History of Indic Languages By K S Krishnan 2 Contents 1 History of Indic Languages 1.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 5 2 Origin of the Indo-European languages 2.1 Linguistic Palaeontology or LP or Palaeolinguistics . . . . . . . . 2.2 Laryngeal Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 8 12 3 Indo-Aryan in South Asia in second millennium BCE 3.1 Horses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.2 Gobekli Tepe . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.3 Further Evidences Against Arrival of Indo-Aryans in South Asia in the Second Millennium BCE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.4 Acculturation A New Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 17 20 4 Genetic Evidence 4.1 Evidence of Studies Based on Y-DNA Haplogroups . . . . . . . . 4.2 Inconsistencies in the Models of Spread of IE Languages in Eurasia 4.3 Arrival of Indo-Aryan Speakers in South Asia - A new Perspective 31 31 35 40 5 Conclusion 45 3 22 23 4 CONTENTS Chapter 1 History of Indic Languages 1.1 Introduction Indic languages or Indo-Aryan languages are the largest groups within the IndoEuropean language family; both in terms of the number of member languages as well as the number of speakers. Geographically, these languages are spoken in the northern parts of South Asia, including Bangladesh, parts of Afghanistan, and also in southern parts of Sri Lanka. The history of this language group or how it came to be in present geographic location is controversial, with discussions on the subject distracted by inflexible ideological positions and emotional reactions. This paper is an effort to look into the issues involved without being buffeted by these as far as possible. Languages belonging to the IE family are spoken by almost half the humanity at present and are present as primary languages in all continents, except maybe the two icy ones. The time and circumstances of this spread to other continents is well known, but the circumstances and dynamics of its unusual spread all over vast Eurasia in pre-historic times is still hazy. Many theories have been proposed trying to explain this process, but none of them has been able to build a respectable consensus in its favor. There are multiple pieces of evidence that just do not add up with any of the models that are proposed at present. 5 6 CHAPTER 1. HISTORY OF INDIC LANGUAGES Chapter 2 Origin of the Indo-European languages There are two dominant theories of the origins of the Indo-European languages; the Pontic-Caspian-Steppe hypothesis and the Anatolian hypothesis. The first of these locate the original speakers of Proto-Indo-European somewhere on the Russian steppes, north of the Black Sea and the Caspian Sea; and posits branching of PIE into daughter languages by around 2500 BCE. This model connects the Proto-Indo-European language with the Yamna or Yamnaya horizon, in the area around the Don River or between Dnieper and Volga rivers in South Russia, using a new approach of combining linguistics and archaeological data. The Yamnaya horizon is an archaeological culture associated with kurgan cemeteries, corded-ware pots, and a system of nomadic pastoralism. This model is known as the ‘Kurgan hypothesis’ and was first proposed by Marija Gimbutas in the 1950s. The alternative theory locates the Indo-European homeland (Urheimat) in Anatolia in modern-day Turkey and suggests a point of divergence in around c.7000-6000 BCE (the Early Neolithic). In this case, the socio-technological development spurring the spread of Proto-Indo-European is the development of agriculture. The displacement of earlier non-Indo-European languages was facilitated by both the population increase enabled by farming and the prestige attached to farming cultures. An advantage of this hypothesis is that the spread of farming techniques has been well-documented by archaeologists. This theory was propounded by the archaeologist Colin Renfrew and is known as the Anatolian hypothesis. The first is essentially based on linguistic evidence and the 7 8 CHAPTER 2. ORIGIN OF THE INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGES second on archaeological data. The ”Indo-Aryan arrival in South Asia in the second millennium BCE” thesis is based on Kurgan or more generally ‘The steppe home-land’ model. The Anatolian hypothesis also posits Indo-Aryan arrival in South Asia, but far back in time; in about fifth millennium BCE. There are other models like Paleolithic Continuity Theory and Out-of-India Theory among others, but these have only limited support at present. There are also many variations of these proposed by different scholars. One important point of contention between proponents of these two theories is whether ‘linguistic paleontology’ is a legitimate methodology. 2.1 Linguistic Palaeontology or LP or Palaeolinguistics The practice of making inferences about the cultures of unattested proto-language users on the evidence of reconstructed languages is called ‘linguistic paleontology’. It can be considered the interface between historical-comparative linguistics and archaeology. It aims at and is thought to be capable of correlating the results of linguistic reconstruction with historical cultures identified in space and time by archaeology. It was argued that by establishing a bridge between historical-comparative reconstruction and archaeology, linguistic palaeontology can take us closer to a fuller picture of a reconstructed proto-language because it may provide a cultural embedding for linguistic reconstruction, thus it can eliminate the sterility and increase the reality of reconstructed etyma. By establishing the time-depth of a proto-language we can set the temporal limits, while the ancient spread-zone of the fauna and flora known from reconstructed terms defines the spatial limits of the center of language dispersal. According to its defenders, it promises the ability to see into the social and material cultures of prehistoric societies and uncover facts about peoples beyond the reach of archaeology. Advocates of this procedure claim that conclusions made based on reconstructed etyma about the cultural background of a proto-language will relate not only to objects known by the speakers of a proto-language but also to abstract phenomena that may not be traced by material remnants accessible by archaeological investigations. Most 19th-century linguists took it for granted that they were reconstructing the actual word forms of some earlier language, that *dekm, (PIE reconstruction for ‘ten’. PIE reconstructions have a star prefix as a convention) for example, was a pronounceable Proto-Indo-European word. Many of their successors have been 2.1. LINGUISTIC PALAEONTOLOGY OR LP OR PALAEOLINGUISTICS9 more skeptical about the phonetic reality of reconstructed etyma. Many linguists maintain that they are no more than formulae summarizing the correspondences observed to hold between attested forms in particular languages and that they are, in principle unpronounceable. According to Pulgram, “no reputable linguist pretends that Proto-Indo-European reconstructions represent a reality”. The reconstructed lexicon is not a reconstructed language. At best what can be said is that different forms of these might have been in use in the dialects of the proto-language. When the shift is made, from phonological reconstruction to drawing inferences about the material and social culture of pre-historical societies, one moves from historical linguistics to a discipline much more closely aligned to archaeology. Its critics see it as essentially flawed and unscientific. The correct establishment of both the temporal and spatial limits in itself can be a very difficult task; and if one of them is incorrect, the other is rendered invalid and we miss the target of the identification of a homeland. The biggest issue related to this procedure is that it is based on the assumed meanings of the reconstructed terms. While we have near exception-less sound laws, there is no equivalent ‘meaning laws’. Given the “vague and variable meanings” of the cognates used to reconstruct Indo-European, conclusions about Indo-European culture must be suspect. ‘A reconstructed meaning does not necessarily mirror the exact reference in the protolanguage’. Furthermore, when dating the spread of these languages, the presence of some reconstructed etyma, particularly of terms referring to technological advances, does not tell us that the language or community sprang into existence at the same point in time that the technology did. Some linguists find some of these methods and processes arbitrary. A. Fox states “we are not necessarily able to establish unambiguously the original meaning of a particular word in the protolanguage, since meaning, as well as phonological form, are subject to change” (Fox 1995, 307). Although phonemes are subject to change, sound changes are mostly found to be regular and therefore recoverable as far as we can establish the laws governing sound changes. Semantic changes, however, are more elusive, and semantic reconstruction, as a result, is not always as reliable as phonological reconstruction. These are often assigned the currently understood meaning of the cognates used for reconstruction, which could lead to serious errors. We cannot expect full semantic congruity of an etymon in descendant languages, just as we do not expect a proto-form to come down to daughter languages without phonological alterations. Many scholars have expressed doubts about this method. Linguist Stefan Zimmer says, “The long dispute about the reliability of this ‘linguistic paleontology’ is not yet fin- 10 CHAPTER 2. ORIGIN OF THE INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGES ished, but is approaching its inevitable end with a negative result, of course.” Gimbutas says when discussing the cultural change enforced by Indo-European expansion: “These changes were expressed as the transition from matrilineal to patrilineal order, from a learned theocracy to a militant patriarchy, from a sexually balanced egalitarian society to a male-dominated hierarchy, and from a chthonic goddess religion to the IE sky-oriented pantheon of gods”. These are quite clearly excessively strong conclusions to derive from linguistic evidence. There were even more expansive claims about the magical efficacy of the technique and as a backlash many people are now suspicious of the methods of linguistics. Words reconstructed for PIE used by Gimbutas and others in formulating the Kurgan hypotheses are those for horse, cow, pig, goat, and sheep, as well as words for piglet, lamb, and cattle; words for riding, milking, wool, and words for the wheel, hub, axle, and transport by vehicle. All these would support the idea of a pastoral society, who domesticated horses and lived during the Early Bronze Age. But there are also words for grain, barley, kernel, broad beans, axes, milling, grinding, sowing, reaping, gathering, ploughing, and fields for ploughing in the earliest layer of PIE, which suggest an agricultural society. Besides, missing from Gimbutas’ analysis are the words for ducks, geese, cranes, salmon, and eels, which are not typical steppes creatures. Words for ships and rowing are unlikely in the language of a pastoral culture and words for ore, gold, and silver also have been reconstructed, even though Gimbutas insists that the Kurgan people only knew copper. If such selective picking and choosing of terms in PIE in support of the model is acceptable, it is also possible to rule out South Russian Steppe as a candidate for PIE homeland, as it can be argued that PIE people were agriculturalists and lived somewhere near the sea. Edwin Francis Bryant (2001) says about the horse evidence; ”using such negative evidence, by the same logic used to eliminate India as a candidate, ultimately any potential homeland can be disqualified