Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica:Volume 40(9)October 1996pp 1073-1086 Awareness in anaesthesia: Incidence, consequences and prevention [Review Article] Heier, T.; Steen, P. A. Department of Anesthesiology, Ullevaal University Hospital, Oslo, Norway. Address: Tom Heier MD, PhD; Department of Anesthesiology; Ullevaal University Hospital; N-0407 Oslo, Norway Awareness has been a concern since the first time ether anaesthesia was demonstrated by William Morton in 1846. The patient was reported to be half-awake during the operation, and later expressed that he had experienced pain. In 1847 a female patient was reported to be aware of the instrument movements in the surgeon's hands (1), and in the early 1900s the surgeon George Washington Crile called attention to the fact that the anaesthetized brain was not unresponsive. "It may be an unpleasant thought that although our patient is unconscious from general anaesthesia, the nerve impulses inaugurated by operative injury of the nerves reach the brain, making a reactive opposition to the surgeon, an attempt to get off the table and escape from the injuries the surgeon is inflicting." (2) With the use of curare in the 1940s "the signs of anaesthesia" disappeared, and many were reluctant to use the drug because of the possibility of having a completely paralysed, but conscious patient. "Care must therefore be taken to deaden sensation and ensure unconsciousness" (3). In 1959 Cheek demonstrated by use of hypnosis that patients under the influence of general anaesthetics may retain intraoperative auditory information (4). The possibility of awareness during anaesthesia and surgery is still a major concern (5-22). Depth of anaesthesia vs. awareness The original descriptions of anaesthesia were based on observed physiological responses to increasing doses of inhaled anaesthetics (23-25). Prys-Roberts defined pain as conscious perception of noxious stimuli, and anaesthesia as a state in which the patient neither perceives nor recalls such stimuli, and thus does not experience pain (26). In 1993 Kissin refined the definition of anaesthesia as consisting of several different components: anxiolysis, analgesia, hypnosis, muscle relaxation, and suppression of somatic (motor) and autonomic (haemodynamic, sudomotor, hormonal) responses to noxious stimulation (27). These various endpoints should not be regarded as a result of one single anaesthetic action (27). Studies both on intravenous and inhaled anaesthetics suggest that they represent different pharmacological actions, with different underlying mechanisms of action. This is most obvious with opioid drugs. The morphine and fentanyl potency (expressed as ED50, the concentration eliminating the measured response in 50% of the animals) ratios in rats between blockade of the response to noxious stimulation and loss of the righting reflex (measure of consciousness in animal studies) are different, which is not consistent with a unitary nonspecific mechanism of anaesthetic action (28). Additionally, in rats, both reserpine and thiopental antagonize the effect of opioids on the movement response to noxious stimulation, while a synergistic effect exists on the righting reflex (29, 30). For thiopental, etomidate, and diazepam the dose-response curves for loss of righting reflex, blockade of noxious stimulationinduced movement response, and suppression of cardic acceleration response to noxious stimulation vary ( 31), again suggesting different mechanisms of action for different components of anaesthesia. Similarly, in mice, the ratio of ED50 for movement response to tail-clamp (MAC) to ED50 for the righting reflex varies between different inhaled anaesthetics (32). In patients, MAC was originally defined as the minimal alveolar concentration of an inhaled anaesthetic required to prevent purposeful movement of head or extremities upon surgical skin incision in 50% of patients in the near steady-state situation (MAC-incision) (33). In a recent clinical study the concentration of isoflurane needed to prevent voluntary response to verbal commands as a fraction of MAC was significantly less than for nitrous oxide (0.38% MAC vs. 0.64 MAC) (34). Although this difference may reflect the stimulatory effect of nitrous oxide on the sympathetic nervous system (35-37), multiple mechanisms of action for different end-points of anaesthesia seem to be involved. This may not be apparent during everyday clinical anaesthesia because inhaled anaesthetics affect all components of anaesthesia more uniformly than opioids (38, 39). It is also suggested that different anaesthetics may induce the same anaesthetic end-point by different mechanisms of action (40-43). This view is supported by results obtained on isolated single-neuron preparations, where various inhaled anaesthetics had differential effects on the discharge activity (44). General anaesthetics are compounds with widely diverse chemical structures (45). The only physiochemical common characteristic is their lipid solubility. Traditionally, they were considered nonspecific, acting by perturbation of the lipid bilayer of the cell membrane in nerve cells (41). Consistent with the view that anaesthetics exert their effects by different mechanisms of action, new data suggest that these drugs act on sensitive proteins in the CNS (41). Unlike with local anaesthetics, impulse conductance in myelinated neuronal fibres is not affected by clinical doses of general anaesthetics, reinforcing the view that excitatory synaptic transmission is disrupted during anaesthesia (43, 46-48). Thin unmyelinated nerve fibres in CNS may, however, be as sensitive to inhaled anaesthetics as excitatory synapses (46, 48), and isoflurane induces hyperpolarization of neuronal cell body/dendrite, which may contribute to the depression of neuronal excitability ( 48, 49). Although the mechanisms of action of general anaesthetics are only partly known, recent data suggest that both cholinergic and amino acid-mediated neurotransmitter systems are involved (45, 50-53). Hypnosis consists of amnesia and unconsciousness (16) which should be considered different components of anaesthesia, as amnesia may be induced by drugs that do not affect consciousness significantly (54-56). In this context, amnesia can be defined as loss of learning and memory functions (12) which appear related to longterm potentiation (LTP), a phenomenon characterized by a long-lasting (days to weeks) postsynaptic response to a brief tetanic stimulation (57, 58). LTP is mostly studied in the hippocampus, which is associated with memory functions (58), and is related to stimulation of the NMDA glutamate postsynaptic receptor (50, 59, 60), which stimulates the production of nitric oxide. It is speculated that inhibition of nitric oxide production creates anaesthesia (51, 52). Ketamine impairs LTP by blocking the NMDA receptor (60), in contrast to other anaesthetics (volatile, barbiturates) which enhance inhibition (GABA as transmitter) of this excitatory synapse (50) either by reducing presynaptic transmitter release, or by postsynaptic hyperpolarization (48, 50). It is suggested that the state of consciousness is determined by neuronal activity in the brain stem reticular formation which connects with every part of the brain and spinal cord, and receives afferent impulses from all sensory modalities (61). General anaesthetics may induce unconsciousness by enhanced inhibition or suppression of excitatory responses of the reticular formation neurons evoked by somatosensory stimuli (62). One MAC halothane thus significantly reduces acetylcholine release in the medial pontine reticular formation, probably the primary region for normal sleep regulation. Direct application of acetylcholine in this part of the brain induces a REM sleep-like state in intact unanaesthetized animals (63), and brain stem mechanisms involved in the generation of naturally occurring sleep may play a key role when anaesthetics induce unconsciousness (64). No clear clinical end-point can serve as a basis for rational administration of drugs to achieve unconsciousness, and presently absence of recall of events is the only objective criterion for unconsciousness (16). Several studies suggest that a consciousness transition zone exists during the induction and emergence from anaesthesia (10, 14, 34, 65, 66). At low brain concentrations of anaesthetics the patient responds to verbal commands and has conscious (explicit) recall of the event. At higher concentrations the patient may remember the event if offered a cue (cued conscious recognition). At even higher concentrations the ability for explicit memory is lost, but the patient may still respond to auditory impulses such as verbal commands (wakefulness), or retain information as unconscious (implicit) memory, only revealed by psychologic testing and hypnosis (12, 67, 68). It is not known if memory of events during anaesthesia and surgery can be completely prevented. It is speculated that implicit memory, especially of auditory impulses, may take place independent of depth of anaesthesia (10, 69), since auditory impulses can be detected in the brain stem despite an isoelectric EEG ( 70). In the absence of surgical stimulation, however, isoflurane 0.2 MAC prevented explicit learning, and isoflurane (71) or desflurane 0.6 MAC (72) prevented both explicit and implicit learning in humans. The concentration required to eliminate memory functions in the presence of surgical stimuli is not known, but it is reasonable to believe that it is higher, as noxious stimuli may increase the level of consciousness (34, 73). Depth of anaesthesia depends on two antagonizing factors: the anaesthetic, which induces different anaesthesia components to a varying degree depending on the specific drug used; and the surgical stimulation, which may activate the sympathetic nervous system and increase the patient's