Classical Conditioning: Mechanisms and Theory Eyeblink Class Study • • • • 60 conditioning trials (blocks of 20) 7 blocks of 4 probe trials C1, P1, C2, P2, C3, P3, P4, P5, P6, P7 Acquisition, extinction Results: Individual % CR in Block 100 75 50 25 1 2 3 4 Blocks (of 4 probe trials) 5 6 7 Results: Averaged % CR in Block 100 75 50 25 1 2 3 4 Blocks (of 4 probe trials) 5 6 7 Unconditional/Conditional • US: elicits response without training • Cs: elicits response due to training (association) • Not quite so clear-cut Consider • Aversive conditioning: tone (CS), mild shock (US) • Pavlov: mild shock(CS), food (US) • Sign tracking: light (CS), saccharin (US) • Taste aversion: flavour of saccharin (CS), illness (US) Novelty • Prior associations • Familiar vs. unfamiliar stimuli • Not “unlearning” of familiar stimuli, per se • Basically, need to learn something different Latent Inhibition/CS Preexposure Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3 Exp. gr. “CS” alone CS-US test Cont. gr. nothing test CS-US CR magnitude • Highly familiar stimuli more difficult to associate with US than novel stimuli • Preexposure group Cont. Exp. Latent Inhibition • Habituation function • Typically we think of habituating to a US; ambiguity in CS/US designation • Attentional processes • CS- could also explain, but doesn’t suppress responding to other CS+ US Preexposure • Subjects exposed to US before CS-US pairings slower to produce CR • Associative interference (Hall 2008) – Association of contextual CS with US during US preexposure – In essence, need to extinguish context CS to associate novel CS with US • Could this be habituation of US, too? • Test methodology? Ayres, Moore & Vigorito (1984) • Stimulus salience • Stimulus novelty • Conditioned suppression Method • Stimuli – CS: tone, light – US: shock • Stage 1: pair CS with US; suppression ratio • Stage 2: pair second CS (novel or familiar) with US; suppression ratio • Stage 3: extinction of second CS Results Stage 2: 2nd stim. & shock Stage 1: 1st stim. & shock Suppression Ratio 0.5 Stage 3: 2nd stim. extinction Tone 0.4 Light 0.3 0.2 0.1 2 4 6 8 10 1 2 3 1 2 Day Familiar T-T L-L Novel L-T T-L Familiars (T-T & L-L) show less suppression than novels (L-T & T-L): preexposure Salience and Intensity • Salience: significance, noticeability, detectability • Salience and intensity often used synonymously – – – – Low to moderate levels, probably interchangable Consider high level stimulus Physiological damage Not salient, but definitely intense • Better to treat intensity as a component of salience Salience • Increase via: • Intensity • Relevance – Physiological needs – Similarity of environmental stimuli (e.g., naturalistic CS”) Belongingness: Stimuli Relevance • Equipotentiality principle • Pavlov • Any stimulus should, relatively, be equally conditionable with any other stimulus – E.g., CS1 easily associated with US1, should also be easily associated with US2 – Easy-to-easy, hard-to-hard • But doesn’t always work this way • Garcia & Koelling’s work on taste aversion Stimuli Relevance • Biological predispostions; evolved • Pigeons – Visual CS associated more easily than auditory CS with food US – But auditory CS easier than visual CS when shock is US • Fear conditioning in primates (rhesus monkeys, human children) – CS of snake vs. flower Wilcoxon et al. (1971) • • • • • • Biological preparedness in conditioning Rats nocturnal, quails diurnal Taste aversion Blue water, sour water Quails: colour --> stronger CS Rats: taste --> stronger CS Higher Order Second-order First-order CS1 US CR CS2 CS1 CR Third-order CS3 CS2 CR • Few pairings, higher-order • Extensive training, CS• Solution: periodic reconditioning of firstorder Rizley & Rescorla (1972) • Extinction of CS1 does not affect CS2 • CS1 = tone, US = shock, CS2 = light • Experimental group: 1. CS1-US, 2. CS2CS1, 3. extinguish CS1, 4. test CS1 & CS2 • Control group: 1. CS1-US, 2. CS2-CS1, 3. nothing, 4. test CS1 & CS2 Results Acquisition CS2 (light) CS2 (light) test CS2 still shows suppression for both exp. & cont. groups… CS1 (tone) test Suppression Ratio 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 1- 2 Exp. gr. Cont. gr. 3-4 5-6 7-8 1-2 3-4 1-4 5-8 Even though CS1 shows no suppression in exp. group. 