in primates. Biology Letters 2(3), 351-354. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0485). 7. Hutchinson J.M.C., Waser P.M. 2007 Use, misuse and extensions of "ideal gas" models of animal encounter. Biological Reviews 82(3), 335-359. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00014.x). 8. Wich S.A., Nunn C.L. 2002 Do male "long-distance calls" function in mate defense? A comparative study of longdistance calls in primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52(6), 474-484. (doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0541-8). 9. Olson M. 1965 The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups. Cambridge, Mass., Harvard University Press. Table ESM 4 Outcome of PGLs analyses presented in the main body of our article in Tables 1, 2 and 3 respectively, using an alternative measure of economic defendability, the Lowen-Dunbar M-index (see introductory note for information on parameter values). All results and conclusions presented in the main body of our article are corroborated by the independent set of analyses presented here. ESM 4.1 Variable Intercept Group size Defendability Activity period Substrate use Habitat type Diet B 0.031 0.207 -0.070 -0.045 -0.088 0.058 -0.061 SE t-value p 0.27 0.12 0.9066 0.05 3.93 <0.0001 0.03 -2.43 0.0167 0.18 -0.25 0.8059 0.11 -0.77 0.4416 0.15 0.38 0.7042 0.13 -0.49 0.6282 λML= 0.544; R2Adj.= 0.212, F(7, 109)= 6.16, p< 0.0001 ESM 4.2 a) Variable B SE t-value p Intercept 0.333 0.23 1.47 0.1449 Social structure: sM-sF sM-mF 0.065 0.10 0.62 0.5338 mM-sF -0.125 0.14 -0.90 0.3718 mM-mF 0.258 0.10 2.64 0.0095 Controlling for: Defendability -0.087 0.03 -3.37 0.0010 λML= 0.688; R2Adj.= 0.175, F(5, 121)= 7.63, p< 0.0001 b) Variable B SE t-value p Intercept 0.321 0.24 1.31 0.1916 ID larger sex: Male Female -0.070 0.09 -0.76 0.4464 n larger sex: One Multiple 0.171 0.07 2.50 0.0139 Controlling for: Defendability -0.078 0.03 -2.77 0.0065 λML= 0.702; R2Adj.= 0.125, F(4, 108)= 5.49, p< 0.0005 ESM 4.3 Variable B SE t-value p Intercept 0.817 0.10 8.05 <0.0001 Dispersal pattern: Dispersal Philopatry -0.377 0.12 -3.05 0.0038 Controlling for: Defendability -0.051 0.04 -1.40 0.1695 λML= 0.000; R2Adj.= 0.171, F(3, 46)= 5.94, p< 0.005