See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/312212692 Measuring Subjective Movie Evaluation Criteria: Conceptual Foundation, Construction, and Validation of the SMEC Scales Article in Communication Methods and Measures · January 2017 DOI: 10.1080/19312458.2016.1271115 CITATIONS READS 15 588 1 author: Frank M. Schneider Universität Mannheim 95 PUBLICATIONS 1,243 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Social media & ostracism: Consequences and coping View project Effects of election promises on trust in politicians and vote choice View project All content following this page was uploaded by Frank M. Schneider on 08 March 2019. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Running head: MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA Please cite as: Schneider, F. M. (2017). Measuring subjective movie evaluation criteria: Conceptual foundation, construction, and validation of the SMEC scales. Communication Methods and Measures, 11, 49–75. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2016.1271115 Measuring Subjective Movie Evaluation Criteria: Conceptual Foundation, Construction, and Validation of the SMEC Scales Frank M. Schneider University of Mannheim Author Note Frank M. Schneider (PhD, University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany), Institute for Media and Communication Studies, University of Mannheim, Germany Correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to Frank M. Schneider, Institute for Media and Communication Studies, University of Mannheim, Haus Oberrhein, Rheinvorlandstr. 5, 68159 Mannheim, Germany. E-mail: frank.schneider@uni-mannheim.de, Phone: +49 621 181-3938, Fax: +49 621 181-393. Acknowledgements I thank Frieder Schmid for assisting in collecting the data, Carina Weinmann for her feedback on an earlier version of this paper as well as the anonymous reviewers and the editor, Jörg Matthes, for their valuable recommendations and thoughtful comments that helped to improve the paper during the editorial process. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 2 Abstract Audiences’ movie evaluations have often been explored as effects of experiencing movies. However, little attention has been paid to the criteria viewers use when they evaluate a movie or its specific features. Adding to this, the present research introduces the idea of subjective movie evaluation criteria (SMEC), conceptualizes SMEC as the mental representation of important attitudes toward specific film features, and describes the scale construction for their measurement and its validation process. Findings from pilot work and 2 studies including over 1,500 participants provide first evidence that 8 dimensions—Story Verisimilitude, Story Innovation, Cinematography, Special Effects, Recommendation, Innocuousness, Lightheartedness, and Cognitive Stimulation—are largely determined by stable individual differences, substantially but differentially related to film-specific constructs and personality traits, and that the SMEC scales are reliable and valid instruments for measuring subjective movie evaluation criteria. Keywords: subjective movie evaluation criteria; important attitudes toward specific film features; scale construction; scale validation MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 3 Measuring Subjective Movie Evaluation Criteria: Conceptual Foundation, Construction, and Validation of the SMEC Scales “I have a foolproof device for judging whether a picture is good or bad. If my fanny squirms, it's bad. If my fanny doesn't squirm, it's good. It's as simple as that.” —Harry Cohn (*1891, †1958), founder and president of Columbia Pictures Is evaluating a movie really as simple as suggested by these famous words from Harry Cohn? Or, do we have a set of more elaborated criteria we can apply when we evaluate a movie? If we talk with others about films, want to buy a DVD at an online store, or visit movie websites like www.rottentomatoes.com or www.IMDb.com, we come across a plethora of movie evaluations. We also find that individuals judge the same movies quite differently and that these judgments are based on different criteria. For example, the movie Babel was reviewed on Rotten Tomatoes as “elegantly written and shot, harrowing to watch and realistic” or as “unusual aesthetic force, no coherent idea or narrative logic” or as “one of the most challenging and saddest movies of the year”, or as “higher kitsch” (see also Chamorro-Premuzic, Kallias, & Hsu, 2014). Such observations give reason to think about the concept of movie evaluation criteria from the perspective of lay audiences because evaluating movies is not limited to film theorists, movie critics, or festival juries—“[it] is something that we all do all of the time” (Carroll, 2003, p. 148). Thus, the present article deals with the notion of criteria lay audiences use for evaluating narrative films, or briefly subjective movie evaluation criteria (SMEC), and addresses the question what is important when we watch and evaluate a movie. Conceptually speaking, it is assumed that SMEC are mental representations of important attitudes toward specific film features that have been formed during our cinematic socialization MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 4 (cf. Valkenburg & Cantor, 2000; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999). These specific film features can be broadly categorized as film-inherent features (e.g., story, cinematography), film-external features (e.g., awards, film critiques), or anticipated effects of use (e.g., features that might be entertaining or require cognitive effort to cope with).1 Like attitudes, SMEC can vary in their importance. Especially important attitudes have been found to be stable over time, resistant to persuasion, influential in information processing, and predictive of behavior (e.g., Boninger, Krosnick, Berent, & Fabrigar, 1995; Eaton & Visser, 2008). Thus, in mimicking important attitudes, SMEC might be consequential for encountering movies and corresponding evaluative responses. After briefly summarizing why movie evaluations and SMEC are relevant and reviewing previous operationalizations of film-related evaluation criteria, the empirical part of this article focuses on the number of SMEC dimensions, their scope, and on the question how we can measure them. Movie Evaluations and the Role of SMEC From motivational frameworks for movie theater attendance (e.g., Austin, 1986; Palmgreen, Cook, Harvill, & Helm, 1988; Tesser, Millar, & Wu, 1988) and mood management (Zillmann, 1988) to more recent and general attempts to delineate the underlying motivations and gratifications of entertainment consumption (e.g., Oliver & Raney, 2011; Tamborini et al., 2011; Wirth, Hofer, & Schramm, 2012), communication scholars have been interested in why people expose themselves to movies. Even more studies have been conducted to investigate the effects of movies on their audience (Young, 2012). However, surprisingly little research has been done with regard to movie evaluations, although they seem to play an important part not only after, but also during, and before movie exposure (for a detailed overview, see Schneider, Vogel, Gleich, & Bartsch, 2014). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 5 Evaluation After Exposure First, movie evaluations have often been operationalized as post-hoc judgments after exposure (e.g., enjoyment or appreciation as evaluative responses to movies; e.g., Oliver & Bartsch, 2010). These evaluations are not only determined by the (perceived) features of the movie itself; they are also determined by characteristics of the situation (e.g., going to the cinema theater alone or with others; discussing a movie with others at a bar or in an online forum; multitasking while watching a movie at home; etc.), and by individual characteristics (e.g., preferences, expectations, attitudes, motivations, traits, and states; e.g., Krcmar, 2009). According to the disposition-content congruency hypothesis (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013, p. 232), the content of a movie is more likely to have an impact if it is congruent to the characteristics of the viewer. For instance, an individual’s greater preference for suspenseful films led to a better evaluation of a suspenseful movie (cf. Vorderer, Knobloch, & Schramm, 2001), viewers who watched clips from film festival submissions and had more movie expertise evaluated these clips as more interesting (Silvia & Berg, 2011), sensation seeking and neuroticism predicted the evaluation of violent and arousing content (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014), and the need for cognition affected mystery enjoyment (e.g., Knobloch-Westerwick & Keplinger, 2008). Thus, if viewers hold stable, important attitudes toward specific film features (i.e., SMEC) and encounter congruent film features, it seems likely that these attitudes affect their evaluation of a movie as well. From this perspective, we can look at movie evaluations as dependent variables that are partly determined by SMEC. Most importantly, if SMEC are causally related to movie evaluations, they may also exert their influence when movies are evaluated while watching. Evaluation During Exposure During movie exposure, the interplay of cognitive and affective processes hints to the role of movie-related evaluations as a mediating variable. For instance, from the perspective of MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 6 appraisal theories of emotions (e.g., Bartsch, Vorderer, Mangold, & Viehoff, 2008; Scherer, 2013; Schramm & Wirth, 2010), cognitive appraisals are the key to understand how emotions are elicited. Movie-specific evaluative processes might be related to these more general cognitive appraisals. In fact, these appraisals do underlie basic evaluation principles (i.e., appraisal criteria or stimulus evaluation checks, cf. Scherer, 2013). These stimulus evaluation checks include, among others, the novelty, the intrinsic pleasantness, and the norm compatibility of an event, and the coping potential with regard to an event. Thus, it seems possible that corresponding SMEC (e.g., novel or pleasant aspects of story-telling or cinematography) influence the set of appraisal criteria or even become some of them. Consequently, the mediational process between the film stimulus and the emotional response via cognitive appraisals might be partly determined by SMEC. Furthermore, it also seems likely that evaluating cinematic information while watching a movie could alter involvement. For example, if someone thinks it is important that a movie does not contain brutal scenes, but perceives cues for brutality, and evaluates them as offensive, she or he might stop or get less involved in watching the movie. Alternatively, these evaluated cues can trigger different modes of reception—for instance, switching from an emotionally-involved to a more production-oriented mode of reception (Suckfüll & Scharkow, 2009). Evaluation Before Exposure Finally, before movie exposure, prior aggregated evaluative information about movies from external sources can be seen as determinants of movie choice and influential factors for responses to films. This is widely acknowledged in the movie marketing literature, especially with regard to awards and nominations, critical acclaim and professional critique, and word-ofmouth recommendations (for overviews, see e.g., Hadida, 2009; Simonton, 2009). In addition, experimental studies stress the importance of critical acclaim and word-of-mouth for evaluative responses to movies (e.g., d'Astous & Touil, 1999; Jacobs, Heuvelman, ben Allouch, & Peters, MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 7 2015; Wyatt & Badger, 1990). In other words, recommendations or prior aggregated movie evaluations from others might influence the way we select, process, and respond to movies. In this sense, movie evaluations can be seen as independent variables. If we deem them to be important for our own choices of and judgments about movies, these recommendations might qualify as SMEC. This might be especially true when we intentionally seek out such filmrelated, external information. With regard to SMEC as potentially dispositional determinants of movie evaluations (see above), individuals may also choose movies congruent to their traits, needs, motivations, preferences, SMEC, or actual states more or less consciously. This has been addressed, for instance, by research on uses and gratifications (U&G), gratifications sought and obtained (GSGO), selective exposure, and mood management (e.g., Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015; Krcmar, 2009). However, these approaches focus rather on the users’ driving forces and anticipated effects of use than on the specific film-inherent content or features like cinematography, aesthetics, or story (cf. Swanson, 1987, p. 245; Wolling, 2009, p. 85). In contrast to these approaches and to explicitly include media-inherent features, Wolling (2004, 2009) proposed the theory of subjective quality assessment (TSQA) that is based on expectancy x value assumptions and GSGO models (e.g., Palmgreen & Rayburn, 1985), but extends them in two major ways: (a) Whereas expectations can be seen as the probability that a media object will possess a specific feature, the TSQA postulates that users desire that a media product should possess specific features. If two or more desired features are in conflict (e.g., perhaps a happy and a realistic ending cannot be desired at the same time), the importance of each desired feature might play a crucial role in predicting the global judgment about a movie across features. Although SMEC are defined as important attitudes, which are conceptually different from desires, the idea