due to lacking some fundamental Proto-Indo-European item or another”. Edward Sapir, N. S. Trubetzkoy, Antonio Tovar, Joseph Greenberg, Georges Mounin, and others have criticized such shortcomings of conventional linguistics, including its claim to reconstruct hypothetical languages of the distant past. Edmund Leach (1990: 243) expresses his reservations thus “The origin myth of the IndoEuropean philologists calls for a lineage of wholly imaginary ancestral protolanguages.” One example of such misinterpretation is the word for salmon in PIE. It was originally claimed that Indo-European term *laks denoted the North Atlantic 2.1. LINGUISTIC PALAEONTOLOGY OR LP OR PALAEOLINGUISTICS11 salmon which is found in the Baltic and North Atlantic. This was based on the cognate etymology of the respective words for salmon in Germanic and BaltoSlavic languages. It was argued that since the term for Atlantic salmon in the Germanic, Baltic and Slavic languages could be derived from the common ProtoIndo-European root *laks, the Urheimat of the Indo-Europeans must be where both the languages and the object it describes can be found: Northern-Central Europe. This precluded the placement of the Indo-European homeland at any real distance from this region or seemed to be a piece of strong evidence for a PIE Home-Land in North Europe. This model also had strong political support from the Nazi rulers of Germany. In later work though, it was argued that *laks denotes the salmon Trout which is much more widespread and would allow for homelands across Eurasia and that the term which originally denoted the salmon Trout was only transferred to the North Atlantic salmon by later tribes of Balts, Slavs, and Germans. This demonstrates how spurious models can be constructed using tools of historical linguistics. Many people hold that the idea of a PIE itself makes little sense, as the regions variously identified with the PIE homeland never seem to have had a high population density. These models assume large migrations from PIE homeland in several directions within a short period; which is very unlikely when the proposed regions had a sparse population. This was particularly so in the case of South Russian Steppe. Apart from such objections raised against linguistic paleontology, many linguists are also uncomfortable with the reconstruction of etyma in unattested proto-languages using the comparative method. Leonard Bloomfield outlined in 1933 the problems inherent in the comparative method. The comparative method worked “only on the assumption of a uniform parent language,” one without dialectal variation, and hypothesized abrupt separations of one speechcommunity from another. Such abrupt changes are unlikely and language separations are likely to be long-drawn-out processes. Furthermore, according to Bloomfield, the likelihood of error increases with the length of time or breadth of the area under investigation. Bloomfield was even more critical of linguistic paleontology, which attempted to reconstruct proto-culture based on the reconstructed lexicon. Linguistic palaeontology was once the great hope of the new ‘science’ of linguistics. Linguists claimed they were able to re-create the Indo-Europeans original environment, down to its flora and fauna. However, this field is now largely discredited. Depending on the approach followed, one can equally well arrive at a warm or a cold climate, the plains of Central Europe or the mountains of the Caucasus; the PIE people can be portrayed as aggressive nomads or as 12 CHAPTER 2. ORIGIN OF THE INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGES peaceful sedentary agriculturists or as a coastal society of sea-farers or almost as anything one fancies. This method gives anyone the freedom to choose the ‘Original Home-land’ and the cultural and lifestyle practices of the PIE people based on their preferences or ideological leanings. The popular chronology of the evolution of IE languages is almost entirely based on this method. And ‘the arrival of Indo-Aryan language in South Asia in the second millennium BCE’ model is again almost entirely based on this model and its chronology, as it has little else to support it at the local level. Linguists claim that the discipline fully qualifies to be treated as a science, as its predictive power was demonstrated in many instances (though the uninitiated might find some of its methods and procedures as based on subjective inferences). This claim is primarily related to sound changes and one example usually quoted is that of Laryngeal theory. 2.2 Laryngeal Theory The laryngeal theory is the name commonly given to an assumption made about the phonological system of an early stage of Indo-European language. It is assumed that this system included some phonemes, usually called laryngeals. It is hypothesized that Proto-Indo-European language (PIE) had a series of phonemes beyond those reconstructed with the comparative method. These phonemes, according to the most accepted variant of the theory, were ”laryngeal” consonants of an indeterminate place of articulation towards the back of the mouth. They have disappeared in all known IE languages, but is believed to have affected their surroundings in typical and to a large extent predictable ways. The hypothesis was first proposed by F. de Saussure more than a century ago (1879), but was mostly rejected by most scholars at that time. It was savagely attacked by many leading linguists of the time, including some senior faculty members of the Leipzig University, which was the acknowledged leader in Indo-European linguistics studies in 19th century. Because of this rejection, the theory was more or less forgotten for the next fifty years. But in a remarkable turnaround, it was accepted by mainstream scholars after Hittite language was deciphered and seemed to contain some such phonemes. The evidence for their existence is indirect, but the theory serves as an elegant explanation for some properties of the PIE vowel system that made no sense until this theory came along. It creates greater regularity in the reconstruction of PIE phonology than from the reconstruction that is produced by the comparative method. Thus it proved to be an exotic tool that supplemented 2.2. LARYNGEAL THEORY 13 comparative method in linguistics. Laryngeal Theory of Proto-Indo-European is widely accepted nowadays, but with different degrees of ‘faith’. Many IE linguists have expressed their doubts about different aspects of the theory and there are many disagreements among those who support it. Linguists have found inconsistencies in PIE Laryngeal Theory and many are completely against it. It is not easy to find articles of these critics, even in mediums like Wikipedia as they are generally ignored by the IE linguistics establishment. One of these authors is the eminent Polish linguist Witold Manczak, who has written a series of articles with strong criticism of the theory. According to Maczak, the first problem is that Saussure’s proposal is untenable, for various important reasons, among them the absurdity of proposing a language with just one vowel. This would be enough to invalidate the whole edifice of PIE laryngeals. In one of the articles, Manczak asks himself why it is that the Laryngeal Theory has been so successful among linguists. According to him, there is a general lack of validity criteria in historical linguistics. The important thing is the ‘authority’ behind the theory, not the validity of the theory itself. It seems strange that linguistics has to depend on such a theory (about a sound nobody has heard) to establish its scientific credentials, when there are such serious disagreement between scholars regarding its validity and applicability. Similarly, R. Jakobson (1957) argued that, in the PIE system reconstructed by use of comparative methods, the existence of the aspirated voiced stops without the presence of the corresponding voiceless aspirated stops was questionable. In short there are serious disagreements among scholars regarding many aspects of PIE reconstruction. The suggestion is not that linguistics is a fake science; it is only that conclusions based on it should not be treated as conclusive in themselves that can override any adverse findings in other disciplines. 14 CHAPTER 2. ORIGIN OF THE INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGES Chapter 3 Indo-Aryan in South Asia in second millennium BCE The principal reasons for many to argue for an ‘Indo-Aryan arrival in South Asia in second millennium BCE’ model are (in the words of Michael Witzel, Professor of Sanskrit at Harvard University; one of the most vocal supporter of the model) 1 The linguistic evidence ‘so clearly speaks for it’ 2 “If RV is to be dated before 2000 BCE, it should not contain evidence of the domesticated horse (not found in the subcontinent before c. 1700 BCE) and of the horse drawn chariot developed only about 2000 BCE in S. Russia”. “The horse-and chariot-rich Vedic texts are characterized by small tribal units of late Bronze Age pastoralists roaming around the Punjab (Witzel 2001a) that must be dated after 1900 BC, given the invention of the spoked-wheeled chariot around 2000 BC and the importation of the first domesticated horses into South Asia by c. 1800 BC.” ‘The linguistic evidences that speak for the model’ include the conclusion based on reconstruction of PIE and linguistic palaeontology that its original home was in an area with temperate climate and that the people who spoke this dialect were nomadic pastoralists who domesticated horses and had horse drawn chariots for transport. Words for flora and fauna found in the ‘home land’ also have been reconstructed. These seem to exclude South Asia as the home of PIE. Since Indic languages are branches of IE, this implies that Indo-Aryan language came to South Asia from outside. It was also similarly concluded, again based on linguistic palaeontology, that PIE evolved in bronze-age and began to branch into separate daughter languages 15 16CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE in middle bronze-age, as chariot construction requires at least middle bronzeage technologies. Since it was also concluded that PIE home was in the South Russian Steppes and the middle bronze-age in that area was in the middle of third millennium BCE and since Indo-Iranian was a later branch of PIE, it could not have reached South Asia before the middle of second millennium BCE. Witzel finds many substrate words in RV which he believes came from the local language spoken in NW India when IA speakers entered the area. There are also many names in RV which he finds are neither possible in Vedic nor Indo-Iranian or Indo-European. Witzel believes this to be evidence for arrival of IE language in South Asia. But some linguists have pointed out that the identification of some of these as substrate may be on account of “wilful imposition by the researching scholar, of the inferred structure of a majority of lexical roots, on a minority of them”. It is difficult to concede that the above linguistic evidences ‘so clearly speaks for’ an ‘Indo-Aryan arrival in South Asia in second millennium BCE’ model, as it is based on linguistic palaeontology, which has been judged as extremely frail and unreliable by many scholars. And without the assumed insights provided by this method, the steppe home land model, including its chronology, loses much of its glamour and credibility. The chronology is mostly dependent on the reconstructed words in PIE related to technologies like use of metals (bronze, copper), chariot construction and horse domestication. But in most of these cases the cognates in the attested languages could be adaptations of terms inherited from their earlier forms with meanings that have nothing to do with these new technologies. The English word ‘internet’ is a combination of two words, inter + net. Both have nothing to do with ‘internet’. If future linguists managed to reconstruct the word internet’, they would be wrong to infer that the English language came into existence around the turn of the second millennium. If words like computer and Android are also reconstructed, and if the linguists follow this logic, it would be treated as final proof that English language could not have existed before twentieth century. The PIE related evidence of chariots and horses are also not entirely reliable. It is said that PIE had words for both horse and wheel and thus it is evident that the people who spoke this form of the dialect had domesticated horses and had wheeled chariots. But many authors have pointed out that the word for horse might have been used for wild horses at the PIE stage. Similarly even if the term wheel was present in Proto-Indo-European, it is uncertain that it denoted the wheels on vehicles rather than potter’s wheels or even the sun disk. It is important to remember that coining an entirely new word for a new concept was always rare. What usually happens in such cases is adaptation of an existing 3.1. HORSES 17 word for the new concept or borrowing it from another language. 