Research Article Predictors of Participation Difficulties in Autistic Children Importance: Participation in meaningful occupations supports quality of life and health. Because quality of life is lower in autistic children than in children without this diagnosis, it is important to consider aspects contributing to the participation difficulties this population experiences. Objective: To identify predictors of participation difficulties in a large data set from autistic children to inform professionals about potential intervention targets. Design: Retrospective cross-sectional design using a large data set with multivariate regression models for home life, friendships, classroom learning, and leisure activities. Setting: 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services data set. Participants: Parents or caregivers of 834 autistic children with co-occurring intellectual disability (ID) and 227 autistic children with no ID. Results: The strongest participation predictors within the scope of occupational therapy practice were sensory processing, emotional regulation, behavioral variables, and social variables. Our results are consistent with those of smaller previous studies and indicate the importance of addressing these areas in occupational therapy intervention in line with client priorities. Conclusion and Relevance: Focusing interventions with autistic children on sensory processing, emotional regulation, behavioral skills, and social skills to address their underlying neurological processing can support their increased participation in home life, friendships, classroom learning, and leisure activities. What This Article Adds: Our findings support a focus in occupational therapy interventions on sensory processing and social skills to increase activity participation in autistic children with and without ID. Emotional regulation and behavioral skills can be supported by interventions that target cognitive flexibility. Positionality Statement: This article uses the identity-first language autistic people. This nonableist language describes their strengths and abilities and is a conscious decision. This language is favored by autistic communities and self-advocates and has been adopted by health care professionals and researchers (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2021; Kenny et al., 2016). Hilton, C. L., Ratcliff, K., & Hong, I. (2023). Predictors of participation difficulties in autistic children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 77, 7702205010. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2023.050068 or optimal growth and development, children and adolescents need to participate in a variety of activities (Bohnert et al., 2019). Diverse activity participation results in the creation of neural pathways that aid the development of competence in a wide range of activities vital for daily life functioning (Demetriou et al., 2018; Reynolds et al., 2011) and the automatization of movements and social communication (Hirata et al., 2014), leading to greater life satisfaction, lifetime health, well-being, and quality of life (McCollum et al., 2016). Autistic F children encounter difficulty participating in many activities (Hilton et al., 2008) and enjoy a less robust range of developmentally supportive activities than children without this diagnosis (Hilton et al., 2019; Taheri et al., 2016). Potvin et al. (2013) found that the out-of-school activity participation preferences of autistic children were similar to those of peers without a diagnosis, except for lower physical activity preferences among the autistic children. Limited diversity in activity participation is not exclusively related to person factors; it can also be THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 1 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 Claudia L. Hilton, Karen Ratcliff, Ickpyo Hong Areas of Participation Home Life As with all of the outcome variables in the current study, home life is not clearly defined in the data set we used from the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services that was conducted by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (hereinafter referred to as “Pathways”; Child and Adolescent Health Measurement Initiative [CAHMI], 2015). However, the literature generally defines the term to include interactions with family members, family mealtime, meal preparation, care of other family members, self-care, chores, homework, personal care, cleanup, and care of animals or pets (Anaby et al., 2013). Home life spans several occupations from the Occupational Therapy Practice Framework: Domain and Process (4th ed.; OTPF–4; AOTA, 2020), including activities of daily living (ADLs), instrumental activities of daily living, education, and social participation. Autistic children participate less in home life than children with no diagnosis, particularly in the areas of personal care management, household chores, and school preparation (McCollum et al., 2016), and difficulties with home life continue into adulthood (Smith et al., 2012). Studies have identified lack of skills in daily living, communication, and socialization as major barriers to home life participation (Liss et al., 2001; Perry et al., 2009). Classroom Learning Previously identified areas of concern regarding classroom learning include behavioral and emotional differences, school performance, social activities, communication, following rules and directions, personal care awareness, task completion, positive social interactions, and safety (Sparapani et al., 2016). Classroom learning is part of the occupation of education in the OTPF–4. Many areas of concern are related to executive function, a frequent area of difficulty for autistic individuals (Demetriou et al., 2018). Sparapani et al. (2016) found that autistic children spent less time emotionally regulated and actively engaged in the classroom than other children. Autistic children experience behavioral and emotional differences from their peers without a diagnosis that are potential predictors of classroom learning, including shorter attention span, anxiety, depression, and behaviors that can be disruptive in the classroom (Ashburner et al., 2010). Aspects of the classroom environment contribute to these behaviors and may be potential targets for intervention (Martin, 2016). Friendships The friendships of autistic children without ID differ from those of children without an ASD diagnosis in self-, peer-, and teacher-rated quality and reciprocity (Kasari et al., 2011). Friendships are part of the occupation of social participation in the OTPF–4. In a study examining the friendships of young adults with ASD only, significantly more were likely to never see friends, to never be called by friends, to never be invited to activities, and to be socially isolated compared with young adults with other diagnoses who received special education services (Orsmond et al., 2013). Leisure Activities Leisure is defined as intrinsically motivated nonobligatory activity that is engaged in during time not committed to obligatory occupations such as work, self-care, or sleep (AOTA, 2020; Parham & Fazio, 1997). This definition is consistent with that of the occupation of leisure in the OTPF–4. Recreation, a category of leisure, consists of an individual’s preferred pleasurable and enjoyable activities engaged in during leisure time (Veal, 1992) and includes both physically active and solitary sedentary activities (e.g., watching television and videos; playing video games; using THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 2 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 an outcome of environmental and task-related factors (Law et al., 1996). Longitudinal studies of severity trajectories in autistic children have shown mixed outcomes. One study that examined autistic children