Psychotherapy Research ISSN: 1050-3307 (Print) 1468-4381 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tpsr20 “It is never okay to talk about suicide”: Patients’ reasons for concealing suicidal ideation in psychotherapy Matt Blanchard & Barry A. Farber To cite this article: Matt Blanchard & Barry A. Farber (2018): “It is never okay to talk about suicide”: Patients’ reasons for concealing suicidal ideation in psychotherapy, Psychotherapy Research, DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1543977 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2018.1543977 Published online: 08 Nov 2018. Submit your article to this journal View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tpsr20 Psychotherapy Research, 2018 https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2018.1543977 EMPIRICAL PAPER “It is never okay to talk about suicide”: Patients’ reasons for concealing suicidal ideation in psychotherapy MATT BLANCHARD∗ & BARRY A. FARBER Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA (Received 22 April 2018; revised 16 September 2018; accepted 22 October 2018) Abstract Objective: To identify psychotherapy clients’ motives for concealing suicidal ideation from their therapist, and their perceptions of how their therapists could better elicit honest disclosure. Method: A sample of 66 psychotherapy clients who reported concealing suicidal ideation from their therapist provided short essay responses explaining their motives for doing so and what their therapist could do to help them be more honest. Content analysis was used to identify major motives and themes in these responses. Results: Seventy percent of suicidal ideation concealers cited fear of unwanted practical impacts outside therapy as the reason they did not disclose. Chief among these unwanted impacts was involuntary hospitalization, a perceived outcome of disclosing even mild suicidal thoughts. Less concrete motives for concealment, such as shame or embarrassment, were significant but secondary concerns. Nearly half of suicide-concealing clients said they would be more honest only if the threat of hospitalization was somehow reduced or controlled. Conclusion: Fostering disclosure of suicidal ideation in therapy may require renewed attention to providing transparent, comprehensive, and easy-to-understand psychoeducation about the triggers for hospitalization and other interventions. Clients make risk-benefit calculations about whether to disclose suicidal ideation, but may operate with exaggerated or inaccurate ideas about the consequences of disclosure. Keywords: Suicide; psychotherapy; disclosure; lying Clinical or methodological significance of this article: This study’s online and anonymous design allowed for SI concealers to describe in their own words the most important factors motivating their concealment. In this way, the study serves as a type of “customer feedback”, offering a constructive critique of suicide risk assessment as it is presently practiced in psychotherapy, from the point of view of patients who conceal their suicidal thoughts. The findings suggest concealment is most often driven by practical concerns such as a fear of being hospitalized. This, in turn, suggests renewed attention to aspects of informed consent that do not get much attention in the research. Clients who believe—often incorrectly—that they will be involuntarily hospitalized or experience other unwanted interventions if they disclose suicidal thoughts may feel strongly motivated to conceal no matter what method of assessment is used, and no matter how skillful the clinician is at normalizing or reducing client shame around being suicidal. For this reason, achieving the goal of honest disclosure may require finding ways for therapists to psycho-educate about the basic rules of confidentiality, reporting, and hospitalization, in such a way that clients feel some measure of control or predictability. In the majority of cases, doing so will likely alleviate unrealistic fears held by the client, and potentially remove or at least mitigate this major barrier to disclosure. This study adds to recent findings on suicide concealment by Hom, Stanley, Podlogar, and Joiner (2017) and Ganzini et al. (2013), and helps to answer a call made by Hom, Stanley, and Joiner (2015) for more research with middle-aged and older adults in a field that is dominated by studies using college undergraduates. Although honest disclosure is central to the process of psychotherapy (e.g., Farber, 2006; Stiles, 1995), it is well established that clients are not always honest and forthcoming. Just like people in all types of settings, ∗ clients in psychotherapy keep secrets (Baumann & Hill, 2016; Kelly, 1998) and lie (Farber, Blanchard, & Love, in press; Martin, 2006); they also minimize discussion of personally salient topics (Farber & Matt Blanchard is now at New York University. Matt Blanchard, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA. Email: mpb2160@tc.columbia.edu; matt.blanchard@nyu. edu © 2018 Society for Psychotherapy Research 2 M. Blanchard and B. A. Farber Sohn, 2007), hide their negative reactions to therapist interventions (Hill, Thompson, Cogar, & Denman, 1993), and sometime mislead therapists about the effectiveness of therapy itself (Blanchard & Farber, 2016). The question of honest disclosure is especially fraught on the subject of suicide. As Jobes and Ballard (2011) point out, the life-and-death nature of suicidality injects fundamental issues of power and control into psychotherapy, stirring up strong emotions for all involved: “the therapeutic stakes are raised to the highest possible level … both parties (clinician