of varying importance is common two both concepts. (b) Distinct MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 8 from these desired features is the second component in the TSQA: the subjective evaluation of the encountered features. In GSGO, these evaluations are derived from experiences with or behavioral responses to the specific features and are weighted against expectations (Wolling, 2009, p. 89), whereas in the TSQA, evaluations of specific features are generated through the interplay between the desired or undesired features and the perceived features (for details, see Wolling, 2009, pp. 89–92). Likewise, the interplay between SMEC and congruently perceived features might influence specific evaluative responses. Most importantly with regard to SMEC, Wolling presents some early thoughts on evaluation criteria of media products in general, that is on which dimensions can the features be systematized (p. 96). For instance, he proposes three preliminary and broad criteria dimensions (i.e., reality, originality, and the effects of the features) that comprise different features. In sum, the TSQA seems to be a useful framework that explicitly incorporates parameters that can be related to evaluative processes and criteria. It extends U&G, mood management, and GSGO by relating evaluations not only to the effects of use but also to specific product features. This distinction is important, especially for delineating SMEC because they are not only related to anticipated effects of these features but also to film-inherent and film-external features. Unfortunately, we cannot derive a set of movie evaluation criteria from the TSQA with regard to movies. Instead, Wolling suggests drawing from existing “catalogues of criteria” (p. 95) as a starting point. Especially previous research on subjective evaluation criteria with regard to TV shows (e.g., Greenberg & Busselle, 1996; Gunter, 1997; Himmelweit, Swift, & Jaeger, 1980; Wolling, 2004) or within specific target groups (e.g., children, Nikken & van der Voort, 1997; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999) provide such catalogues that are briefly reviewed because they might be useful for the purpose of the present study. Previous Research on Evaluation Criteria MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 9 Previous research on evaluation criteria is scarce and has focused mainly on criteria in the context of film journalism or TV quality research. Table 1 gives a summary of the relevant studies that will be briefly introduced in the following paragraphs. To my best knowledge, Linton and Petrovich (1988) conducted the only study that focused specifically on movie evaluation criteria. They asked students to rate the importance of movie attributes when evaluating a movie and found two underlying dimensions: foreground (e.g., storyline, acting, scenery, music) and background (e.g., editing, photography, director). A more elaborated and multidimensional approach evolved from a survey on newspaper film critics (Wyatt & Badger, 1988). The surveyed film journalists rated the importance of review elements, movie characteristics for writing film critiques, and the functions of a movie. In sum, the approach of Wyatt and Badger seems to offer first interesting insights into the professional perspective on film criticism and evaluation criteria. However, it is questionable whether these results can be applied to the ordinary movie audience. A study from a project that dealt with films in daily newspapers examined this question more closely (Rössler, 1997). Among other studies in this project, Rössler surveyed German newspaper film critics and found that their ratings on the importance of movie evaluation criteria differed from the ratings of moviegoers. In line with these results, a US-American study showed only a weak correspondence between the ratings of film critics and consumers (Holbrook, 1999). Only a few studies that have dealt with TV evaluation criteria focused on fictional formats that might be related to movies in general (e.g., Gehrau, 2008; Greenberg & Busselle, 1996; Himmelweit et al., 1980; Wolling, 2004). The major drawback of these studies lies in the simultaneous measurement of predictor (evaluation criteria) and criterion variables (evaluative responses)—both are explicitly related to the program watched. Hence, it is impossible to draw any conclusions about the causal relationship between general evaluation criteria and the specific MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 10 evaluative responses to a particular film. Unfortunately, what is even worse, we cannot infer anything about independent criteria or their consistency from these results. Furthermore, Gunter (1997) questioned the content validity of the often used adjective lists. Instead, he suggested to “go directly to viewers themselves even before the attribute scales are produced as a source of insights concerning what those scales might be” (p. 14). In his own research on soap operas, Gunter (1997) developed genre-specific scales in two steps, consisting of a qualitative focus group and a quantitative survey approach. One branch of TV quality research has focused on the criteria children use to evaluate TV programs. Nikken and colleagues (e.g., Nikken & van der Voort, 1997; Nikken, van der Voort, & van Bochove, 1996) found seven types of quality criteria when asking mothers how a good children’s TV program should be. These types plus two additional criteria were then used to assess children’s quality criteria for TV programs (Nikken & van der Voort, 1997). The results resembled those of the mothers-study, especially when taking only fictional TV program genres into account. However, as Valkenburg and Janssen (1999) argued, the study is limited in that Nikken and van der Voort (1997) largely used items from the list of the mothers-study. Therefore, Valkenburg and Janssen (1999) used a focus group approach to collect children’s criteria and then conducted a paper-and-pencil questionnaire study. Their analyses resulted in an eight-dimensional solution and were quite similar for Dutch and US children (see Table 1). Summary and Purpose of the Present Research Because there is no approach to the SMEC of lay audiences, it is important to first shed some light on criteria found in the context of film critics and reported on selected work from TV quality research. Although TV research has accumulated a large body of literature, it is limited to this medium and thus cannot be merged with film audience research without hesitation: Watching TV comprises many more formats and genres irrelevant for movie evaluation (e.g., MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 11 news, magazines, and talk shows). Furthermore, it is supposed that TV is associated with different evaluations and viewing motives (Finn, 1997). Despite these constraints, we could gain insights into research methods applied to quality assessments that might be useful for the purpose of the present research—especially the combination of qualitative and quantitative approaches during a multi-step development of measures and the call for an inductive and data-driven development of the criteria by asking the lay audience (e.g., Gunter, 1997; Himmelweit et al., 1980; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999). Moreover, the TV quality studies included to some amount narrative and fictional stimuli. Thus, if carefully interpreted with regard to movies, these findings give us a first impression of the research done so far. Although previous research has accumulated evidence for the existence of subjective evaluation criteria, taken together, findings from film criticism and TV quality research show some shortcomings with respect to the goals of the present research: (a) Film critics might have different criteria than laypeople (e.g., Holbrook, 1999; Rössler, 1997). (b) TV programs contain specific and varying features. Their one-to-one application to movies hardly seems justifiable (e.g., viewers might apply different criteria to evaluate news, talk shows, game shows). (c) Even children seem to already have developed sophisticated, multidimensional criteria for assessing TV programs (Nikken & van der Voort, 1997; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999). Thus, it seems reasonable to assume that more experienced viewers apply even more or more elaborated criteria. Furthermore, it is also possible that the criteria children use differ from those adolescents and adults apply. (d) Adjective lists seem to be inappropriate as a measurement instrument because they might be too abstract to capture specific film features (e.g., Gunter, 1997). (e) The simultaneous assessment of criteria importance and evaluation of stimuli already watched might lead to erroneous conclusions due to confounding (e.g., Gunter, 1997; Wolling, 2009). (f) Most of the instruments applied were ad hoc scales and thus, maybe only appropriate to the respective samples. (g) The use of MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 12 exploratory data analysis to examine the construct space was not followed by further confirmation or validation procedures. The present research differs from previous research in some important aspects: Of course and first of all, the topic is movies across media, not TV shows. Second, the individual steps in developing a measurement instrument are carried out in separate studies (e.g., item development, scale construction). In addition, confirmatory and construct validation studies are conducted. Moreover, detailed reporting on the psychometric properties of the scales is warranted. Finally, the broad conceptualization of SMEC as important attitudes toward specific film features offers the opportunity to connect them to a plethora of attitude theories (e.g., Gawronski, 2007; Schneider, 2012) and also to use them for further examination of the criteria dimensions within the TSQA (Wolling, 2009). In line with the recommendations of previous approaches (e.g., Gunter, 1997; Himmelweit et al., 1980; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999) and because we have only little theoretical and empirical knowledge about SMEC, as a first step, an inductive, data-driven approach to scale development seems to be the most appropriate strategy to gain a deeper understanding of the content domain.2 Although previous research has tried to answer the question what is important when we watch and evaluate a movie and accumulated evidence for the existence of SMEC, we still do not know their number, scope, or stability—and we do not have any proven measure. Thus, it seems fruitful to start exploring them in more detail and develop a methodologically sound instrument for their measurement. Pilot Studies for Item Pool Development To collect everyday language terms of movie evaluation criteria, 258 participants (age: M = 26.44, SD = 9.68, range 18–73; 73% female) filled out questionnaires with an open-ended item (“In general, what do you think what kind of criteria can be applied to evaluate a movie?”). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 13 Responses to this open-ended question informed the construction of 117 items that broadly covered all aspects that were investigated or found in previous research (see above). Every item consisted of three components: (a) “When evaluating movies, how important for you personally is/are...”, (b) derived from the answers to the open-ended question, a phrase including the criterion (e.g., an item concerning the verisimilitude of a film’s story was worded “...that the story the film tells is realistic?”), and (c) a rating scale. All items were positively phrased to avoid artifacts only due to the negative wording. The rating scales were constructed as 5-point unipolar with construct-specific labels for the response options: 0 (not at all important), 1 (slightly important), 2 (moderately important), 3 (very important), 4 (extremely important). The items were pretested applying the item panel method with 14 participants (mostly graduates). In an item panel, panelists review and discuss in several rounds if the content and wording of items are comprehensible and appropriate for the measured construct (cf. Wilson, 2005, pp. 59–61). Results led to minor wording revisions and the exclusion of some items. The final version—including 93 items for measuring SMEC—was created as an online-questionnaire and subjected to another pilot test. This pilot test was conducted as a mix of thinking-aloudtechnique (n = 2) and exit interviews (n = 6) by two interviewers (cf. Wilson, 2005). The interviewers’ discussion of the results led to minor changes in item wording and the inclusion of an example how to fill out the online questionnaire. Study 1 Study 1 aimed to explore the latent structure of SMEC and to reduce the number of items. Method Sample and Procedures. Cover letters including the link to an online questionnaire were distributed to students via mailing lists. Participants were offered a chance to win shopping vouchers for Amazon as an incentive to take part in the study. To further increase the number of MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 14 participants, all recipients were asked to forward the link to other persons they knew. The “Start”-button on the welcome page was clicked by 659 people; 506 of them completed all SMEC items (N), 500 participants completed the entire questionnaire (age: M = 30.4, SD = 11.9, range = 16–77; 65% female; 93% had at least finished secondary school). Measures. Besides socio-demographic items, the major part of the online questionnaire consisted of 93 items to measure SMEC and two items to check for methodological issues (i.e., perceived difficulty of responding to questions, specific