James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 1 of 48 Chapter Six Vision James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 2 of 48 General Principles of Perception • • Water strikes iron and iron experiences rust, not water Light from a tree leaf strikes your eye and you experience green, not colorless light – visual receptors are specialized to absorb light and transduce it into an electrochemical pattern in the brain – not a duplicate, picture-like pattern of the object James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 3 of 48 Law of Specific Nerve Energies • • Müller (1838): the neuron action potential always conveys the same kind of information – brain “sees” the activity of optic neurons and “hears” the activity of the auditory neurons Von Melchner, Pallas & Sur (2000) – routed the optic nerve on left side of ferret’s immature brain to the auditory area of the thalamus – the adult ferret “saw” activity of optic nerve in the thalamus and cortex normally used for auditory input James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 4 of 48 Figure 6.1 Figure 6.1 Behavior of a ferret with a rewired temporal cortex. First the normal (right) hemisphere is trained to respond to a red light by turning to the right. Then the rewired (left) hemisphere is tested with a red light. The fact that the ferret turns to the right indicates that it regards the stimulus as light, not sound. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 5 of 48 The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain • • • Pupil: opening in the center of the eye that allows light to pass through Lens: focuses the light on the retina Retina: back surface of the eye that is lined by visual receptors – light from above strikes bottom and light from below strikes top – light from left strikes right side and vice versa James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 6 of 48 Figure 6.2 Figure 6.2 Cross section of the vertebrate eye. Note how an object in the visual field produces an inverted image on the retina. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 7 of 48 Route Within the Retina • • Light passes through ganglion and bipolar cells, without distortion, to visual receptors – bipolar cells receive input from visual receptors – ganglion cells receive input from bipolar cells – amacrine cells exchange information with bipolar cells and send information to ganglion and other amacrine cells • provides many options for complex processing of information Optic nerve is made up of axons of ganglion cells – the point where optic nerve leaves the eye does not have receptors and is our blind spot James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 8 of 48 Figure 6.3 Figure 6.3 Visual path within the eyeball. The receptors send their messages to bipolar and horizontal cells, which in turn send messages to the amacrine and ganglion cells. The axons of the ganglion cells loop together to exit the eye at the blind spot. They form the optic nerve, which continues to the brain. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 9 of 48 Fovea and Periphery of Retina • • Macula: 3mm X 5mm center of retina with greatest ability to resolve detail – fovea, the center of the macula, provides most detail – each visual receptor has direct pathway to the brain through to one bipolar cell and one midget ganglion cell • provides exact location of a point of light Periphery of retina provides better sensitivity to dim light – multiple receptors converge onto bipolar and ganglion cells – can’t detect exact location or shape of light but convergence enables detection of very faint light James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 10 of 48 Figure 6.5 Figure 6.5 Types of cells in the vertebrate retina. Note the huge variation among amacrine cells, which have diverse functions. Note also the variation among ganglion cells. Those in and near the fovea have a narrow span of dendrites and receive input from few receptors. Those in the periphery integrate input from a wide field of receptors. (Source: Masland, 2000.) James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 11 of 48 Fovea and Periphery of Retina cont. • • Many bird species have two foveas per eye – one pointing ahead and one pointing to the side Visual receptors in some predators and prey are designed to facilitate survival – hawks have greater density on top half (looking down) than on the bottom half (looking up) – rats have greater density on the bottom half (looking up) James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 12 of 48 Rods and Cones • • Rods: visual receptors that are abundant in the periphery of the retina – respond best to low light conditions – bleached by bright light Cones: visual receptors that are abundant in and around the fovea – respond best to bright light conditions – essential for color vision James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 13 of 48 Rods and Cones cont. • • • • 100 million cones and 6 million rods But rods send 10 times more responses to brain than cones Each cone has direct line to brain while many rods share same line Both rods and cones contain photopigments, chemicals that release energy when struck by light – light is absorbed and 11-cis-retinal is