and patient) may feel vulnerable, powerful, scared, angry, worried, wary – and sometimes all at the same time” (p. 51). There is evidence that many clients avoid these issues by hiding or denying suicidal ideation (SI). In a sample of 547 adults in individual psychotherapy, Blanchard and Farber (2016) found that 31% recalled having lied to their therapist about suicidal thoughts at some time in the past. Similarly, a study of 355 students seeking therapy at a college counseling center found that 13.8% denied SI when asked on an intake questionnaire but later admitted it when given a full suicide assessment by a counselor (Morrison & Downey, 2000). How many more of these “hidden ideators” (we will use the term “SI concealers” for clarity) continued to conceal during the face-to-face assessment is impossible to know. Neither study was able to assess the true prevalence of SI in their samples. These findings are in keeping with a body of research that suggests concealment of suicidal thoughts is common outside of therapy as well. In a sample of 26,000 college undergraduate and graduate students, 1321 responded in the affirmative when asked if they had “seriously considered attempting suicide in the past 12 months”, and 46% of this subsample decided to tell no one about these thoughts (Drum, Brownson, Burton Denmark, & Smith, 2009). It is very difficult to predict who in the general population will attempt suicide, in part because it is a low base-rate phenomenon (Murphy, 1984). Even among those who experience SI, the majority will never go on to form a suicide plan nor make a suicide attempt (CDC, 2015; Klonsky & May, 2014; Nock et al., 2008). While numerous risk factors have been identified (e.g., previous attempts), these do not translate into an ability to predict suicide-related behaviors. A recent meta-analysis of the last 50 years of research on risk factors suggests there is no category of predictors which functions much better than chance at predicting suicide-related outcomes, generating calls for a more complex algorithms that can account for combinations of perhaps 50 different risk factors at once (Franklin et al., 2017). In the absence of better tools, most clinical settings rely on self-report questionnaires (e.g., PHQ-9) and, even more so, the clinical interview. This involves asking patients directly about the frequency and intensity of suicidal thoughts, planning or preparations, desire to die and intent to attempt suicide, as well as baseline and acute risk factors, protective factors, and any warning signs of suicide (e.g., Jobes, 2006; Joiner, Van Orden, Witte, & Rudd, 2009; Maltsberger, 1986; Mays, 2004; Shea, 1999; Welton, 2007). Clients who are strongly motivated to conceal these types of information can usually stymie even the most skillful interviewer without great difficulty. Indeed, we respect the autonomy of all people to disclose only what they wish when speaking with mental health professionals. Honest disclosure of SI has benefits, however. For those who may go on to attempt suicide, disclosing SI allows their therapist to conduct a fuller assessment of suicide risk, monitor any intensification of suicidality, provide appropriate psychological treatments, and intervene in a potentially lifesaving manner. For those who will never attempt, fostering honest disclosure would at the very least mean relief from the burden of concealment and a greater opportunity to benefit from their mental health treatment. Understanding clients’ motivations for concealment can help clinicians address unspoken concerns about disclosure that are acting as roadblocks. Clearing these impediments, in turn, may increase the likelihood that clients will honestly disclose suicidal ideation when it is experienced. In theory, identifying these roadblocks to disclosure requires only that researchers ask patients who are concealing suicidal ideation to voice their concerns. Nevertheless, it is rare to find studies in which SI concealers are invited to explain their reasons for concealment or provide feedback to the mental health professions who are trying to assess them. One such study, Ganzini and colleagues’ (2013) research on brief suicide screening in US Department of Veterans Affairs medical centers, involved interviewing 34 Iraq and Afghanistan War veterans about their decision to conceal or reveal suicidal thoughts. These veterans felt that shame and fear of career repercussions were valid reasons not to mention suicidal thoughts. They also noted concerns about unwanted hospitalization or medication recommendations. Veterans stressed trust and respect as the keys to fostering honesty about a topic they regarded as intimate and shameful. As one veteran put it: “Those who are nice to me and treat me with respect … will get all the information they need from me” (p. 1218). The authors concluded that professionals who put aside the standardized screener to establish interpersonal trust with Psychotherapy Research 3 the veterans were deemed to be safe for disclosure. These gestures may have helped foster a stronger therapeutic alliance, which has been found to be positively correlated with greater overall disclosure (Farber & Hall, 2002). Hom et al. (2017) looked at why undergraduates decide to respond accurately or not when asked about suicidal thoughts by various parties, including family members, friends, teachers, religious leaders, and mental health providers. Their sample included 306 undergraduate students who reported some lifetime history of suicidal ideation. Of this group, 77 had been asked about suicide by a psychologist, therapist, or counselor, and 49 had been asked by a