reference movies for answering the items). The response scale was provided as described above. Data analytic procedure. Prior to analysis and using SPSS, all 93 variables were examined for multivariate outliers, univariate normality by assessing skewness and kurtosis (skew < 2, kurtosis < 7), and item difficulties (cut-off criteria for exclusion were < .20 or > .80). No outliers were excluded. The distributions of 13 items were severely skewed, had a highly positive kurtosis, or showed unsatisfactory item difficulties; therefore, these items were excluded. The remaining 80 criteria items were analyzed with CEFA software (Browne, Cudeck, Tateneni, & Mels, 2010). Following the recommendations of Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, and Strahan (1999), exploratory factor analysis (EFA; maximum likelihood [ML] extraction, oblique Geomin rotation, no row standardization; Browne, 2001) was run to assess the dimensionality of the movie criteria and identify reasonable corresponding indicators.3 RMSEA, RMSEA “close” fit (CFit; RMSEA ≤ .050), and parallel analysis were investigated to determine the number of factors. Items without salient loadings (less than |.40| or less than |.50| for the upper bound of the 90% CI) or with multiple salient cross-loadings were removed considering 90% CI for the loadings in the pattern matrix. The primary goal was to achieve a simple structure. Results MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 15 For the 80 items included in EFA, a good model fit was obtained for a 10-factor solution (RMSEA = .047, 90% CI of RMSEA [.045, .048], CFit = .999; parallel analysis also suggested a 10-factor solution). However, 25 items had no salient loading on any of these factors. Thus, these items were excluded, and EFA was run again with the remaining 55 items. This procedure was repeated until adequate fit, salient loadings, and non-salient cross-loadings were achieved. After a total of eight processing cycles, an adequate fit was achieved for an eight-factor solution with 32 items (RMSEA = .052, 90% CI of RMSEA [.046, .057], CFit = .291; parallel analysis also suggested an eight-factor solution). The final solution is presented in Table 2, which also provides latent factor correlations and Raykov's ρ.4 Discussion The first factor is labeled Story Verisimilitude (SV) and comprises three items reflecting correspondence to (contemporary) reality. Similar factors representing realism have been found in previous studies (e.g., Greenberg & Busselle, 1996; Gunter, 1997; Nikken & van der Voort, 1997; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999). The second factor is labeled Story Innovation (SI) and consists of two items reflecting the originality of the story. This is also consistent with dimensions found in previous research like originality (e.g., Greenberg & Busselle, 1996). The third factor is labeled Cinematography (CI) and comprises four items reflecting core cinematic techniques. It is related to film theoretical approaches but has been found in audience research too (e.g., Gunter, 1997). The fourth factor is labeled Special Effects (FX) and consists of four items also reflecting cinematic techniques, but focusing more on technical effects. It has been investigated as a film-inherent factor or an element of film critiques before (e.g., Neelamegham & Jain, 1999; Rössler, 1997). The fifth factor is labeled Recommendations (RE) and comprises five items reflecting external resources for film evaluation. Indicators of RE were found to be important factors in influencing movie attendances and evaluations (e.g., Möller & Karppinen, MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 16 1983; Neelamegham & Jain, 1999). The sixth factor labeled Innocuousness (IN) consists of four items reflecting lack of potentially unpleasant characteristics. This dimension represents anticipated audience responses and is consistent with previous research on children’s TV quality criteria (e.g., Nikken & van der Voort, 1997; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999) and also related to factors that represent the opposite pole of this dimension like sex and violence (e.g., Wyatt & Badger, 1988). The seventh factor is labeled Light-heartedness (LH), comprises five items, and reflects amusement and escapism and has also been found in previous studies (e.g., Greenberg & Busselle, 1996; Nikken & van der Voort, 1997; Valkenburg & Janssen, 1999). Finally, the eighth factor is labeled Cognitive Stimulation (CS), consists of five items, and reflects the viewer’s cognitive processes such as cogitation or learning and is consistent with previous factors (e.g., Himmelweit et al., 1980; Nikken & van der Voort, 1997). Taken together, most of these factors are not independent of each other. However, they share only little variance (Mdn of all latent correlations is .14). Therefore, it seems to be justified to interpret them as distinct dimensions. Because none of the factors is highly correlated with another one, it is also unlikely that there are underlying higher factors. Study 2 Given the initial findings in Study1 concerning the factorial structure and scope of the SMEC dimensions, Study 2 had three goals: (1) to cross-validate the latent structure discovered in Study 1 with another sample and to improve structural validity, (2) to provide evidence for the substantive validity of the different SMEC (i.e., to show that these criteria can be better described as rather enduring and cross-situationally consistent constructs than transient constructs that are susceptible to situational influences), and (3) to locate the SMEC in the nomological network of related constructs by examining their discriminant and convergent validity. Although nothing is yet known about possible relationships to other constructs relevant to SMEC, it stands to reason MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 17 that—based on the disposition-content congruency hypothesis (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013)—the different SMEC dimensions have their own particular relationships with (a) more general personality traits (e.g., the Big Five), (b) thematically related constructs from specific domains (e.g., need for cognition and sensation seeking), and (c) film-specific constructs like film expertise, film genre preferences, and film-specific evaluations. The Nomological Network of SMEC and Related Constructs Big Five personality factors. Although there is surprisingly little research on how the Big Five contribute to movie-related motives, preferences, and evaluations, the scarce empirical evidence clearly demonstrates that individuals seek movies that reflect and reinforce facets of their personalities (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014; Rentfrow, Goldberg, & Zilca, 2011). The Big Five dimensions—Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness—describe personality traits on an abstract and broad level (e.g., Goldberg, 1990). As research on Conscientiousness and movie-related motives, preferences, and evaluations is scarce and connections are not theoretically clear, it is not further considered here. Neurotic (or emotionally instable) individuals are more likely to experience negative affective states as anxiety, anger, or depressed mood. They are self-conscious, impulsive, and vulnerable to stress. Neuroticism was found to be negatively correlated with preferences for action-adventures, intellectual and mainstream films, and violent media content (e.g., Kallias, 2012; Krcmar & Kean, 2005; Weaver, 1991). Extraverts tend to be friendly, gregarious, assertive, active, excitement-seeking, and cheerful. In a few studies, Extraversion was positively associated with the liking of surrealist (Swami, Stieger, Pietschnig, & Voracek, 2010) and violent films (Krcmar & Kean, 2005). People who are open to experiences appreciate aesthetics, reflect on their emotional states, are interested in adventurous activities, are intellectually curious and liberal, and have a MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 18 vivid imagination. They also seem to like surrealist films (Swami et al., 2010) and violent content if it was of aesthetical value (Krcmar & Kean, 2005). Furthermore, Openness was also positively related to artistic and information seeking viewing motivations and to preferences for art, drama, and science-fiction genres as well as negatively related to hedonistic viewing motivations (Kallias, 2012). This is also in line with a strong positive relationship to eudaimonic entertainment motivations (Oliver & Raney, 2011). Moreover, Silvia and Berg (2011) demonstrated that Openness to Experience is also related to expert knowledge about movies. Agreeable people think that most other people are trustworthy. They are friendly, empathetic, generous, and helpful. People who scored high on Agreeableness preferred lighthearted and entertaining programs, such as romances and comedies, whereas low agreeableness was related to viewing or liking action-oriented, suspenseful, violent, dramatic, or horror movies (for overviews, Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014; Kallias, 2012). Furthermore, Agreeableness was positively related to affiliation and hedonistic viewing motivations, preferences for mainstream genres and negatively related to aggressive viewing motivations (Kallias, 2012). Sensation seeking. Sensation seeking, “the need for varied, novel, and complex sensations and experiences and the willingness to take physical and social risks for the sake of such experience” (Zuckerman, 1979, p. 10), manifests itself in individually different levels of optimal arousal. To establish these levels, high sensation seekers search for stimulating sensations to avoid boredom, whereas low sensation seekers rather enjoy calm environments instead of excitement. For instance, whereas high sensation seekers liked horror, suspenseful, violent, action, or sexual content more, and preferred high-arousal films, low sensation seekers preferred light films, and avoided violent and sexually explicit content (for overviews, see Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014; Kallias, 2012). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 19 Need for cognition. The need for cognition, the “tendency to engage in and enjoy thinking” (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982, p. 130), has been found to be positively correlated with the enjoyment of complex narratives (Knobloch-Westerwick & Keplinger, 2008), a productionoriented mode of reception (Suckfüll & Scharkow, 2009), preference for an unspoiled story (Rosenbaum & Johnson, 2016), and with eudaimonic entertainment motivations (Oliver & Raney, 2011). Film expertise. Art expertise is widely known to influence the perception and evaluation of visual arts (e.g., Augustin & Leder, 2006). With regard to movies, Silvia and Berg (2011) argued that the expert’s domain knowledge should facilitate the processing of aesthetic, challenging, and complex stimuli and should allow them to more easily build representations and connect them to other experiences. Film genre preferences. Media genre preferences result from positive experiences with specific genres and evolve over time (Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015, p. 15). To subsume a movie under a specific genre category might help the potential viewers, for instance, to choose among a vast selection of movies according to their preferences, thereby reducing uncertainty, and to facilitate the comprehension of the plot during or after watching a new movie (e.g., interpreting a misfortune in a comedy as funny, in a drama as tragic, or interpreting open endings). Some findings from research on movie genre preferences and broader personality traits were presented above and showed that individuals differ in their movie preferences (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014; Kallias, 2012; Rentfrow et al., 2011). In addition, preferences for dramas, science fiction, and non-fiction (e.g., documentaries) were positively related to eudaimonic gratifications, whereas preferences for comedies and action adventures were negatively correlated with eudaimonic gratifications, but positively associated with hedonic motivations (Igartua & Barrios, 2013; Oliver & Raney, 2011). However, prior MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 20 research on movie preferences did not have the opportunities to go beyond the broad and fuzzy categories of film genres, which are often open to subjective speculation and interpretation. Thus, leaving aside violent film content (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014; Krcmar & Kean, 2005), specific film features across genres have not been examined in detail yet, with one exception: Möller and Karppinen (1983) showed that the viewers’ tendencies to stress characteristics like film critiques and viewing motives (e.g., extending views and opinions and aesthetic experiences) were positively correlated with the preference for drama. Film-specific evaluations. In entertainment research, recent developments distinguish between two distinct but most often associated types of evaluations concerning effects of use, hedonic and eudaimonic entertainment experiences (e.g., Bartsch & Schneider, 2014; Lewis, Tamborini, & Weber, 2014; Oliver & Bartsch, 2010; Wirth et al., 2012). Whereas hedonic entertainment experiences refer to longstanding concepts like enjoyment and pleasure (cf. Vorderer, Klimmt, & Ritterfeld, 2004), the concept of appreciation is prevalent in research on eudaimonic entertainment experiences. It is defined as “an experiential state that is characterized by the perception of deeper meaning, the feeling of being moved, and the motivation to elaborate