3.1 Horses In 1930, Sir John Marshall first suggested that Aryans introduced the horse into South Asia as part of the Aryan Invasion Theory. Since then, it has been near taboo in the academic circles to go against this thinking. But there are multiple reasons to suspect that this conclusion was hasty and ill-informed. When Falconer and Cautley documented equus from the Indian Subcontinent (1858), they named three species: equus sivalensis; bones of which were discovered in the Siwalik Hills, part of outer Himalayas and equus namadicus and equus palaeonus. The last two were mostly native to Narmada Valley. Within the stratigraphic range of E. sivalensis, remains of a smaller horse with slender metapodials have also been found. Thus, it is possible that South Asia had many sub-species belonging to equus sivalensis and E. namadicus. Equus sivalensis is the oldest known true horse. The Arabs, barbs and the thoroughbred horses might have evolved from equus sivalensis or at least may have ancestry elements from it. It could be the largest of the old world fossil horses. Until early twentieth century, equine specialists studying the taxonomy of horses favoured the idea that equus sivalensis and the probably derived equus namadicus of India were ancestors of modern domesticated horses. But most palaeontologists now hold that the Indian species (E. sivalensis and E. nomadicus) became extinct, and that E. stenosis through E. robustus, gave rise to the modern breeds. It may be noted that the generic term Equus caballus included many varieties or races of horse. India had sivalensis and namadicus, while in Europe it was generally the stenonis. During the glacial peak between 25,000 and 19,000 years ago, and then again between 12,700 and 11,500 years ago, there was extreme cold and aridity in the northern latitudes of Eurasia. This could have resulted in mass extinction of horses in these areas, as all kinds of fodder would have withered. But horses might have survived the glacial period in the southern locations of Eurasia. West Asia and Iran were extremely arid cold deserts, and were not habitable for horses during this period. But horses in South Asia could have survived this period. Given this, the consensus that horses in India became extinct, while those in the steppes, Central Asia and West Asia survived during this period, seem illogical. I have not come across any explanation why horses became extinct in South Asia, except that horse remains have not been found anywhere in South Asia after 10000 BP. If equus sivalensis became extinct in South Asia 18CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE during this period, it could not have been due to adverse climatic conditions in India. There is a possibility that these species became extinct much later. The presence of similar physical features in the Java, Sulu Archipelago, and Borneo horses make it appear that some modified descendants of E. sivalensis survived. Similarly, there are indications that Indian caballus horse breeds like Marwari, Spiti, Bhutia, Manipuri and Zanskari have substantial local ancestry. Wild true horse bones were found from 20,000 BP strata of Bolan and Son valleys (G.R. Sharma:110 ff.; Kazanas 1999:33-34), and domesticated horse bones from dates 7500 BP and 3500 BP of the Bolan and Son valleys (Sharma:110 ff.). R.S. Sharma (1996:17) too noted domesticated horse bones from Mahagara Neolithic complex in district Prayagraj, UP of 7000 BP and Bagor (Rajasthan) 6500 BP. Many of these finds are dated to c.4500 to 3500 YBP. But artifacts from a few deeper trenches yielded dates of c.7500 BP. Many equine-like terracotta figurines found from Harappan sites were initially considered as depicting the horses. Later as the horse-riding Aryans versus equid-ignorant non-Vedic Harappan binary became dominant; these began to be treated as being ambiguous. The identification of a terracotta figurine from Mohenjodaro as that of the horse could be doubtful. But those from the Harappan levels at Nausharo have yielded terracotta figurines of horse. Also, the middle Harappan levels at Lothal have yielded a couple of terracotta figurines. The Bhimbetka Rock Paintings are usually dated between 30000 BCE to early historical period. According to one report, out of the 428 animal figures found among these, 185 are of horses. This should normally mean that horse was the animal most familiar to the painters. Some of paintings are quite crude and fading. At least one of a horse is more detailed and colourful. The cruder paintings among them should be older. For example, one painting of fighting men with club-like weapons on horse-back should be quite old and was earlier thought to be from Mesolithic period (6000 to 8000 YBP). But the paintings have now been dated later than 3,500 BP because the general view of the historians does not accept horse in India before the assumed Aryan Invasion. This would mean that aboriginal inhabitants of Central India were riding horses just as Vedic Aryans were bringing them into NW India for the first time. (It is possible that the aborigines had observed other people fighting on horse-back and painted the figures.) Cave paintings cannot be dated directly. Dating these to 3500 YBP or later is based on extraneous reasons or historians’ stubborn resistance to considering an earlier date. Given the findings of the domesticated ‘true’ horse bones from the Central India (Mahagara, Bolan and Son Valley), the pre-agricultural Mesolithic paintings depicted in the Bhimbetka Rock Paintings (petroglyphs and pictographs) can well be dated 5,000 BP or earlier. 3.1. HORSES 19 Then there is the case of Surkotada horse bone findings. The late eminent Hungarian archaeozoologist, Sandor Bokonyi, was in India to attend a workshop and later he spent some time in Delhi where someone showed him six samples from Surkotada, which consisted of mostly teeth. After examining the artifacts, he concluded that they were not of a half-ass, but a real ‘domesticated horse’. In 1994, archaeologist (Zooarchaeology) Richard H Meadow examined the same remains and came to a different conclusion. He thought the samples came from an onager and not the true horse. Meadow believes that horses could have come to the region, maybe by 2000 B.C.E since there is figurine evidence and painted shreds in Swat, but the find from Surkotada was not that of true horse. Richard Meadow raised his objections to Sandor Bokonyi during a conference in Konstanz, Germany, but Bokonyi was not convinced. With so many reports of archaeological finds of horse bones, terracotta figurines of horses and the Bhimbetka paintings of horses, one cannot simply reject the possibility of presence of domesticated horses in South Asia before second millennium BCE. Surprisingly this rejection seems to be on very thin grounds. It is said and rhetorically repeated that there is no evidence for presence of horses in South Asia, until it was brought here by the Vedic Aryans, using the same as argument, evidence and conclusion. The logical course would be not to use the ‘horse argument’ in the context of ‘Aryan’ debate until we have more clarity in the matter. Colin Renfrew (1999) remarked, ”The significance of the horse ... has been much exaggerated”. As regards as chariots, earliest dates of archaeological evidence for spoke wheeled horse driven chariots is from c. 2000 BCE. But there are evidences for wheeled vehicles from at least mid 4th millennium BCE from many parts of the world, including Harappan settlements. In any case, it seems implausible that small bands of nomadic pastoralist could have brought true chariots into South Asia in the second millennium BCE. Consider the following 1 The Indian chariots described in Vedas and Itihasa/Puranas were very different from the steppe or West Asian ones in style as well as materials used. Rigvedic chariots are described as made of the wood of Kimsuka and Samali (RV 10.85.20), Khadira and Simsapa (RV 3.53.19) trees. These trees are mentioned in various texts/treatise on Ayurveda. Some of these trees are also mentioned in Buddhist and Jain texts. These are still found, though rare, in some parts of South Asia, Burma and South East Asia, but not found in Central Asia or the steppe. 2 It would have been very difficult for a few nomads to bring chariots from Central Asia crossing the difficult Hindu Kush. It might have been possible in the case of a larger ‘Aryan invasion’. But since most scholars have now abandoned 20CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE such a model, we will have to discard the chariot related arguments also. 3 Constructing chariots locally would have required an ecosystem that included identification and availability of the required types of wood and metals, implements to fashion these as required and trained artisans experienced in such work. This would have taken generations of the nomads to collect together. By this time the locals would have been familiar with it and it would hardly have the value of a ‘status kit’. And Witzel believes that ”the composers of RV were small tribal units of late Bronze Age pastoralists roaming around the Punjab”. It is argued that since archaeology has not found evidence for chariots, domesticated horses and iron in South Asia before 1900 BCE and since there are references to these in RV, the text cannot be dated before that date. Such an assertion can be made only if the entire area (Eurasia) has been thoroughly investigated and we can confidently say that nothing more is likely to be found. Besides, there is no archaeological evidence for presence of horses and chariots in South Asia in the second millennium BCE also. These begin to appear only in the first millennium BCE. Should we then conclude that Aryans came to South Asia in the middle of first millennium BCE? Absence of archaeological evidence is not evidence of absence of artefact found and the technology used to create it at any time before it. It can just as well be that we have failed to find it so far. It is best to reserve judgement in such cases. The instance of Gobekli Tepe may be an example. 