ages 3 to 11 yr found symptom severity changes in 51% of participants, with 27% experiencing increased and 24% decreased severity (Waizbard-Bartov et al., 2022). In a different type of data analysis, an older group, ages 12 to 23 yr, showed an increase in intelligence while autism symptoms remained unchanged (Simonoff et al., 2020). Although autistic people may not consider reducing symptom severity a priority (Leadbitter et al., 2021), their symptoms can limit diversity in their activity participation, preventing the creation of neural pathways that facilitate competence in adult roles and expectations (Reynolds et al., 2011). As autistic children age into adolescence, their participation differences increase compared with peers without a diagnosis (Ratcliff et al., 2018). These differences may contribute to difficulty as adults in living independently, becoming employed, going to college, and getting married (Baldwin et al., 2014). Previously, Hilton and colleagues (2021) compared parent-reported difficulties experienced by children with intellectual disability (ID) with those experienced by autistic children with and without ID in four areas: home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities. Friendships was the most commonly reported area of difficulty for all of these children. Autistic children with ID experienced the greatest difficulties in all four areas, followed by those with autism alone. In the current study, we examined predictors of difficulties in these four areas in autistic children with and without ID to inform potential intervention targets to support broader participation interests. We selected the same outcome areas as in the previous study because they are often identified as participation categories of concern in the autism spectrum disorder (ASD) literature. computers, cellphones, and other electronic devices). Although many studies have found lower levels of physically active recreational activity in autistic children (Askari et al., 2015; Hilton et al., 2008; Potvin et al., 2013), Ratcliff et al. (2018) found no significant difference in recreation participation between children with and without ASD, but they did not differentiate between physically active and solitary sedentary leisure. To promote greater diversity of participation by autistic children and adolescents, occupational therapy practitioners should understand the factors that influence participation so they can design interventions in collaboration with clients and care providers to address the barriers. We used a large cross-sectional database to examine predictors of parent-reported participation difficulties in the types of activities in which autistic children engage. On the basis of prior research findings, we selected the following predictor variables: Contextual variables: age, sex, race or ethnicity, parent education level, household income, health insurance coverage ADLs and functional and adaptive skills Skill-related variables: fine motor, learning (i.e., understanding, paying attention) Behavioral variables: conduct problems, hyperactivity, acting out (i.e., fighting, bullying, arguing) Emotional regulation variables: emotional regulation problems, anxiety or depression Social variables: social problems, prosocial behaviors Sensory processing variables: sensory seeking, sensory avoidant, low registration (i.e., disorganized, overreactive), repetitive behaviors. Our aims in the current study were to identify and compare the strength of these predictors of activity participation difficulties in home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities in autistic children with and without co-occurring ID in order to better inform professionals about potential targets of intervention. We formed five hypotheses: 1. Sensory processing, emotional regulation, and behavioral variables and household income predict participation difficulties in home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities for both groups. 2. Social variables predict participation difficulties in home life, friendships, and leisure activities for both groups. 3. Social variables, parent education level, household income, and health insurance coverage predict classroom learning and friendships for both groups. 4. Sensory processing and emotional regulation variables are stronger predictors than contextual variables. 䊏 䊏 䊏 䊏 䊏 䊏 䊏 Method We used a retrospective cross-sectional complex sample design (N 5 4,032) to examine predictive factors for the four outcome variables of home life, friendships, classroom learning, and leisure activities. We sought to identify characteristics of autistic children that can be addressed in intervention targeting more robust participation and to reveal any potential confounding demographic variables. Participants We examined data from the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services data set (CAHMI, 2015). This survey is part of a repeated cross-sectional survey of parents and caregivers of children with special health care needs who have ever had a diagnosis of ASD, ID, or developmental delay from all 50 U.S. states; data were weighted to represent the U.S. population. Survey respondents were 6,090 parents or caregivers of children ages 6 to 17 yr, and phone interviews were conducted with 4,032 respondents. We excluded 1,112 respondents who did not complete questions about home life, friendships, classroom learning, and leisure activities. We analyzed data from reports having no missing observations (N 5 2,920) for children with ASD only (n 5 834), ASD and ID (n 5 227), ID only (n 5 508), developmental disorder only (n 5 995), and no current diagnosis (n 5 356; see Appendix Figure A.1). Missing responses for 236 children were imputed in the data analyses. This study did not require approval or oversight from the institutional review board at the University of Texas Medical Branch (No. 18–044) because we used publicly available, deidentified data. Study Variables The Pathways data include 25 social–behavioral items categorized into five social–behavioral areas: emotional regulation problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/ inattention, social problems, and prosocial behavior (see Appendix Table A.1). Higher scores indicate greater social behavioral difficulties. We also examined four sensory domains—sensory seeking, sensory avoidant, low registration/disorganized/overreactive, and repetitive behaviors—using the 15 Pathways sensory items (Lee et al., 2019). Lee and colleagues (2019) calibrated the sensory items with item response theory models and found good psychometric properties, including structural and known group validity. Higher scores represent greater sensory differences. ADL, skill-related, and behavioral variables from the Pathways data set included ADLs (eating, dressing, bathing), fine motor difficulty (using hands), learning (understanding, paying attention), anxiety or THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 3 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 Potential Predictors of Activity Participation Difficulties 5. Poorer ADLs and functional and adaptive skills predict greater participation difficulties in home life and classroom learning. depression, and behavior problems (acting out, fighting, bullying, arguing). These items were rated using a 3-point scale (1 5 no difficulty, 2 5 a little difficulty, 3 5 a lot of difficulty). Higher scores represent more difficulties. Contextual variables consisted of parent education level, race or ethnicity, household income, and health insurance coverage and were included as the study covariates. Outcome Variables Data Analysis We conducted population-weighted descriptive data analyses for the demographic characteristics using t tests for the continuous variable (age) and chi-square tests for the categorical variables (sex, race or