on thoughts and feelings inspired by the experience” (Oliver & Bartsch, 2010, p. 76). As discussed above with regard to movie evaluations after exposure, film-specific evaluations might not only be related to features of the movie but also to the viewer’s traits (e.g., Krcmar, 2009). Characterizing the associations between SMEC and related constructs. SV reflects the importance of realistic and contemporary elements in a story. Therefore, a positive relationship with preference for documentaries and negative correlations with preferences for genres that deal with unrealistic or fantastic stories (e.g., science fiction and animation) and with film expertise, which focuses on aesthetic and artistic aspects, are expected. As Bartsch and Schneider (2014) pointed out, contrary to the mood management factor lack of semantic affinity MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 21 to real world concerns (Zillmann, 1988) that contributes to hedonic entertainment experiences, eudaimonic entertainment is related to realistic and socially relevant topics and truth-seeking motivations (Oliver & Raney, 2011). Thus, SV should be positively associated with Appreciation. SI represents original and novel aspects of a film’s story. This should lead to positive associations with traits like Sensation Seeking, Extraversion, and Openness to Experiences. SI is also assumed to be positively correlated with film expertise because film experts are knowledgeable about many stories and thus value new and innovative aspects of movies. Innovative stories are often based on ideas about the future or include some kind of “experimental” adaptation of archetypical content (e.g., a hero who saves the world). Thus, SI should be positively related to preferences for science fiction and avant-garde genres. CI focuses on visual and aesthetic aspects of movies such as camera or cutting. Thus, associations with film expertise, Openness to Experiences, Extraversion and Sensation Seeking should be positive. Suckfüll and Scharkow (2009) found a positive relationship between need for cognition and a production-oriented mode of reception. Thus, it seems likely that CI is also positively associated with Need for Cognition. Furthermore, CI should be positively related to preferences for avant-garde and other genres in which visualization is important (e.g., science fiction, animation). FX is also characterized by visual aspects but focuses more on additional technical effects. Special effects are often assumed to be innovative parts of a movie (e.g., trailers contain the most eye-catching, arousing, and exciting special effects). Thus, FX should be positively associated with Sensation Seeking and Extraversion. It is also likely that FX is positively related preferences for light, non-serious genres (e.g., action, animation, and action comedies) as movies of these genres, which are most often produced to be entertaining mainstream blockbusters, MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 22 contain special effects and action at the same time. In contrast, negative relationships with more serious genres like drama, documentary or avant-garde are expected and FX might also be negatively correlated with constructs that reflect more aesthetic and cognitive effort (i.e., film expertise and Need for Cognition, respectively). In addition, it is assumed that the nuances of CI and FX become obvious in different magnitudes of correlations, especially with the genre preferences described above. RE takes the importance of external information about films into account. Therefore, it should be positively related to film expertise and to preferences for genres that receive critical acclaim in the media and awards (e.g., dramas; cf. Oliver, Ash, Woolley, Shade, & Kim, 2014). Furthermore, as neurotic people are more anxious and submissive, and cannot easily make up their minds, they might rely more on film-external features like word of mouth or film critiques when evaluating movies. Finally, people who have a high need for cognition might be more likely to scrutinize cinematic information than to enjoy motion pictures that have already been interpreted or spoiled by others (cf. Rosenbaum & Johnson, 2016). IN focuses on the absence of aversive scenes (i.e., film elements that are disgusting, frightening, violent, or enraging). Thus, IN should show negative associations with Emotional Stability and Sensation Seeking as well as with preferences for genres that usually include scenes with explicit or negatively arousing content (e.g., thriller, horror, tragedy). In addition, IN should be positively connected to preferences for not offensive, harmless genres (e.g., romance and most often comedy), and consequently, to Agreeableness. Because LH mainly represents positive mood and fun, it should also be positively associated with Fun and preferences for light entertainment genres. However, these positive relationships are not expected for serious or negative genres (e.g., avant-garde, drama, horror). With regard to personality traits, a positive correlation with Agreeableness but negative MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 23 correlations with Openness for Experience and Need for Cognition are assumed. Moreover, LH should also negatively correlate with film expertise. Finally, CS is characterized by thought-provocation, broadening knowledge, communicating values, and taking action. Thus, contrary to LH, CS should be positively correlated with Appreciation and preferences for serious genres (e.g., drama, avant-garde), and negatively related to mainstream genres like comedy and action. Moreover, CS should also be positively related to cognitive effort in information processing (e.g., Need for Cognition) as well as to traits that reflect engaging in stimulating activities (e.g., Sensation Seeking, Extraversion, and Openness to Experiences). Method Participants and samples. Two samples from different studies were combined to validate the latent structure. In Study 2a, 152 students filled out questionnaires including the SMEC items; 147 completed all SMEC items (age: M = 22.65, SD = 4.26, range = 18–47; 64% female; more than 85% were undergraduates enrolled in social sciences courses). In an additional sample, an online-study (Study 2b) was conducted that used recruiting strategies different from Study 1 (e.g., undergraduates received course credit for distributing a link to the online questionnaire via their social network profiles or mailing lists; a press release including a link to the study was distributed; a variety of special interest online platforms about cinema and movies posted the link on their news website, in their weekly newsletter, on Facebook, Twitter, or online forum). Participants were offered a chance to win shopping vouchers for Amazon as an incentive. The “Start”-button on the welcome page was clicked by 849 people; 659 of them completed all SMEC items, 587 of them completed the entire questionnaire (age: M = 29.74, SD = 10.64, range = 14–72; 55% female; 86% had at least finished secondary school; Mdn of cinema attendances per month was 1). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 24 Together, both samples yielded a total of 806 valid cases (147 from the first, 659 from the second sample). The total sample was randomly split into two halves R1 and R2—each comprising 403 cases (see below). The 587 participants who completed the entire questionnaire in Study 2b were used for investigating the nomological network of SMEC. Exactly four weeks after completing the online-questionnaire in Study 2b (t1), participants, who wanted to take part in the lottery (n = 371), were invited for the second part of the study (t2). On average, the time between t1 and t2 was 31.08 days (SD = 6.81). The “Start”button on the welcome page was clicked by 282 people (participation rate: 76%); 275 of them completed all SMEC items (age: M = 29.88, SD = 10.97, range = 14–70; 60% female; 88% had at least finished secondary school); 273 participants completed the questionnaire at both measurement occasions. Measures. All final 32 SMEC items from Study 1 were included in Study 2a and 2b (t1 and t2). Moreover, one item (Item si3: “. . . that a movie shows something that has never been shown in a film before?”) that was supposed to load on SI was added as a third indicator for this factor. Besides socio-demographic items, the online-questionnaire in Study 2b also included the following measures to investigate external validity: If not indicated otherwise, all measures were applied using 5-point Likert scales ranging from 0 (completely disagree) to 4 (completely agree). The Big Five dimensions Extraversion (ρ = .72), Emotional Stability (ρ = .65), Openness to Experience (ρ = .50), and Agreeableness (ρ = .53) were assessed using the German version of the Ten-Item Personality Inventory (Muck, Hell, & Gosling, 2007). Zuckerman’s (1979) sensation seeking construct was measured by the eight-item Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (Hoyle, Stephenson, Palmgreen, Lorch, & Donohew, 2002), which was transferred into German via translation–back-translation method (ρ = .80). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 25 To assess need for cognition, the German version of the 16-item Need for Cognition scale (Bless, Wänke, Bohner, Fellhauer, & Schwarz, 1994) was applied (ρ = .87). Film expertise operationalized as a knowledge base for facilitating aesthetic experiences was measured using an adaptation of the Aesthetic Fluency in Film scale (Silvia & Berg, 2011). Participants rated their subjective film expertise on 10 terms reflecting important names related to film production, history, and theory (e.g., “Sergej Eisenstein”, “Mise-en-scéne”, or “The Cabinet of Dr. Caligari”) on 5-point scales ranging from 0 (I have never heard of this artist or term) to 4 (I can talk intelligently about this artist or idea in art). Raykov’s ρ was .90. Furthermore, Pearson correlations with film viewing frequency (r = .30, N = 563) and preferences for avant-garde films (r = .42, N = 587) provided preliminary support for criterion validity.5 Eleven items for measuring film genre preferences—action, animation, avant-garde, comedy, documentary, drama, horror, romance, science-fiction/fantasy, thriller, and tragedy—on a response scale ranging from 0 (completely dislike) to 3 (completely like) were included. To get a fine-grained picture of how SMEC are related to different types of genres, it was necessary to take into account subtle nuances that differentiate between similar genres (e.g., action, sciencefiction, and thriller). Thus, only the genres drama and tragedy, which were highly correlated (r = .67), were combined; all other genre preferences were represented as single item measures. For measuring film viewers’ evaluative responses to specific movies at t2, the German version of the Oliver and Bartsch (2010) scales—Appreciation (Raykov’s ρ = .90) and Fun (ρ = .94), each consisting of three items—was included. Before filling out the scales, participants were asked to recall the last movie they had seen and to respond to the gratification items with that movie in mind. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 26 Data analytic procedures. To confirm the structural validity of the SMEC scales, EFA within the confirmatory factor analysis framework (E/CFA) was conducted for analyzing Sample R1. One of the reasons for not directly moving into a strict CFA framework was the question if indeed all cross-loadings and error covariances could be fixed to zero—as it is done in CFA. Because the latent structure is far from being established at this early stage of scale development, E/CFA is seen as an appropriate intermediate step between EFA and CFA (Brown, 2006, pp. 193–202). Compared to EFA, E/CFA provides more tools for inspecting misspecifications (e.g., a variety of fit indices, standardized residuals, or modification indices). To avoid capitalization on chance, Sample R2 was used to replicate the findings from E/CFA in Sample R1. All analyses were conducted with EQS 6.1 (Bentler & Wu, 2005) and robust ML estimation because of violation of multivariate normality (Satorra & Bentler, 1994). Robust test statistics and cutoff values for assessing model fit were used (Kline, 2011). To accomplish the second goal and to examine whether the SMEC scales assessed rather trait- or state-like constructs, latent state–trait (LST) analyses were conducted (e.g., Schneider, Otto, Alings, & Schmitt, 2014; Steyer, Geiser, & Fiege, 2012). Testing an LST model requires (at least) two parallel test halves at two points of measurement for each of the SMEC. Test halves for each SMEC were generated by randomly assigning the indicators of each SMEC construct to either one or the other half. Several restrictions had to be imposed to obtain good fitting models (for details, see Figure 1). Finally, convergent and discriminant validity were examined by inspecting partial correlations between the SMEC and relevant constructs in their nomological network, controlling for age and gender. Results MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 27 Validity of the latent structure. Although Model 1b clearly replicated the pattern found in Study 1 (see Table 3), modification indices suggested that freely estimating some error covariances would improve the model. Additionally, several cross-loadings were salient (see Table 4). Thus, five items were removed.6 The E/CFA of Model 2 (R1) includes the remaining 28 items and shows improved model fit (e.g., ∆rCFI = .016; rRMSEA includes zero as lower bound CI). Further support for structural validity comes from the replication of Model 2 via CFA (R2; see Table 3). All indices indicate good or acceptable model fit. Stability