Riskperception – modeller och principer The Swedish Risk Academy Annual Meeting May 14, 2013 Lennart Sjöberg Center for Risk Research Stockholm School of Economics Sweden Outline • • • • • • • • • • Why study risk perception? Experimental work Factors in risk perception Specifics of risk perception: example of chemicals Trust and antagonism Risk targets Demand for risk mitigation Affect (attitude) and emotions Experts and the public Conclusions 2 As you will see, my research questions the “received view” from well-known work on risk perception, critical comments are invited Why research on risk perception? • Risk is a very common issue in policy deliberations • This is true both for decision makers, experts and the public • Several risk related issues have created great economic and political turbulence • There is therefore a need to know more about how people perceive and react to risks 4 Risk perception (RP) and risk communication (RC) • Beliefs (”perceptions”) constitute the basis: What RC should be about • Beliefs are both the motives for RC and targets of RC • The sucess of RC is therefore dependent on the validity of RP models applied 5 Some experimental work • Does exposure to well crafted “risk movies” lead to availability of the risks depicted, and hence to increased perceived risks – To our surprise no such effect could be found! • A similar theme was studied in a quasiexperiment in 5 countries where we followed media reports on nuclear risks 10 years after the Chernobyl accident. Intense media attention should have increased availability and hence perceived risk. – To our surprise no such effect could be found! Interpretation • What looks like availability effects is really effects of new information, cp. the news about “mad cow disease” in the spring of 1996. This was NEW information and it had great RP effects. • Just bringing up an old and well known risk does not make it see more threatening. Data sources (examples) from our work on risk perception surveys at the Center for Risk Research in Stockholm • Several projects on attitudes and perceived risks with regard to nuclear power and nuclear waste – both EU and Swedish data • EU project on chemicals in consumer products • Perceived risk of food, genetically modified food, alcohol and smoking • Political and social risks of EU membership • Perceived risks of terrorism • Selected results from surveys will be presented, but first some experimental work on availability… 8 Surveys: Factors in risk perception • ‘Interfering with nature’ is an important factor and so are moral aspects • Reactions to new technology are not driven by ‘novelty’ per se but by other factors, such as perceived benefit, or whether the technology brings about unique advantages and is hard to replace • Social trust is important, but epistemic trust, trust in Science, is even more so • Attitude or ”affect” plays an important role • Risk sensitivity is an aspect of individual differences which is quite important – some people rate risks as large, others rate them as small • Attitude to precautionary policy is another important factor • Various hazards, some new (such as terrorism), require their own specific factors 9 10 Typically social trust (in experts or organisations) has only a weak effect on perceived risk – correlations of 0.3 or less But at the aggregate level it is easier to see a relationship. See graph based on surveys of chemical risks, next slide 11 12 Factors beyond social trust • Trust in science, as distinct from social trust, has a stronger effect – epistemic trust • Level of education is also important • Another important factor is perceived antagonism 13 Effect of social trust is mediated by epistemic trust (model of nuclear waste risk) Risk sensitivity 0.28 -0.23 -0.22 R2=0.41 R2=0.37 0.35 0.38 Social trust -0.50 Epistemic trust R2=0.56 -0.51 Perceived risk -0.19 0.12 Antagonism GFI=0.95, AGFI=0.93, RMSEA=0.030 14 Conclusion about trust • Epistemic trust is more important than social trust • The effect of social trust is mediated by epistemic trust • In other words: trust in people and institutions is important to the extent that it promotes belief in the substance of their message 15 Risk target: Whose ‘risk’ – more specifically? • Personal and general risk differ both as to level and rank order –General risk is important for lifestyle (smoking etc.,) –Personal for environmental risks, and technology hazards • Research shows that such risk ratings with a non-specified target are close to general risk • But, general risk