level of consciousness and the somatic and autonomic reactivity (73, 74). Adequate depth of anaesthesia requires a sufficient amount of the agent to secure unconsciousness and other components of anaesthesia as needed for that particular surgical procedure, without jeopardizing vital organ functions. Ideally, depth of anaesthesia should be assessed in relationship to each anaesthetic component separately, tested by specific stimuli (16, 27). Based on the above discussion, awareness during general anaesthesia can be defined as a degree of consciousness, revealed by the occurrence of explicit or implicit memory of intraoperative events. Intraoperatively, eye opening or movements indicate impending awareness (12). During such an episode information may be stored temporarily into short-term memory (STM), and may or may not be stored permanently into long-term memory (LTM) (10). Both explicit and implicit memory are stored in LTM (10). Postoperatively, an interview will reveal explicit awareness. Detection of implicit awareness requires psychological testing, which is impractical during routine anaesthesia. Some patients have dreams during anaesthesia, implying a mental state in the transition zone between explicit and implicit awareness ( 12, 19). To ensure that the dreaming has not occurred postoperatively, the episode should be revealed shortly after conclusion of anaesthesia. Incidence of awareness There is a great variability in the reported incidence of awareness during anaesthesia and surgery, due to several reasons: 1. The incidence is dependent on the diagnostic criteria use to identify awareness. For explicit memory the incidence may be zero (75). If any movement response during surgery suggests impending awareness, a frequency as high as 100% may be observed (76). If dreaming suggests awareness, the frequency is in the range 0-60% (Table 1 and 2). Response to verbal commands during surgery (wakefulness) was observed in 70% of the patients in one study (77). Table 1. Incidence of awareness during anaesthesia for general surgery Table 2. Incidence of awareness during anaesthesia in special groups 2. Different combinations of drugs used in anaesthetic practice depress consciousness to a varying extent. 3. The need of the patients for anaesthetics may depend on the intensity of the noxious stimuli. 4. The pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of a particular anaesthetic agent vary between patients, leading to varying dose requirements to obtain the same level of unconsciousness. 5. Certain surgical procedures require light anaesthesia, increasing the risk of patient awareness. Incidence of conscious (explicit) awareness with different anaesthetics (Table 1) Inhaled anaesthetics When nitrous oxide 60-70% alone was used for maintenance of routine anaesthesia, a technique used in the 60s and 70s, the reported incidence of conscious awareness was 0-7% (78-84). In investigations where either volatile anaesthetics, opioids or ketamine are added to nitrous oxide 50-70% the incidence is <2% (85-89), although occasionally higher percentages may be reported from studies including only few individuals ( 90, 91). Recently, in a study of more than 3400 patients, Jordening and Pedersen found that the incidence was 0.2% for nitrous oxide 60-70% combined with fentanyl and droperidol in diazepam-premedicated patients (92). Similar results were obtained in a study of 1000 patients by Liu et al. in 1991, where unfortunately the anaesthetic techniques used were not described in detail (93). Only a few case reports are available of conscious awareness during routine anaesthesia with volatile agents administered in inspired doses >1% (92, 94-96). At least one of those was caused by a vaporizer leakage (96). These findings are consistent with studies showing that inhaled anaesthetics are relatively more potent than nitrous oxide on consciousness compared to pain suppression (34). Intravenous agents Awareness with propofol as a sole anaesthetic is described (97), but the dosage was not higher than that recommended in the presence of nitrous oxide (12 mg·kg-1·h-1 (98). No conscious awareness was reported when propofol as recommended by the manufacturer was administered in conjunction with alfentanil and topical application of lidocaine for laryngoscopy procedures in 15 patients (99). In a recent retrospective study of 1727 patients 0.3% experienced conscious awareness during total intravenous anaesthesia with propofol (612mg·kg-1·h-1) and alfentanil (40-80 µg·kg-1·h-1) (100). In 2 of those 5 cases less than the intended doses of propofol were administered inadvertently. Zero percent and 3% conscious awareness was observed in two studies including 11 and 32 female patients, respectively, undergoing breast or gynaecological surgery with midazolam (0.15mg·kg-1·h-1)and alfentanil (1 µg·kg-1·h-1) (77, 101). In a study of 24 orthopaedic patients 13% experienced conscious awareness when ketamine was administered as a sole anaesthetic ( 85). The frequency of conscious awareness was 8% during bronchoscopy in 130 patients with thiopental-oxygen-succinylcholine anaesthesia (102). Incidence of conscious (explicit) awareness in special groups (Table 2) Cardiac surgery There are several case reports of documented awareness during ketamine-diazepam (103), ketamine-nitrous oxide (104), high-dose opioid-oxygen (105, 106), and cyclopropane (107) anaesthesia. In a series of 30 patients, 23% had explicit awareness during high-dose fentanyl anaesthesia with or without halothane or nitrous oxide supplementation (108). Maunuksela (109) compared pethidine-nitrous oxide, droperidol-fentanyl and halothane anaesthesia, and observed an explicit awareness frequency of 5%, 13%, and 0%, respectively. In a large survey including 700 patients 1.1% experienced explicit awareness (110). The awareness episodes occurred mostly secondary to DC-shocks during cardiopulmonary bypass, or other painful procedures such as sternal split, aortic root dissection and electrocauterization (108, 109). This tendency to conscious awareness during cardiac surgery is not completely eliminated by adding an amnesic drug like diazepam (111). Caesarean section The pre-delivery part of caesarean section requires light anaesthesia. In one study, all 6 patients anaesthetized with 75% nitrous oxide in oxygen as the sole anaesthetic pre-delivery experienced conscious awareness (112). When pentothal 4mg·kg-1+ nitrous oxide 50-70% was used, the reported frequency of explicit awareness varies between 2 and 12% (113-115). When this anaesthetic regimen was supplemented by fentanyl 100 µg or morphine 0.2mg·key-1+ diazepam 0.1 mg·kg-1 postdelivery 1.5-8% experienced awareness (116, 117). Several authors report no conscious awareness when 0.5% halothane is added to 50% nitrous oxide after tracheal intubation in studies including 30 to 245 patients (75, 76, 113, 115), while 3% could recall intraoperative event when 1% enflurane was added (118). In 1991 Lyons and Macdonald reported an incidence of awareness of 1.3% and 0.4% when retrospectively evaluating 3000 caesarean section patients who received either thiopental 4mg·kg-1+50-70% nitrous oxide with halothane 0.5%(terminated postdelivery) or thiopental 6mg·kg 1 +50-70% nitrous oxide with isoflurane 1% until end of surgery (119). Trauma surgery Surgery for major trauma in haemodynamically unstable patients will frequently require light anaesthesia. Conscious awareness was experienced by 4 of 37 patients during emergency surgery with ketamine for induction and volatile anaesthetics for maintenance (120), and by 6 of 14 patients who received no anaesthetics for tracheal intubation and during the first approximately 20 min of surgery as they were unconscious/haemodynamically unstable on arrival (120). Incidence of unconscious (implicit) awareness during anaesthesia (Table 1 and 2) The use of recall as a measure of cognitive processing during anaesthesia has led to the conclusion that awareness is a rare event. This might be erroneous as it does not take into account that memories may be inaccessible for conscious recall (6). The frequency of 'dreams', which may indicate unconscious intraoperative awareness, is much higher than recall of intraoperative events. However, it is difficult postoperatively to ascertain that the episode really occurred during anaesthesia (19, 81). In nonobstetrical patients the reported incidence of dreaming during unsupplemented nitrous oxide anaesthesia is 3-57% (78, 81, 83, 84, 88). When either inhaled anaesthetics, opioids or ketamine are added to nitrous oxide the incidence varyies from 0 to 16% (81, 84, 85, 89, 91). Total intravenous anaesthesia (propofol or midazolam, in conjunction with alfentanil, administered as continuous infusions), is associated with a dreaming frequency of 0-9% (77, 99, 101). Eight of 24 (33%) orthopaedic patients anaesthetized with ketamine as a sole agent experienced dreaming (85). This is in contrast to an incidence of 0.9% in a recent study of 1000 nonobstetrical patients interviewed 20-36 hours postoperatively, where unfortunately the anaesthetic techniques were not specified (93). In obstetrical patients the reported incidence of dreaming was 31% during unsupplemented nitrous oxideoxygen anaesthesia in 150 patients (114), 4-11% when nitrous oxide 70% was supplemented with opioid and diazepam after delivery of the infant (116, 117), and 5-8% when supplemented by inhaled anaesthetics (118, 119). In one study none of 129 patients had unpleasant dreams during nitrous oxide 70% + 0.5% halothane anaesthesia (75). Adequate responses to verbal commands during anaesthesia, without any conscious memory of the event, may also suggest implicit or unconscious memory (34). When neuromuscular blocking agents are used during anaesthesia the incidence may be investigated using the isolated forearm technique with a tourniquet applied to one upper extremity before