Donald Winnicott Some wiki-factoids:Lived 1896 – 1971 in England, and eventually became President of the British Psychoanalytic Association. Variously described as a pediatrician, psychiatrist, sociologist, and psychoanalyst, gave BBC talks. Apart from a distinct way of describing child development, he introduced the following key terms: holding environment, transitional objects, and the true and false self. Your two hand-outs are intended to give you an overview of Winnicott’s version of developmentalism, and so you have here another critical object that falls into our second category – and into Walkerdine’s sights. But as with any developmental theory, to really start your critique you need to grasp the imaginative simplification which typically lies at the heart of your ‘object’. For instance, Freud’s theory is driven by the tension between individual selfish instinct and the survival need to stay within the group, with instinctive maturation forcing the pace. Piaget’s theory emphasises the individual’s exploratory capacity in relation to an environment, and the individual’s capacity to react to that environment with increasing sophistication so that both the individual, and its relationship to the environment, becomes ever more complex – again, a form of instinctive maturity forces the pace, but are his ‘schema’ really instincts. With Winnicott, you may find it helpful to compare what he says about transitional objects with Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development. Both men seem less interested in maturational changes and more interested in how social contexts are ‘internalised’ – assuming, therefore, that their explanations hold true for the whole of maturation. So perhaps the starting point for today is something like this: while Piaget is interested in environments of all sorts, and has been accused of not taking the social dimension very seriously, Vygotsky clearly does take it very seriously indeed, as does Winnicott. The most striking difference between Vygotsky and Winnicott is that Winnicott locates the focus of his theory in the experiences of much younger children than does Vygotsky. However, despite their differences, both use their particular explanations as ways of also stressing the significance of play and creativity. For Winnicott, then, the greatest mystery of early childhood life is how children come to recognise the meaningfulness of things how, in other words, they come to accept that their entire experience is not only meaningful to them but - by a process of mediation using abstract symbols – how they can share experiences with others. Clearly this involves language, but Winnicott’s focus is much more general – he wants to explain (like the French psychoanalysts Jacques Lacan) how we enter the whole of the symbolic world, how we maintain ourselves there, how we may develop within it – and of course, being a psychoanalyst, how such developments can go wrong. N.B. If you do decide to make psychological developmentalism your critical object, you might find it helpful to ask yourself a) how a particular theory suggests development can go wrong, and b) how this ‘wrong’ development expresses itself. This last point introduces a new idea that we have not really discussed, although we touched upon it last week in relation to Autism. Each of these developmental theory describes a process of supposedly ‘normal’ development, and by implication also says something about ‘abnormal’ development. This falls very much within the scope of Foucault’s writings ( as Wakerdine indicates in her chapter). Here, perhaps rather than reaching for his Discipline and Punish, you may find it illuminating (and not too hard) to look at another of his books: Madness and Civilisation. There are others by Foucault and more contemporary authors - Emile and I can advise. Please also recognise that you may find parallel work that you have done, or are doing, with Stephanie, Simon, or Wayne relevant here – do please make use of this in your second assignment if you find it helpful to bring it into your discussion and analysis. More information on Winnicott’s technical vocabulary. Three matters of context:Firstly, the group of psychoanalysts he worked with during the Forties and Fifties included John Bowlby – take a second look at the YouTube clip and ask yourself if what Bowlby is saying still seems consistent with the kinds of descriptions Sarah Blaffer-Hrdy wants to make. Secondly, during the Second World War, he was employed as a consultant psychiatrist for the evacuee programme – the planned evacuation of all children away from the urban centres most likely to be bombed – typically, these children were taken into rural and small-town communities that were very different from the communities from which they had come. Inevitably there were many instances in which the children were very upset and failed to ‘settle down’ and these were Winnicott’s first patients. Thirdly, he became an early and popular ‘guru’ on the BBC during the Fifties – some criticism has suggested that his emphasis on the importance of the mother was used by the authorities as propaganda to get women to stay at home, i.e. to free-up places in the work force. A Holding Environment Winnicott’s concept of the ‘good-enough mother’ assumes what might be described as an ordinary level of devotion and loving care which Winnicott identifies as the most basic foundation for psychological health. An aspect of this is the ‘holding environment’, and Winnicott describes in unusual detail the processes by which a baby is picked up, handled, bathed, cleaned, played with, etc. – all of which contributes to the baby’s first idea of the mother. He argues that the child’s sense of its own embodiment – its recognition of its own body as the place where its experience is focussed – cannot develop adequately without the experience of its own unrecognised dependency being met by consistent forms of loving handling so as to allow in to continue living within an illusion of omnipotence until it can begin to interact in initially limited ways with the environment unaided by the mother (see also slide 10). More on the ‘holding environment’: Winnicott also uses the concept as an extended metaphor to explain a necessary pre-condition for all subsequent development. He argues that as the child develops, so too do its forms of dependency. For normal development to take place there has to be an equivalent to the infant’s initial holding environment, only now this holding is done by other carers, by siblings, by the family, and eventually extended social groups, such as school, university, and work-place. (Compare this with the Bowlby/Blaffer-Hrdy notion of the environment of evolutionary adaptedness. You might also find certain novels illuminating here, for example, J. P. Hartley’s The Shrimp and the Anemone introduces a form of holding by a sibling, the start of Jeannette Winterson’s Oranges are not the Only Fruit introduces an extended evangelical social