is not the most relevant in policy contexts 16 Ge ne ra l risks ra te d a s m uch la rge r tha n pe rsona l risks datfromhepublic. Mean general risk 6 5 4 3 Regression line 2 y=x 1 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Mean personal risk 17 Personal and general risk, alcohol Mean risk rating 5 4 3 Personal risk General risk 2 <15 16-20 21-37 38-75 75+ Alcohol consumption per week (corresponding to cl vodka) 18 Personal and general risk, smoking Mean risk rating 5 4 3 Personal risk General risk 2 <12 13-20 21-30 31-55 Number of cigarettes per day 19 Different dynamics of personal and general risk • General risk is related to policy for hazards perceived to be under one’s personal control • Personal risk is related to policy for hazards not under one’s personal control • Examples: alcohol and nuclear power 20 21 How important is ”risk”? 22 Focus on risk – traditional approach • People are asked to rate the ‘risk’ • It is assumed that perceived risk, as defined in this way, is the factor driving risk-related behaviour – such as demand for risk reduction • But the assumption is usually implicit 23 Risk mitigation – the problem • What drives demand for risk reduction? • Is perceived risk the important factor? • If not, what factor is most important ? 24 Example • Consider the risk for a Swedish citizen, age 30–45, to 1. get a severe cold during the next 12 months 2. become infected with the HIV virus during the same time period • Which risk is the largest? • From which risk is it more important to be protected? 25 Risk perception studies show that • Risk and probability are closely related • Severity and demand for risk reduction are closely related • Risk and demand for risk reduction are only moderately related (“probability neglect”) 26 Implications • In risk communication it should be clear that the public wants to hear about severity of consequences, not so much about probabilities: –Probability is hard to understand –Precise estimates of very small probabilities must rely on many assumptions and are seldom very credible • In risk perception research, it is necessary to broaden the scope – just studying ‘risk’ is not sufficient 27 Individual differences in risk perception 28 Distribution of the number of product types checked as risky, across all respondents. 29 ”Risk sensitivity” or attenuation- amplification of perceived nuclear waste risk 30 The level of risk judgments varies strongly across individuals and is a very important explanatory factor in risk perception models: Risk Sensitivity 31 Specific risk factors 32 Recent EU project on perceived risks of chemicals in consumer products (extensive data collected in the beginning of 2009) • • • • N=26,718 All 27 member states of the EU participated Among other things, participants were asked: According to what you know, which of the following consumer products contain chemicals posing a risk to the user? • 13 product categories were listed 33 Specific reactions, not to general concept Pesticides or herbicides Bleach and heavy duty cleaners Bathroom cleaners Hair dyes Interior paints Timber preservatives Laundry or dishwashing detergents Washing powder Fabrics: Synthetic fibers Sunscreens Hair shampoo Fabrics: Wool cotton or linen Toothpaste 0 20 40 60 80 Percentage check a product category, all states 34 Comment: ”Chemicals” is a highly variable concept. Sometimes very risky, sometimes not. 35 Study of the perceived risk of terrorism 36 Conclusion • Traditional explanatory factors (dread and new risk), as well as background factors, only explained 10-15 % of the variance of perceived risk • Twice that level was reached by adding specific factors Emotions and affect • “Affect” is a word with several distinct meanings: emotions or values (attitudes) • It is necessary to clarify which one is investigated – they are psychologically quite different • Both are related to risk perception 38 Attitude (affect), trust, risk sensitivity and attitude towards nuclear power (1991 study) 39 Emotions – study of emotional reactions to a nuclear waste repository • Two candidate municipalities, one control and a national sample • About 800 respondents from two communities where site studies are now carried out • Several emotions were rated, not only one • Negative and positive emotions were rated 40 Model of the attitude to a nuclear waste repository Model of attitude to the repository explaining 65% of the variance Attitude to nuclear power Social trust 0.06 Positive emotions 0.13 0.26 Attitude to the repository - 0.21 