administration of the neuromuscular blocking agent, enabling the patient to move the arm. Seventy-two percent of 32 nonobstetrical patients during total intravenous anaesthesia with midazolam-fentanyl (77), and more than 90% of 30 (76) and 74 (118) patients during nitrous oxide plus halothane or enflurane for caesarean section, responded one or several times to verbal commands without recall of the event. The real incidence of implicit awareness during anaesthesia may therefore be high. Intensity of noxious stimulation vs. the incidence of awareness There are no data available on the relationship between the intensity of noxious stimulation and the level of consciousness during anaesthesia. However, surgical manipulation and tracheal intubation are strong stimulants of the sympathetic nervous system, and therefore considered to increase the level of consciousness during anaesthesia (8, 73, 74). This is consistent with the observation that conscious awareness during cardiac surgery frequently occurs during sternotomy or other painful procedures (105, 107, 109). Similarly, in a study of 160 premedicated patients 3 incidents of conscious awareness (2%) occurred during tracheal intubation after induction with phenoperidine, droperidol, thiopentone and succinylcholine (121). Influence of patient-related differences in pharmacokinetics on awareness Patient-related factors, such as age, smoking, acute or chronic use of alcohol or drugs such as opiates or amphetamines, may increase the anaesthetic dose needed to obtain a certain level of unconsciousness, especially during opioid-based anaesthesia (8, 122-128). Patients with a compromized circulation require less anaesthetics as a larger proportion of their cardiac output is delivered to the brain than in healthy individuals (129). In conclusion, the incidence of awareness depends on the diagnostic criteria used, and varies with different anaesthetic techniques. If the frequency of response to verbal commands during isolated forearm studies is indicative, the incidence may be very high. Consequences of awareness Awareness during surgery may affect the patient adversely, but may also have a potential for patient benefit with programmed passive learning during anaesthesia. Conscious awareness - adverse effects In 1993 Moerman et al. (130) interviewed 26 patients who had experienced awareness with explicit recall of intraoperative events. Most patients felt panic and helplessness related to the inability to move or call for help, and some had frightening sensations of impending death, being left unattended, or that pain might be experienced. Seventy percent suffered significant aftereffects, including day-time anxiety, sleep disturbances and nightmares, and 3 needed psychotherapeutic help. Ninety percent of the patients who experienced pain during the awareness episode suffered after-effects. Similar findings are observed in other studies (92, 119, 130, 131) and case reports (132-139), also describing patients disabled after the incident. Sometimes the conscious awareness incident is less obvious. In these patients, a syndrome of traumatic neurosis may occur (5, 135), characterized by anxiety, irritability, preoccupation with death, and repetitive nightmares. The patients are reluctant to talk about their problems, as they are unable to connect them with the anaesthesia episode and fear being considered insane. Strikingly, in many patients the symptoms subside when fragments of their nightmares are connected to events occurring during surgery (5). Unconscious awareness - adverse effects Intraoperative events or auditory information presented to patients under general anaesthesia may be remembered subconsciously (4-6, 108, 140-144), but the connection to the event is subtle and cannot be recalled. Consequently, the influence of implicit memory on postoperative behaviour is not well studied clinically. Case reports suggest that operating room conversation, especially rude remarks related to the patient, may adversely influence the postoperative course without the patient's conscious knowledge (4, 6). The connection to the intraoperative event may be revealed by hypnosis (5, 6, 143, 144), based on the assumption that memory formed in one state may be recalled better in the same than in a different state (state-dependent memory) (145). An analogy between the state of general anaesthesia and that of hypnosis has been considered. These studies (5, 6, 143, 144) were not controlled or blinded, and may be of limited value, as confabulations cannot be excluded (146). Also, the period of dreaming may have occurred postoperatively during recovery from anaesthesia, rather than during surgery (147). Drugs with amnesic effects used as premedicants or during anaesthesia may influence the patient's ability to recall intraoperative events, without eliminating the ability of implicit memory (54, 56, 81, 148-150). It has also been suggested that a patient can have a conscious awareness episode, but be unable to recall the incident postoperatively, if the anaesthesia is subsequently deepened (151), or if the retrieval process is blocked by psychological or physiological mechanisms (152). Therefore, conclusive evidence in the literature of unconscious memory of intraoperative events adversely affecting postoperative behavior is essentially lacking. Learning during anaesthesia For implicit memory formation, auditory perception must still be functioning during anaesthesia, and physiological studies suggest this is the case (10, 69, 70). In unstimulated volunteers, implicit memory is suppressed at 0.45 MAC isoflurane (153), slightly higher if nitrous oxide is administered simultaneously (154). Implicit memory during anaesthesia considered adequate for surgery has been demonstated by some investigators (155-158), while others have failed (71, 72, 159-163). This discrepancy may at least in part be explained by the great variability in anaesthetic techniques. Since unconsciousness during anaesthesia is considered a graded phenomenon (10, 14, 34, 65, 66), factors known to influence the depth of anaesthesia, especially the anaesthetic technique used and the level of surgical stimulation, must be similar when results from different studies are compared. Subconscious memory building during anaesthesia is also studied with a variety of tests, but it is not known which test is most sensitive to learning ability during anaesthesia (12). Priming is greater when the target material and the implicit test are presented in the same sensory modality, i.e. both auditory (12, 164). The salience of the information presented during anaesthesia might be important and insignificant stimuli may not be memorized (6). Consequently, differences in tests and procedures may also explain varying results (12). In unstimulated volunteers, 1.5-2 times the concentration of desflurane or propofol required to prevent response to verbal commands, suppressed memory of emotionally charged information (165). The drug concentration needed to suppress such information during surgical stimulation is not known. If implicit memory occurs during anaesthesia, learning processes may take place, and learning skills may even be improved by the relaxed state achieved during anaesthesia, analogous to the increased responsiveness to verbal suggestions observed during hypnosis (12, 166). In prospective double-blind experiments with volatile anaesthetics, it appears that intraoperative suggestions influence postoperative behaviour (i.e. the number of times a suggested body part is touched) (156, 167, 168). These studies have been criticized for the lack of preanaesthesia baseline assessment of the behaviour tested (169). Bethune et al. (158) and Jansen et al. (170) could not replicate these results when a pre-operative behaviour assessment was included in patients anaesthetized with either methohexitone or propofol. Both a reduction (171-174) and no effect (175-179) on duration of hospital stay has been reported for patients presented during anaesthesia with positive suggestions predicting a rapid and comfortable post-operative recovery. There is no clear explanation for the variability in the results. The level of consciousness affected by the anaesthetics and the surgical stimulation may have varied (71, 173). One of the positive studies (171) has been criticized for using an inappropriate control group (179), but statistical arguments can also be used the other way. The number of patients included in the referred studies were relatively small, and the complexity of factors influencing the duration of hospital stay (type of surgery, surgical skills, intrinsic patient and illness variability, and discharge policy), may easily obscure any effect of positive suggestions. The fact that some studies show effect of intraoperative positive suggestions at all may therefore be considered strong evidence of a real effect, not precluded by reports of no benefits (12). Studies with a high statistical power, which require inclusion of a large number of patients, standardized anaesthetic and surgical techniques and exclusion of patients having complications secondary to surgery, are unfortunately lacking (173). Some authors find reduced postoperative analgesic requirements after intraoperative positive suggestions ( 180184), others not (185-187). Most investigators only recorded requests for intramuscular opiates, which probably is an unreliable measure of the patient's real needs (188), but both positive (183, 184) and no effect (186, 187) have also been reported with patient-controlled analgesia. In conclusion, there is solid evidence that implicit memory function exists during anaesthesia, based on experimental studies with doses of anaesthetics less than those required for surgery. During anaesthesia for surgery the picture is more obscure. The situation is multifactorial and the depth of anaesthesia is influenced