converted to alltrans-retinal James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 14 of 48 Color Vision • The trichromatic, or Young-Helmholtz, theory – we can perceive color only within the visible wavelengths of light, ranging from 400-700 nm (violet to red) – we see a specific color by comparing responses from 3 kinds of cones, each most sensitive to a short, medium, or long wavelength of light • Ex: blue-green excites short wavelength to 15% of maximum, medium to 65% and long to 40% James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 15 of 48 Color Vision cont. • The trichromatic, or Young-Helmholtz, theory cont, – fewer short wavelength cones (blue) so we see red, yellow, and green colors better – when all 3 cones are equally active we see white or gray – Incomplete theory, e.g., can’t explain negative color afterimage James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 16 of 48 Figure 6.9 Figure 6.9 A beam of light separated into wavelengths. Although the wavelengths vary over a continuum, we perceive them as several distinct colors. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 17 of 48 Color Vision cont. • The opponent-process theory – brain sees color on a continuum from red to green and another from yellow to blue – we perceive color in terms of “paired opposites” redgreen, black-white and yellow-blue • explains why we can’t see reddish green or bluish yellow • explains negative color afterimages – not a complete explanation since afterimages depend not only on the retina but also on the area of the brain that produces it James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 18 of 48 Figure 6.13 Figure 6.13 Possible wiring for one bipolar cell. Short-wavelength light produces more excitation than inhibition and the result is seen as blue. Other wavelengths produce mostly inhibition, perceived as yellow. White light produces about equal excitation and inhibition. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 19 of 48 Color Vision cont. • The retinex theory – color perception requires some reasoning – the cortex compares information from various areas of the retina to determine the brightness and color perception for each area • color constancy: we see the right colors despite lighting changes, e.g., we subtract green tint to see white house and red rose but we only see green house if viewed in isolation • brightness requires a comparison with other objects James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 20 of 48 Color Vision Deficiency • Inability to perceive color differences – genetic: lack of short- medium- or long-wavelength cones • some people lack two types of cones • some people have low number of all three – inability to distinguish red from green is most common deficiency • recessive gene on X chromosome • 8% in men and 1% in women James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 21 of 48 The Mammalian Visual System • Within the eyeball – rods and cones synapse to horizontal cells and bipolar cells – horizontal cells make inhibitory synapse onto bipolar cells – bipolar cells synapse to amacrine and ganglion cells – axons of the ganglion cells leave the back of the eye James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 22 of 48 The Mammalian Visual System cont. • The inside half of the axons of each eye cross over in the optic chiasm – most visual information goes through the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus – some goes to the superior colliculus – lateral geniculate inputs to other parts of thalamus and to visual areas of cerebral cortex, which sends back axons to modify input • number of neurons within this loop varies widely among people by a factor of 2 or 3 James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 23 of 48 Figure 6.17 Figure 6.17 Major connections in the visual system of the brain. Part of the visual input goes to the thalamus and from there to the visual cortex. Another part of the visual input goes to the superior colliculus. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 24 of 48 Mechanisms of Processing in the Visual System • Receptive field: the point in space from which incoming light strikes a receptor – receptors have both excitatory and inhibitory regions since receptive field is normally an array of light patterns • Ex: light in center of ganglion cell might be excitatory, with the surround inhibitory James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 25 of 48 Figure 6.18 Figure 6.18 Receptive fields. The receptive field of a receptor is simply the area of the visual field that strikes that receptor. For any other cell in the visual system, the receptive field is the collective receptors feeding the neural pathway to the cell. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 26 of 48 Mechanisms of Processing in the Visual System cont. • Lateral Inhibition: each active receptor and it’s visual path tends to inhibit the visual path of neighboring receptors – an active receptor excites both a bipolar and horizontal cell; in turn, horizontal cell inhibits bipolar cell, but net potential is excitatory on bipolar – but, horizontal cell does inhibit neighboring bipolar cells on border of visual field – effect is to heighten contrast: receptors inside visual field are excited and those on border tend to be inhibited James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 27 of 48 Figure 6.20 Figure 6.20 An illustration of lateral inhibition. Do you see dark diamonds at the “crossroads” due to inhibition from all four corners of intersection? James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 28 of 48 Retina and Lateral Geniculate Pathways • • Parvocellular: smaller ganglion cell bodies and small receptive fields, located near fovea – detect visual detail and color – all axons go to lateral geniculate nucleus Magnocellular: larger ganglion cell bodies and receptive fields, distributed fairly evenly throughout retina – respond to moving stimuli and patterns – not color sensitive – most axons go to lateral geniculate nucleus James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 29 of 48 Retina and Lateral Geniculate Pathways cont. • • Koniocellular: small ganglion cell bodies that occur throughout the retina – many functions – axons go to lateral geniculate nucleus, thalamus and superior colliculus Many different types of ganglion cells implies analysis of information from the beginning James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 30 of 48 Pathways in Cerebral Cortex • • • Most visual information from lateral geniculate nucleus goes to primary visual cortex (V1) – first stage of visual processing Output of V1 goes to secondary visual cortex (V2) – second stage of visual processing which transmits visual information to additional areas – feedback loop to V1 – V1 and V2 also exchange information with other cortical areas and thalamus 30-40 visual areas reported in brain of macaque monkey James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 31 of 48 Pathways in Cerebral Cortex cont. • Magnocellular and parvocellular paths split into three paths – Magnocellular path • ventral branch to temporal cortex is sensitive to movement • dorsal branch to parietal cortex integrates vision with action – Parvocellular path to temporal cortex is sensitive to details of shape – Mixed parvo/magnocellular path to temporal cortex is sensitive to brightness and color James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 32 of 48 Figure 6.21 Figure 6.21 Three visual pathways in the cerebral cortex. (a) A pathway originating mainly from magnocellular neurons. (b) A mixed magnocellular/parvocellular pathway. (c) A mainly parvocellular pathway. Neurons are only sparsely connected with neurons of other pathways. (Sources: Based on DeYoe, Felleman, Van Essen, & McClendon, 1994; Ts’o & Roe, 1995; Van Essen & DeYoe, 1995.) James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 33 of 48 Pathways in Cerebral Cortex cont. • • Visual paths in temporal cortex form the ventral stream – the “what” path, specialized for identifying and recognizing objects – if damaged, we can find and pick up objects but cannot describe them Visual path in parietal cortex is the dorsal stream – the “where” or “how” path, helps motor system find objects, move toward them and pick them up – if damaged, we can describe object but can’t find and pick up object James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 34 of 48 The Shape Pathway in the Cerebral Cortex • • • Simple cells (fixed, small receptive field) – have fixed excitatory and inhibitory zones – most have bar- or edge-shaped receptive fields Complex cells (large receptive field) – respond to a particular orientation anywhere within large receptive field – receive input from combination of simple cells End-stopped or hypercomplex cells – resemble complex cells with a strong inhibitory area at one end of its bar-shaped receptive field James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 35 of 48 Figure 6.24 Figure 6.24 The receptive field of a complex cell in the visual cortex. Like a simple cell’s, its response depends on the angle of orientation of a bar of light. However, a complex cell responds the same for a bar in any position within the receptive field. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 36 of 48 Shape Pathway in the Cerebral Cortex cont. • • Cells within a column process similar information, e.g, they may respond best to lines of a single orientation Are visual cortex cells in V1 and V2 feature detectors? – yes, prolonged exposure fatigues cells – but, cells that respond to single bar respond more to gratings of bars or lines suggesting neurons respond to spatial frequencies, not bars or edges – perhaps a series of spatial frequency detectors could represent anything we see – still not a full explanation and object perception is still a puzzle James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 37 of 48 Shape Analysis Beyond Area V1 • • • In area V2 some cells respond to circles, lines at right angles or other complex patterns In V4, many cells respond to the slant of a line in threedimensional space Shape constancy detected in inferior temporal complex – responds selectively to complex shapes – detects objects based on shape, not light or darkness – responds equally strong to mirror images • but that may interfere with reading where we need to treat mirror image letters differently, e.g, b and d James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 38 of 48 