Negative emotions 0.18 Epistemic trust - 0.16 Risk to the municipality 41 Correlations between emotional reactions and the attitude to nuclear power Emotion Own emotional reaction to nuclear power The anticipated emotional reaction of others to nuclear power Anger -0.62 -0.08 Contempt -0.55 -0.10 Fear -0.65 -0.06 Interest 0.28 0.17 Sadness -0.58 -0.12 Satisfaction 0.57 0.21 Guilt -0.25 0.03 Shame -0.29 0.04 Worry -0.61 -0.12 42 Mean emotional reactions attributed to others versus own reactions Others’ emotional reaction 3.5 3.0 2.5 2.0 1.5 1.4 1.6 1.8 2.0 2.2 2.4 2.6 2.8 3.0 3.2 3.4 Own emotional reaction 43 A methodological point • Instructions to rate “dread” do not specify WHOSE dread • This probably leads to the interpretation to rate the emotional reactions of OTHERS • In turn, data therefore reflect only a weak link between emotional reactions and perceived risk 44 Conclusions about emotions • Specific and current emotional reactions do seem to explain much of attitudes and policy behaviour, attitude (“affect”) somewhat less • Compare these strong effects with the almost zero importance of anticipated ‘dread’ of others • Both positive and negative emotions are important • Note that ‘worry’ contributes beyond the effect of ‘fear’ • Anger seems to be more important than fear in policy contexts 45 Experts versus the public • Original work suggested, in some interpretations, that experts make ‘correct’ and ‘objective’ risk judgements used a very small group of ‘experts’ with questionable competence • Later work with substantive experts has shown that they have similar structure of risk perception, but lower level • Risk perception is related to experts’ field of responsibility – not to knowledge 46 Ratings of risk dimensions of nuclear waste by the public, and male and female experts 5 A 5 B 4 4 3 Meanratingsbyfemaleexperts Meanratingsbymaleexperts 3 2 1 0 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 M e a nra tin g sb ym e n ,m e m b e rso fth ep u b lic 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 M e a nra tin g sb yw o m e n ,m e m b e rso fth ep u b lic Expert-public difference for both genders 47 4.0 No gender difference among experts Mean ratings by male experts 3.5 3.0 2.5 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 Mean ratings by female experts 48 Regression coefficients in model of perceived nuclear waste risk, results from analyzing data from experts (A) and engineers (B) plotted against results from analyzing data from the public. 0 .4 0 .4 B 0 .3 0 .3 0 .2 0 .2 Regresioncoeficients,enginers Regresioncoeficient,experts A 0 .1 0 .0 0 .1 0 .0 0 .1 0 .2 R e g r e s s io n c o e f f ic ie n t ,p u b lic 0 .3 0 .4 0 .1 0 .0 0 .1 0 .1 0 .0 0 .1 0 .2 0 .3 0 .4 R e g r e s s io n c o e f f ic ie n t s ,p u b lic Very similar models for experts and the public 49 Correlations between risk perception ratings and the psychometric factors (genetically modified food) for the public and experts Personal risk Explanatory variable General risk Public Experts Public Experts Dread 0.40 0.43 0.19 0.01 New risk 0.44 0.51 0.31 -0.06 Interfering with nature 0.47 0.53 0.47 0.08 Immoral risk 0.51 0.56 0.61 0.38 Severity of consequences 0.50 0.54 0.47 0.08 Epistemic trust -0.24 -0.31 -0.27 -0.08 Social trust -0.38 -0.42 -0.52 -0.30 Experts’ risk ratings unrelated to “subjective factors, But only for Dread and Novelty and for general risk 50 Conclusion – public and experts • Experts judge personal risk in a manner similar to the public • However, their judgements of general risk seem to be less correlated with the ‘subjective’ factors • Other studies have shown that personal risk is most important in policy related to technology and the environment • Experts judge risks to be smaller when they are within 51 their general area of responsibility What does our research imply for risk communication? • Emotions are important to take into account – but not only strong fear • Concern about ‘Interfering with Nature’ is a major factor – but not novelty of a risk • People’s understanding and trust in science is very important – social trust is somewhat less important • ‘Risk’ and ‘probability’ are marginal to people – they respond to notions about anticipated consequences and whether a technology has unique advantages • Experts are not that different from the public in how they react to hazards outside their field of responsibility 52 For more information… •See my homepage http://www.dynam-it.com/lennart/ •Several papers and reports can be downloaded from that site 53