group, while the beginning of Gunter Grass’ The Tin Drum leads you back to the Second World War and a truly extraordinary version of ‘holding’.) The Anti-Social Tendency: An implication of Winnicott’s extended concept of holding is that dependency itself changes as the individual matures. This entails that the form of holding appropriate to a particular state of dependency must also change, i.e., the mother, or the family, or the school, may no longer offer an adequate form of holding. Anti-social behaviour is therefore, at root, a cry for help, a search for a new form of holding, and also an expression of a sense of loss of a previously successful social integration – a lost balance between need and response. Rather like Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development and creative play, Winnicott sees creative play as a strategy by which the child constructs for itself a form of holding environment. The Sense of Being – the major ‘driver’ of the Winnicott’s theory: Overview: Without an adequate initial holding environment the child’s sense of being can be lost and this means, according to Winnicott, that instead of a true sense of being developing a false one will. The characteristic aspect of this falsity will be various forms of docile compliance relative to whichever holding environment the child is currently experiencing. This diagnosis implies that the development and maintenance of a true sense of being will always result in a level of conflict between individuals within the relevant holding environment. (It may be helpful to compare this with Rousseau’s notion of amour-de-soi and its development.) Perhaps one of the most interesting further comments Winnicott makes in relation to the notion of the false self is his suggestion that as development proceeds so too does play, and play staves off the threat of developing a false self relative to a particular form of dependency – recall again Vygotsky’s comments on play – and see also slide 12. Playing and Reality: Winnicott described playing as taking place in the potential ‘space’ between the ‘baby’ and the mother figure – the quotes are here to remind you that for Winnicott the structure of critical events in early infancy give a specific form to all subsequent equivalent developments. Initially, the play situation refers to the good-enough mother’s readiness to initiate play with the baby and for the baby to recognise this initiation as coming from a trusted mother figure. Given the baby’s initially weak sense of self, much depends on the nature of the mother’s responsiveness – indeed, for Winnicott the whole momentum of development is dependent on the mother’s behaviour and attitude. The good-enough mother is consistently responsive to the infant, allowing its sense of self to develop through an illusion of omnipotence which she can at least temporarily sustain. The true sense of self can only flourish in an environment that relies on the mother’s optimal responsiveness to the infant’s expressions. (In relation to later forms of play, its creativity therefore entails the recreation of a ‘maternal’ environment.) Transitional phenomena: In play Winnicott identifies the ‘transitional object’ – an object that is not-me and yet not not-me either – a familiar example would be the favourite teddy bear or doll, etc. which the child needs to an almost obsessive degree in times of stress and almost always when going to sleep. Winnicott argued that it was the means by which the child coped with separation – and as the child developed, the identification of its equivalent was a vital ingredient in healthy development. Only through this structure of experience could the child (or adult) become strong enough to withstand being denied access to an appropriate holding environment. But like Vygotsky, Winnicott recognised the positive and creative aspects of play, rather than just its capacity to act as a form of self-administered therapy for stress. In these situations, it was the child’s interpretative leap into new holding environments that was at issue, and the transitional object – whether found or self-made – became the means by which the shock of the new was mediated in a transitional space so that the child could reach towards it from the familiarities of its present holding formation. For Winnicott, then, play led to creativity, and creativity led to the opening up of new possibilities – new ways of being. Given the enormous importance of imitation in human learning, what Winnicott had to say about those situations when the transitional object failed to materialise may seem odd. He argued that just as in early infancy, where a false sense of being is a constant developmental danger, in play lacking a transitional object one ends with a child (or adult) making imitative leaps that lacks substance silly or embarrassing impersonations, assumed forms of maturity which cannot be sustained, i.e., a developed form of infant compliance. These comments seem Rousseauesque – as does his view that a weak sense of true being exposed one to the risk of falling prey to the expectations of others. Given the significance of the true self, it is helpful to develop the concept a little further. For Winnicott, the true self is close to that residuum that even Locke recognised as making the essence of an individual – but in fact Winnicott followed Freud in wanting to think of this as each individual’s unique patterning of instinct. But note especially Winnicott’s descriptive words – integrity, connected wholeness – these characterise an ‘authentic’ sense of aliveness. Rousseau’s Émile offers few grounds for compromise until Émile has become a self sufficient adult – Winnicott is more flexible. He extends the notion of the false self so as to use it as a means to healthily cope with the kinds of social sacrifices Freud wrote about in Civilisation and its Discontents. A healthy false self allows for social compromise without falsifying the integrity of the true self, but in each of this there is a continuum between the two kinds of self, and the false sense can become a source of danger. The false self presents a mask, or persona (character) which attempts to anticipate the demands made by the social situation on the individual in order to maintain social position – and clearly, one is really describing the experience of most adolescents in the family home! For the infant matters may be more serious. If the mother is not goodenough, she is unable to respond adequately to the individuality of her infant’s needs and instead looks for the reproduction of her own gestures, attitudes, etc. Repeated compliance by the infant becomes the grounds for a false sense of being becoming misunderstood for the true self. If this happens, an entire set of social relations can be built up by the child until it ends by living in a world which lacks personal reality – the child grows up to be just like its mother, brother, or whoever dominated this initial holding environment at the time.