3.2 Gobekli Tepe Mainstream consensus on reports of civilizations that flourished before 5000 BCE is that these are ‘idiocracies of mystery mongers’. But the archaeological finding of Gobekli Tepe seems to challenge this consensus. Gobekli Tepe means ‘belly hills’ in Turkik. It is located in the Southeastern Anatolia Region of Turkey. Under what was for long a small hillock, archaeologists have uncovered a strange group of structures dated to 12000 YBP. We believed that at the time these were being built, human beings were roaming about in small groups as hunter gatherers. The tools they had were just sharp stones, wooden clubs and pieces of bone. Based on everything we know about how modern civilization got its start, Gobekli Tepe should not exist. However, exist it does. These structures are made of huge carved megaliths of limestone arranged in circles. Each circle has a roughly similar layout: in the center are two large Tshaped stone pillars, encircled by slightly smaller stone pillars. The tallest pillars 3.2. GOBEKLI TEPE 21 tower 16 feet and weigh between seven and ten tons. This circle is surrounded with a stone wall, with a corridor or passage extending outward towards what could have been an entrance. The whole structure might have been covered with a roof, but there is no trace of such a roof at present. Beyond, on the hillside, are four other rings of partially excavated pillars. More than 200 pillars in about 20 circles are currently known through geophysical surveys. The surrounding area contains of lot of archaeological remains of the period, so we are dealing with a lot more than just one site. Yet another find in Boncuklu Tarla, some 300 kilometres east of Gobekli Tepe is at least 1000 years older than Gobekli Tepe. It has some similarities with Gobekli Tepe. The pillars are not roughly hewn blocks but are cleanly carved limestone ones, splashed with bas-relief of animals; a cavalcade of gazelles, snakes, foxes, scorpions, and ferocious wild boars. Amazingly, the structure’s builders were able to cut, shape, and transport 16-ton stones hundreds of feet without any wheels, tools or beasts of burden. The work also would have required cutting, carving, transporting for distances of hundreds of meters of huge stone blocks weighing 8 to 10 tons and erecting them in rock pits cut to the exact size. The fit was so exact that the pillars stood as they were erected for 12000 years, many thousands of years more than any other man-made structure. All these would have called for designing, planning, supervising and managing capabilities of a very high order. If our understanding of the hunter gatherer life style and technological capabilities is correct, the building of Gobekli Tepe would have been simply impossible. It would have been impossible to collect together a large work-force and feed them for long periods, as hunter-gatherers would have lived in small groups. They also could not have generated food surpluses. Another incomprehensible aspect of it, among several others, is how the hunter gatherers cut and carved hard rock so cleanly. It would have been impossible without some metallic tools. Our understanding so far was that the earliest date for bronzeage anywhere in the world was about 3300 BCE. The structure at Gobekli Tepe was built 7000 years before that. Hence, it now looks far more logical to admit that we just do not know when human being learned to make metal tools, rather than adamantly insisting that any date before 3300 BCE is impossible as there is no archaeological evidence for it. Archaeology could tell us if a particular pre-historic community used certain innovations. But claiming that it has to be the earliest such use might be being dump-headed. This could well apply to our present understanding of domestication of horses and use of chariots, as also the dates of evolution of PIE. To sum-up, the evidence for presence of horses and horse drawn chariots in 22CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE South Asia is ambiguous. Evidences for the former seem to be evenly matched. As for the chariots, I will opt for a 7:3 verdict; 7 parts for the absence of it in South Asia before 2000 BCE. But the arguments of the opposing side cannot be just rubbished and rejected. It can at best be used as a debating point, but it is hardly conclusive evidence. 3.3 Further Evidences Against Arrival of IndoAryans in South Asia in the Second Millennium BCE Apart from the above, there are multiple reasons why a large Indo-Aryan migration or invasion could not have taken place in the 2 MBCE. Some of these are listed below. 1 After the discovery of IVC in the first half of twentieth century, there were a series of archaeological discoveries in the Urals, Central Asia, Afghanistan, NW India and places to the east. All these were immediately declared as evidences of Aryan migration from the Steppe towards South Asia. This was mostly because the estimated dates of these appeared to coincide with the time-space expectations of linguists of where the Indo-Aryans might have reached at that time. But later closer scrutiny of the evidence by well-known archaeologists concluded that all these were more likely to be local developments. These include Sintashta, the BactriaMargiana Archaeological Complex, Gandhara grave culture, Ochre Coloured Pottery culture (OCP), Cemetery H culture, Black and Red-ware culture (BRW) and Painted Grey Ware culture (PGW). In the last thirty years, linguists realized that archaeological evidence do not support a large Aryan migration or invasion of South Asia in the second millennium BCE. Witzel says, “So far, clear archaeological evidence has just not been found. Yet, any archaeologist should know from experience that the unexpected occurs and that one has to look at the right place”. “If the so-called ‘invasion’ of IA speakers is not (yet) visible in the archaeology, it must be stressed that such movements rarely leave clear physical traces. – It is just a matter of time before such small camps are also discovered in the north-west of the subcontinent.” It is twenty years since Witzel wrote this and we still do not have any archaeological discovery of ‘such small camps’. 2 In the absence of any such evidences, linguists have abandoned the invasion/large migration model and fine-tuned it as trickling in of small groups of 3.4. ACCULTURATION A NEW APPROACH 23 nomads and a process of acculturation that resulted in language substitution by the locals as well as major changes in their culture, life-style and religious practices. “The horse-and chariot-rich Vedic texts are characterized by small tribal units of late Bronze Age pastoralists roaming around the Punjab”. “For the past five decades or so, the best specialists have no longer seen the influx of IA speakers as an ‘invasion.’ Linguists first, and archaeologists somewhat later, have stressed that such a scenario is too simple-minded and largely wrong. Evidence for the so-called massacre of the population of MohenjoDaro (Wheeler 1966) was actually found in several archaeological layers, and the irregular arrangement of skeletons may very well be due to flooding that swept the corpses into a street corner and buried them here and there. Linguists and philologists have now proposed to re-evaluate the mode of influx of IA speakers into the subcontinent. The immigration of such fringe groups visible in Near Eastern written sources turns out to be a combination of various scenarios: there is imperceptible influx of pastoral people, there are clashes with the settled agriculturalists over water and grazing rights, there is cattle raiding, there is a gradual influx into the cities (often by hired soldiers, with palace coups), and there is outright invasion by motley groups of border peoples, who are not necessarily ethnically homogeneous.” Witzel. 3.4 Acculturation A New Approach Many authors are now of the opinion that it was a process of acculturation that resulted in a language substitution in North-India in the second millennium BCE. Some go further and suggest that it might have happened even without a demographic change or that the change could have occurred owing to cultural exchanges alone without population admixture. Acculturation is a process of social, psychological, and cultural change that stems from the balancing of two cultures or is a process in which an individual adopts, acquires and adjusts to a new cultural environment. It is defined by Brown. H. Douglas as “the process of being adapted to a new culture” which involves a new orientation of thinking and feeling on the part of an individual. It also results in changes to culture, religious practices, health care, and other social institutions. There are also significant ramifications on the food, clothing, and language of those becoming introduced to the overarching culture. It occurs when individuals adopt the cultural norms of a dominant or host culture. Acculturation occurs when a minority culture, in contact with a dominant one, changes but still retains unique cultural markers of language, food and 24CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE customs. This type of change is much more likely to happen in an instance of voluntary migration or peaceful coexistence, rather than as a result of the conquests or forced coexistence that typically characterize assimilation. If this was the process that happened in the present case, we should have two or more distinct groups with some cultural similarity. The authors who support such a model in the present case probably believe that the higher and lower castes in India represent such distinct groups (I have not come across such an assertion so far, but assume such may be the case, as I do not see any other option). But in such cases, it would be the cultural markers of the dominant group that will prevail. In the two way exchanges, the target group will borrow far more of the markers than its reverse flow. In the present case, a few illiterate nomads could in no way be the dominant group. The locals would have been incomparably larger in demographic size and culturally advanced and they are unlikely to change their mother tongue, religious practices and beliefs and own names in such an exchange. Thus, it is likely that the common language that evolved after the process was closer to the language of the locals rather than that of the new migrants. If his was indeed the case, the language of the locals before the process must have been an early form of Indo-Aryan. Witzel further fine-tunes his acculturation model as follows, “Ehret (1988) underlines the relative ease with which ethnicity and language shift in small societies, due to the cultural/economic/military choices made by the local population in question. The intruding/influencing group bringing new traits may initially be small and the features it contributes can be fewer in number than those of the pre-existing local culture. The newly formed, combined ethnic group may then initiate a recurrent, expansionist process of ethnic and language shift. The material record of such shifts is visible only insofar as new prestige equipment or animals (the ‘status kit’, with new, intrusive vocabulary!) are concerned. This is especially so if pottery; normally culture-specific; continues to be made by local specialists of a class-based society.” “Just one ‘Afghan’ Indo-Aryan tribe that did not return to the highlands but stayed in their Panjab winter quarters in spring was needed to set off a wave of acculturation in the plains, by transmitting its ’status kit’ (Ehret) to its neighbours.” Such a model could be valid in isolated small societies. But NW India, ‘The Land of Five Rivers’ and the Gangetic Plaines were probably far more thickly populated compared to any other part of the world at that time. In such a population, living in more or less continuous habitations, such a model simply cannot work. ”We actually do know that one group after the other has entered the Indian 3.4. ACCULTURATION A NEW APPROACH 25 subcontinent, as immigrants or as invaders, in historical times. They include tribal groups such as the Yue Ji (Tukhara), Kushana, Abhira, (The Abhira tribes were a people mentioned in epics and other texts, including Vedas. They were probably some tribal people of Afghanistan) Gurjara as well as large armies, such as those of Dariu’s Persians, of Alexander’s and the Bactrian Greeks in the first mill. BCE, of both the Chinese via