and reliability. Based on the estimated variance components, the LST coefficients in Figure 1 were calculated.7 First, all SMEC scales demonstrate high reliabilities across both measurement occasions. On average, 88% is due to systematic variance indicating that the scales are reliable. Second, reliabilities are largely determined by stable interindividual differences (i.e., common consistencies were 65% on average). Third, indicators contain small, but statistically significant proportions of test-half-specific variance (i.e., method specificities were 6% on average). Last, on average, 16% variance is due to systematic, but unstable effects of the situation or interaction. For instance, occasion specificities for IN and LH vary about 10 percentage points between t1 and t2. Additionally, SV, CI, and RE show high proportions of occasion specificity compared to common consistency. Convergent and discriminant validity. Previous research on the uses and effects of movies found differences in age and gender (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2014). Thus, Table 5 presents the partial correlations between the SMEC and the constructs relevant to the nomological network, after controlling for gender and age. Discussion The latent structure found in Study 1 was replicated in Study 2. Furthermore, the results of the LST analyses clearly demonstrate that the scales reliably measure stable individual MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 28 differences in SMEC that depend to a small extent on systematic situational or interactional effects present at the measurement occasion. Concerning construct validity, most of the examined partial correlations between related constructs and SMEC, controlling for gender and age, were as expected. More specifically, SV was negatively correlated with preferences for genres that deal with rather unrealistic content (e.g., science fiction, animation, and action) and with film expertise. Furthermore, as hypothesized, it was positively associated with Appreciation. Unexpectedly, it was not statistically significantly related to preference for documentaries. One reason for this might be that the story in a film is considered being mainly fictional and narrative. Although it seems to be important that such stories are based on historical facts, are realistic, and address contemporary issues, this does not mean that these stories need to be real. SI was expectedly associated with film expertise, Openness to Experiences, Extraversion, Sensation Seeking; and preferences for avant-garde and science fiction. The additional positive relationship with Fun and preferences for drama/tragedy might hint to the importance of innovative stories for entertainment experiences as well as for serious genres. As expected, CI was closely positively connected to film expertise. Furthermore, as hypothesized, positive relationships with preferences for genres that typically focus on visual and aesthetic styles (e.g., for avant-garde, science fiction, and animation) as well as with the Need for Cognition, Openness to Experiences, Extraversion and Sensation Seeking clearly draw a distinction between CI and FX as expected. CI did not significantly correlate with Fun or Appreciation. This might be due to the fact that aesthetic aspects build a distinct factor of audience gratifications in the Oliver and Bartsch (2010) studies that could be included in future studies. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 29 FX was, as assumed, positively related to preference for action, comedy, and animation and negatively associated with preference for avant-garde and dramas. Remarkably, FX was not significantly correlated with Sensation Seeking and Extraversion, which might hint to the fact that not all special effects need to be highly arousing. Instead, FX was positively associated with Agreeableness. This is in contrast to previous findings that showed a positive correlation between preferences for and enjoyment of action-oriented content and Psychoticism, Sensation Seeking, and Extraversion. However, more recent findings also suggested a positive relationship between Agreeableness and a preference for mainstream genres (Kallias, 2012). Furthermore, as expected FX was negatively correlated with Need for Cognition and film expertise, emphasizing the distinction between FX and CI. As expected, RE was positively associated with preference for drama/tragedy and negatively related to Need for Cognition and Emotional Stability. Unexpectedly, it was positively but not significantly correlated with film expertise. Maybe due to the fact that film experts are already knowledgeable and competent they do not need to rely on the recommendations of others. However, they must surely have the external sources to keep their expertise up-to-date. Perhaps items concerning these resources are not included in the RE scale. Thus, adding more items to investigate the uniqueness of the facets of the scale might be also an effective way to cope with the substantial method specificity (see Figure 1). Participants who tend to rate IN as important had less film expertise. As expected, IN is positively associated with harmless genres (e.g., romance) and Agreeableness, and negatively with genres that are assumed to include frightening and violent content (e.g., horror) and Emotional Stability and Sensation Seeking. Contrary to expectations, IN correlated only low with preference for comedy but positively with Appreciation. Despite drawing a distinction between IN and LH, this result is difficult to explain. Perhaps this is so because comedies can MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 30 also include scenes in which disgusting things happen, schadenfreude is evoked, or aggressive behavior is celebrated (e.g., Dumb and Dumber), thereby leading to rather mixed emotions. With regard to LH, nearly all correlations are in line with previous assumptions and reflect the semantic closeness to mood management factors or U&G dimensions like entertainment or escapism as well as to the Fun scale, and Agreeableness. At the same time, the magnitudes are rather low or moderate. Thus, the results point in the right direction but clearly draw a distinction between the constructs. In addition, LH is negatively connected to constructs or genres that reflect some kind of aesthetical interest or cognitive effort (e.g., film expertise, preference for avant-garde, Need for Cognition, Openness to Experiences). As expected, CS was negatively related to preferences for action and comedy (although the latter correlation was not statistically significant) and positively related to serious genres (e.g., drama, avant-garde, documentary). Furthermore, CS was as assumed positively related to Appreciation as well as to Need for Cognition and broader aspects of stimulation (e.g., Openness to Experience, Extraversion, and Sensation Seeking). The scope of CS and the results indicate a close connection to the meaningful and thought-provoking aspect of eudaimonic entertainment. Taken together all partial correlations in the nomological network showed only small to moderate magnitudes. Hence, the constructs share rather little proportions of variance with SMEC. These low partial correlations indicate discriminant validity and also help to draw distinctions between the SMEC themselves (e.g., SV and CS, CI and FX, IN and LH). However, it is quite evident that measuring the evaluative importance of, for instance, aesthetic movies has a very different level of specificity than such broad personality traits like Openness to Experiences. Therefore, in future studies, a higher level of correspondence, for example, between CI and the openness-facet aesthetics could be achieved by measuring personality traits with the many-faceted scales of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 31 A final note concerns the theoretical relationship between SMEC and film-specific evaluations. If SMEC are conceptualized as stable and important attitudes toward specific film features, individual differences in SMEC might predict film-specific evaluation like enjoyment or appreciation. Similar ideas have been proposed by GSGO models (e.g., Palmgreen & Rayburn, 1985). However, as argued in the TSQA (Wolling, 2009), such an influence might be only exerted when viewers also perceived the features they consider as important. As participants in the present study only rated their experiences with the last movie they have seen, it is unknown whether they perceived the corresponding features. Although the SMEC were developed to be generally applicable across a variety of movies, there might be movies, for example, without any special effects. Thus, future research should not only include featurespecific evaluations but also ask if these specific film-features have been perceived. General Discussion The major goal of the present research was to delineate what is important when we watch and evaluate a movie. Taken together, the studies provide evidence that subjective movie evaluation criteria are much more complex than the introductory quote from Harry Cohn suggested. Individuals differ in the criteria they use when they evaluate movies. Eight distinct dimensions reflect film-inherent features (Story Verisimilitude, Story Innovation, Cinematography, Special Effects), film-external features (Recommendation), or (anticipated) effects of use (Innocuousness, Light-heartedness, Cognitive Stimulation). These findings are in line with previous studies on film critiques and TV quality but extend them by exploring SMEC in the field of movies and applying current standards in scale construction and validation. Furthermore, LST analyses supported the assumption that individual differences in SMEC are rather trait-like. Future Research Directions MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 32 Besides the preliminary support for the conceptualization of SMEC as important attitudes toward specific film features, it remains an open question how these SMEC emerge and develop during the life-span. Research provides some evidence that media preferences crystallize during different developmental levels (Valkenburg & Cantor, 2000). During socialization, we do not only develop likes and dislikes for film genres. Rather, it seems plausible that we even develop fairly stable likes and dislikes for specific film features. Moreover, in the course of time we gain knowledge about movies, film production, and cinematic experiences. Furthermore, film features are not independent of each other; specific features frequently appear together with certain other features because the movies are produced in correspondence with a specific film genre category (e.g., a romantic comedy combines a happy ending, funny scenes, maybe a less innovative story, no special effects, etc.). Consequently, the development of SMEC is accompanied by the development of mental representations of genre categories as well as genre preferences and distinguishing between SMEC and genre preferences is a creative, longitudinal endeavor. Thus, applying theories that deal with cognitive as well as socio-emotional development might help to disentangle both constructs and clarify their relationships (Valkenburg & Cantor, 2000). In addition, answering these questions about how SMEC develop and what the antecedents of SMEC are, would contribute considerably to the substantive validity of the construct. As outlined in the introductory paragraphs, SMEC are only one part of the puzzle concerning movie evaluations but they could help to examine evaluative processes before, during, and after movie exposure more deeply. Thus, we must carefully differentiate between SMEC and perceived features that might result from the interaction of a viewer with a specific movie, leading not only to evaluations of multiple experiences (cf. Oliver & Bartsch, 2010) but also to evaluations of film-inherent and film-external features. Whereas the SMEC can be MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 33 conceptualized as mental representations of important attitudes towards specific film features, the evaluative responses can be seen as resulting from those activated representations. The scales developed here provide an appropriate measurement tool only for SMEC. Thus, future steps should include the development of appropriate tools for measuring perceptions of and evaluative responses to specific film features. This approach is similar to the TSQA (Wolling, 2009) which proposes that the interplay of desired and perceived media product features results in the evaluation of these features. In search for a catalogue of criteria that could be tested within the TSQA, the SMEC provide a first step and a sound instrument. Hence, SMEC can be used to address research questions such as: How do different SMEC contribute to the selection or global judgment of a movie, dependent or independent from perceived features? Furthermore, SMEC might work as a fine-grained alternative to preferences and sought gratifications because they do not only refer to anticipated effects of use (i.e., IN, LH, and CS) but also to film-external (RE) and film-inherent features (SV, SI, CI, FX), thereby taking aspects of content into account (Swanson, 1987; Wolling, 2009). Future studies should investigate whether SMEC might be better predictors of movie evaluations compared to genre preferences within and across specific genres. In addition, as SMEC are conceptualized as mental representations of important attitudes, the concept can easily be connected to and explored from the perspective of recently proposed theories in the field of social cognition (e.g., Gawronski, 2007; see also Schneider, 2012). With these contributions and research questions in mind, it