ethnicity, Results Table 1 presents the population-weighted descriptive statistics for the two diagnosis groups. The estimated Table 1. Participant Demographics Weighted % [95% CI] Variables ASD Age, M (SE) ASD and ID 11.1 (0.18) 12.0 (0.38) 62.6 [60.0, 65.2] 18.2 [16.1, 20.3] 14.4 [12.4, 16.4] 4.7 [3.6, 5.8] Non-Hispanic White (n 5 808) 55.0 [52.3, 57.7] 12.6 [10.9, 14.3] Non-Hispanic Black (n 5 53) 6.8 [5.1, 8.5] 2.2 [1.5, 2.9] Hispanic (n 5 85) 8.5 [6.7, 10.3] 4.2 [2.9, 5.5] Non-Hispanic other (n 5 115) 6.7 [5.4, 8.0] 3.9 [2.9, 4.9] Less than high school (n 5 147) 16.7 [14.4, 19.0] 8.0 [6.3, 9.7] More than high school (n 5 914) 60.3 [57.6, 63.0] 14.9 [13.2, 16.6] <100% FPL (n 5 141) 12.3 [10.2, 14.4] 4.4 [3.3, 5.5] ≥100% FPL (n 5 920) 64.8 [62.1, 67.5] 18.4 [16.3, 20.5] 75.1 [72.8, 77.4] 22.1 [19.9, 24.3] 2.0 [1.1, 2.9] 0.7 [0.3, 1.1] Sex Male (n 5 833) Female (n 5 227) Missing (n 5 1) a Race or ethnicity Parent education level Household income Health care coverage Yes (n 5 1,035) No (n 5 26) Note. ASD 5 autism spectrum disorder; FPL 5 federal poverty level; ID 5 intellectual disabilities; SE 5 standard error. Data are from the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services (Child and Adolescent Health Measurement Initiative, 2015). a Data are missing for ASD only. Not applicable for ASD and AD. THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 4 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 The outcome variables were home life (Item C27CA), friendships (Item C27CB), classroom learning (Item C27CC), and leisure activities (Item C27CD). For those four items, parents or caregivers were asked, “Do the difficulties interfere with your child’s everyday life?” Respondents used a 4-point rating scale (1 5 not at all, 2 5 only a little, 3 5 quite a lot, 4 5 a great deal), which we operationally dichotomized as 0 5 not at all/only a little, 1 5 quite a lot/a great deal to estimate odds ratios. parent education level, household income, health insurance coverage). Next, we performed four individual multivariate logistic regression models for the outcome variables and identified significant predictor variables. For the multivariate logistic regression analyses, we examined whether the study variables violated the multicollinearity assumption, and we checked the model fit. To control for the multilevel sampling structures of the complex survey database, we conducted subpopulation approaches using the entire sample with the sampling weight, strata, and cluster information; however, the primary analyses were produced using data solely for the autistic children with and without ID. For the multivariate logistic regression models, we used the SAS MI and MIANALYZE procedure modules to conduct the fully conditional specification as multiple imputations for the 236 missing observations in the study variables (Allison, 2002). In the individual multivariate regression models, we calculated the point estimations as adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals. We used SAS Version 9.4 for data management and all study analyses. Discussion Large data research provides opportunities to examine patterns of responses among a large number of participants. The focus of this study was on parent-reported participation difficulties specific to children and adolescents; readers should note that parent-identified priorities may differ from the self-reported priorities of the children and adolescents themselves and from the priorities of autistic adults. Our findings support those of past and current research on areas that clinicians have targeted in therapeutic interventions, add additional directions for intervention development, and include some outcomes that were unexpected. We found the strongest predictors of participation difficulties across the contexts examined to be sensory processing, emotional regulation, and behavioral variables; social variables; contextual variables; and ADLs and functional and adaptive skills. Differences between autistic children with and without ID were generally minor. Sensory Processing, Emotional Regulation, and Behavioral Variables We hypothesized that sensory processing, emotional regulation, and behavioral variables would predict participation difficulties in home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities for autistic children with and without ID. This hypothesis was supported by some of the variables in these categories. Sensory processing, emotional regulation, and behavioral variables are interconnected and consistent with theoretical understandings of sensory processing; sensory information registered from the environment is processed in the brain, which directs emotional and behavioral responses (Blair & Diamond, 2008; Smith Roley et al., 2007). Management of emotional and behavioral responses is governed by executive function systems that work to control attentional processes, cognitive flexibility, goal setting, and information processing and enable individuals to control themselves physically, cognitively, and emotionally (Monteiro, 2021). These processes support a person’s ability to participate in home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities and may play a role as predictors of participation in these activities. The role of sensory processing as an activity participation predictor was identified by Loh et al. (2021), who found that sensory processing was related to participation in childhood occupations, and by Choi and Jung (2021), who found sensory processing to be a predictor of leisure participation in early adolescents with no diagnosis. Werkman et al. (2020) examined the relationships among sensory processing, emotional and behavioral problems, and social participation and found that autistic individuals with higher cognitive abilities and sensory processing difficulties demonstrated more emotional and behavioral problems and that social participation was restricted by sensory processing difficulties regardless of cognitive abilities. Other studies have linked emotional and behavioral responses to participation. In a study examining home life participation, Reynolds et al. (2011) found that fewer autistic children completed chores compared with unaffected peers and that sensory processing contributed to participation in both play and leisure activities and job or chore activities. Another study found that the social and cognitive demands of activities limited participation in home life (Egilson et al., 2018). Sparapani et al.’s (2016) study supports our findings of the importance of emotional regulation and behavioral variables, finding less time spent being emotionally regulated and actively engaged in the classroom among the autistic children in comparison with the other children. Freeman et al. (2017) found that the autistic children who were rated by teachers as having poor initiation, working memory, and planning and organization skills spent more time engaging in solitary play when on the playground. Several studies found that lower emotional regulation and behavioral skills (executive function) contributed the most to lower participation in classroom learning (Kheirollahzadeh et al., 2021; Zingerevich & LaVesser, 2009). Sensory processing (Zingerevich & LaVesser, 2009) and motor proficiency (Kheirollahzadeh et al., 2021) were also identified as contributors to reduced participation in classroom learning. The ability to manage emotional and behavioral responses has been shown to be important for friendship participation. Fong and Iarocci (2020) found the executive function skills of emotional regulation and behavioral regulation to be associated with social competence in a study of 77 autistic children. In another study, increased participation in organized activity and better friendship quality were demonstrated by youth with better emotional control (Bohnert et