Tibet, Ladakh and Nepal, and the Arabs into Sindh in the 7-8th c. CE; further the Ahom Tai in Assam, and the Huns, Turks, Moguls, Iranians, and Afghans via the north-western passes in the first and second mill. CE. In addition, small-scale semi-annual transhumance movements between the Indus plains and the Afghan and Baluchi highlands continue to this day (Witzel 1995: 322, 2000). Why, then, should all immigration, or even mere transhumance trickling in, be excluded in the single case of the Indo-Aryans, especially when the linguistic evidence so clearly speaks for it?” What is denied is not the possibility of a migration or invasion, but the wholesale substitution of the language, culture, and religious practices in South Asia on account of such an event. None of the events mentioned by Witzel had resulted in such a change. Many of these were quite successful and in the case of the Muslim invasions, the invaders dominated life in South Asia for next 1000 years. These invaders/migrants also had their own ‘status kits’, probably far more substantial than that of the illiterate nomads. Yet they had only marginal impact on the languages, lifestyle and religious practices of the majority of the locals. Yet we are told that a few illiterate nomads caused a wave of acculturation in South Asia in the second millennium BCE that resulted in a complete language substitution and even change of personal names. The possibility of such changes should be considered negligible, if not non-existent. The argument about ‘status kits’ also needs to be taken with a pinch of salt. The status kit of the Indo-Aryans was said to include horses, chariots and some early RV hymns, which the locals came to believe had some magical effects. The locals were unlikely to be impressed with horses, as they would have been familiar with it on account of their close interaction with Central Asia and Mesopotamia, if it was not common locally. Central Asia was always known for excellent horses as well as for the horsemanship of the people. According to Surahmaniam et.al., (2019) people from NW India were migrating into Central Asia in large numbers in third millennium BCE. As for chariots, as mentioned earlier, it would have taken many generations for the small bands of nomads to begin construction of chariots locally and by that time the locals would have been quite familiar with it. Given the enormous difference in the population size, it is likely that the locals would adopt and adapt these ‘status kit’ from the new-comers. I have nothing to say about the belief of locals in the magical 26CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE powers of RV hymns. Speculation has a habit of taking flight because of its seductive appeal. The whole issue thus boils down to whether such small groups of illiterate nomads can dominate an incomparably larger population who were far more culturally and technologically advanced and who would have looked down upon the nomads as vagabonds. My firm answer would be ‘NO! 3 If the steppe nomads had a dominating presence in North-West India in the subsequent centuries, it would certainly be reflected in the culture and lifestyle of the locals. But what is apparent in the area has nothing in common with those of second-millennium steppes. But there are many similarities between the culture and life style of IVC settlements and those in the Ganga Yamuna Doab and areas further east in later centuries. There are obvious signs of cultural continuity between IVC and these later settlements and many of these can be found even in present-day Indians. 4 The Rgvedic pantheon have different layers of ‘Devas’ or divinities, which include ‘nature gods’ with human-like attributes like Indra, Varuna, Vayu and Agni, and Gods who maintain universal order like the Adityas. Above all these, there are abstract concepts like ‘Rta’ and ‘Dyauh’. Others which are possibly of this genre are ‘Sat’ and ‘Soma’. Particularly Rta seems to be very close to ‘laws of nature’. Everything in the universe, including humans and gods function in accordance with Rta. It is often considered as identical to later Hindu concept of ‘Dharma’, but this seems to be an incorrect interpretation. It is inconceivable that a supposedly nomadic pastoralist culture like Vedic Aryan could think up such a deeply philosophical and abstract concept. Nomads usually have simple ‘nature gods’ or ‘hero gods’. In more advanced settled societies, creator gods, who are compassionate and keeps universal order evolved. All these divinities have human attributes or even form. But Rta and Dyauh have no human attributes. No other known culture, (except Zoroastrians, who also had a similar concept, as have many things in common with Vedic Culture) had such abstract concepts as Rta (and Dyauh), which is the ultimate controller of nature as well as divinities. Pre-historic nomadic pasteralists could not have developed such abstract concepts. 5 The examination of human remains from the IVC settlements by wellknown anthropologists has come to the conclusion that “the anthropometric and cranial variables of the people of the area have remained the same from the third millennium BCE to the present”. This seems to rule out the possibility of a large migration of steppe nomads into the area during this period and their subjugation and domination of the locals in subsequent centuries. A recent paper by Won Joon Lee et.al published in Japanese Association of Anatomists 3.4. ACCULTURATION A NEW APPROACH 27 2019 reports details of first ever craniofacial reconstruction of the Indus Valley Civilization individuals found at the 4500 year old Rakhigarhi cemetery. These were of a male of 16-18 years age and a female of 35-50 years. The cranium and the facial features are very similar to present population of the area. These people were unusually tall for the period; the young man was almost 6 feet and the woman was 5 feet 8 inches in height. 6 Most words for flora and fauna, names of rivers and mountains in Indic languages have clear Indo Aryan/Indo European etymologies. This would mean that if Indo Aryan languages had first reached South Asia in the middle of the second millennium BCE, the new migrants forced a complete change in these names, which is very unlikely. We have the parallel cases of Americas, Australia and many others, where the new migrants had overwhelming military, technological and organizational superiority over the locals and yet most river, mountain and place names were adopted from those used earlier by the locals. Even more unlikely, in the case of most of South Asia, even personal names were changed to Indo Aryan ones. The most plausible inference that can account for the above would be that Proto Indo Aryan language reached North India at a time when these areas were sparsely populated by very primitive communities, which should be before sixth millennium BCE. There is no reference in Rgveda to any flora and fauna that is not found in India. Some probably later hymns seem to be aware of the geography of parts of Afghanistan. But there is no indication of knowledge of Northern Afghanistan and areas further north or west. This should mean that Vedic Aryans did not know lands beyond Hindkush. 7 Illiterate nomads are unlikely to have the kind of rich vocabulary found in RV. Rgveda uses about 15000 to 18000 separate words and roots. This would make it one of the richest languages that were in use before Common Era. Languages of nomadic pastoralists are unlikely to have more than 3000-5000 words. The inference one can draw from this is that Indo-Aryans were a settled people for a very long time with a developed literary tradition. Many of the hymns of Rgveda are highly poetic, musical and often philosophical. Many words have multiple synonyms. For example RV uses four synonyms for ‘horse’. 8 There are many references in Rgveda to River Sarasvati. Many hymns describe it as a very large river. RV 10.75 says it flowed between River Yamuna and Satlej. A palaeo-canal that fits the description is now visible in the area. But this river had become a small rain-fed one by 3000 BCE and dried up by 2000 BCE, just about the time Harappan civilization was disintegrating. There is no reason to deny that this canal is what remains of the legendary River Sarasvati, except that it will be against the currently popular South Russia 28CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE Homeland model of Proto IE language. The river had completely dried up by 1500 BCE (the hypothetical time of arrival of Vedic Aryans in South Asia) that the migrants would not have seen anything that looked like a river in the area. Thus, it could be argued that the references to a large full-flowing River Sarasvati in Rgveda actually rules out the possibility of arrival of Vedic Aryans in South Asia in the second MBCE. If River Sarasvati had had become a small river by 3000 BCE and dried up by 2000 BCE, Rgveda will have to be dated to the fourth millennium BCE or earlier. 9 Many ancient Vedic texts describe an unmoving star in the northern sky which was called ‘Dhruva Nakshatra’. The meaning of the word ‘Dhruva’ itself is ‘fixed’ or ‘unmoving’. The Brahmanda Purana uses the simile of “a lump of clay at the center of the potters wheel” to describe all celestial bodies in the sky circling ‘Dhruva’. Many ‘Grhya Sutra’ and other texts talk about and describe it in detail. There were only two instances in the last 10000 years of a star close enough to north celestial pole (NCP) to be considered as ‘fixed’ or all celestial bodies to be seen as circling it; Thuban or α Draconis in about 2900 BCE and Polaris in the last three hundred years. But many authors assume that either Polaris (α Ursa Minor) or Kochab (β Ursa Minor) was the Dhruva Nakshatra mentioned in the Hindu texts, for no reason other than the fact that these were the visible stars closest to NCP in the first millennium BCE. But Kochab was 80 and Polaris 170 away from NCP in 800 BCE (at the time the ‘Brahmanas’ were being composed as per the popular chronology) that it could not have been perceived as a ‘fixed’ stars or entire sky as circling either of these stars. Many of these texts describe the position of Dhruva in detail. It is said that it was in ‘Simsumara’ constellation. The constellation is stated to have 14 stars and all are named after various Vedic Devas and demigods, though the names often differ in different texts. Dhruva was the last in the tail of Simsumara or Dolphin. This description matches that of what we now call the Draco constellation in every respect. Draco or dragon also has 14 visible stars. Thuban is the last in the tail of Draco. Kochab and Polaris do not match this description in any way. For example Kochab is in the middle of Ursa Minor constellation or is in the middle of the body of the ‘small bear’ close to its neck. There can be no doubt about this identification, as some Grhya Sutra texts explain how to identify Dhruva in the sky. Thus identifications of Simsumara as Draco and Dhruva as Thuban are on very firm ground and if Dhruva was Thuban, there can be no doubt that the originals of these texts were composed in the first half of third millennium BCE. There are several astronomical references in the Vedic literature texts that point to very deep antiquity to these texts. Western authors mostly rubbish 3.4. ACCULTURATION A NEW APPROACH 29 these as unreliable. But these form a consistent set of data with its relative chronology fully in agreement with the currently accepted chronology of these texts; that is RV first; followed by other Vedas followed by Brahmanas and then the Sutra texts. It is difficult to see haw this consistency is possible unless these references were based on actual observation. 