is also important to mention limitations and propose solutions. First, the convenience samples were predominantly conducted online and largely consisted of younger and higher-educated participants, thereby leading to a sample bias and presumably to systematic bias. Although moviegoer statistics show that the highest frequency of movie attendances in Germany is among adolescents and students (FFA MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 34 Filmförderungsanstalt, 2011), this certainly calls for further studies, which might be conducted under other circumstances (e.g., in field studies at cinema theaters) and include samples with different socio-demographic or ethnic backgrounds. Secondly and as it is true with every scale, the conceptualization and measurement of SMEC need to prevail over time. More studies should be conducted to broaden the nomological network by taking recent theoretical advances into account. For example, eudaimonic entertainment motivations and experiences (e.g., Oliver & Raney, 2011; Wirth et al., 2012) might help to disentangle the antecedents and consequences of specific SMEC (e.g., CS and LH). Moreover, they might enrich the conceptualization of SMEC. Due to the fact that the modelling of the construct space and the SMEC dimensions were data-driven and based on the subjective ideas of lay audiences, it is important to prove that more parsimonious models with fewer dimensions do not compete with the present structure. This could be accomplished by testing the predictive and incremental power of the SMEC and complementary constructs, for example, with regard to specific evaluations of movies belonging to different genres. Finally, as attitude importance is only one strength-related attribute among many others (e.g., attitude accessibility) and is based on self-reports, it might be interesting to examine whether multi-methodological approaches can enrich the measurement (e.g., by developing and applying indirect measures; cf. Hefner, Rothmund, Klimmt, & Gollwitzer, 2011). The SMEC scales are developed to address the question what is important when we evaluate a movie. Hopefully, a number of researchers will elaborate on the SMEC construct and scales, improve them, and find them useful to delineate the role of these criteria in the underlying processes and effects of evaluating movies. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 35 References Augustin, M. D., & Leder, H. (2006). Art expertise: a study of concepts and conceptual spaces. Psychology Science, 48, 135–156. Austin, B. A. (1986). Motivations for movie attendance. Communication Quarterly, 34, 115– 126. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463378609369627 Bartsch, A., & Schneider, F. M. (2014). Entertainment and politics revisited: How non-escapist forms of entertainment can stimulate political interest and information seeking. Journal of Communication, 64, 369–396. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12095 Bartsch, A., Vorderer, P., Mangold, R., & Viehoff, R. (2008). Appraisal of emotions in media use: Toward a process model of meta-emotion and emotion regulation. Media Psychology, 11, 7–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213260701813447 Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. C. (2005). EQS 6 structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software. Retrieved from www.mvsoft.com Bless, H., Wänke, M., Bohner, G., Fellhauer, R. F., & Schwarz, N. (1994). Need for Cognition: Eine Skala zur Erfassung von Engagement und Freude bei Denkaufgaben [Need for cognition: A scale measuring engagement and happiness in cognitive tasks]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 25, 147–154. Boninger, D. S., Krosnick, J. A., Berent, M. K., & Fabrigar, L. R. (1995). The causes and consequences of attitude importance. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 159–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York, NY: Guilford Press. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 36 Browne, M. W. (2001). An overview of analytic rotation in exploratory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 36, 111–150. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327906MBR3601_05 Browne, M. W., Cudeck, R., Tateneni, K., & Mels, G. (2010). CEFA: Comprehensive Exploratory Factor Analysis (Version 3.04). Retrieved from http://faculty.psy.ohiostate.edu/browne/ Burisch, M. (1984). Approaches to personality inventory construction: A comparison of merits. American Psychologist, 39, 214–227. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.3.214 Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116 Carroll, N. E. (2003). Engaging the moving image. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Cattell, R. B. (1988). Psychological theory and scientific method. In J. R. Nesselroade & R. B. Cattell (Eds.), Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology (2nd ed.) (pp. 3–20). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Kallias, A., & Hsu, A. (2014). What type of movie person are you? Understanding individual differences in film preferences and uses: A psychographic approach. In J. C. Kaufman & D. K. Simonton (Eds.), The social science of cinema (pp. 87–122). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Ressources. d'Astous, A., & Touil, N. (1999). Consumer evaluations of movies on the basis of critics' judgment. Psychology & Marketing, 16, 677–694. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)15206793(199912)16:8<677::AID-MAR4>3.0.CO;2-T MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 37 Eaton, A. A., & Visser, P. S. (2008). Attitude importance: Understanding the causes and consequences of passionately held views. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1719–1736. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00125.x Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272– 299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.4.3.272 FFA Filmförderungsanstalt. (2011). Der Kinobesucher 2010: Strukturen und Entwicklungen [German Federal Film Board: The Moviegoers 2010]. Berlin. Retrieved from http://www.ffa.de/download.php?f=b35f7381f893e02729cfac5c62bfa735&target=0 Finn, S. (1997). Origins of media exposure: Linking personality traits to TV, radio, print, and film use. Communication Research, 24, 507–529. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365097024005003 Gawronski, B. (Ed.). (2007). What is an attitude? [Special Issue]. Social Cognition, 25(5). Gehrau, V. (2008). Fernsehbewertung und Fernsehhandlung: Ansätze und Daten zu Erhebung, Modellierung und Folgen von Qualitätsurteilen des Publikums über Fernsehangebote [Television evaluation and television action: Approaches and data relating to the recording, modeling and consequences of quality assessments of television programs by viewers]. München: Reinhard Fischer. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative 'description of personality': The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216 Greenberg, B. S., & Busselle, R. W. (1996). Audience dimensions of quality in situation comedies and action programmes. In S. Ishikawa (Ed.), Quality assessment of television (pp. 169–196). Luton, UK: University of Luton Press; John Libbey Media. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 38 Gunter, B. (1997). An audience-based approach to assessing programme quality. In P. Winterhoff-Spurk & T. H. A. van der Voort (Eds.), New horizons in media psychology: Research cooperation and projects in Europe (pp. 11–34). Wiesbaden: Westdeutscher Verlag. Hadida, A. L. (2009). Motion picture performance: A review and research agenda. International Journal of Management Reviews, 11, 297–335. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14682370.2008.00240.x Hefner, D., Rothmund, T., Klimmt, C., & Gollwitzer, M. (2011). Implicit measures and media effects research: Challenges and opportunities. Communication Methods and Measures, 5, 181–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2011.597006 Himmelweit, H. T., Swift, B., & Jaeger, M. E. (1980). The audience as a critic: A conceptual analysis of television entertainment. In P. H. Tannenbaum (Ed.), The entertainment functions of television (pp. 67–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Holbrook, M. B. (1999). Popular appeal versus expert judgments of motion pictures. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 144–155. https://doi.org/10.1086/209556 Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, R. L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 401–414. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0 Igartua, J.-J., & Barrios, I. (2013). Hedonic and eudaimonic motives for watching feature films. Validation of the Spanish version of Oliver & Raney’s scale. Communications, 38, 411– 431. https://doi.org/10.1515/commun-2013-0024 Jacobs, R. S., Heuvelman, A., ben Allouch, S., & Peters, O. (2015). Everyone's a critic: The power of expert and consumer reviews to shape readers’ post-viewing motion picture evaluations. Poetics, 52, 91–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2015.07.002 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 39 Kallias, A. (2012). Individual differences and the psychology of film preferences (Doctoral thesis). University of London. London. Retrieved from http://research.gold.ac.uk/7813/ Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2015). Choice and preference in media use: Advances in selective exposure theory and research. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis. Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Keplinger, C. (2008). Murder for pleasure: Impacts of plot complexity and need for cognition on mystery enjoyment. Journal of Media Psychology, 20, 117–128. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105.20.3.117 Krcmar, M. (2009). Individual differences in media effects. In R. L. Nabi & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), The Sage handbook of media processes and effects (pp. 235–250). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Krcmar, M., & Kean, L. G. (2005). Uses and gratifications of media violence: Personality correlates of viewing and liking violent genres. Media Psychology, 7, 399–420. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0704_5 Lewis, R. J., Tamborini, R., & Weber, R. (2014). Testing a dual-process model of media enjoyment and appreciation. Journal of Communication, 64, 397–416. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12101 Linton, J. M., & Petrovich, J. A. (1988). The application of the consumer information acquisition approach to movie selection: An exploratory study. In B. A. Austin (Ed.), Current research in film. Audiences, economics and law (Vol. 4, pp. 24–45). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 40 Möller, K. E. K., & Karppinen, P. (1983). Role of motives and attributes in consumer motion picture choice. Journal of Economic Psychology, 4, 239–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(83)90029-6 Muck, P. M., Hell, B., & Gosling, S. D. (2007). Construct validation of a short five-factor model instrument: A self-peer study on the German adaptation of the Ten-Item Personality Inventory (TIPI-G). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 166–175. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.166 Neelamegham, R., & Jain, D. (1999). Consumer choice process for experience goods: An econometric model and analysis. Journal of Marketing Research, 36, 373–386. https://doi.org/10.2307/3152083 Nikken, P., & van der Voort, T. H. A. (1997). Children's views on quality standards for children's television programs. Journal of Educational Media, 23, 169–188. https://doi.org/10.1080/1358165970230206 Nikken, P., van der Voort, T. H. A., & van Bochove, E. (1996). Maternal quality standards for children's television programs. Journal of Educational Media, 22, 41–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/1358165960220104 Oliver, M. B., Ash, E., Woolley, J. K., Shade, D. D., & Kim, K. (2014). Entertainment we watch and entertainment we appreciate: Patterns of motion picture consumption and acclaim over three decades. Mass Communication and Society, 17, 853–873. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2013.872277 Oliver, M. B., & Bartsch, A. (2010). Appreciation as audience response: Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human Communication Research, 36, 53–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01368.x MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 41 Oliver, M. B., & Raney, A. A. (2011). Entertainment as pleasurable and meaningful: Identifying hedonic and eudaimonic motivations for entertainment consumption. Journal of Communication, 61, 984–1004. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01585.x Palmgreen, P., Cook, P. L., Harvill, J. G., & Helm, D. M. (1988). The motivational framework for moviegoing: Uses and avoidances of theatrical films. In B. A. Austin (Ed.), Current research in film. Audiences, economics and law (Vol. 4, pp. 1–23). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Palmgreen, P., & Rayburn, J. D., II. (1985). An expectancy-value approach to media gratifications. In K. E. Rosengren, L. A. Wenner, & P. Palmgreen (Eds.), Media gratifications research. Current perspectives (pp. 61–72). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21, 173–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/01466216970212006 Rentfrow, P. J., Goldberg, L. R., & Zilca, R. (2011). Listening, watching, and reading: The structure and correlates of entertainment preferences. Journal of Personality, 79, 223– 258. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00662.x Rosenbaum, J. E., & Johnson, B. K. (2016). Who’s afraid of spoilers? Need for cognition, need for affect, and narrative selection and enjoyment. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5, 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000076 Rössler, P. (1997). Filmkritiker und Publikum: Diskrepanzen und Übereinstimmungen. Ergebnisse einer Befragung von Filmrezensenten und Kinogängern [Film critics and the public: discrepancies and agreements: Results of a survey of film critics and moviegoers]. Media Perspektiven, 28, 133–140. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye & C. C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 42 analysis: Applications for developmental research (pp. 399–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Scherer, K. R. (2013). The nature and dynamics of relevance and valence appraisals: Theoretical advances and recent evidence. Emotion Review, 5, 150–162. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912468166 Schneider, F. M. (2012). Measuring subjective movie evaluation criteria. Conceptual foundation, construction, and validation of the SMEC scales (Dissertation). Universität KoblenzLandau. Landau. Retrieved from http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:hbz:lan1-7813 Schneider, F. M., Otto, L., Alings, D., & Schmitt, M. (2014). Measuring traits and states in public opinion research: A latent state–trait analysis of political efficacy. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 26, 202–223. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edu002 Schneider, F. M., Vogel, I. C., Gleich, U., & Bartsch, A. (2014, May). Evaluating movies: A review of movie evaluations and a framework for investigating underlying psychological processes. Paper presentation at the 64th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association (ICA). Seattle, WA. Schramm, H., & Wirth, W. (2010). Exploring the paradox of sad-film enjoyment: The role of multiple appraisals and meta-appraisals. Poetics, 38, 319–335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2010.03.002 Silvia, P. J., & Berg, C. (2011). Finding movies interesting: How appraisals and expertise influence the aesthetic experience of film. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 29, 73–88. https://doi.org/10.2190/EM.29.1.e Simonton, D. K. (2009). Cinematic success criteria and their predictors: The art and business of the film industry. Psychology & Marketing, 26, 400–420. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20280 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 43 Steyer, R., Geiser, C., & Fiege, C. (2012). Latent state-trait models. In H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol 3: Data analysis and research publication (pp. 291–308). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association. Suckfüll, M., & Scharkow, M. (2009). Modes of reception for fictional films. Communications, 34, 361–384. https://doi.org/10.1515/COMM.2009.023 Swami, V., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2010). The disinterested play of thought: Individual differences and preference for surrealist motion pictures. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 855–859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.013 Swanson, D. L. (1987). Gratification seeking, media exposure, and audience interpretations: Some directions for research. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 31, 237–254. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838158709386662 Tamborini, R., Grizzard, M., Bowman, N. D., Reinecke, L., Lewis, R. J., & Eden, A. (2011). Media enjoyment as need satisfaction: The contribution of hedonic and nonhedonic needs. Journal of Communication, 61, 1025–1042. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14602466.2011.01593.x Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Exploring personality through test construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment. Personality measurement and testing (Vol. 2, pp. 261–292). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Tesser, A., Millar, K. U., & Wu, C.-H. (1988). On the perceived functions of movies. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 122, 441–449. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1988.10542949 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 44 Valkenburg, P. M., & Cantor, J. (2000). Children's likes and dislikes of entertainment programs. In D. Zillmann & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Media entertainment: The psychology of its appeal (pp. 135–152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Valkenburg, P. M., & Janssen, S. C. (1999). What do children value in entertainment programs? A cross-cultural investigation. Journal of Communication, 49, 3–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1999.tb02790.x Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication, 63, 221–243. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12024 Vorderer, P., Klimmt, C., & Ritterfeld, U. (2004). Enjoyment: At the heart of media entertainment. Communication Theory, 14, 388–408. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14682885.2004.tb00321.x Vorderer, P., Knobloch, S., & Schramm, H. (2001). Does entertainment suffer from interactivity? The impact of watching an interactive TV movie on viewers' experience of entertainment. Media Psychology, 3, 343–363. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0304_03 Weaver, J. B. (1991). Exploring the links between personality and media preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 1293–1299. https://doi.org/10.1016/01918869(91)90203-N Wilson, M. (2005). Constructing measures: An item response modeling approach. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Wirth, W., Hofer, M., & Schramm, H. (2012). Beyond pleasure: Exploring the eudaimonic entertainment experience. Human Communication Research, 38, 406–428. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2012.01434.x Wolling, J. (2004). Qualitätserwartungen, Qualitätswahrnehmungen und die Nutzung von Fernsehserien: Ein Beitrag zur Theorie und Empirie der subjektiven Qualitätsauswahl von MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 45 Medienangeboten [Quality expectations, quality perceptions and viewing of television serials: A contribution to the theoretical and empirical study of the subjective selection of media content]. Publizistik, 49, 171–193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11616-004-0035-y Wolling, J. (2009). The effect of subjective quality assessments on media selection. In T. Hartmann (Ed.), Media choice. A theoretical and empirical overview (pp. 85–101). New York, NY: Routledge. Wyatt, R. O., & Badger, D. P. (1988). What newspaper film critics value in film and film criticism: A national survey. In B. A. Austin (Ed.), Current research in film. Audiences, economics and law (Vol. 4, pp. 54–71). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Wyatt, R. O., & Badger, D. P. (1990). Effects of information and evaluation in film criticism. Journalism Quarterly, 67, 359–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769909006700213 Young, S. D. (2012). Psychology at the movies. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management through communication choices. American Behavioral Scientist, 31, 327–340. https://doi.org/10.1177/000276488031003005 Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 46 Footnotes 1 See Neelamegham and Jain (1999), Linton and Petrovich (1988), or Wolling (2009), for instance, for similar categorizations. 2 Burisch (1984) distinguishes three primary approaches to the development of self-report inventories (external, inductive, and deductive). In an inductive approach, no preconceived set of dimensions are derived from theory. Instead, empirical data analysis should reveal important dimensions and their interrelationships. Furthermore, whereas deductive scale constructionists aim at accommodating the initial constructs, inductive scale developers examine them (Tellegen & Waller, 2008, p. 261). Thus, the inductive approach can be characterized as theory-building rather than theory-testing. In personality psychology, there is a long tradition of inductive scale development, for example, several Big Five inventories based on the lexical approach or the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. For more details, the interested reader is referred to Tellegen and Waller (2008, p. 261–263), who discuss some advantages of an inductive approach, especially as a first step in the “inductive–hypothetico–deductive method” (Cattell, 1988). 3 The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin coefficient was good (.85), measure of sample adequacy coefficients were all above .70, and the Bartlett test of sphericity was significant, thus indicating applicability of EFA. Mardia’s coefficient for multivariate skewness was not statistically significant, thus indicating no violation of multivariate normal distribution. The coefficient for multivariate kurtosis was statistically significant. However, the pattern matrix of the ML extraction correlated over .98 with the pattern matrix of a principal axis factor analysis, which requires no distributive assumptions, thus indicating no bias. 4 Throughout the text the composite reliability estimator Raykov’s ρ is reported because Cronbach’s α underestimates reliability in case of congeneric measures models (Raykov, 1997). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 47 5 More details on the psychometric properties of the scale are available from the author. 6 Freely estimating error covariance parameters or excluding items needs theoretical justification. On the one hand, the reasons for correlated error variances seem to lie in redundant item content. For instance, Item cs1* (“that the movie is thought-provoking”) and Item cs2 (“that the movie is intellectually challenging”) are obviously synonymous. In a similar vein, Items ci3 and ci4* as well as lh4* and lh5 seemed to be semantically related (for item wording see Table 2). Based on their psychometric properties, the items with asterisk* were preferred, while the competing ones were dropped. On the other hand, two items (fx4 and re3) were excluded because their loading patterns were ambiguous. For instance, re3 had only low loadings on RE (λ = .29), but also loaded similarly on LH (λ = .19). One reason might be that friends provide information concerning the movie’s entertainment value, whereas the remaining items of the RE dimension cover aspects of the movie itself. The Item fx4 seems to be a summary item for the FX dimension; it also statistically significantly cross-loaded on CI (λ = .36) and RE (λ = .12) and showed the lowest loading on FX (λ = .57) compared to the other FX-items. Therefore, it was excluded, too. 7 As in classical test theory, the reliability (Rel) represents the ratio of the true score variance to the observed variance. However, in LST theory, the true score variance can be further decomposed into a latent trait component representing stable individual differences (its proportion is reflected in the common consistency coefficient cCon), a latent state residual representing systematic situational influences (occasion specificity coefficient Spe), and—in case of method effects—a method factor component representing systematic variance of the measurement instrument or, as here, the test halves (method specificity coefficient mSpe). For further details, see Schneider et al. (2014) or Steyer and Schmitt (1990). MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 48 Table 1 Overview of selected studies dealing with movie evaluation criteria or related constructs Source Object Criteria dimensions (Examples for operationalization) Gehrau Evaluative Adjectives: (2008) responses to TV Emotional Entertainment (e.g., entertaining, pleasant, good) shows Cognitive Information (e.g., realistic, serious, credible) inconsistent findings for further dimensions across the different stimuli Content and production quality: Technical production (e.g., photography, editing, sound) Content (e.g., story, relevance, characters) Greenberg Evaluative & Busselle responses to TV (1996) shows Gunter (1997) Attributes of soap operas Himmelweit a) Evaluative et al. (1980) responses to TV shows Appreciation (e.g., enjoyable, entertaining) [for comedies & action–adventures] Real vs. funny (e.g., realistic, serious–light) [only for comedies one combined factor] Fairness (e.g., fair, gentle) [only for comedies] Modern (e.g., new, modern) [only for comedies] Originality (e.g., unusual, original) [for comedies & action–adventures] Study details Survey after viewing excerpts from TV shows and films (N = 563; German TV research panel); principal component analyses with 20 adjectives regarding the general quality and 14 terms regarding the quality of the content and production Survey after viewing situation comedy/action program (N = 1,345 US students); factor analysis with 44 adjective pairs regarding general attributes of the program Real-time responses and Verisimilitude (e.g., “The characters were true to life.”) focus group discussions for Established Characters (e.g., “All the characters clearly belonged to the local area.”) item development and survey Tension/drama (e.g., “There were a number of problems going on that surprised and (N = 3,000; British TV intrigued me.”) research panel); no factor Entertainment/Involvement (e.g., “I couldn’t wait to find out what happened next.”) Coherence/Cohesion (e.g., “Each of the storylines in this episode had a clear beginning, analyses, only internal consistencies of the eight middle, and end.”) Technical Professionalism (e.g., “The camera work balanced long and close-up shots.”) dimensions comprising 26 items developed in the focus Contrast and Balance (e.g., “It included both light and serious storylines.”) group study were assessed in Plot/Setting (e.g., “The setting was recognizable and reasonably familiar.”) the survey Impact Reactions (informative, moving, exciting, disturbing, trivial, annoying, Survey (N > 1,000; BBC convincing, and absorbing) viewing panel); ratings on 17 Stylistic Attributes (e.g., realistic, lighthearted, funny, informative, complicated, brutal, attributes and violent) (table continues) MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 49 Table 1 (continued) Source Object Criteria dimensions (Examples for operationalization) Study details b) Stylistic Approach to/Avoidance of Potentially Upsetting Stimuli (e.g., “A sad ending just preferences with makes me miserable.”) regard to TV Aloofness or Involvement in Programs (e.g., “I prefer programs that appeal to my series heart.”) Preferences for Low/High Cognitive Effort (e.g., “I prefer programs that you have got Factor analysis with 20 items to make a real effort to understand.”) Preference for Real vs. Fantasy Content/Presentation (e.g., “I prefer programs set in everyday surroundings.”) Liking for Thriller/Action Content (e.g., “What makes a plot interesting is the action.”) Linton & Petrovich (1988) Important Foreground (e.g., storyline, characters, acting, scenery, music, etc.) attributes for Background (e.g., editing, photography, casting, director, etc.) movie evaluation Survey (N = 192; students); factor analysis with 15 items developed in a pilot study Nikken & Children’s Van der quality standards Voort in TV programs (1997) Credibility (e.g., “A children’s program should give a balanced image of reality.”) Comprehensibility (e.g., “. . . should be understandable for all children.”) Entertainment (e.g., “. . . should be funny.”) Aesthetic Quality (e.g., “. . . should contain beautiful images.”) Involvement (e.g., “. . . should capture a child’s attention.”) Presence of Role Models (e.g., “ . . . should feature persons a child wants to be like.”) Innocuousness (e.g., “. . . should not make a child sad.”) Restfulness (e.g., “. . . should set a child at ease.”) Thought Provocation (e.g., “. . . should make a child curious.”) Survey (N = 427 Dutch children); principal components analysis of 52 items mainly derived from maternal quality criteria of children’s TV programs (Nikken et al., 1996) Rössler (1997) a) Important evaluation criteria in film critiques Technical Qualities (e.g., sound, camera, music, and editing) Plot (e.g., director, actors, dramaturgy, and plausibility) Educational Engagement (e.g., political ambition, social relevance) Entertainment (e.g., entertainment value, stars, and special effects) Survey on German newspaper film critics (N = 150); factor analysis with 15 items b) Important evaluation criteria from viewers’ perspective Entertainment (suspense and fun), educational engagement (political and social relevance), aesthetic value Survey on German moviegoers (N = 225); five items selected from a) (table continues) MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 50 Table 1 (continued) Source Object Valkenburg Children’s & Janssen criteria for a (1999) good TV program Criteria dimensions (Examples for operationalization) Interestingness (e.g., “show somebody I would really like to be”) Romance (e.g., “be about love”) Realism (e.g., “show things that are real”) Violence (e.g., “be about brave and strong heroes”) Humor (e.g., “be full of jokes”) Innocuousness (e.g., “not show any violence that children can imitate”) Comprehensibility (e.g., “be easy to understand”) Action (e.g., “contain a lot of action”) Wolling (2004) Quality dimensions of TV shows Wyatt & Badger (1988) a) Film critics’ Sex/Violence (e.g., vulgarity, sex, violence, nudity, MPAA rating) important movie Production Elements (e.g., editing, design, cinematography) characteristics Performance Elements (e.g., lead actor, director, plot, screenplay) Production Difficulty (e.g., production difficulty, cost, genre) Survey (N = 166; US newspaper film critics); factor analysis with 22 items b) Content Personal Impression/Judgment (e.g., subjective responses, evaluative adjectives) elements of film Objective Reporting (e.g., facts of the film) critiques Audience Reaction (e.g., discussing audience reaction, viewers who might enjoy) Factor analysis with 13 items c) Film critics’ important functions of a movie Conflict Management (e.g., harmony, aggression) Relation to Reality (e.g., authenticity) Story (e.g., temporal closeness, suspense, erotic) Production (e.g., details, humor) Study details Focus group discussions and paper-and-pencil questionnaires (N = 200 Dutch and US children); principal components analysis with 44 items Survey (N = 100); principal component analysis with 23 items (analyses within each not across dimensions) Aesthetic Experience (e.g., artistic experience, aesthetic experience, and self-education) Entertainment (e.g., diversion, escape, and relaxation) Factor analysis with 25 items Arousal (e.g., danger, emotional arousal, and sexual stimulation) Subject of Conversation (e.g., basis of conversation, what’s talked about) Ethical Value (e.g., model of behavior, reinforce values) MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 51 Table 2 Latent structure of subjective movie evaluation criteria: Descriptive statistics (M, SD), results of ML-EFA (loadings in the pattern matrix, CI, communalities h², and latent factor correlations), and Raykov’s ρ (N = 506) No.a Wording M SD SV SI sv1 that the story the movie tells is based on .73 true facts (e.g., based on a historic event 0.86 0.98 [.66, .81] or tells the life story of a real person)? sv2 .65 that the story the film tells is realistic? 1.72 1.21 [.58, .72] sv3 that the movie addresses contemporary .56 0.88 0.91 issues? [.49, .64] si1 that the movie tells a story in a novel .71 1.98 1.07 way? [.61, .82] si2 .69 that the story the film tells is unusual? 2.12 1.06 [.59, .80] ci1 the film’s camera work and the shots? 2.54 1.04 ci2 the way how the film is cut or how individual shots are cut together? ci3 the illumination and lighting in the movie? ci4 the color scheme in the movie (e.g., the use of black-and-white, red as a symbol or signal color, etc.)? fx1 that the production of the film was extravagant? fx2 the digital (post-)editing of the film? 2.43 1.08 2.11 1.10 2.27 1.09 1.12 1.03 1.42 1.13 fx3 the movie illusions (e.g., special effects such as fire, explosions, stunts, combat 1.73 1.17 scenes)? fx4 the technical design of the film 1.91 1.08 altogether? CI FX RE IN LH CS h² .55 .43 .40 .56 .54 .90 [.87, .93] .83 [.80, .86] .79 [.75, .83] .79 .71 .64 .58 [.52, .64] .49 .68 [.62, .74] .20 .67 [.13, .27] [.60, .73] .50 .57 .66 [.60, .73] .54 .23 .64 [.16, .31] [.57, .71] .53 (table continues) MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 52 Table 2 (continued) No.a Wording re1 the award(s) for the film (e.g., Oscar, Golden Globe, Golden Palm, Golden Bear, German Film Award, Grimme Award)? re2 the reviews of the film in the “media” (e.g., press, radio & TV, movie sites in the Internet)? re3 the opinions of friends, acquaintances, etc. about the film? re4 how the film is advertised (e.g., on TV or movie trailers, posters, newspaper, and magazine ads)? re5 the fact that the film is considered a classic movie? that the movie is free of scenes that you in1 find disgusting? that the movie is free of scenes that in2 make you angry? that the movie is free of scenes that you in3 find frightening? that the movie is free of scenes in4 containing violence? that the film puts you in a cheerful lh1 mood? M SD CI FX RE IN LH CS h² .79 [.73, .84] .61 1.19 1.04 .71 [.65, .77] .47 1.72 1.04 .58 [.51, .64] .39 1.02 0.96 .25 .51 [.16, .33] [.44, .58] .38 1.14 1.08 .45 [.38, .52] .30 1.81 1.53 1.41 1.35 1.43 1.36 1.10 1.21 2.24 1.18 2.37 1.00 lh3 that you find the movie entertaining? 3.03 0.84 lh4 that you find the movie relaxing? 2.21 1.17 that the movie takes your mind off everyday things? SI 1.03 0.98 lh2 that you find the movie humorous? lh5 SV 2.60 1.13 .24 [.15, .32] .92 [.89, .95] .83 [.79, .87] .73 [.69, .78] .52 [.45, .58] .83 .73 .61 .39 .83 [.78, .89] .70 [.63, .77] .59 [.52, .67] .57 [.50, .65] .43 [.34, .51] (table continues) .70 .49 .43 .38 .26 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 53 Table 2 (continued) No.a Wording cs1 that the movie is thought-provoking? cs2 that the movie is intellectually challenging? cs3 that the movie communicates values? that watching the film broadens your knowledge? that the movie motivates you to do cs5 something (e.g., politically, socially, search for information)? cs4 M SD SV SI CI FX RE 2.73 1.00 2.41 1.03 1.98 1.16 2.15 1.08 .20 [.11, .29] 1.65 1.10 IN LH CS .82 [.78, .87] .82 [.76, .87] .57 [.50, .64] .58 [.50, .65] The items were introduced with the phrase “When evaluating movies, how important for you personally is/are...” *p < .05 .69 .67 .43 .47 .56 .41 [.49, .63] Latent factor correlations SV SV .71 SI SI –.04 .76 CI CI –.01 .14* .82 FX FX –.02 .16* .25* .75 RE RE .19* .17* .06 .13* .72 IN IN .15* -.10* –.02 .06 .08 .86 LH LH .11* .17* .04 .35* .28* .34* .78 CS CS .36* .20* .17* –.02 .15* –.03 .08 .79 Note. ML-EFA = exploratory factor analysis with maximum likelihood extraction and Geomin-rotation, no row standardization; factor loadings < .20 are omitted; factor loadings > .40 are in bold typeface. 90% CI are reported in brackets. Raykov's ρ for each factor is presented in the diagonal in italics in the lower part of the table; latent factor correlations below the diagonal. SV = Story Verisimilitude, SI = Story Innovation, CI = Cinematography, FX = Special Effects, RE = Recommendation, IN = Innocuousness, LH = Light-heartedness, CS = Cognitive Stimulation. a h² MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA Table 3 Fit Indices for Model Comparison of EFA, E/CFA, and CFA Models of Eight SMEC Dimensions Model Analysis Extraction N (Sample) No. Items SB-χ²/df rRMSEA rRMSEA 90%CI SRMR rCFI a 1a EFA ML 506 (Study 1) 32 2.35 .052 .046, .057 – – b 1b E/CFA Robust ML 403 (R1) 33 1.39 .031 .024, .038 .020 .977 b 2 E/CFA Robust ML 403 (R1) 28 1.16 .020 .000, .031 .017 .993 2 CFA Robust ML 403 (R2) 28 1.92 .048 .042, .053 .061 .926 Note. SB-χ²/df = Satorra–Bentler χ²/df; rRMSEA = robust root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; rCFI = robust comparative fit index. a For Model 1a non-corrected fit indices are presented (i.e., χ²/df, RMSEA, and RMSEA 90% CI). Differences compared to Model 1a: Model 1b (Item si3 added), Model 2 (Item si3 added; Items ci3, fx4, re3, lh5, and cs2 removed). b 54 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA Table 4 Latent Structure of the SMEC Scales: Exploratory Factor Analysis within the Confirmatory Factor Analysis Framework (E/CFA; nR1 = 403) and Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA; nR2 = 403) SV SI CI FX RE IN LH CS Item E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA E/CFA CFA sv1 .72 .71 sv2 –.21 .57 .61 sv3 .65 .55 si1 .71 .70 si2 –.21 .20 .82 .66 si3 .15 .74 .70 ci1 .90 .88 ci2 .80 .89 ci4 .58 .62 fx1 .13 .62 .72 fx2 .23 .64 .77 fx3 .65 .76 re1 .71 .70 re2 .17 .23 –.17 .69 .75 re4 .20 .51 .59 re5 .54 .52 in1 .84 .82 in2 .59 .71 in3 .84 .83 in4 .20 .68 .81 lh1 .82 .82 lh2 .74 .81 lh3 –.23 .70 .52 lh4 .70 .70 cs1 –.25 .71 .65 cs3 .22 .80 .75 cs4 .78 .72 cs5 -.22 .88 .79 Note. Factor loadings with p > .05 are omitted; factor loadings > .40 are in bold. For abbreviations of the SMEC dimensions see text; for item wording see Table 2. 55 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 56 Table 5 Partial Correlations Between the SMEC Scales and External Criterion Constructs, Controlling for Gender and Age Validation Construct SV SI CI FX RE IN Emotional Stability –.02 .02 –.03 –.02 –.10 –.11 Extraversion .05 .01 .06 –.03 .09 .11 Openness to Experiences –.01 –.03 –.07 –.07 .17 .16 Agreeableness –.03 .05 .00 .06 .11 .12 Sensation seeking .02 .00 .02 .13 .13 –.12 Need for Cognition –.07 .04 .14 –.14 –.14 –.17 Film expertise .07 –.12 .21 .33 –.12 –.28 Film genre preferences Romance .02 –.01 –.04 .10 .14 .20 Comedy .02 .03 .02 .04 .19 .14 Action .01 .04 .05 –.12 .09 .37 Animation .08 .03 .01 –.18 .13 .22 Thriller –.02 .07 .06 .11 .11 –.20 Science-Fiction/Fantasy –.03 –.05 –.25 .12 .14 .17 Horror –.08 .07 .05 –.04 .08 –.35 Drama/Tragedy .04 .11 .13 –.20 .13 –.23 Avant-garde –.04 .00 .19 .23 –.29 –.19 Documentary .06 –.01 .03 –.10 –.09 –.17 Film specific evaluationsa Fun .00 .01 .11 .01 .04 .17 Appreciation .01 .04 .04 .07 .20 .12 Note. N = 587. Significant correlations (p < .05, two-sided) are in bold. a Film specific evaluations were measured at t2 (N = 273). LH .01 .02 –.10 .16 –.08 –.16 –.41 CS .00 .13 .18 .03 .13 .15 .06 .19 .41 .28 .11 –.02 .05 –.09 –.37 –.43 –.17 –.05 –.07 –.15 –.03 –.01 –.10 –.10 .20 .16 .15 .10 -.02 –.02 .23 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 57 MEASURING SUBJECTIVE MOVIE EVALUATION CRITERIA 58 Figure 1. LST coefficients reliability (Rel), common consistency (cCon), occasion specificity (Spe), and method specificity (mSpe) for SMEC dimensions on two points of measurement (N = 275; aggregation across test-halves within occasions; for formulas see Steyer & Schmitt, 1990). The LST models were specified as follows: Error variances were freely estimated for SV, SI, and FX, constrained on each occasion for CI and LH and constrained on all occasions for RE, IN, and CS. Both method factors were freely estimated for SV and SI and constrained for RE and IN. One method factor was fixed to zero due to lack of significance for CS (test half 1) and for CI, FX, and LH (test half 2). All model fit indices indicated good fit (i.e., all ps > .05, all RMSEAs < .06 and all 90% CIs included 0; all SRMRs < .06, and all CFIs > .99). View publication stats