9-12 Holland & Rescorla (1975) • • • • CS1 = light, CS2 = tone, US = food CS1-US then CS2-CS1 Then satiate (devalue) US Test: – CS1’s CR weakened – CS2’s CR unaffected • Manipulating CS1-US relationship doesn’t seem to affect CS2’s representation Sensory Preconditioning • Pair two stimuli (e.g., light and tone) • Pair one with US… becomes a CS • Now second stimulus also makes CR Blocking • Pair CS1 and US repeatedly • Make compound CS1-CS2 and keep pairing with US • CS1 gives strong CR • CS2 gives weak CR Value of Classical Conditioning • Preparedness • Evolution, survival mechanisms • Foresight, anticipation Zamble et al. (1985) • Male rats • Give male repeated access to receptive females; pair with explicit CS • With CS, initiates copulation sooner, ejaculates quicker • Competitive advantage over other males Hollis (1984) • • • • Blue gourami Males hold territory Attack intruders Condition light with intruder – Resident attacks intruder sooner – Resident won conflict more often Learning and Homeostasis • • • • Preparation Homeostatic systems Feedback lag in control system Classical conditioning associations can influence homeostatic systems • Prepare for events that will perterb the system • Minimize lag Effects • • • • • Generally, very adaptive However, sometimes difficulties Conditioned compensatory responses Drug tolerance S. Siegel’s work on drug tolerance – Contextual CS prepare opposing CR to maintain homeostasis – Difficulties if contextual CS absent Stimulus Substitution Theory • Pavlov’s theory • Through repeated pairings of the CS and the US the CS becomes a substitute for the US so that all responses initially elicited only by the US are now also produced by the CS Jenkins & Moore (1973) • Pigeons • Food or water as US • CRfood = pecked response key as if eating; rapid pecks with open beak • CRwater = pecked response key as if drinking; slower pecking with beak closed, often with swallowing water food Problems with Stimulus Substitution Theory • CS not a complete substitute for US – E.g., eyeblink differences – Magnitudes • CSs produce different responses – Omissions and additions – E.g., conditioned suppression in rats • US = shock, UR = flinch, CS = tone, CR = freeze • Conditioned compensatory responses What is Learned in Classical Conditioning? • US centre, Response centre, CS centre Nervous System US Response Centre US Centre S-S S-R CS CS Centre Response S-S or S-R Connections? • Stimulus-Stimulus (S-S) Theory US Centre Response Centre – Two associations • Learned CS centre to US centre • Innate US centre to response centre CS Centre • Stimulus-Response (S-R) Theory US Centre – One association • CS centre to response centre CS Centre Response Centre Rescorla’s (1973) Experiment • Habituation to weaken US-response link • Conditioned suppression procedure – Loud noise • Experimental protocol Group Phase 1 Phase 2 Test Habituation Light Noise Noise (habituation) Light: low CR Light Noise No stimuli Light: high CR Control Results Control: high CR Habituation: low CR Supports S-S theory CS/US Influence on CR • Timberlake & Grant (1975) – Second rat as CS in sign tracking – CR --> social, not consumatory behaviour • Akins (2000) – – – – Male quails’ behaviour sequence General or focal search for female CS = visual stimulus, US = female quail CS-US interval short (1 min.) or long (20 min.) Akins (2000) General Search % Time near CS Crossings (Pacing) Focal Search 1 20 CS-US Interval (min.) 1 20 CS-US Interval (min.) Paired CS &US Unpaired CS & US Re: S-S Theory • Requires flexibility in CR-UR relationship • CSs not associated with all aspects of US • CS and US can interact • CR depends on sensory properties of CS and presentation context Rescorla-Wagner Model • Learning is a discrepancy between – Expectation – Occurrence • Level of surprise --> degree of conditioning – More surprising, more learning – Early vs. later trials Mathematical Model Vn = k( - Vn) • V = CS-US associative strength V = change in associative strength per trial • k = salience of stimuli = asymptotic maximum of V (due to US) - V = “suprisingness” R-W and Blocking • CS1 paired with US repeatedly • Vcs1 approaches l • By the time CS2 added, very little associative strength left to be acquired • CS1 very predictive of US; little “surprise” left, so not much need for CS2 Overexpectation Effect • Predicted by Rescorla-Wagner model before being empirically demonstrated Group Phase 1 Phase 2 Test Overexpectation L ... 1 food L+T ... 1 food L T ... 1 food T L ... 1 food no stimuli L Control T ... 