al., 2019). Findings from the current study support those of previous research identifying sensory processing as a predictor of participation. Hochhauser and EngelYeger (2010) found lower leisure participation to be THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 5 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 population samples were 98,679 (21.5%) for autistic children with ID and 360,170 (78.5%) for autistic children without ID. The demographics of the two groups were similar, except for parent education level in the ASD group (p 5 .017). There was no significant difference in household income between the two groups (p 5 .675). The multivariate logistic regression models did not violate the multicollinearity assumption and revealed good model fit (all ps > .05). Results indicated some differences in predictors (emotional regulation variables, social variables, behavioral variables, sensory processing variables, ADLs and skill-related variables, and contextual variables) for autistic children with and without ID and across participation areas. Results are categorized by participation areas (Table 2). Predictors 0.82 [0.40, 1.69] 1.02 [0.92, 1.14] ASD 1.01 [0.54, 1.89] 0.50 [0.20, 1.27] 0.94 [0.37, 2.39] 1.01 [0.21, 4.94] 1.32 [0.78, 2.24] Learning 1.10 [0.96, 1.27] 1.34 [0.91, 1.97] Hyperactivity Acting out Social problems 1.05 [0.91, 1.21] 1.34 [0.87, 2.07] Anxiety or depression Social variables 1.15 [1.02, 1.30]* Emotional regulation problems Emotional regulation variables 1.31 [1.09, 1.57]* Conduct problems Behavioral variables 1.00 [0.63, 1.60] 1.24 [0.79, 1.95] Fine motor skills Skill-related variables Activities of daily living No Health insurance coverage (Ref. 5 yes) ≥100% FPL Household income (Ref. 5 <100% FPL) High school or less Parent education level (Ref. 5 more than high school) Other Race or ethnicity (Ref. 5 non-Hispanic White) Male Sex (Ref. 5 female) Age Contextual variables ASD + ID 0.92 [0.71, 1.18] 0.79 [0.31, 2.04] 1.32 [0.92, 1.91] 0.94 [0.40, 2.24] 1.18 [0.84, 1.65] 1.54 [1.07, 2.21]* 0.95 [0.20, 4.37] 0.48 [0.23, 1.01] 0.85 [0.36, 1.98] 0.23 [0.03, 1.79] 5.44 [1.16, 25.47]* 0.83 [0.19, 3.59] 1.27 [0.34, 4.66] 2.91 [0.82, 10.34] 0.93 [0.79, 1.10] Home Life 1.58 [1.35, 1.85]* 0.95 [0.60, 1.48] 1.31 [1.13, 1.52]* 1.43 [0.86, 2.38] 1.09 [0.92, 1.29] 0.98 [0.78, 1.23] 1.29 [0.74, 2.26] 1.35 [0.81, 2.23] 1.15 [0.64, 2.08] 2.48 [0.39, 15.86] 1.69 [0.54, 5.24] 0.56 [0.23, 1.34] 0.90 [0.42, 1.94] 0.56 [0.24, 1.33] 0.99 [0.90, 1.10] ASD 1.19 [0.51, 2.77] 0.87 [0.41, 1.85] 1.66 [0.83, 3.34] 0.67 [0.34, 1.34] 1.02 [0.93, 1.11] ASD 1.89 [1.24, 2.89]* 0.79 [0.35, 1.78] 0.79 [0.48, 1.31] 1.67 [0.55, 5.06] 1.54 [1.07, 2.21]* 1.11 [0.77, 1.59] 0.11 [0.01, 1.15] 0.60 [0.22, 1.62] 1.41 [0.44, 4.47] 1.04 [0.92, 1.17] 0.82 [0.55, 1.24] 1.09 [0.96, 1.25] 0.91 [0.57, 1.47] 1.22 [1.06, 1.40]* 1.05 [0.88, 1.26] 1.71 [1.00, 2.91]* 0.96 [0.61, 1.51] 1.36 [0.85, 2.17] 0.53 [0.26, 1.07] 1.04 [0.95, 1.13] ASD 0.68 [0.35, 1.32] 1.09 [0.83, 1.43] 1.37 [0.48, 3.92] 1.20 [0.81, 1.79] 0.24 [0.09, 0.65]* 1.39 [1.04, 1.85]* 1.34 [0.88, 2.05] 3.08 [0.30, 31.88] 0.82 [0.32, 2.10] 1.71 [0.53, 5.51] 1.07 [0.95, 1.21] 0.93 [0.58, 1.50] 1.22 [1.06, 1.40]* 0.93 [0.61, 1.41] 1.02 [0.90, 1.17] 1.22 [1.04, 1.44]* 1.30 [0.76, 2.21] 0.91 [0.60, 1.39] 1.16 [0.75, 1.79] 1.43 [0.31, 6.65] 4.16 [1.21, 14.36]* 0.94 [0.40, 2.20] 0.49 [0.13, 1.84] (Continued) 1.07 [0.95, 1.21] 0.41 [0.17, 0.97]* 1.37 [1.07, 1.75]* 0.80 [0.38, 1.68] 1.02 [0.90, 1.17] 1.07 [0.81, 1.40] 0.41 [0.09, 1.93] 0.20 [0.09, 0.45]* 1.66 [0.69, 4.00] 1.19 [0.08, 16.98] 0.82 [0.22, 3.11] 0.94 [0.33, 2.71] 0.24 [0.07, 0.75]* 0.93 [0.31, 2.78] 0.86 [0.71, 1.03] ASD + ID Leisure Activities 4.97 [1.12, 22.07]* 1.15 [0.62, 2.15] 1.13 [0.22, 5.73] 1.22 [0.93, 1.60] ASD + ID Classroom Learning 1.29 [0.03, 50.33]* 4.90 [1.26, 19.10]* 2.00 [0.07, 58.62] 4.53 [0.95, 21.64] 0.55 [0.07, 4.08] 0.70 [0.21, 2.34] 1.22 [0.27, 5.55] 1.08 [0.83, 1.41] ASD + ID Friendships Adjusted Odds Ratioa [95% CI] Table 2. Multivariate Regression Models With Multiple Imputations for Predictors of Participation Difficulties in Home Life, Friendships, Classroom Learning, and Leisure Activities Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 6 Note. ASD 5 autism spectrum disorder; CI 5 confidence interval; ID 5 intellectual disabilities; Ref. 5 reference group. Data are from the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services (Child and Adolescent Health Measurement Initiative, 2015). a Odds ratios were weighted to account for the complex survey sampling structures. b Prosocial behaviors predicted lower difficulty scores, so scores were reversed. *p < .05. 1.01 [0.72, 1.41] 1.15 [0.98, 1.35] 0.78 [0.64, 0.93]* 1.06 [0.89, 1.26] Repetitive behaviors 1.32 [0.94, 1.86] 1.02 [0.68, 1.53] 1.15 [0.96, 1.36] 1.17 [0.85, 1.60] 1.12 [0.81, 1.55] 1.12 [0.95, 1.33] 1.11 [0.91, 1.37] 1.13 [0.95, 1.35] Low registration 1.72 [1.08, 2.74]* 1.61 [0.97, 2.66] 1.27 [1.06, 1.52]* 1.64 [1.10, 2.45]* 0.97 [0.81, 1.17] 1.22 [0.88, 1.69] 1.08 [0.97, 1.21] 0.92 [0.76, 1.12] 0.92 [0.70, 1.22] 1.23 [0.72, 2.10] 0.88 [0.72, 1.07] 1.06 [0.94, 1.20] 0.90 [0.70, 1.16] 0.81 [0.54, 1.22] 1.15 [1.00, 1.32]* 1.02 [0.84, 1.23] 0.73 [0.57, 0.93]* 1.09 [0.76, 1.56] 0.95 [0.85, 1.06) 0.89 [0.75, 1.04] Sensory seeking Sensory processing variables Sensory avoidant 1.57 [1.16, 2.13]* 1.30 [1.15, 1.48]* 1.19 [1.02, 1.39]* Prosocial behaviors b 1.14 [1.00, 1.29]* 1.57 [1.17, 2.12]* 1.30 [0.93, 1.81] 1.04 [0.92, 1.17] 0.92 [0.66, 1.27] ASD + ID Leisure Activities ASD ASD + ID Classroom Learning ASD ASD + ID Friendships ASD ASD + ID Home Life ASD Predictors Adjusted Odds Ratioa [95% CI] Social Variables We hypothesized that social problems and lower prosocial behaviors would predict participation difficulties in home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities for both groups. This hypothesis was supported for all outcomes except classroom learning. Lower prosocial behaviors was a significant predictor of participation problems in home life and leisure activities for both groups and of friendships for the ASD-only group. Our finding regarding friendships supports the results of previous studies showing that successful peer relationships require social skills such as person perception, empathy, and prosocial behavior (Cillessen & Bellmore, 2011; Vetter et al., 2013). In other studies, social competence was shown to be important for obtaining and maintaining friendships (Flannery & Smith, 2017), and social impairment was found to be a mediator in the relationship between executive function and friendship quality (Lieb & Bohnert, 2017). Social problems, such as preferring to play alone, not being liked by other children, and being picked on or bullied, predicted friendship difficulties for both groups in our study. Salters et al. (2022) found that instructors reported social challenges as the largest barrier to participation, although they identified cognitive and fine motor skills as well. It is possible that parents in the Pathways study were not aware of the impact of social skills on difficulties in classroom learning or interpreted that outcome narrowly to focus only on school attendance and not on interactions with others. Contextual Variables We hypothesized that parent education level, household income, and health insurance coverage would predict participation difficulties in classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities for autistic children with and without ID, but we did not expect race or ethnicity to be a predictor. This hypothesis was generally not supported. Surprisingly, higher household income was a strong predictor of greater participation difficulties in home life and classroom learning for autistic children with and without ID. Poverty has been shown to slow cognitive and language development and is associated with lower