10 There is no reference to a migration event from the North West in any texts of Vedic literature or Itihasa/Puranas. As Pargiter has pointed out, the northWest frontier never had any ancient sacred memories and was never regarded with reverence. On the contrary, it was thought of as land of barbarians or as ‘Mlecha’ country; a very unlikely attitude if their ancestor’s home-land was in that direction. These people had retained their old memories for a very long time in the form of stories in these texts and if their ancestors had undertaken a very long, difficult and dangerous migration from the South Russian Steppe or Central Asia to North West India, some version of it would certainly be found in some of these texts. The only ‘migration myth’ mentioned in Vedic literature is the one by the first man; Manu, from the south; across the seas. The absence of any such reference can only mean that, if a migration from the Central Asia had indeed taken place in the second MBCE, it had little impact on the local languages, religious practices or culture; an impact significant enough to find at the least a passing mention in some of these texts. 30CHAPTER 3. INDO-ARYAN IN SOUTH ASIA IN SECOND MILLENNIUM BCE Chapter 4 Genetic Evidence 4.1 Evidence of Studies Based on Y-DNA Haplogroups Unravelling of the pre-history of IE languages has evaded the determined efforts of hundreds linguists, historians, archaeologists, and anthropologists for the past hundred and fifty years. But in the coming days we might get better insight into it through genetic studies; particularly those from ancient-DNA research. (But many archaeologists consider results of a-DNA studies as the ‘work of the devil’ as it seems to upend and rubbish many established conclusions and consensus built after many decades of hard work.) It all started from the turn of the century from advances made in our ability to identify mutations in Y-DNA and compare these with those present in different individuals. It is based on the fact that human Y-DNA and Mitochondrial DNA (Mt-DNA) preserves a written record of their mutations for thousands of years because neither gets mixed up or randomized as they are not involved in meiosis and gene crossover, as genes in other chromosomes in nucleus do. Any mutation to the Mt-DNA is passed on in a strict maternal line; and mutation in the Y chromosome is passed down in a direct paternal line of descent. Furthermore, the historical sequence of these mutations can also be inferred. Technology is now available to estimate the time when such a mutation occurred. Thus, it is now possible to determine the present descendants of a person who lived in the very distant past, and as those descendants may be found all over the world, it is possible to trace human migrations and its chronology since the time of the 31 32 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE ancestor. This technique has now emerged as a very powerful tool in the study of ancient pre-historic human migrations. I have listed here findings of some of the early studies that are relevant to our subject. *It is found that majority of individuals all over the world whose mothertongue belongs to the IE family, have Y-DNA mutations termed as Haplogroup (HP) R1 and its subclades. We now have two major sub-groups of HP R1; R1a mostly found in the Central and Eastern Europe, Iran, Afghanistan and South Asia; and R1b mostly in Southern and Western Europe. Also, those with R1a ancestry mostly speak ‘Satem’ branches and those with R1b the ‘Kentum’ branches of IE languages. *Many studies based on y-DNA mutations have concluded that there was no large scale genetic input into South Asia for the past 10000 years or more. Several studies have concluded that the data is at least consistent with South Asia as the likely original point of dispersal of what is believed to be the IndoEuropean Haplogroup; namely HG R1. For example, Kivisild et al. (2003), Mirabal et al. (2009) and Underhill et al. (2009), Sharma et al. (2009) have come to such conclusions. *The most frequent mtDNA haplogroups in South Asia are M, R and U. Stephen Oppenheimer in his book (2004) titled “The Real Eve” says that it is highly probable that nearly all human maternal lineages in Central Asia, the Middle East and Europe descended from only four mtDNA lines that originated in South Asia 50,000-100,000 years ago. The major South Asian Y-chromosome DNA haplogroups are H, J2, L, R1a1 and R2. Innumerable sub-clades of these are present in the Indian population. Geographical origin of Y-DNA HP H and R2 is probably South Asia. J2 probably originated in West Asia. Opinion about R1a, the probable IE marker, is divided. Most studies place it in Iran, South Asia or Central Asia. *It was the colonial historians, anthropologists and ethnographers who introduced the thesis that the Indian caste system is based on racial differences and this was treated as a historical fact until recently. But the admittedly limited genetic studies on Indian population groups have failed to find clear, consistent and statistically significant variations at the sub-continental scale between different castes. According to Sanghamitra Sahoo et al (2006 National Academy of Sciences, USA) “Even though more than one explanation could exist for genetic differentiation between castes and tribes in India, the Indo-Aryan migration scenario rested on the suggestion that all Indian caste groups are similar to each other while being significantly different from the tribes. Using a much more representative data set, numerically, geographically, and definitively, it was not possible 4.1. EVIDENCE OF STUDIES BASED ON Y-DNA HAPLOGROUPS 33 to confirm any of the purported differentiations between the caste and tribal pools. Although differences could be found to occur within particular regions, between particular caste and tribal groups, consistent and statistically significant variations at the sub-continental scale were not detected.” Today almost 50% of the South Asian population carries what is known as Ancestral North Indian (ANI) ancestry that include every population segments even in remote areas of the sub-continent. For example, tribal groups like the ‘Chenchus’ of Andhra Pradesh and the ‘Saharias’ of Madhya Pradesh show anomalously high proportions of R1a. The Chenchus speak a Dravidian language, and the Saharias probably spoke an Austro-Asiatic one not long ago. Most population groups in South Asia were practicing strict endogamy for the past c.3000 years. Thus, such deep population admixture must be a legacy of many thousands of years. It is unlikely that this could have happened in the last 3500 years, which will give only about 500 years for such deep admixture. Sahoo et al. (2006) and Sengupata et al. (2006) suggest that Indian caste populations have not been subject to any recent admixtures. Sanghamitra Sahoo concludes his study thus, “It is not necessary, based on the current evidence, to look beyond South Asia for the origins of the paternal heritage of the majority of Indians at the time of the onset of settled agriculture. The perennial concept of people, language, and agriculture arriving to India together through the northwest corridor does not hold up to close scrutiny. Recent claims for a linkage of haplogroups J2, L, R1a, and R2 with a contemporaneous origin for the majority of the Indian caste’s paternal lineages from outside South Asia are rejected, although our findings do support a local origin of haplogroups F* and H. Of the others, only J2 indicates an unambiguous recent external contribution, from West Asia rather than Central Asia.” Studies by Watkins et al. (2005) and Kivisild et al. (2003) based on autosomal markers also conclude that Indian caste and tribal populations have a common ancestry. * Mait Metspalu et al (2011) makes the following observation “Here we report data for more than 600,000 SNP markers genotyped in 142 samples from 30 ethnic groups in India. Our simulations show that one can detect differences in haplotype diversity for a migration event that occurred 500 generations ago, but chances to distinguish signals for older events will apparently decrease with increasing age because of recombination. In terms of human population history, our oldest simulated migration event occurred roughly 12,500 years ago and predates or coincides with the initial Neolithic expansion in the Near East. Thus, regardless of where this component was from (the Caucasus, Near East, Indus Valley, or Central Asia), its spread to other regions must have occurred well 34 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE before our detection limits at 12,500 years. Accordingly, the introduction of k5 to South Asia cannot be explained by recent gene flow, such as the hypothetical Indo-Aryan migration.” Another study that searched for West Eurasian groups most closely related to the Ancestral North Indian (ANI) ancestors of Indians, failed to find any evidence for shared ancestry between the ANI and groups in West Eurasia within the past 12,500 years. * An increasing number of studies have found South Asia to have the highest level of diversity of Y-STR haplotype variation within R1a1a. Most societies are found to demonstrate a high level of genomic differentiation between cohorts and different signatures of natural selection. This is particularly so in the case of South Asia. On this basis, several studies have concluded that the data is at least consistent with South Asia as the likely original point of dispersal of the IE marker R1a1. Top-level Haplogroups like R*, R1and R2 based SNP individuals are found in some pockets in Pakistan and NW India. These markers are rarely found in other parts of Eurasia. The only logical explanation for this is that the locus of origin of HP R is somewhere in this part of Eurasia. A 2015 study by Peter A. Underhill et al., using 16,244 individuals from over 126 populations from across Eurasia, concluded that “there was compelling evidence that the initial episodes of haplogroup R1a diversification likely occurred in the vicinity of present-day Iran. The split of R1a (M420) is computed to ca. 22,000, or 25,000 years ago.” “Whole Y-chromosome sequence analysis of eight R1a and five R1b individuals suggests a divergence time of c.25 000 (95% CI: 21 300-29 000) years ago” “Of the 1693 European R1a-M417 samples, more than 96% were assigned to R1a-Z282, whereas 98.4% of the 490 Central and South Asian R1a lineages belonged to hg R1a-Z93.” “The corresponding diversification in the Middle East and South Asia is more obscure. However, early urbanization within the Indus Valley also occurred at this time (4600 years before present (YBP)) and the geographic distribution of R1a-M780 may reflect this.” “We estimate the splintering of R1a-M417 to have occurred rather recently, c.5800 years ago (95% CI: 48006800). The slowest mutation rate estimate would inflate these time estimates by one-third, and the fastest would deflate them by 17%.” So the slowest mutation rate estimate can take the date for M780 to about 9000 YBP. “We caution against ascribing findings from a contemporary phylogenetic cluster of a single genetic locus to a particular pre-historic demographic event, population migration, or cultural transformation. The R1a TMRCA estimates 4.2. INCONSISTENCIES IN THE MODELS OF SPREAD OF IE LANGUAGES IN EU we report have wide confidence intervals and should be viewed as preliminary; one must sequence tens of additional R1a samples to high coverage to uncover additional informative substructure and to bolster the accuracy of the branch lengths associated with the more terminal portions of the phylogeny.” The report says that the splintering of R1a-M417 into z282 (European clade) and z93 (Asian clade) may have occurred in around 5800 YBP, but warns us about possible large error in the estimate of the time of the event. Besides, it warns us against “ascribing findings from a contemporary phylogenetic cluster of a single genetic locus to a particular pre-historic demographic event, population migration, or cultural transformation. Dr. Chaubey says that ‘there is a common ancestor for the European branch of R1a (Z282) and Indian branch of R1a (M780) but neither is a parent of the other’. Some researchers have pointed out that ‘Phylogenetically M 780, the subclade of R1a mostly found in South Asia, is not nested under any other R1a branch in the world or it evolved from some early stage of R1a’. Taking the reports of Kivisild et al. (2003), Mirabal et al. (2009) and Underhill et al. (2009), Sengupata et al. (2006), Sharma et al. (2009), Mait Metspalu et al (2011), Underhill et al.