1 food T Results moderate CR moderate CR strong CR strong CR Conditioned Inhibition • • • • Excitatory CS paired with inhibitory CS Opponent process system Example: tone = CS+, light = CSStart values: Vtone = 100.0, Vlight = 0.0, k = 0.2, = 0.0 • We define Vsum = Vtone + Vlight • And Vn = k(Vmax - Vsum) CS+ CS- 100 Vn Vtone Vlight Vsum 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 -20.0 -12 -7.2 -4.3 -2.6 -1.6 -0.9 -0.6 -0.5 100 80 68 60.8 56.5 53.9 52.3 51.4 50.8 50.4 0.0 -20 -32 -39.2 -43.5 -46.1 -47.7 -48.6 -49.2 -49.6 100 60 36 22 13 8 5 3 2 0 V1=0.2(0-100)=-20 V2=0.2(0-60)=-12 V3=0.2(0-36)=-7.2 60.8 -39.2 = 22 Associative Strength Trial 80 -20 = 60 Vtone 50 Vsum 0 Vlight -50 Trials CS Preexposure Effect • • • • Preexposure group: give CS alone Control group: pair CS with US Test: how long to get CR in both groups “Habituation” in preexposure group – Learn to pay less attention to CS – CS irrelevant, nonpredictive – Must “unlearn” during test phase CS Preexposure Effect • Not explained by Rescorla-Wagner model • First preexposure trial – No prior conditioning Vn = 0, = 0 • But, something is learned • Salience variable, not constant – Salience of CS decreases during preexposure – Pay less attention to CS Other Issues • Extinction – Not return to zero • CS– Extinguished not by being presented by itself but by extinguishing CS+ • Stimuli must be either CS+ or CS-, not both depending on context Attentional Models • Numerous stimuli in environment • Can’t attend to them all • Selectively process (remove signal from noise) • E.g., cocktail party effect – Attend to one conversation at a time – Your name Mackintosh’s Theory • Treats salience as a variable, not a constant • Consider two stimuli, L and T • If L is a better predictor of the US, then the salienceL will increase and salienceT will decrease • Attend to the more informative stimulus Mackintosh: Blocking • Trained on CSL; salienceL high because CSL is predictive of US • In compound CS phase this means salienceT will drop towards zero • CST will receive little attention, hence the weak CR Pearce & Hall’s Theory • CSs become ineffective whenever the US is already well predicted • If situation changes so that US is surprising then more learning about the CS • Attention to CS depends on surprisingness of US on previous trial • Assume surprisingness of US will alter attention paid to CSs on subsequent trials – Contrast to R-W (surprisingness of US on trial determines what is learned on that same trial) Attentional Theories and Blocking • On first compound CS trial, CS2 should be quite surprising (previously only CS1 paired with US) • Should result in heightened attention to (and learning about) CS2 on subsequent trials • But, CS1 blocks learning about CS2 on first compound CS trial Temporal Coding Hypothesis • Contiguity • ISI: short delay vs. long delay and trace • ITI: generally, stronger CR with it is spaced further apart • CS duration: can also influence learning • Learn not only that CS is paired with US but also when the US will occur • US = food CS = noise CR = time at food cup • Two CS durations (trial duration = T) of 10 or 20 seconds • Six it is (15 to 960 seconds) • Results explainable by I/T ratio Time at Food Cup (CR) Holland (2000) 1.5 3 6 12 I/T Ratio T = 10 sec. T = 20 sec. 24 48 Rate Estimation Theory • Extension of relative-waiting-time hypothesis – CS only informative about US if you spend less time waiting for US when CS is present • Nonassociative theory • CRs reflect subjects estimates of rate of US presentations during CS and absence of CS • Doesn’t fit will with neurophysiological data on associative learning • Heavy computational burden – May work in controlled (restricted) laboratory environment, but in real world environment? Comparator Hypothesis • Traditional interpretation is that blocking from failure to learn about CS2 • Comparator assumes that subject learns about CS2, but ability to respond is blocked • Revaluation effects • Extinguishing CS1 can result in CS2 now producing a CR Comparator Hypothesis • Theory of performance, not learning • Condioned responding depends on: – Associations between CS and US – Associations between US and other stimuli (comparator cues; may include experimental context) Comparator Hypothesis • Only allows formation of excitatory associations with US • Excitation or inhibition determined by relative strengths of excitatory conditioning to target CS as opposed to other comparator stimuli • If excitatory value of CS greater than that of comparator stimuli, then CS+; if lower, then CS• In essence, another opponent process model