academic achievement (Hernandez, 2011), and household income has been associated with autistic children’s physical and play activities (Memari et al., 2015). It is possible that, in our analysis, parents and caregivers with higher household income were THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 7 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 Table 2. Multivariate Regression Models With Multiple Imputations for Predictors of Participation Difficulties in Home Life, Friendships, Classroom Learning, and Leisure Activities (Cont.) correlated with atypical sensory processing in autistic children, and Choi and Jung (2021) found sensory processing abilities to be predictive of participation in leisure activities for autistic adolescents. In a scoping review, Askari et al. (2015) identified a pattern of sensory, cognitive, and behavioral factors as a barrier to participation for autistic children. Activities of Daily Living and Functional and Adaptive Skills We hypothesized that the group of skills consisting of poorer ADLs and functional and adaptive skills would predict participation difficulties in home life and classroom learning. This hypothesis was supported, although only fine motor skills were a protective factor for leisure participation among autistic children with ID and learning difficulties were a predictor for classroom learning. Difficulty learning, understanding, and paying attention predicted classroom learning in children with ASD only. Fine motor skills, including handwriting, manipulation of materials, and scissor skills, are required in the classroom environment, and difficulty with fine motor skills curtails a child’s ability to perform these classroom tasks and thus their classroom learning. We expected the ability to complete ADLs such as dressing and toileting to be a predictor of home life and classroom learning participation. Holloway et al. (2021) found that better gross motor skills were associated with greater participation in self-care, leisure, and social interactions. In a study of adolescents, physical demands were among the factors that parents identified as barriers to participation in home and school life (Lamash et al., 2020). Surprisingly, we found that fine motor skills and ADLs were not predictive of participation in home life or classroom learning. Fine motor difficulties were actually protective for leisure activity difficulties for the autistic children with ID. It is possible that, because parents completed the survey and did not often observe their children in the classroom, they may not have been aware of the impact of these skills; in addition, they may have been accustomed to managing ADLs at home and did not think of fine motor problems as a limiter in home life. In addition, the question asked whether using their hands interfered with the child’s classroom participation. Parents may have interpreted the question as referring to school attendance rather than measures of success such as good writing skills. Many leisure activities do not require fine motor skills; research findings indicate that many autistic children and adolescents play computer and video games and watch TV, videos, and DVDs (Lamash et al., 2020; Must et al., 2014; Stiller & M€ oßle, 2018). The fine motor skills required for watching TV and videos or playing computer and video games may be less demanding than the fine motor skills required for ADLs and other home life activities. Limitations and Directions for Future Research The 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services provides data from a large number of participants, but the nature of secondary data sources limits the precision of some of the data. As with all secondary analyses of survey data that were collected for another purpose, inherent bias is a potential limitation of our analysis. It is also possible that critical sensory items were not included in the 15 sensory items found in THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 8 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 more able to focus on their children’s needs and to identify areas of participation difficulty than those with income at or below the poverty level, who likely experienced more money-related distractions. Having no health insurance coverage predicted greater participation problems for children with ASD only in classroom learning and for autistic children with ID in friendships. It is possible that the families with no health insurance felt they had inadequate access to services for their children and therefore more frequently identified participation problems. Only 2% of families in this data set did not have health insurance, so the data may not adequately represent the opinions of that group. Contrary to our hypothesis, identification with a race or ethnicity other than non-Hispanic White was a significant predictor for classroom learning participation problems for autistic children with and without ID but was a protective factor for leisure activities. It is possible that non-Hispanic White parents have higher expectations for their children in the classroom and consider a wider range of activities as leisure than other parents. A previous study found that some Black parents were concerned about institutional racism in schools (Fields-Smith & Williams, 2009), which may have increased the observations of problems with classroom learning in this portion of our sample. Parent education level was not a significant predictor of any of the participation areas for either group. This was surprising because higher income and highly educated parents are more likely to be involved in their children’s education, which is a key factor in educational success (Cabrera et al., 2018). It is possible that the parent perspective on their child’s difficulty in the data we used is not the same as actual school performance measured in other studies. It is also possible that the high representation of educated and higher income parents in this sample meant that too few lower educated (less than high school) and poverty-level income parents were surveyed to adequately represent the differences in lower education and lower income conditions. We also hypothesized that sensory processing and emotional regulation variables would be stronger predictors of participation difficulties than contextual factors. This hypothesis was supported; sensory processing and emotional regulation factors were strong predictors in all participation areas for autistic children both with and without ID. Previous studies have identified emotional regulation (Pugliese et al., 2016) and sensory responsiveness (Hilton et al., 2010) as potential contributors to participation. Memari et al. (2015) identified financial burden and lack of opportunities as stronger barriers than specific autistic characteristics, but they examined only physical activity participation. Implications for Occupational Therapy Practice The findings from this study suggest the following implications for occupational therapy practice with autistic children and adolescents: Incorporating strategies that help them manage their emotional and behavioral responses, including environmental and occupational adjustments, can support their increased participation in home life, classroom learning, friendships, and leisure activities. Addressing sensory differences, particularly low registration, disorganized, overresponsive, and sensory seeking, can help this client group increase their participation in home life and friendships. Supporting the development of social skills can help these clients increase participation in home life, friendships, and leisure activities. As a cornerstone of occupational therapy practice, client-centered approaches are important aspects of assessment and intervention for autistic children and adolescents. Practitioners need to include collaboration with clients and their families and care providers in evaluation and intervention (AOTA, 2020). 