Figure 6.27 Figure 6.27 Three transformations of an original drawing. In the inferior cortex, cells that respond strongly to the original respond about the same to the contrast reversal and mirror image but not to the figure-ground reversal. Note that the figure-ground reversal resembles the original very strongly in terms of the pattern of light and darkness; however, it is not perceived as the same object. (Source: Based on Baylis and Driver, 2002.) James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 39 of 48 Disorders of Object Recognition • • Visual agnosia: inability to recognize some objects – can describe object but doesn’t know what they are, e.g., key, stethoscope, smoking pipe Prosopagnosia: inability to recognize faces – can still read and recognize person by their voice – inferior temporal cortex area, fusiform gyrus, especially active in recognition of faces • Also activated when recognizing other complex shapes, e.g., cars and birds James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 40 of 48 Color, Depth and Motion Perception • • • Cells sensitive to color are found in parts of V1 known as blobs, which also have cells that contribute to brightness perception Area V4 or nearby is important for color constancy – monkeys with damage here can’t find yellow banana if light is changed from white to blue Cells of magnocellular path are specialized for stereoscopic depth perception James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 41 of 48 Color, Depth and Motion Perception cont. • • Cells in area MT respond selectively to stimulus moving in a particular direction regardless of size, shape or color – motion blind people who cannot determine if objects are moving may have damage here Cells on MST respond best to expansion, contraction or rotation of large visual scene James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 42 of 48 Figure 6.33 Figure 6.33 Stimuli that excite certain cells in the ventral part of area MST. Cells in this area respond when an object moves relative to its background. They therefore react either when the background is steady and the object moves or when the object and the background move. James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 43 of 48 Visual Attention • Attention is dependent on amount and duration of activity in a cortical area – a brief response to stimulus produces activity in V1 area – focused attention produces additional activity in V2 area – similar focus on color or motion produces additional activity in visual cortex area responsible for color and motion perception James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 44 of 48 The Binding Problem Revisited • • • How does visual cortex bind color, shape and movement to an object, e.g., a rabbit and bring it into consciousness? – evidence for synchronized activity in both hemispheres when an object is recognized Some visual processing without consciousness – “blindsight”: loss of visual field and person can still point out objects or light in the blind field • some healthy tissue may remain to provide blindsight Dominant hypothesis is that consciousness is distributed over several cortical areas James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 45 of 48 Infant Vision • • • Infants two days old already prefer to look at faces, circles and stripes than patternless displays Great difficulty in shifting attention up until about 6 months In newborn mammals many properties will develop even if the eyes are damaged or raised in darkness – but if darkness continues, these properties diminish – visual experience is required to maintain and fine tune connections James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 46 of 48 Early Development • • In newborn kitten, lack of stimulation: – of one eye for 4-6 weeks and that eye became blind – of both eyes up to three weeks still left cortex responsive • but if for a longer period of time, loss of sharp receptive fields is noted Sensitive or critical period for normal vision – if congenital blindness is not restored for years, newly gained vision is almost useless – removal of cataracts within 6 months of birth still leaves deficits James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 47 of 48 Early Development cont. • • Vision can be restored when kitten is deprived of vision in one eye for only a few days – but, kitten recovers better if normal eye is covered – suggests using patch over normal eye for amblyopia Alternating normal stimulation one eye at a time and cortex learns to respond to one or the other, resulting in loss of binocular vision – accounts for strabismus, where eyes point in different directions, in humans James W. Kalat Biological Psychology, 8th Edition Chapter 6: Vision 48 of 48 Early Development cont. • • • Kitten became responsive only to horizontal lines when wearing goggles with horizontal lines during sensitive period – similar to development of astigmatism in humans, which can be corrected if glasses are used early Kitten became motion blind when raised with only strobe lighting for 4-6 month period Kitten receiving no visual stimuli became more responsive to auditory and tactile stimuli than normal cats – more true for children or infants than when blindness occurs in adults