(2015) mentioned above and also Shinde.et.al.(2019) together, it can be concluded that HP R1a-M780, often described as the steppe ancestry element, reached South Asia before 10000 YBP. I have not come across any major reports of Y-DNA based studies after this, as the emphasis seems to have shifted to autosomes and a-DNA data. 4.2 Inconsistencies in the Models of Spread of IE Languages in Eurasia The current majority view is that groups with Y-DNA Haplogroups R1b-M269 and R1a ancestry expanded from the West Eurasian Steppe (Yamna horizon), along with the Indo-European languages sometime in the middle of third MBCE into different parts of Eurasia. The people with R1b ancestry travelled to South, West and North-West Europe as speakers of Kentum branches of IE languages and those with R1a based ancestry spread into Central and Eastern Europe as well as places like Iran and South Asia as speakers of Satem branches. This pattern is apparent all over the world at present, with majority of people with R1b ancestry speaking Kentum languages and majority of those with R1a ancestry speaking the Satem verity. Further, it is found that incidence of R1b is near 100 % in many places in 36 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE Western and Southern Europe. Similarly, in many areas of the eastern parts of Eurasia R1a ancestry is substantial, but incidence of R1b is very low or often completely absent. This leads to the inference that when these two groups began their expansion into various parts of Eurasia as speakers of Satem or Kentum dialects of IE, their ancestry profile was near 100% of either HP R1a or R1b. Entropy can only increase. If incidence of R1b is 90% in parts of Ireland and Northern Spain now, it must have been higher earlier. Genetic bottlenecks arguably could have limited the gene pool of the migrating groups, but it is impossible that it could have eliminated R1a or R1b selectively in many different locations, and that too most R1a from Western and Southern Europe and R1b from eastern parts of Eurasia. There is yet another aspect of the issue that just does not seem to add-up. Underhill et al. (2015), mentioned above, report that the split between R1a and R1b occurred some 25000 YBP in the vicinity of present-day Iran. Given this, Kurgan hypothesis will require that some 20000 years later, elements from these two branches came close to each other and were living in geographic proximity in the steppes. They then must have jointly developed PIE. (The idea of joint development of a language, with its basic words, grammatical, and phonetic forms itself is problematic, as it is unclear if it is possible at all) This scenario forces us to reconcile a paradox. Such a co-development of PIE would require very intimate social/cultural contacts between the two groups, and such close contacts would surely result in some mutual gene flow. But as mentioned in the preceding paragraph, ancestry profile of the groups must have been near 100% of either HP R1a or R1b as the groups again separated and moved away. After the split between R1a and R1b some 25000 YBP, it is apparent that the two groups moved to many parts of Eurasia in the subsequent millennia. Reports based on a-DNA tell us that these reached many areas of Europe and Anatolia by late Paleolithic and Mesolithic. But the two groups seem to have remained separate in most places. “Villabruna 1”, who lived c.14,000 YBP in north-east Italy, had R1b-L-754 and samples from central Germany dated to 5,800 to 5,350 YBP were found to be HP I and R1a. Such reports are available from other areas like Anatolia, The Caucus and areas around Zagros Mountains. The oldest forms of R1b (M343, P25, L389) are found dispersed at differing frequencies from Western Europe to India. What is relevant here is that there were many pockets in Eurasia with population with these ancestry profiles; and not just in the steppes. After the two groups separated in steppes, they moved away as speakers of Kentum and Satem branches of IE. Magically, they went only to places where the population had similar ancestry. That is; R1a groups went to places where other 4.2. INCONSISTENCIES IN THE MODELS OF SPREAD OF IE LANGUAGES IN EU R1a people lived and R1b groups also did the same. Also, both groups forced the locals to change their mother-tongue and become speakers of IE languages. This inference should follow from the fact that, at present the vast majority of people with R1a ancestry all over the world are speakers of Satem languages and those with R1b ancestry are speakers of Kentum languages. Also, people with these ancestry profile, but who are speakers of non-IE languages are only a fraction of the former. It was as if these people had some divine inspiration and knew exactly where to migrate. It is of course possible that everyone with R1 based ancestry, except those who were in the steppes died out. But a more reasonable inference would be that such groups were present in different areas of Eurasia at that time. The spread of IE languages in Europe also is not as clear as it is often made out to be. One of the pivotal unresolved issues of the Indo-European Urheimat or ‘Home land’ problem, is the origin and dispersal of Corded Ware culture, also often termed as Battle Axe culture. It comprised a broad archaeological horizon of Europe from the Rhine on the west to the Volga in the east between c. 3000 BCE -2350 BCE; from the late Neolithic, through the Copper Age, and ending in the early Bronze Age. The earliest radiocarbon dates for Corded Ware come from Kujawy and Lesser Poland in central and southern Poland, which point to a period around 3000 BCE. The characteristic shared elements of Corded Ware groups include their burial practices, pottery with ‘cord’ decoration and unique stone-axes. There is sharp disagreement between archaeologists regarding the origins of Corded Ware. Some archaeologists believe it is local in origin and sprang from central Europe while others saw an influence from nomadic pastoral societies of the steppes. In favour of the first view is the fact that Corded Ware coincides considerably with the earlier north-central European Funnel beaker culture (TRB 4500 -3000 BCE) and lobular Amphora culture (3400-2800 BCE). Corded Ware could be a continuation of these. Haplogroup R1a is the most common paternal haplogroup among males of the Corded Ware horizon, and has earlier been found among Eastern HunterGatherers (EHGs) of the Mesolithic. A November 2015 genetic study of the Funnel beaker culture published in Nature reports two male samples, which were found to carry the paternal haplogroups I and R1b1a or L754. Other 5,800 to 5,350 YBP samples from central Germany were found to be HP I and R1. (Due to poor quality of a-DNA, it is often impossible to tell whether or not the ancients carried the mutations that define subclades.) Subclades of HP I can be found in most present-day European populations, with peaks in some Northern European and South East European countries. Its origin could be somewhere 38 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE in the eastern parts of Eurasia and might have reached Europe c. 20000 YBP. Thus it appears that the Corded Ware people had IE related ancestry (R1a, R1b, I) long before the steppe nomads could have reached these areas, as Kurgan Hypothesis chronology posits split of PIE into daughter languages in the middle of third MBCE. Similarly, archaeological evidence shows that most of Southern, Western and North-West Europe had what is known as ‘Bell Beaker culture’ at the beginning of the late Neolithic and early European Bronze Age in about 3000 BCE till about 2300 BCE. These people had ancestry based on Y-DNA HP R1b and HP I and its subclades. The earliest find of R1b anywhere is what is known as ‘Villabruna 1’, who lived c. 14,000 YBP in north east Italy and it had Y-DNA HP R1b-L754. R1bM269 is now the most common Y-DNA lineage in European males. L754 is a basal or ancestor clade of M269 Phylogenetic tree. There are several other finds from Southern Europe, Balkans and Anatolia which are also mostly L754 or its subclades that are dated to Mesolithic and early Neolithic periods. The steppe nomads could not have reached Southern and Western Europe before the middle of third MBCE, and it appears that the Beakers had IE related ancestry long before that. There is little clear archaeological evidence for influence of migrants from the steppe in Southern, western and North-Western Europe during the third millennium BCE. Thus ‘The beaker phenomenon’ could well be a local development, just as Corded Ware in Central and Eastern Europe. A-DNA studied so far from the steppes of Yamna period belonged to R1bM269 or R1b-L23, which are similar to the present European clades. But this in itself may not be enough to argue for a migration from there to Southern and Western Europe in the third MBCE. Besides, nobody seem to have any clear idea of how this 5000 KM journey from areas around River Dnieper, north of Black Sea to Southern Europe was undertaken 4500 YBP; the exact route followed by the migrants; whether it was a large wave or migration by small groups; whether it was a few quick movement or of many small groups that lasted many centuries and how it was completed without much mutual genetic transfer with the people whom they might have encountered along the route. Such a movement also would surely have resulted in a series of clashes with local communities along the route. Some authors seem to be eager to deny that IE related ancestry was present in Europe before the arrival of steppe nomads in the third MBCE. They propose some complicated models for this purpose. It is said that those people with R1b ancestry migrated from Southern Europe through Anatolia, The Caucus and reached the steppe. After the development of PIE, they again migrated to 4.2. INCONSISTENCIES IN THE MODELS OF SPREAD OF IE LANGUAGES IN EU Western and Southern Europe as speakers of Kentum IE languages. There are at least two problems with this model. First, there is no evidence for the first part of the migration from Europe to the steppes. Secondly, it will still leave out the presence of I and R1a based ancestry found in Northern and Central Europe from at least the Mesolithic. It is said that archaeology has “documented a massive migration into the heartland of Europe from its eastern periphery” in the third MBCE. But at least part of this interpretation of archaeological data is because archaeologists were looking for evidence for such a migration. They were trained to assign migrations, invasions and population displacements as reasons for differences in one archaeological layer and the next. But many of these could just as well be due to gradual changes of cultural practices and subsistence strategies. These might also be the result of innovations or borrowing and adaptations from other groups with which they were in contact. It is often very difficult to determine what resulted in the apparent cultural differences between two archaeological layers. Invasions, battles and ‘heroic’ victories have a masculine, romantic appeal, but we forget that these have often ended in mass genocides. In contrast, an inference of local innovation may be mundane and have a lot less appeal. Many such changes all over the world were declared as the result of invasions and ‘great’ victories in the past. But there is now rethinking in many such cases. The reason why most people insist on an influx of steppe migrants into Europe in third MBCE is that they believe the Kurgan Hypothesis and its chronology is a fact of history. It is treated as a maxim which by definition needs no proof and is unquestionable. But it should be kept in mind that the chronology is mostly based on linguistic paleontology, which is suspected to be quite fragile and not entirely dependable. Thus it may be prudent to take a closer look at the entire model. It is difficult to see how such an unlikely series of events described above is possible. The only rational explanation could be that PIE is much older than what is assumed now and the founder group of Y-DNA HP R1 spoke an early form of PIE 25000 Years before Present, before the split into R1a and R1b. But such a conclusion will destabilize “the co development of PIE in the steppe in the third MBCE” part or even the ‘steppe home land’ model itself. 