Basic Color Theory Susan Farnand farnand@cis.rit.edu Tell me about color: How would you define color? How does color happen? Where does color happen? Why do things look colorful? At least two things needed for color to happen: A source… …and a sensor Visible Light Spectrum 400 nm 700 nm Light Sources Compared 350 typical daylight incandescent mercury vapor lamp 300 6500K fluorescent Relative Spectral Power 250 200 150 100 50 0 300 400 500 600 Wavelength, nm 700 800 Basic Structure of the Human Eye Schematic of the Retina Photoreceptors Horizontal, bipolar, and amacrine cells Ganglion cells Incoming light Relative Spectral Sensitivity of Cones & Rods Relative Sensitivity 100 S rods M L 50 0 350 450 550 Wavelength, nm 650 750 Spectral sensitivity Rods S Cones R ML Color Mixing–Additive Adding light Mixing light sources Red, Green, Blue primaries Additive Color Mixing: Color Mixing–Additive Adding light Mixing light sources Red, Green, Blue primaries Can you think of examples of systems using additive light Color Mixing– Subtractive Removing light Mixing dyes, pigments or other colorants Cyan, magenta, & yellow primaries Can you think of examples of systems using subtractive light? What happens to the light? Transmitted Absorbed Reflected Subtractive Color Mixing Measuring Color Are these two patches the same? Reflected Light Reflectacne 100 50 0 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 Wavelength, nm 650 700 750 800 Sensation of Color Light Source Object Observer Light Sources Compared 350 typical daylight incandescent mercury vapor lamp 300 6500K fluorescent Relative Spectral Power 250 200 150 100 50 0 300 400 500 600 Wavelength, nm 700 800 Cone mosaic data from experiment by Hofer, Singer, and Williams (2005). Different sensations from cones with the same photopigment. Journal of Vision, 5(5):5, 444–454. CIE Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage (International Commission on Illumination), an international organization that establishes and maintains standards of light and color. Its system of describing color is based on standardization of illuminants and observers, not physical samples. CIE Illuminants 300 Illuminant A illuminant C 250 illuminant D50 Relative Spectral Power illuminant D65 illuminant D75 200 150 100 50 0 300 400 500 600 Wavelength, nm 700 800 Color Matching Experiment Color Matching Functions Color Matching Experiment Color Matching Functions 1931 Standard Observer 2 CIE 2° x 1.8 CIE 2° y 1.6 CIE 2° z 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 380 480 580 Wavelength, nm 680 Computing CIE Tristimulus Values Illuminant Object Observer The CIE tristimulus values X, Y, and Z are obtained by multiplying together the power (P) of a CIE standard illuminant, the reflectance (R) of the object, and the standard observer functions and then summing the products. Computing CIE Tristimulus Values Doing the math… The CIE tristimulus values X, Y, and Z are obtained by multiplying together the power of a CIE standard illuminant, the reflectance of the object, and the standard observer functions and then summing the products. Colorimetric Match Colorimetric match is defined as when the tristimulus values for two stimuli match: X1 = X2 Y1 = Y2 Sample 1 Z1 = Z2 Sample 2 Relative Sensitivity Metamerism 10 8 6 Patch 1 4 Patch 2 2 0 350 450 550 650 750 Wavelength (nm) Two colored patches that have different reflectance curves may appear the same when viewed under one illuminant, but may appear different when viewed under a second illuminant. Colorimetric Match Colorimetric match is defined as when the tristimulus values for two stimuli match: X1 = X2 Y1 = Y2 Sample 1 Z1 = Z2 Sample 2 Chromaticity Values and the Chromaticity Diagram The CIE tristimulus values X, Y, and Z are can be transformed to chromaticity values (x, y) and displayed on a unit plane is known as the chromaticity diagram. Chromaticity values are calculated by: x = X/(X+Y+Z), y = Y/(X+Y+Z) It is also possible to calculate a chromaticity for z, where z=Z/(X+Y+Z). Since x+y+z = 1, it is redundant (i.e. z=1-x-y) Chromaticity Diagram Color Differences on the 1931 CIE Chromaticity Diagram