䊏 䊏 䊏 Conclusion Participation in a wide range of activities is important for development and for quality of life. Autistic children and adolescents participate in fewer activities than their peers without this diagnosis. Occupational therapy practitioners work to enhance and promote client participation, and information about the predictors of limited participation can support them in designing the most effective intervention plans. Large data research provides opportunities to examine patterns of responses among large numbers of participants, and the patterns that emerged from the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services data set through its design and sampling provide generalizable ideas of factors that influence participation for autistic children and adolescents. Most of the predictors of participation difficulties we found are within the scope of occupational therapy intervention. The strongest predictors were sensory processing, emotional regulation, behavioral, and social variables, supporting previous smaller studies and indicating the importance of addressing these areas in intervention. The connections between these areas and their relationship to participation also suggest the importance of focusing occupational therapy interventions on emotional regulation, sensory processing, and social skills to address the underlying neurological processing in autistic children for the purpose of supporting increased participation in occupations. Acknowledgments This research was supported in part by the American Occupational Therapy Foundation (AOTF) Health Services Research program (Grant No. AOTFHSR2019HILTON) and the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (Grant No. THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 9 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 this data set. However, the sensory scale was validated for this population survey (Lee et al., 2019). Age and gender differences across diagnostic groups potentially bias the findings from this study. Use of parentreported survey data is another potential limitation of this study and did not allow for examination of interrater reliability. In addition, the survey did not include questions for the autistic children and adolescents, so the survey data may not reflect their perspectives on the activities that were difficult for them. Diagnoses were based on parent report; confirmatory diagnostic assessments and behavioral observations might have improved interrater reliability, but it was not possible to conduct these types of assessments with secondary data. Participation variables were based on single questions that were broad in scope, so parent responses may have been biased because of misinterpretation of the questions and lack of specific categories for each (e.g., physically active vs. sedentary leisure activities). Finally, our findings were limited by the dichotomous nature of the ID status and the absence of actual IQ data, which would have allowed for more robust statistical analyses. Nevertheless, our use of a large cross-sectional data set was a strength of this study, allowing us to complete the first examination of a range of predictors of participation difficulties in autistic children. Our results provide professionals with potential intervention targets to address participation difficulties in this population. Future studies that include quantitative IQ data, diagnostic measures, and sensory assessments would extend the understanding of the relationships identified in this study. The results of more extensive participation assessments could better explain the participation limitations identified in this analysis. Examining longitudinal data will shed light on the relationships between the predictor and outcome variables and the impact of interventions. It is possible that sensory differences are mediators between other predictors and participation outcomes, and the relationships among emotional regulation, behavioral, and cognitive strategy use require further exploration. Other child factors that might affect participation, such as use of medication to manage ASD symptoms, presence of siblings, executive function factors, and interoception abilities, are potential topics for future studies to help practitioners better understand and support activity participation among autistic children. Finally, data from surveys of autistic children and adolescents would add a valuable perspective on their participation difficulties. NRF-2021S1A3A2A02096338). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of AOTF. This study used a publicly available, deidentified data set that was supported by the U.S. Data Resource Center for Child and Adolescent Health and the U.S. National Center for Health Statistics. The data set and survey questionnaire are available at www.childhealthdata.org. Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., . . . Guastella, A. J. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular Psychiatry, 23, 1198–1204. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.75 Egilson, S. T., Jakobsd ottir, G., & Olafsd ottir, L. B. (2018). Parent perspectives on home participation of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder compared with a matched group of children without autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 22, 560–570. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316685555 Fields-Smith, C., & Williams, M. (2009). Motivations, sacrifices, and challenges: Black parents’ decisions to home school. Urban Review, Allison, P. D. (2002). Missing data. Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/ 9781412985079 American Occupational Therapy Association. (2020). Occupational therapy practice framework: Domain and process (4th ed.). American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 74(Suppl. 2), 7412410010. https:// doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2020.74S2001 Anaby, D., Hand, C., Bradley, L., DiRezze, B., Forhan, M., DiGiacomo, A., & Law, M. (2013). The effect of the environment on participation of children and youth with disabilities: A scoping review. Disability and Rehabilitation, 35, 1589–1598. https://doi.org/10.3109/ 09638288.2012.748840 Ashburner, J., Ziviani, J., & Rodger, S. (2010). Surviving in the mainstream: Capacity of children with autism spectrum disorders to perform academically and regulate their emotions and behavior at school. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 18–27. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.rasd.2009.07.002 Askari, S., Anaby, D., Bergthorson, M., Elsabbagh, M., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2015). Participation of children and youth with autism spectrum disorder: A scoping review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2, 103–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s40489-014-0040-7 Baldwin, S., Costley, D., & Warren, A. (2014). Employment activities and experiences of adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2440–2449. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2112-z Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 899–911. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000436 Bohnert, A., Lieb, R., & Arola, N. (2019). More than leisure: Organized activity participation and socio-emotional adjustment among adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49, 2637–2652. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10803-016-2783-8 Bottema-Beutel, K., Kapp, S. K., Lester, J. N., Sasson, N. J., & Hand, B. N. (2021). Avoiding ableist language: Suggestions for autism researchers. Autism in Adulthood, 3, 18–29. https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2020.0014 Cabrera, A. F., Peralta, A. M., & Kurban, E. R. (2018). The invisible 1%: A comparison of attaining stepping stones toward college between military and civilian children. Journal of Higher Education, 89, 208–235. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221546.2017.1368816 Child and Adolescent Health Measurement Initiative. (2015). 2011 Pathways outcome measures SPSS codebook, Version 1. www. childhealthdata.org Choi, Y. E., & Jung, H. (2021). Sensory processing as a predictor of leisure participation in early adolescents. Children, 8, 1005. https:// doi.org/10.3390/children8111005 Cillessen, A. H. N., & Bellmore, A. D. (2011). Social skills and social competence in interactions with peers. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (pp. 393–412). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/ 9781444390933.ch21 41, 369–389. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11256-008-0114-x Flannery, K. M., & Smith, R. L. (2017). Are peer status, friendship quality, and friendship stability equivalent markers of social competence? Adolescent Research Review, 2, 331–340. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s40894-016-0042-z Fong, V. C., & Iarocci, G. (2020). The role of executive functioning in predicting social competence in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 13, 1856–1866. https://doi.org/ 10.1002/aur.2350 Freeman, L. M., Locke, J., Rotheram-Fuller, E., & Mandell, D. (2017). Brief report: Examining executive and social functioning in elementary-aged children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 1890–1895. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10803-017-3079-3 Hernandez, D. J. (2011). How third grade reading skills and poverty influence school graduation. Annie E. Casey Foundation. Hilton, C. L., Crouch, M. C., & Israel, H. (2008). Out-of-school participation patterns in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 62, 554–563. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.62.5.554 Hilton, C. L., Harper, J. D., Kueker, R. H., Lang, A. R., Abbacchi, A. M., Todorov, A., & LaVesser, P. D. (2010). Sensory responsiveness as a predictor of social severity in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 937–945. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0944-8 Hilton, C. L., Ratcliff, K., Collins, D. M., Flanagan, J., & Hong, I. (2019). Flourishing in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 12, 952–966. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2097 Hilton, C. L., Ratcliff, K., & Hong, I. (2021). Participation difficulties in autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disabilities: Findings from the 2011 Survey of Pathway to Diagnosis and Services. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51, 1210–1223. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s10803-020-04591-4 Hirata, S., Okuzumi, H., Kitajima, Y., Hosobuchi, T., Nakai, A., & Kokubun, M. (2014). Relationship between motor skill and social impairment in children with autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 60, 251–256. https://doi.org/10.1179/2047387713Y.0000000033 Hochhauser, M., & Engel-Yeger, B. (2010). Sensory processing abilities and their relation to participation in leisure activities among children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HFASD). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 746–754. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. rasd.2010.01.015 Holloway, J. M., Long, T. M., & Biasini, F. J. (2021). The intersection of gross motor abilities and participation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Infants and Young Children, 34, 178–189. https:// doi.org/10.1097/IYC.0000000000000192 Kasari, C., Locke, J., Gulsrud, A., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2011). Social networks and friendships at school: Comparing children with and without ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 533–544. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1076-x Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism? THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 10 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 References Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism, 20, 442–462. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315588200 Kheirollahzadeh, M., Alizadeh Zarei, M., Amini, M., & Dehghan, F. (2021). The relationship of school participation with motor proficiency and executive function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Iranian Journal of Child Neurology, 15, 65–76. https://doi. org/10.22037/ijcn.v15i2.19721 Lamash, L., Bedell, G., & Josman, N. (2020). Participation patterns of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder compared to their peers: Parents’ perspectives. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 83, 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0308022619853518 Law, M., Cooper, B. A., Strong, S., Stewart, D., Rigby, P., & Letts, L. Occupational Therapy, 63, 9–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 000841749606300103 Leadbitter, K., Buckle, K. L., Ellis, C., & Dekker, M. (2021). Autistic selfadvocacy and the neurodiversity movement: Implications for autism early intervention research and practice. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 635690. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635690 Lee, M. J., Ratcliff, K., Hilton, C. L., & Hong, I. (2019). Validation of Sensory Outcome Measure: Findings from the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services. Korean Journal of Occupational Therapy, 27, 167–182. https://doi.org/10.14519/kjot.2019.27.4.13 Lieb, R. W., & Bohnert, A. M. (2017). Relations between executive functions, social impairment, and friendship quality on adjustment among high functioning youth with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 2861–2872. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3205-2 Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., . . . Rapin, I. (2001). Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 219–230. https://doi.org/10.1023/A: 1010707417274 Loh, S. W., Ee, S. I., & Marret, M. J. (2021). Sensory processing and its relationship to participation among childhood occupations in children with autism spectrum disorder: Exploring the profile of differences. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 20473869.2021.1950484 Martin, C. S. (2016). Exploring the impact of the design of the physical classroom environment on young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 16, 280–298. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-3802.12092 McCollum, M., LaVesser, P., & Berg, C. (2016). Participation in daily activities of young adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 987–997. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2642-z Memari, A. H., Panahi, N., Ranjbar, E., Moshayedi, P., Shafiei, M., Kordi, R., & Ziaee, V. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorder and patterns of participation in daily physical and play activities. Neurology Research International, 2015, 531906. https://doi.org/ 10.1155/2015/531906 Monteiro, E. M. (2021). An ecologically valid understanding of executive functioning. Psychology in the Schools, 59, 1390–1401. https://doi.org/ 10.1002/pits.22627 Must, A., Phillips, S. M., Curtin, C., Anderson, S. E., Maslin, M., Lividini, K., & Bandini, L. G. (2014). Comparison of sedentary behaviors between children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. Autism, 18, 376–384. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1362361313479039 Orsmond, G. I., Shattuck, P. T., Cooper, B. P., Sterzing, P. R., & Anderson, K. A. (2013). Social participation among young adults with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 11 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 (1996). The Person–Environment–Occupation Model: A transactive approach to occupational performance. Canadian Journal of Developmental Disorders, 43, 2710–2719. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10803-013-1833-8 Parham, L. D., & Fazio, L. S. (Eds.). (1997). Play in occupational therapy for children. Mosby. Perry, A., Flanagan, H. E., Dunn Geier, J., & Freeman, N. L. (2009). Brief report: The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in young children with autism spectrum disorders at different cognitive levels. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1066–1078. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10803-009-0704-9 Potvin, M.-C., Snider, L., Prelock, P., Kehayia, E., & Wood-Dauphinee, S. (2013). Recreational participation of children with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 445–457. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1589-6 Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L. G., Strang, J. F., Dudley, K., Wallace, G. L., Naiman, D. Q., & Kenworthy, L. (2016). Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 467–477. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2584-5 Ratcliff, K., Hong, I., & Hilton, C. (2018). Leisure participation patterns for school age youth with autism spectrum disorders: Findings from the 2016 National Survey of Children’s Health. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48, 3783–3793. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10803-018-3643-5 Reynolds, S., Bendixen, R. M., Lawrence, T., & Lane, S. J. (2011). A pilot study examining activity participation, sensory responsiveness, and competence in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1496–1506. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1173-x Salters, D., Rios, P. C., & Sharoun Benson, S. M. (2022). Instructors’ perspectives of social and motor influences on participation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 93, 467–478. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 02701367.2020.1864260 Simonoff, E., Kent, R., Stringer, D., Lord, C., Briskman, J., Lukito, S., . . . Baird, G. (2020). Trajectories in symptoms of autism and cognitive ability in autism from childhood to adult life: Findings from a longitudinal epidemiological cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59, 1342–1352. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.11.020 Smith, L. E., Maenner, M. J., & Seltzer, M. M. (2012). Developmental trajectories in adolescents and adults with autism: The case of daily living skills. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 622–631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.03.001 Smith Roley, S., Mailloux, Z., Miller-Kuhaneck, H., & Glennon, T. (2007). Understanding Ayres Sensory Integration. OT Practice, 12(17), CE1–CE8. Sparapani, N., Morgan, L., Reinhardt, V. P., Schatschneider, C., & Wetherby, A. M. (2016). Evaluation of classroom active engagement in elementary students with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 782–796. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s10803-015-2615-2 Stiller, A., & M€ oßle, T. (2018). Media use among children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 5, 227–246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-018-0135-7 Taheri, A., Perry, A., & Minnes, P. (2016). Examining the social participation of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities and autism spectrum disorder in relation to peers. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 60, 435–443. https://doi.org/10.1111/ jir.12289 Veal, A. J. (1992). Definitions of leisure and recreation (Working Paper No. 4). School of Leisure, Sport and Tourism, University of Technology, Sydney, Australia. (Reprinted from “Definitions of leisure and recreation,” 1992, Australian Journal of Leisure and Recreation, 2[4], 44–48, 52.) children with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 429–437. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.09.002 Claudia L. Hilton, PhD, MBA, OTR, FAOTA, is Associate Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston. Karen Ratcliff, PhD, OTR, is Associate Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston. Ickpyo Hong, PhD, OTR, is Associate Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, College of Software and Digital Healthcare Convergence, Yonsei University, Wonju-si, Gangwon-do, Republic of Korea; ihong@yonsei.ac.kr Appendix Figure A.1. Flow diagram of participants through the study. N = 4,032: Phone interviews with the Parents/caregivers in the 2011 Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services n = 1,112 (27.6%) Incomplete data for the four outcome measures n = 2,920: Analytic cohort n = 834: ASD alone n = 227: ASD and ID n = 508: ID alone n = 995: Developmental disorder alone n = 356: No current diagnosis Note. ASD 5 autism spectrum disorder; ID 5 intellectual disabilities. THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 12 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 Vetter, N. C., Leipold, K., Kliegel, M., Phillips, L. H., & Altgassen, M. (2013). Ongoing development of social cognition in adolescence. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 615–629. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 09297049.2012.718324 Waizbard-Bartov, E., Ferrer, E., Heath, B., Rogers, S. J., Nordahl, C. W., Solomon, M., & Amaral, D. G. (2022). Identifying autism symptom severity trajectories across childhood. Autism Research, 15, 687–701. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2674 Werkman, M. F., Brouwer, S., Dijkxhoorn, Y. M., van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A., Reijneveld, S. A., Landsman, J. A., & Begeer, S. (2020). The moderating effect of cognitive abilities on the association between sensory processing and emotional and behavioural problems and social participation in autistic individuals. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 78, 101663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. rasd.2020.101663 Zingerevich, C., & LaVesser, P. D. (2009). The contribution of executive functions to participation in school activities of Table A.1. Social–Behavioral Items Social–Behavioral Area Emotional regulation problems Items ▪ Often complains of headaches, stomachaches, sickness ▪ Has many worries or often seems worried ▪ Is often unhappy, depressed, or tearful ▪ Is nervous or clingy in new situations ▪ Has many fears, is easily scared Conduct problems ▪ Often loses temper ▪ Is generally well-behaved ▪ Often lies or cheats ▪ Steals from home, school, or elsewhere Hyperactivity/inattention ▪ Is restless, overactive, cannot stay still for long ▪ Is constantly fidgeting or squirming ▪ Is easily distracted ▪ Thinks things out before acting ▪ Has good attention span, finishes chores or homework Social problems ▪ Prefers to play alone, would rather be alone ▪ Has at least one good friend ▪ Is generally liked by other children/youth ▪ Is picked on or bullied by other children/youth ▪ Gets along better with adults than with other children/youth Prosocial behavior ▪ Is considerate of other people’s feelings ▪ Shares readily with other children/youth ▪ Is helpful if someone is hurt, upset or feeling ill ▪ Is kind to younger children/youth ▪ Often offers to help others (parents, teachers, other children/youth) Note. Respondents use a 3-point scale: 1 5 not true, 2 5 somewhat true, 3 5 certainly true. Positive items are reverse scored. Higher scores indicate more difficulties. THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY MARCH/APRIL 2023, VOLUME 77, NUMBER 2 13 Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/77/2/7702205010/77411/7702205010.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 26 April 2023 ▪ Often fights with other children or bullies them