psychiatrist, although it was not clear whether they were in ongoing psychotherapy. Student motives for not giving accurate information differed slightly between psychiatrists and other mental health providers. Fear of hospitalization was most commonly reported among those probed by psychiatrists, while embarrassment was the most common reason for concealment from psychologists or other nonmedical therapists. Other common motives for concealment included being judged, others finding out, and not being taken seriously. When these undergraduates did give accurate accounts of their suicidal thoughts to mental health professionals, it was most commonly because they wanted emotional support or desired help in getting treatment or medications. These studies suggest there are many possible reasons for concealment of suicidal ideation, some of which are universal and some of which vary across the contexts in which patients are assessed (e.g., military personnel conceal out of worry about career implications). Expectations regarding confidentiality may also affect individuals’ tendencies to conceal suicidal thoughts. In this regard, psychotherapy clients are likely to have higher expectations of privacy and confidentiality than, for example, undergraduates being briefly assessed in a hospital emergency room or veterans answering standardized screening questions during a medical check-up. What remains unstudied, though, is how these and other considerations influence the extent to which and reasons for which psychotherapy clients conceal their suicidal ideation from their therapists. More generally, the literature lacks research on suicide concealment among middle-aged and older adults. In their comprehensive review of the factors affecting help-seeking among suicidal individuals, Hom et al. (2015) note that the majority of studies in this area focus on adolescents and young adults, whose involvement with institutions such as colleges make them relatively easy to study. While suicide is a leading cause of death among young people in the United States, middle-aged adults accounted for the largest proportion of suicides (56%) in 2011, and from 1999 to 2010, the suicide rate among this group increased by nearly 30% (CDC, 2015). Hom et al. call for research to understand the barriers and facilitators to care unique to adults, writing “it is essential for this research gap to be addressed” (p. 35). Ultimately, the goal of understanding client motives for suicide concealment is to improve clinical treatment. Only one previous study (Ganzini et al., 2013) asked SI concealers to provide suggestions about what therapists could do to help them be more honest. Thus, in order to learn how psychotherapists can better elicit honest disclosure of suicide-related material, the current study directly queried SI concealers on their motives for concealment and their ideas about what could facilitate their honesty in the context of their psychotherapy. Specifically, the study’s two research questions were (a) what most commonly motivates clients experiencing suicidal ideation to conceal this from their psychotherapist? and (b) what do suicidal ideation concealers believe their therapist could do to help them be more honest? Method Participants Of a total of 798 respondents completing an online survey about dishonesty in psychotherapy, 171 reported dishonesty specifically about suicidal ideation. Among these, 66 elected to answer a series of in-depth questions about concealment. These 66 constitute the “SI Concealer” sample for this study, defined as those who avoided disclosure of suicidal thoughts by either actively lying or passively omitting information on this topic. As Table I indicates, the average age among these self-acknowledged SI concealers was 31, ranging in age from 18 to 71. This sample consisted primarily of women (79%), with most respondents reporting their ethnicity as White (77%) or African-American (11%). The majority of this sample (53%) reported having a Bachelors or more advanced degree. Among the 66 SI concealers, 39 were currently in therapy and the remaining 27 had been in therapy within the previous 12 months. The median number of therapy sessions with their current or most recent therapist was 35; the mean duration of treatment with this therapist was 22.7 months (SD = 30.3), with durations ranging from 2 months to 15 years. The most commonly reported reasons these clients entered therapy were depression (58%), stress (32%), and anxiety (30%), with 11% specifically mentioning suicidal ideation. The most commonly reported therapeutic modalities were 4 M. Blanchard and B. A. Farber Table I. Participant demographics, SI concealer sample (N = 66). Age Gender Female Male Other Race/Ethnicity African-American Asian-American Hispanic/Latino/a White/Caucasian Biracial Other Education Level Grade School High School or GED Some college Associate’s Degree Bachelor’s Degree Master’s Degree PhD/MD/JD n/mean %/SD 31.1 years 13.04 52 12 2 79% 18% 3% 7 – 4 51 4 – 11% – 6% 77% 6% – 1 13 14 3 22 13 – 2% 20% 21% 5% 33% 20% – CBT (38%), psychodynamic/psychoanalytic (21%), and integrative/eclectic (11%), while a number also reported “other” (17%). Measures The original Difficult Disclosures Survey (Blanchard & Farber, 2016) is an online, self-report instrument, consisting of a total of 107 items developed with the Qualtrics survey software, and designed to elicit information from psychotherapy clients about the topics they tend to be dishonest about in therapy, their reasons for doing so, the perceived clinical consequences of their dishonesty, and their sense of what their therapist could do to facilitate greater