40 4.3 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE Arrival of Indo-Aryan Speakers in South Asia - A new Perspective The followings quotes are from two recent (2019) a-DNA based studies; the first based on hundreds of samples from sites in Southern Central Asia (BMAC sites) and parts of Afghanistan. The second is primarily based on one 4500 year old sample from Raghigarhi; an important Harappan settlement. These bring out some very interesting results. Narasimhan et.al. “The primary ancestry source of all these ancient samples from Central Asia and Eastern Iran was the Iranian farmer related ancestry, the same ancestry type which was also the main ancestry component of IVC people. This Iranian farmer related ancestry was the main ancestral source in Central Asia even in Chalcolithic period (4000-3000 BC) which means that already by the Chalcolithic period the populations of Central Asia and NW South Asia had a lot of shared ancestry before the IVC migration into Central Asia during the Bronze Age. How old is this shared ancestry should be a matter of future research.” An interesting finding of this report is that there were many people from IVC settlements living in Central Asia and BMAC in c.2500 BCE. But there is no evidence of a reverse flow. Shinde.et.al. (2019) “Our evidence that the Iranian-related ancestry in the IVC Cline diverged from lineages leading to ancient Iranian hunter-gatherers, herders, and farmers prior to their ancestors separation, places constraints on the spread of Iranian-related ancestry across the combined region of the Iranian plateau and South Asia, where it is represented in all ancient and modern genomic data sampled to date. The Belt Cave individual dates to c.10,000 BCE, definitively before the advent of farming anywhere in Iran, which implies that the split leading to the Iranian-related component in the IVC Cline pre-dates the advent of farming there as well. Even if we do not consider the results from the low-coverage Belt Cave individual, our analysis shows that the Iranian-related lineage present in the IVC Cline individuals split before the date of the c.8000 BCE Ganj Dareh individuals, who lived in the Zagros mountains of the Iranian plateau before crop farming began there around c.7000-6000 BCE. Thus, the Iranian-related ancestry in the IVC Cline descends from a different group of hunter-gatherers from the ancestors of the earliest known farmers or herders in the western Iranian plateau.” The relative figure given in the report shows that the Iranian-related ancestry found in the IVC as well as most parts of South Asia at present separated from the parent body well before 10000 BCE. Mait Metspalu et. al. (2011) mentioned 4.3. ARRIVAL OF INDO-ARYAN SPEAKERS IN SOUTH ASIA - A NEW PERSPECT above also comes to similar conclusion. An interesting conclusion of Shinde.et.al. is that this separation was well before agriculture was invented in the Fertile Crescent or areas east of Zagros Mountains in Iran. Because of this, the report concludes that agriculture was invented in South Asia independently; and not through migrating farmers from these areas. It also suggests another possibility that it might be a case of ‘knowledge transfer’ through trade/stray contacts. Thus, primary ancestry source of IVC, Central Asia, Afghanistan and Eastern Iran were the same from very ancient times, although the South Asian branch separated from Iranian branch before 10000 YBP as per Shinde.et.al.(2019). This common ancestry was unrelated to those from Fertile Crescent, Mesopotamia or the steppe. We also find that later most of these people in Iran, Afghanistan and South Asia were speakers of closely related IE languages and had very similar cultural and religious practices. Since we are now told by Peter A. Underhill et al. (2014) that “the initial episodes of haplogroup R1a diversification likely occurred in the vicinity of present-day Iran before 25000 YBP”, this could well be the source of the R1a based ancestry found in these areas at present. The straightforward conclusion should be that the ethnicity and languages of the area did not undergo substantial changes after 10000 BCE. There is no reference either in the Avesta or Vedic texts of a migration event from the NW. Avesta seems to be unaware of lands north of southern parts of Central Asia. Greek chroniclers mention that Iranians were unaware about the Aral Sea, just about 1000 KM north of North Eastern border of Iran, in the Uzbek/Kazakh border. For Vedic Aryans, Aryan home-land or ‘Aryavarta’ was in the Gangetic plains (Interestingly, the highest incidence of R1a in South Asia is in ‘Aryavarta’ or areas east of River Ganges) and for Zoroastrians, the home-land of Aryas or ’Airiya’ was in ‘Airyanem Vaejah’; the location of which is uncertain and controversial. Neither had any retained memory of a distant home-land in the north-west. This is unlikely if the ancestors of Vedic-Aryans and Zoroastrians had come from that direction. The vast area that had this similar ancestry and languages also has some of the remotest locations in the world and it would have been difficult to completely replace the language, customs and even ethnic identity from the entire area, with little trace of the original. Any model that assumes complete substitution of languages, culture, religious practices, apart from ancestry in the area during this period will require many special pleadings, particularly as there is no firm evidence for such a change, except for some controversial linguistic hypotheses. Besides, complete demographic displacements or language substitution could occur only in very special circumstances, particularly when large population size and vast geographical area are involved. We should be skeptic of any models suggesting or based on such changes. 42 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE Some studies have also found evidence to support substantial steppe genetic input into South Asia in the second MBCE. “aDNA study indicated the Steppe MLBA and not the Steppe EMBA genomes to be the plausible source of Steppe ancestry among South Asians genomes”. Narasimhan et al., 2019 “However, a natural route for Indo-European languages to have spread into South Asia is from Eastern Europe via Central Asia in the first half of the 2nd millennium BCE, a chain of transmission that did occur as has been documented in detail with ancient DNA. The fact that the Steppe pastoralist ancestry in South Asia matches that in Bronze Age Eastern Europe (but not Western Europe [de Barros Damgaard et al., 2018; Narasimhan et al., 2019]) provides additional evidence for this theory, as it elegantly explains the shared distinctive features of Balto-Slavic and Indo-Iranian languages” Shinde et al., 2019. But other studies (Underhill et al.) have found that z286 (European R1a) and z93 (Asian R1a) had separated in around 6000 YBP. Z282 is rarely found east of Urals and z93 is rare in Eastern Europe. From this it was concluded that there was no gene flow from South Russian Steppe towards Iran and South Asia after 6000 YBP. The first only talks about a “plausible source of Steppe ancestry among South Asians genomes” and second report is not based on data from within South Asia, except one sample from IVC (Raghigarhi). Besides, large migration is now ruled out by most linguists and archaeologists. These also contradict many other reports, some of which I have quoted above. Nicole Boivin offered this critique of genetic studies: “In reading the genetics literature on South Asia, it is very clear that many of the studies actually start out with some assumptions that are clearly problematic, if not in some cases completely untenable. Perhaps the single most serious problem concerns the assumption, which many studies actually start with as a basic premise that the IndoAryan invasions are a well established (pre)historical reality.” (Boivin, 2007: 352)). It should also be kept in mind that the findings mentioned in such papers are the best possible subjective inferences based on the data analyzed, but might fall short of being rigorous objective conclusions, the reason being that we are only just beginning to unravel the complexity of the structure of DNA. Errors can occur due to quantity and quality issues of the data acquired, and also interpretation of it. Space for subjective Interpretation can become less as quantity and quality of the data improves and it acquires the ability to speak for itself. For example, U. Esposito et.al. (Biorxiv preprint) make the following remark “Half of all published ancient human genomes lack reliable and direct dates, which results in obscure and contradictory reports.” But we are now witnessing exponential growth in related research, which is also leading to improving tech- 4.3. ARRIVAL OF INDO-ARYAN SPEAKERS IN SOUTH ASIA - A NEW PERSPECT niques and technologies. Hopefully these should result in more robust results in the days to come. 44 CHAPTER 4. GENETIC EVIDENCE Chapter 5 Conclusion To sum-up, there are compelling evidences to conclude that a wave of migrants belonging to Y-DNA Haplogroup R1a or its subclades, who spoke an early form of Proto-Indo-Aryan language arrived (or were present) in South Asia before 10000 BCE and these migrants provided a major component of the present South Asian ancestry. It would then follow that Harappan and Vedic civilizations as well as Hindu religion were the result of the gradual cultural evolution of the admixed population of these migrants with local communities. This is not a perfect model, as it does not explain many adverse facts and data, but I find other models discussed at present even less acceptable. Critics might revile the model I am proposing, as none of the evidences/issues I have discussed above can be treated as clinching or conclusive, but the combined effect of all these should be considerable, if not decisive. It may be just one of the possibilities, but one of the better ones. In the end we should have the humility to admit that at present there are serious gaps in our knowledge of how Indo-Aryan dialects came to be the languages of people living in the northern parts of South Asia. Discussion on the subject at present is often like those related to ‘flat Earth theory’ in some parts of the world - anyone who tries to propose a new model or a variation in a model with a large following, is often shouted down by a chorus of believers. But it is a common human failure that we are always quite sure that our own opinions and beliefs are always correct and any contrary view must be on account of ideological bias, untenable or dubious logic or deficient scholarship or simply ignorance. Such bias tends to creep in without one being aware of it. It may be that I am also a victim of the bug. 45