both by the anaesthetic used and the intensity of the surgical stimulation. No conclusive evidence exists on the presence or lack of influence on behavior, active learning, hospital stay, or need for postoperative analgesics. Prevention of awareness Most anaesthesiologists would probably agree that if anaesthesia is deep enough (isoelectric EEG) there will be no conscious or unconscious awareness. As the margin of safety of most anaesthetic agents is narrow, administration of high doses of anaesthetics is not always compatible with safe conduct of anaesthesia. We are constantly balancing positive and negative effects of the anaesthetic agents, and since at present a reliable monitor for consciousness is not available, we might have to accept some incidents of awareness to avoid higher morbidity and mortality from deep levels of anaesthesia. Patient - anaesthesiologist relationship The first step toward prevention of awareness is probably the anaesthesiologist's acceptance and awareness of this potentially disabling experience! The ASA has recommended considering informing the patient of the possibility of awareness, especially when intentionally light anaesthesia is anticipated (189). In one survey, 50% of the patients expressed concern about the possibility of being awake during surgery (190). Preoperative assurance that qualified personnel are watching the patient continuously might be beneficial should an awareness situation occur. The anaesthesiologist should also see the patient the day before surgery to relieve any patient anxiety, since increased sympathetic activity may increase the potential for awareness during anaesthesia ( 74). While the patient is asleep the anaesthesiologist should take precautions that patient related remarks by operating room personnel do not occur. There are case reports showing that conscious or unconscious memory formation of salient information may take place during otherwise adequate anaesthesia ( 4, 6, 130). Avoidance of unintentional light anaesthesia The endtidal gas concentrations of inhalational anaesthetics should ideally be monitored, and the vaporizer checked at regular intervals. Pumps for intravenous anaesthetics must have volume and pressure alarms, and the infusions should preferably be administered via separate iv lines. Patient characteristics known to increase the anaesthetic requirements during opioid-based anaesthesia, such as cigarette smoking, chronic alchohol or drug abuse, should be accounted for in the choice and dosage of anaesthetic agents (123, 125, 126). Administration of drugs with known amnesic qualities as premedicant or during anaesthesia, i.e. scopolamine or benzodiazepines, has been recommended (12, 189), but they may not block all implicit memory formation during anaesthesia (54, 56, 81, 148-150). In order to prevent pollution in the operating room, anaesthetic gases are frequently avoided during induction of anaesthesia. The hypnotic drugs used normally redistribute quickly, and the brain concentration may be less than optimal when tracheal intubation is performed. Tracheal intubation is a very potent stimulus, and very high anaesthetic concentrations are needed to prevent movement responses secondary to the procedure, if muscle relaxants are not used (191). Consequently, awareness episodes are likely to occur in association with tracheal intubation (121, 130, 192). In an attempt to secure unconsciousness during tracheal intubation, it is adviseable to combine an opioid with the hypnotic, and administer an extra dose of the hypnotic drug just prior to this procedure (189). Administration of intentional light anaesthesia A minimum of anaesthetics, i.e. 4-5mg/kg pentothal and nitrous oxide 50%, is often administered during the first stage of a caesarean section. The addition of low doses of volatile anaesthetics reduce the frequency of awareness significantly, without adversely affecting the newborn (75). In hypovolaemic trauma, patients' awareness episodes may occur during induction of anaesthesia or surgery, even if the patient is unconscious on arrival at the operating room (120). The preferred anaesthetic drug in this situation is ketamine, due to its stimulatory effects on the sympathetic nerve system. However, awareness episodes are described also during ketamine anaesthesia (85, 86, 120). Assessment of anaesthesia depth At present there is no monitor available that can determine if the patient is unconscious or not during anaesthesia (193). Depth of anaesthesia is clinically judged by the observation of somatic (patient movements) and autonomic reflexes (increased heart rate and blood pressure, tearing and pupil dilation). There are only a few case reports of awareness during surgery in unparalyzed patients where somatic reflexes were absent ( 92, 94, 194). Consequently, the observation of movement responses is the best clinical measure available for detecting impending awareness during surgery. In the presence of muscle relaxants adequacy of anaesthesia is most often assessed by the observation of autonomic reflexes, although a firm relationship to awareness is not established (193). Use of muscle relaxants Muscle relaxants are used for tracheal intubation and to obtain surgical relaxation, but most surgical procedures do not require complete neuromuscular blockade. Consequently, the ability of limb movement may be preserved during most surgery, although the precise relationship between degree of neuromuscular blockade and limb movement ability is not determined. As muscle relaxation depends both on the degree of neuromuscular blockade and the anaesthetic depth (195, 196), the dosage of muscle relaxants needed to obtain a certain level of muscle relaxation gradually decreases with increasing concentrations of anaesthetics ( 196). This is specifically shown for volatile inhaled anaesthetics (196), which block stimulatory impulses on alpha motor neurons in the spinal medulla (197), and potentiate the effect of muscle relaxants at the neuromuscular junction (198-202). A nerve stimulator should be used whenever muscle relaxants are given, enabling the anaesthesiologist to titrate the administration according to surgical needs (203). If the patient moves in response to noxious stimuli, more anaesthetics should be administered, not a muscle relaxant. When deep neuromuscular blockade is required during surgery, the activity of muscle groups known to be more resistent to the effect of muscle relaxants, i.e. diaphragm, larynx, facial or neck muscles ( 204-206), should be observed, and surface EMG monitoring from facial or neck muscles may be helpful (207, 208). Inhaled vs. nitrous oxide-opioid or total iv anaesthesia Awareness has been observed with both inhaled, nitrous oxide-opioid and total iv anaesthesia (see incidence section). There are at least three reasons why volatile anaesthetics may be less associated with awareness than other anaesthetic regimens in patients who cannot move in response to noxious stimuli: A. a. There is significant interindividual variability in the pharmacokinetics of intravenous anaesthetics (209-214), and during routine anaesthesia the plasma drug concentration cannot adequately be predicted from the dose administered. As an example, the plasma concentration obtained during a constant rate infusion of propofol (6.5mg·kg-1 8 patients varied between 2.5 and 5 µg·ml-1 (213). Even with a computer-controlled infusion pump, using population-based pharmacokinetic data for drug delivery, the plasma concentration can deviate significantly from the target (211), and online plasma drug concentration determination is not yet available. Awareness might therefore occur during intense noxious stimulation in patients who pharmacodynamically require a high brain drug concentration, but pharmacokinetically achieve a low concentration from a given dose. B. For volatile anaesthetics the brain drug concentrations can be adequately estimated intraoperatively in normoventilated patients, using continuous endtidal concentration measurements (215). Therefore, anaesthetic depth can probably more adequately be adjusted according to the patient's needs than during total intravenous anaesthesia. C. b. Volatile inhaled anaesthetics more potently suppress consciousness (MAC-awake relative to MACincision) than nitrous oxide (34). An anaesthetic regimen based on a combination of nitrous oxide and opioids may therefore be associated with a higher frequency of awareness than other anaesthetic regimen. Some clinical studies suggest that this is the case (90, 91). D. c. The interindividual variability in drug concentration needed to prevent movement response to noxious stimulation may be less with volatile than with nitrous oxide-opioid or total intravenous anaesthesia (9, 16, 209211). This must now be questioned as a recent study found no difference in the interindividual variability in the concentration of anaesthetics required to obtund the response to command for desflurane (MAC-awake) and propofol (Cp50-awake) (216). Summary Awareness during anaesthesia is a state of consciousness that is revealed by explicit or implicit memory of intraoperative events. Although large clinical surveys indicate an incidence of explicit awareness of <0.3% during anaesthesia for general surgery, this adverse effect should be a great concern, because patients may be permanently disabled by the experience of being awake during surgery. Prevention of awareness during anaesthesia starts with an appropriate preoperative visit to the patient. The anaesthetic delivery machines must be properly checked before and during anaesthesia. The anaesthetic depth should be assessed by observation of movement responses, and consequently a minimum of muscle relaxants used. Because the anaesthetic depth can be controlled by determination of endtidal drug concentration, volatile inhaled anaesthesia may be associated with a lower frequency of awareness than other anaesthetic regimens. REFERENCES 1. Unattributed. Operations without pain. Lancet 1847: i: 77-79. [Context Link] 2. Crile GH. George Crile: An autobiography. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1947: 147. [Context Link] 3. Editorial. Curare in anesthesia. Lancet 1945: ii: 81-82. [Context Link] 4. Cheek DB. Unconscious perception of meaningful sounds under surgical anesthesia as revealed under hypnosis. Am J Clin Hypn 1959: 1: 101-113. [Context Link] 5. Blacher RS. On awakening paralyzed during surgery. JAMA 1975: 234: 67-68. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 6. Goldmann L. Information-processing under general anesthesia: A review. J Roy Soc Med 1988: 81: 224-227. [Context Link] 7. Hug CC. Does opioid "anesthesia" exist? Anesthesiology 1990: 73: 1-4. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 8. Bailey PL, Stanley TH. Are opioids anesthetics? Intravenous opioid anesthetics. In: Miller RD, ed. Anesthesia. New York: Churchill Livingstone, 1994: 290-293. [Context Link] 9. Stanski DR. Monitoring depth of anesthesia. In: Miller RD, ed. Anesthesia. New York: Churchill Livingstone, 1994: 1001-1029. [Context Link] 10. Griffith D, Jones JG. Awareness and memory in anesthetized patients. Br J Anaesth 1990: 65: 603-606. [Context Link] 11. Jessop J, Jones JG. Conscious awareness during general anesthesia - what are we attempting to monitor? Br J Anaesth 1991: 66: 635-637. [Context Link] 12. Ghoneim MM, Block RI. Learning and consciousness during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992: 76: 279305. [Context Link] 13. Cormack RS. Conscious levels during anesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1993: 71: 469-471. [Context Link] 14. Newton DEF. Depth of anesthesia. Anesthesia 1993. 48: 367-368. [Context Link] 15. Roizen MF, Saidman LJ. Redefining anesthesia management. Anesthesiology 1994: 80: 251-252. [Context Link] 16. Stanski DR. Monitoring depth of anesthesia. In: Miller RD, ed. Anesthesia. New York: Churchill Livingstone, 1994: 1127-1159. [Context Link] 17. Bennett HL. The mind during surgery: The uncertain effects of anesthesia. Advances 1993: 9: 5-16. [Context Link] 18. Blacher RS. Awareness during surgery. Anesthesiology 1984: 61: 1-2. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 19. Breckenridge JL, Aitkenhead AR. Awareness during anaesthesia: a review. Ann Roy Coll Surg Engl 1983: 65: 9396. [Context Link] 20. Editorial. Advertizing during anesthesia? Lancet 1986: ii: 1019-1020. [Context Link] 21. Saunders D. Anaesthesia, awareness and automation. Br J Anaesth 1981: 53: 1-3. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 22. Mori K. The EEG and awareness during anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1987: 42: 1153-1155. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 23. Snow J. On the inhalation of the vapors of ether in surgical operations. Containing a description of the various stages of etherization, and a statement of the result of nearly eighty operations in which ether has been employed in St. George's and University College Hospitals. London: John Churchill, 1847. Reproduced by Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia, 1959. [Context Link] 24. Guedel AE. Inhalational anesthesia. A fundamental guide. New York: Macmillan, 1937. [Context Link] 25. Artusio JF, Jr. Di-ethyl ether analgesia: a detailed description of the first stage of ether analgesia in man. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1954: 111: 343. [Context Link] 26. Prys-Roberts C. Anesthesia: A practical or impractical construct? Br J Anaesth 1987: 59: 1341-1345. [Context Link] 27. Kissin I. General anesthetic action: An obsolete notion? Anesth Analg 1993: 76: 215-218. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 28. Kissin I, Kerr CR, Smith LR. Assessment of anaesthetic action of morphine and fentanyl in rats. Can Anaesth Soc J 1983: 30: 623-628. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 29. Kissin I, Brown PT. Reserpine-induced changes in anesthetic action of fentanyl. Anesthesiology 1985: 62: 597600. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 30. Kissin I, Mason JO III, Bradley EL Jr. Morphine and fentanyl hypnotic interactions with thiopental. Anesthesiology 1987: 67: 331-335. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 31. Kissin I, McGee T, Smith LR. The indicies of potency for intravenous anaesthetics. Can Anaesth Soc J 1981: 28: 585-590. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 32. Deady J, Koblin DD, Eger EI II. Anesthetic potencies and the unitary theory of narcosis. Anesth Analg 1981: 60: 380-386. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 33. Eger EI II, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration: A standard of anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology 1965: 26: 756-763. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 34. Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI II, Heilbron D. Effects of isoflurane and nitrous oxide in subanesthetic concentrations on memory and responsiveness in volunteers. Anesthesiology 1992: 77: 888-898. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 35. Pelligrino DA, Miletich DJ, Hoffman WE, Albrecht RF. Nitrous oxide markedly increases cerebral cortical metabolic rate and blood flow in the goat. Anesthesiology 1984: 60: 405-412. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 36. Fukunaga AF, Epstein RM. Sympathetic excitation during nitrous oxide-halothane anesthesia in the cat. Anesthesiology 1973: 39: 23-36. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 37. Smith NT, Eger EI II, Stoelting RK. The cardiovascular and sympathomimetic responses to the addition of nitrous oxide to halothane in man. Anesthesiology 1970: 32: 410-421. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 38. Stoelting RK, Longnecker DE, Eger EI II. Minimum alveolar concentrations in man on awakening from methoxyflurane, halothane, ether and fluroxene anesthesia: MAC awake. Anesthesiology 1970: 33: 5-9. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 39. Quasha AL, Eger EI II, Tinker JH. Determination and applications of MAC. Anesthesiology 1980: 53: 315-334. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 40. Richards CD. In search of the mechanism of anesthesia. Trends Neurosci 1980: 3: 9-13. [Context Link] 41. Franks NP, Lieb WR. What is the molecular nature of general anesthetic target sites. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1987: 8: 169-175. [Context Link] 42. Roth SH. Membrane and cellular actions of anesthetic agents. Fed Proc 1980c: 39: 1595. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 43. Richards CR. Actions of general anesthetics on synaptic transmission in the CNS. Br J Anaesth 1983: 55: 201207. [Context Link] 44. Weston GA, Roth SH. Differential actions of a volatile anesthetic agents on a single isolated neurone. Br J Anaesth 1986: 58: 1390-1396. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 45. Keane PE, Biziere K. The effects of general anaesthetics on GABAergic synaptic transmission. Life Sci 1987: 41: 1437-1448. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 46. Berg-Johnsen J, Langmoen IA. The effect of isoflurane on unmyelinated and myelinated fibres in the rat brain. Acta Physiol Scand 1986: 127: 87-93. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 47. Berg-Johnsen J, Langmoen IA. Isoflurane effects in rat hippocampal cortex: A quantitative evaluation of different cellular sites of action. Acta Physiol Scand 1986: 128: 613-618. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 48. Langmoen IA, Larsen M, Berg-Johnsen J. Volatile anaesthetics: Cellular mechanisms of action. Eur J Anaesth 1995: 12: 51-58. [Context Link] 49. Berg-Johnsen J, Langmoen IA. Mechanisms concerned in the direct effect of isoflurane on rat hippocampal and human neocortical neurones. Brain Res 1990: 507: 28-34. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 50. Pearce RA, Stringer JL, Lothman EW. Effect of volatile anesthetics on synaptic transmission in the rat hippocampus. Anesthesiology 1989: 71: 591-598. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 51. Johns RA, Moscicki JC, DiFazio CA. Nitric oxide synthase inhibitor dose-dependently and reversibly reduces the threshold for halothane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992: 77: 779-784. [Context Link] 52. Johns RA. Nitric oxide and minimum alveolar concentration. TKO or Knockout? Anesthesiology 1995: 83: 6-7. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 53. Keifer JC, Baghdoyan HA, Becker L, Lydic R. Halothane anesthesia causes decreased acetylcholine release in the pontine reticular formation and increased EEG spindles. Neuroreport 1994: 5: 577-580. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 54. Frith CD, Richardson JTE, Samuel M, Crow TJ, McKenna PJ. The effects of intravenous diazepam and hyoscine upon human memory. QJ Exp Psychol (A) 1984: 36: 133-144. [Context Link] 55. Hug CC. Lipid solubility, pharmacokinetics, and the EEG: Are you better off today than you were four years ago? Anesthesiology 1985: 62: 221-226. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 56. Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP. Bezodiazepines and human memory: A review. Anesthesiology 1990: 72: 926-938. [Context Link] 57. Teyler TJ, DiScenna E Long-term potentiation. Ann Rev Neurosci 1987: 10: 131-161. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 58. Gustafsson B, Wigstrom H. Physiological mechanisms underlying long-term potentiation. Trends Neurosci 1988: 11: 156-162. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 59. Cotman CW, Monaghan DT, Ganong AH. Excitatory amino acid neurotransmission: NMDA receptors and Hebbtype synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Neurosci 1988: 11: 61-80. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 60. Kemp JA, Foster AC, Wong EH. Non-competitive antagonists of excitatory amino acids. Trends Neurosci 1988: 11: 294-298. [Context Link] 61. Angel A. Central neuronal pathways and the process of anesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1993: 71: 148-163. [Context Link] 62. Shimoji K, Fujioka H, Fukazawa T, Hashiba M, Maruyama Y. Anesthetics and exitatory/inhibitory responses of midbrain reticular neurons. Anesthesiology 1984: 61: 151-155. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 63. Baghdoyan HA, Spotts JL, Snider SG. Simultaneous pontine and basal forebrain microinjections of carbachol suppress REM sleep. J Neurosci 1993: 13: 227-240. [Context Link] 64. Lydic R, Biebuyck JF: Sleep neurobiology. Relevance for mechanistic studies of anesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1994: 72: 506-508. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 65. Jones JG, Konieczko K. Hearing and memory in anesthetized patients. Br Med J 1986: 292: 1291-1293. [Context Link] 66. Newton DEF, Thornton C, Konieczko K, Frith CD, Doré CJ, Webster NR, et al. Level of consciousness in volunteers breathing sub-MAC concentrations of isoflurane. Br J Anaesth 1990: 65: 609-615. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 67. Schacter DL. Implicit memory: History and current status. J Exp Psych Learn Mem Cogn 1987: 13: 501-518. [Context Link] 68. Squire LR. Mechanisms of memory. Science 1986: 232: 1612-1619. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 69. Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA. Human learning during general anesthesia and surgery. Br J Anaesth 1991: 66: 170-178. [Context Link] 70. Drummond JC, Todd MM, Hoi Sang U. The effect of high dose sodium thiopental on brain stem auditory and median nerve somatosensory evoked responses in humans. Anesthesiology 1985: 63: 249-254. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 71. Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI II, Peterson N. Isoflurane anesthesia prevents unconscious learning. Anesth Analg 1992: 75: 107-112. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 72. Gonsowski CT, Chortkoff BS, Eger EI II, Bennett HL, Weiskopf RB. Subanesthetic concentrations of desflurane and isoflurane suppress explicit and implicit learning. Anesth Analg 1995: 80: 568-572. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 73. Sebel PS. Are our patients listening? ASA Newsletter (American Society of Anesthesiologists) 1994: 58: 7-9. [Context Link] 74. Weinberger NM, Gold PE, Sternberg DB. Epinephrine enables Pavlovian fear conditioning under anesthesia. Science 1984: 223: 605-607. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 75. Crawford JS, Lewis M, Davies P. Maternal and neonatal responses related to the volatile agent used to maintain anaesthesia at caesarean section. Br J Anaesth 1985: 57: 482-487. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 76. King H, Ashley S, Brathwaite D, Decayette J, Wooten DJ. Adequacy of general anesthesia for cesarean section. Anesth Analg 1993: 77: 84-88. [Context Link] 77. Russel IF. Midazolam-alfentanil: An anaesthetic? An investigation using the isolated forearm technique. Br J Anaesth 1993: 70: 42-46. [Context Link] 78. Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE. A simple study of awareness and dreaming during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1970: 42: 535-542. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 79. Scott DL. Awareness during general anaesthesia. Can Anaesth Soc J 1972: 19: 173-183. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 80. Agarwal G, Sikh SS. Awareness during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1977: 49: 835-838. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 81. Utting JE. Awareness: Clinical aspects. In: Rosen M, Lunn JN, eds. Consciousness, awareness and pain in general anaesthesia. London: Butterworth, 1987: 171-179. [Context Link] 82. Mainzer J. Awareness, muscle relaxants and balanced anaesthesia. Can Anaesth Soc J 1979: 26: 386-393. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 83. Harris TJB, Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE. Dreaming associated with anaesthesia: The influence of morphine premedication and two volatile adjuvants. Br J Anaesth 1971: 43: 172-179. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 84. Browne RA, Catton DV. Awareness during anaesthesia: A comparison of anaesthesia with nitrous oxide-oxygen and nitrous oxide-oxygen with innovar. Can Anaesth Soc J 1973: 20: 763-768. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 85. Garfield JM, Garfield FB, Stone JG, Hopkins D, Johns LA. A comparison of psychologic responses to ketamine and thiopental-nitrous oxide-halothane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1972: 36: 329-335. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 86. Vaughan RW, Stephen CR. Abdominal and thoracic surgery in adults with ketamine, nitrous oxide, and dtubocurarine. Anesth Analg 1974: 53: 271-280. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 87. Eisele V, Weinreich A, Bartle S. Perioperative awareness and recall. Anesth Analg 1976: 55: 513-518. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 88. Hutchinson R. Awareness during surgery. Br J Anaesth 1960: 33: 463-469. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 89. Wilson SL, Vaughan RW, Stephen CR. Awareness, dreams, and hallucinations associated with general anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1975: 54: 609-617. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 90. Russel IF. Balanced anesthesia: Does it anesthetize? Anesth Analg 1985: 64: 941-942. [Context Link] 91. Russel IF. Comparison of wakefulness with two anaesthetic regimens. Br J Anaesth 1986: 58: 965-968. [Context Link] 92. Jordening H, Pedersen T. The incidence of conscious awareness in a general population of anesthetized patients. Anesthesiology 1991: 75: A1055. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 93. Liu WHD, Thorp TAS, Graham SG, Aitkenhead AR. Incidence of awareness with recall during general anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1991: 46: 435-437. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 94. Saucier N, Walts LF, Moreland JR. Patient awareness during nitrous oxide, oxygen. and halothane anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1983: 62: 239-240. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 95. Bahl CP, Wadwa S. Consciousness during apparent surgical anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1968: 40: 289-291. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 96. Tantisira B, McKenzie R. Awareness during laparoscopy under general anesthesia: A case report. Anesth Analg 1974: 53: 373-374. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 97. Kelly JS, Roy RC. Intraoperative awareness with propofol-oxygen total intravenous anesthesia for microlaryngeal surgery. Anesthesiology 1992: 77: 207-209. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 98. Bennett S. Propofol and awareness III. Anesthesiology 1992: 77: 1232-1233. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 99. De Grood PMRM, Mitsukuri S, Van Egmond J, Rutten JMJ, Crul JF. Comparison of etomidate and propofol for anaesthesia in microlaryngeal surgery. Anaesthesia 1987: 42: 366-372. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 100. Sandin R, Nordstrøm O. Awareness during total IV anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1993: 71: 782-787. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 101. Desiderio DP, Thorne AC. Awareness and general anesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1990: 34: 48-50. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 102. Moore JK, Seymore AH. Awareness during bronchoscopy. Ann R Coll Surg Engl 1987: 69: 45-47. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 103. Hatano S, Keane DM, Boggs RE. Diazepam-ketamine anaesthesia for open-heart surgery: A 'micro-mini'drip infusion technique. Can Anaesth Soc J 1976: 23: 648-656. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 104. Kumar SM, Pandit SK, Jackson PF. Recall following ketamine anesthesia for open-heart surgery: Report of a case. Anesth Analg 1978: 57: 267-269. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 105. Hilgenberg JC. Intraoperative awareness during high-dose fentanyl-oxygen anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1981: 54: 341-343. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 106. Mummanemi N, Rao TLK, Montoya A. Awareness and recall with high-dose fentanyl-oxygen anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1980: 59: 948-949. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 107. Mendelsohn D, MacDonald DW, Nogueira C, Kay EB. Anesthesia for open-heart surgery: Acquired valvular diseases. Anesth Analg 1960: 39: 110-120. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 108. Goldman L, Shah MV, Hebden MW. Memory of cardiac surgery. Anaesthesia 1987: 42: 596-603. [Context Link] 109. Maunuksela E-L. Hemodynamic response to different anesthetics during open-heart surgery. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1977: 65: 43. [Context Link] 110. Phillips AA, McLean RF, Devitt JH. Recall of intraoperative events after general anesthesia and cardiopulmonary bypass. Can J Anaesth 1993: 40: 922-926. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 111. Mark JB, Greenberg LM. Intraoperative awareness and hypertensive crisis during high-dose fentanyl-diazepamoxygen anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1983: 62: 698-700. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 112. Bergstrøm H, Bernstein K. Psychic reactions after analgesia with nitrous oxide for caesarean section. Lancet 1968: ii: 541-542. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 113. Abboud TK, Kim SH, Henriksen EH, Chen T, Eisenman R, Levinson G, Shnider SM. Comparative maternal and neonatal effects of halothane and enflurane for cesarean section. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1985: 29. 663-668. [Context Link] 114. Wilson J, Turner DJ. Awareness during caesarean section under general anaesthesia. Br Med J 1969: 1: 280283. [Context Link] 115. Moir DD. Anaesthesia for caesarean section. Br J Anaesth 1970: 42: 136-142. [Context Link] 116. Schultetus RR, Hill CR, Dharamraj CM, Banner TE, Berman LS. Wakefulness during cesarean section after induction with ketamine, thiopental, or ketamine and thiopental combined. Anesth Analg 1986: 65: 723-728. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 117. Abouleish E, Taylor FH. Effect of morphine-diazepam on signs of anesthesia, awareness, and dreams of patients under nitrous oxide for cesarean section. Anesth Analg 1976: 55: 702-705. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 118. Bogod DG, Orton JK, Oh TE. Detecting awareness during general anaesthetic caesarean section. Anaesthesia 1990: 45: 279-284. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 119. Lyons G, Macdonald R. Awareness during caesarean section. Anaesthesia 1991: 46: 62-64. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 120. Bogetz MS, Katz JA. Recall of surgery for major trauma. Anesthesiology 1984: 61: 6-9. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 121. McKenna T, Wilton TNP. Awareness during endotracheal intubation. Anaesthesia 1973: 28: 599-602. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 122. Bailey PL, WilbrinkJ, Zwanikken P, Pace NL, Stanley TH. Anesthetic induction with fentanyl. Anesth Analg 1985: 64: 48-53. [Context Link] 123. Stanley TH, deLange S. The effect of population habits on side effects and narcotic requirements during highdose fentanyl anaesthesia. Can Anaesth Soc J 1984: 31: 368-376. [Context Link] 124. Shafer A, White PF, Schuettler J, Rosenthal MH. Use of fentanyl infusion in the intensive care unit: Tolerance to its anesthetic effects? Anesthesiology 1983: 59: 245-248. [Context Link] 125. Tammisto T, Takki S. Nitrous oxide-oxygen-relaxant anaesthesia in alcoholics: A retrospective study. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1973: (suppl) 53: 68-75. [Context Link] 126. Tammisto T, Tigerstedt I. The need for fentanyl supplementation of nitrous oxide-oxygen-relaxant anaesthesia in chronic alcoholics. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1977: 21: 216-221. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 127. Lemmens HJM, BovillJG, Hennis PJ, Gladines MPRR, Burm AGL. Alcohol consumption alters the pharmacodynamics of alfentanil. Anesthesiology 1989: 71: 669-674. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 128. Bovill JG, Sebel PS, Wauquier A, Rog P, Schuyt HC. Influence of high-dose alfentanil anaesthesia on the electroencephalogram: Correlation with plasma concentrations. Br J Anaesth 1983: 55: 199S-209S. [Context Link] 129. Stanley TH, Webster LR. Anesthetic requirements and cardiovascular effects of fentanyl-oxygen and fentanyldiazepam-oxygen anesthesia in man. Anesth Analg 1978: 57: 411-416. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 130. Moerman N, Bonke B, Oosting J. Awareness and recall during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1993: 79: 454-464. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 131. Evans JM. Patients' experiences of awareness during general anaesthesia. In: Rosen M, Lunn JN, eds. Consciousness, Awareness and Pain in General Anaesthesia. London: Butterworth, 1987: 184-192. [Context Link] 132. On being aware. Editorial. Br J Anaesth 1979: 51: 711-712. [Context Link] 133. Tracy JM. Personal account of awareness. ASA Newsletter (American Society of Anesthesiologists) 1994: 58: 10-11. [Context Link] 134. A case report. ASA Newsletter (American Society of Anesthesiologists) 1994: 58: 14. [Context Link] 135. Macleod AD, Maycock E. Awareness during anaesthesia and post traumatic stress disorder. Anaesth Intensive Care 1992: 20: 378-382. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 136. Grunshaw N. Anaesthetic awareness. Br Med J 1990: 300: 821. [Context Link] 137. Silbergleit I-L. Letter on awareness. JAMA 1976: 235: 1209-1210. [Context Link] 138. Messer HD. Letter on awareness. JAMA 1976: 235: 1210. [Context Link] 139. Archer J. Letter on awareness. JAMA 1976: 235: 1211. [Context Link] 140. Cheek DB. Further evidence of persistence of hearing under chemo-anesthesia: Detailed case report. Am J Clin Hypn 1964: 7: 55-59. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 141. Cheek DB. Surgical memory and reactio.n to careless conversation. Am J Clin Hypn 1964: 6: 237-240. [Medline Link] [Context Link] 142. Trustman R, Dubrovs. ky S, Titley R. Auditory perception during general anesthesia - myth or fact? Int J Clin Exp Hypn 1977: 25: 88-105. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 143. Howard JF. Incidents of auditory perception during anaesthesia with traumatic sequelae. Med J Austr 1987: 146: 44-46. [Context Link] 144. Levinson BW. States of awareness during general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1965: 37: 544-546. [Context Link] 145. Mewaldt SP, Ghoneim MM, Choi WW, Korttila K, Peterson RC. Nitrous oxide and human state-dependent memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1988: 30: 83-87. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 146. Council report. Scientific status of refreshing recollection by the use of hypnosis. JAMA 1985: 253: 1918-1923. [Context Link] 147. Larson PC. Letter on awareness. JAMA 1976: 235: 1209. [Context Link] 148. Pandit SK, Heisterkamp DV, Cohen PJ. Further studies of the anti-recall effect of lorazepam. Anesthesiology 1976: 45: 495-500. [Context Link] 149. Fang JC, Hinrichs JV, Ghoneim MM. Diazepam and memory: Evidence for spared memory function. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1987: 28: 347-352. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 150. Cherkin A, Harroun E. Anesthesia and memory processes. Anesthesiology 1971: 34: 469-474. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 151. Ruhpret J. Awareness with amnesia during total intravenous anaesthesia with propofol. Anaesthesia 1989: 44: 1005. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 152. O'Hara MW, Ghoneim MM, Hinrichs JV, Mehta MP, Wright EJ. Psychological consequences of surgery. Psychosom Med 1989: 51: 356-370. [Context Link] 153. Chortkoff BS, Bennett HL, Eger EI II. Subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane suppress learning as defined by the category-example test. Anesthesiology 1993: 79: 16-22. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 154. Chortkoff BS, Bennett HL, Eger EI II. Does nitrous oxide antagonize isoflurane-induced suppression of learning? Anesthesiology 1993: 79: 724-732. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 155. Millar K, Watkinson N. Recognition of words presented during general anaesthesia. Ergonomics 1983: 26: 585594. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 156. Bennett HL, Davis HS, Giannini JA. Non-verbal response to intraoperative conversation. Br J Anaesth 1985: 57: 174-179. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 157. Kihlstrom JF, Schacter DL, Cork RC, Hurt CA, Behr SE. Implicit and explicit memory following surgical anesthesia. Psychol Sci 1990: 1: 303-306. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 158. Bethune DW, Ghosh S, Gray B, Kerr L, Walker IA, Doolan LA, et al. Learning during general anaesthesia: Implicit recall after methohexitone or propofol. Br J Anaesth 1992: 69: 197-199. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 159. Eich E, Reeves JL, Katz RL. Anesthesia, amnesia, and the memory/awareness distinction. Anesth Analg 1985: 64: 1143-1148. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 160. Woo R, Seltzer JL, Marr A. The lack of response to suggestion under controlled surgical anesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1987: 31: 567-571. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 161. Standen PJ, Hain WR, Hosker KJ. Retention of auditory information presented during anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1987: 42: 604-608. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 162. Oddby-Muhrbeck E, Jakobsson J. Recall of music: A comparison between anaesthesia with propofol and isoflurane. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1993: 37: 33-37. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 163. Parker CJR, Oates JDL, Boyd AH, Thomas SD. Memory for auditory material presented during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1994: 72: 181-184. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 164. Graf P, Shimamura AP. Priming across modalities and priming across category levels: Extending the domain of preserved function in amnesia. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1985: 11: 386-396. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 165. Chortkoff BS, Gonsowski CT, Bennett HL, Levinson B, Crankshaw DP, Dutton RC, et al. Subanesthetic concentrations of desflurane and propofol suppress recall of emotionally charged information. Anesth Analg 1995: 81: 728-736. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 166. Wadden TA, Anderton CH. The clinical use of hypnosis. Psychol Bull 1982: 91: 215-243. [Context Link] 167. Goldmann L. Factors determining the probability of recollection of intraoperative events. In: Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K, Rockland MA, eds. Memory and awareness in anaesthesia. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger, 1990:45-49. [Context Link] 168. Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA. Human learning during general anaesthesia and surgery. Br J Anaesth 1991: 66: 170-178. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 169. Millar K. Letter to editor. Br J Anaesth 1986: 58:1334. [Context Link] 170. Jansen CK, Bonke B, Klein J, van Dasselaar N, Hop WCJ. Failure to demonstrate unconscious perception during balanced anaesthesia by postoperative motor response. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1991: 35: 407-410. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 171. Evans C, Richardson PH. Improved recovery and reduced postoperative stay after therapeutic suggestions during general anaesthesia. Lancet 1988: ii: 491-493. [Context Link] 172. Bonke B, Schmitz PIM, Verhage Z, Zwaveling A. Clinical study of so-called unconscious perception during general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1986: 58: 957-964. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 173. Jelicic M, Bonke B, Millar K. Effect of different therapeutic suggestions presented during anaesthesia on postoperativ cours. Eur J Anaesth 1993: 10: 343-347. [Context Link] 174. Pearson RE. Response to suggestions given under general anesthesia. Am J Clin Hypn 1961: 4: 106-114. [Context Link] 175. Boeke S, Bonke B, Bouwhuis-Hoogerwerf ML, Bovill JG, Zwaveling A. Effects of sounds presented during general anaesthesia on postoperative course. Br J Anaesth 1988: 60: 697-702. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 176. Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA. Efficacy of therapeutic suggestions for improved postoperative recovery presented during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1991: 75: 746-755. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 177. Liu WHD, Standen PJ, Aitkenhead AR. Therapeutic suggestions during general anaesthesia in patients undergoing hysterectomy. Br J Anaesth 1992: 68: 277-281. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 178. Oddby-Muhrbeck E, Jakobsson J, Enquist B. Implicit processing and therapeutic suggestion during balanced anaesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1995: 39: 333-337. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 179. Millar K. Efficacy of therapeutic suggestions presented during anaesthesia: Re-analysis of conflicting results. Br J Anaesth 1993: 71: 597-601. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 180. Wolfe LS, Millet JB. Control of post-operative pain by suggestion under general anesthesia. Am J Clin Hypn 1960: 3: 109-112. [Context Link] 181. Hutchings DD. The value of suggestion given under anesthesia: A report and evaluation of 200 consecutive cases. Am J Clin Hypn 1961: 4: 26-29. [Context Link] 182. Furlong M. Positive suggestions presented during anaesthesia. In: Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K. Rockland MA, eds. Memory and awareness in anaesthesia. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger, 1990: pp. 170-175. [Context Link] 183. McLintock TTC, Aitken H, Downie CFA, Kenny GNC. Postoperative analgesic requirements in patients exposed to positive intraoperative suggestions. Br Med J 1990: 301: 788-790. [Context Link] 184. Caseley-Rondi, Merikle PM, Bowers KS. Unconscious cognition in the context of general anesthesia. Consciousness and Cogn 1994: 3: 166-195. [Context Link] 185. Munch F, Zug H-D. Do intraoperative suggestions prevent nausea and vomiting in thyroidectomy-patients? An experimental study. In: Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K. Rockland, MA, eds. Memory and awareness in anaesthesia. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger, 1990: 185-188. [Context Link] 186. Steinberg ME, Hord AH, Reed B, Sebel PS. Study of the effect of intraoperative suggestions on postoperative analgesia and wellbeing. In: Sebel PS, Bonke B, Winograd E, eds. Memory and awareness in anesthesia. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PTR Prentice-Hall, 1991: 205-208. [Context Link] 187. van der Laan WH, van Leeuwen BL, Sebel PS, Winograd E, Bonke B, Bauman P. Intraoperative therapeutic suggestion has no effect on postoperative morphine requirements. Anesthesiology 1995: 83: A310. [Context Link] 188. Dodson ME. A review of methods for relief of postoperative pain. Ann R Coll Surg Engl 1982: 64: 324-327. [Context Link] 189. McLeskey CH, Aitkenhead AR. Prevention of awareness. ASA Newsletter 1994: 58: 16-21. [Context Link] 190. McCleane GJ, Cooper R. The nature of pre-operative anxiety. Anaesthesia 1990: 45: 153-155. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 191. Hung OR, Varvel JR, Shafer SL, Stanski DR. Thiopental pharmacodynamics. Anesthesiology 1992: 77: 237244. [Context Link] 192. Gild M. Review of ASA Closed Claims Project. ASA Newsletter June 1993. [Context Link] 193. Heier T, Steen PA. Depth of anaesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1996. In press. [Context Link] 194. Sebel PS: Awareness during anesthesia: An introduction. ASA Newsletter 1994: 58: 7-9. [Context Link] 195. Tammisto T. Neuromuscular block vs. muscle relaxation: Clinical applications of the difference. Finnanest 1992: 18: 11-18. [Context Link] 196. Tammisto T, Olkkola KT. Dependence of the adequacy of muscle relaxation on the degree of neuromuscular block and depth of enflurane anesthesia during abdominal surgery. Anesth Analg 1995: 80: 543-547. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 197. Ngai SH. Action of general anesthetics in producing muscle relaxation: Interaction of anesthetics with relaxants. In: Katz RL ed. Muscle relaxants. New York: North Holland Publ., 1975: 279-297. [Context Link] 198. Gissen AJ, Karis JH, Nastuk WL. Effects of halothane on neuromuscular transmission. JAMA 1966: 197: 770774. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 199. Kennedy RD, Galindo AD. Comparative site of action of various anesthetic agents at the mammalian myoneural junction. Br J Anaesth 1975: 47: 533-540. [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 200. Waud BE, Waud DR. The effect of diethyl ether, enflurane and isoflurane at the neuromuscular junction. Anesthesiology 1975: 42: 275-280. [Fulltext Link] [Medline Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 201. Miller RD, Eger EI II, Way WL, Stevens WC, Dolan WM. Comparative neuromuscular effects of forane and halothane alone and in combination with d-tubocurarine in man. Anesthesiology 1971: 35: 38-42. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 202. Brett RS, Dilger JP, Yland KF. Isoflurane causes 'flickering'of the acetylcholine receptor channel: Observations using patch clamp. Anesthesiology 1988: 69: 161-170. [Context Link] 203. Viby-Mogensen J. Neuromuscular monitoring. In: Miller RD ed. Anesthesia. New York: Churchill Livingstone, 1990: 1209-1226. [Context Link] 204. Debaene B, Beaussier M, Meistelman C, Donati F, Lienhart A. Monitoring the onset of neuromuscular block at the orbicularis oculi can predict good intubating conditions during atracurium-induced neuromuscular block. Anesth Analg 1995: 80: 360-363. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 205. Pansard J-L, Chauvin M, Lebrault C, Gauneau P, Duvaldestin P. Effect of an intubating dose of succinylcholine and atracurium on the diaphragm and the adductor pollicis muscle in humans. Anesthesiology 1987: 67: 326-330. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 206. Donati F, Meistelman C, Plaud B. Vecuronium neuromuscular blockade at the adductor muscles of the larynx and adductor pollicis. Anesthesiology 1991: 74: 833-837. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 207. Tammisto T, Toikka O. Spontaneous EMG activity for detection of arousal during general anaesthesia comparison between recordings from frontal and neck musculature. Eur J Anaesthesiol 1991: 8: 109-114. [Context Link] 208. Edmonds HL Jr, Yli-Hankala A, Heine MF, Tsueda K, Strickland TJ Jr. Anesthetic adequacy, surface EMG and quantitated EEG. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1993: 37: 103-104. [Context Link] 209. Glass PSA, Doherty M, Jacobs JR, Goodman D, Smith LR. Plasma concentration of fentanyl, with 70% nitrous oxide, to prevent movement at skin incision. Anesthesiology 1993: 78: 842-843. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 210. Ausems ME, Hug CC, Stanski DR, Burm AGL. Plasma concentrations of alfentanil required to supplement nitrous oxide anesthesia for general surgery. Anesthesiology 1986: 65: 362-273. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 211. Vuyk J, Lim T, Engbers FHM, Burm AGL, Vletter AA, Bovill JG. Pharmacodynamics of alfentanil as a supplement to propofol or nitrous oxide for lower abdominal surgery in female patients. Anesthesiology 1993: 78: 1036-1045. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 212. Philbin DM, Rosow CE, Schneider RC, Koski G, D'Ambra MN. Fentanyl and sufentanil anesthesia revisited: How much is enough? Anesthesiology 1990: 73: 5-11. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 213. Spelina KR, Coates DP, Monk CR, Prys-Roberts C, Norley I, Turtle MJ. Dose requirements of propofol by infusion during nitrous oxide anaesthesia in man. Br J Anaesth 1986: 58: 1080-1084. [CrossRef] [Context Link] 214. Sear JW. Continuous infusions of hypnotic agents for maintenance of anaesthesia. In: Kay B, ed. Total intravenous anaesthesia. Amsterdam: Elsevier Publ., 1991: 15-56. [Context Link] 215. Eger EI II, Bahlman SH. Is end-tidal anesthetic partial pressure an accurate measure of the arterial anesthetic partial pressure? Anesthesiology 1971: 35: 301-303. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] 216. Chortkoff BS, Eger EI II, Crankshaw DP, Gonsowski CT, Dutton RC, Ionescu P. Concentrations of desflurane and propofol that suppress response to command in humans. Anesth Analg 1995: 81: 737-743. [Fulltext Link] [CrossRef] [Context Link] Keywords: Anesthesia: general; anesthetics: volatile, intravenous; complications: awareness; memory: explicit, implicit; monitoring: EEG, evoked potentials, peripheral nerve stimulation; neuromuscular blockade.