honesty. In order to help respondents access memories of dishonesty in their therapy, a list of 33 common disclosure topics is presented, among them suicidal thoughts; these items were chosen after reviewing prior research on what clients discuss in therapy. Respondents are asked to rate their degree of honesty for each topic they have discussed with their current or most recent therapist on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “not at all honest”, 5 = “completely honest”). For topics they indicated they have not discussed, respondents are asked to provide one of three reasons: “It does not apply to me”, “I would discuss this but it has not come up,” and “I purposely avoid this topic”. Respondents who either indicate low levels of honesty (“1” or “2” on the Likert scale) or acknowledge purposeful avoidance about a specific topic (e.g., suicidal ideation) are given the opportunity to answer a detailed set of follow-up questions. These respondents first encounter open-text essay questions, including “What makes it hard to be honest about this?” and “How could your therapist make you feel more comfortable being honest about this?” Later, respondents encounter multiple-choice questions about motives for dishonesty and potential facilitators for honesty, in order to provide corroboration for earlier essay responses. Procedure Participants were recruited through postings to Craigslist sites serving 13 large metropolitan areas of the United States, as well as 18 more rural areas. The posting message invited them to participate in a “survey on psychotherapy”, and contained a link to the survey. Data were collected between June 2015 and March 2016. The survey was administered online in order to collect the largest and most geographically diverse sample possible within resource constraints, and to offer a private, anonymous format in which respondents would feel most comfortable disclosing what they might not say in a face-toface interview. All respondents were entered into a drawing to win one of six $50 Amazon gift cards. Completers (N = 798) and dropouts (N = 247) showed no statistically significant differences on any demographic measure. Data Analysis Content analysis was used to identify and thematically categorize reasons (i.e., motives) for participants’ avoidance and dishonesty regarding suicidal ideation in psychotherapy. Content analysis is the systematic, objective, quantitative analysis of message characteristics (Neuendorf, 2002). The idea of a “theme” in the qualitative analysis has been variously defined, but in essence, it is “a phrase or sentence that identifies what a unit of data is about and/or what it means” (Saldaña, 2009). For the first research question (“What makes it hard to be honest about this?”), the unit of analysis consisted of phrases and/or sentences that described or conveyed a motive for dishonesty. The senior author examined written responses of all 66 respondents and identified 284 separate statements that appeared to describe potential motives for dishonesty. In order to develop the themes, a team consisting of the senior author and a doctoral student familiar with the research on disclosure in therapy created the first draft of a codebook, which was then reviewed by the second author, a researcher who has published extensively in the area of disclosure and non-disclosure in psychotherapy. Coding was done by a team of three Psychotherapy Research 5 graduate students in clinical psychology. As described by Neuendorf (2002), coder training was an iterative process in which coders were trained on the codebook, attempted to code sample data, and offered feedback and revisions to the codebook itself. The final codebook contained 21 codes. Coding took place in ten rounds, each coding roughly 10% of the data, with an average Krippendorf’s alpha of .80, meeting the generally-accepted standard for intercoder reliability. Disagreements were resolved through whole-team discussion and consensus. Finally, the team agreed upon four higher-order “code groups” to which each of the 21 codes logically belongs. These higher-order code groups were meant to express overarching domains of motives for lying. Code groups included reasons related to practical impacts, reasons related to emotional impacts, reasons specific to therapy or the therapist, and reasons relating to the client’s beliefs about self or suicide. Each is described in greater detail below. Content analysis for the second research question (“How could your therapist help you be more honest?”) was conducted by precisely the same process, with a separate team of three clinical psychology graduate students. The data set for question two consisted of 85 message-units produced by 66 respondents. As before, a preliminary codebook was produced with 10 thematic coding categories. During coder training, this was reduced to 8 final codes. Intercoder reliability on the coded data set was .87, above the accepted standard. As before, the codes for message units on which raters had disagreed were resolved by consensus. Results The percentage of the overall sample (N = 798) who reported dishonesty or avoidance about suicidal thoughts was 21.4%. This number consists of the 10.1% who reported speaking dishonestly and 11.3% who reported deliberately avoiding the topic. Suicidal thoughts was the third most commonly reported topic of dishonesty or avoidance among 33 topics offered to respondents (after two sex-related topics). The remaining participants in the overall sample fell into three categories: 46%who reported speaking about suicide in therapy with “moderate” or greater honesty (i.e., had scores of 3 or more on a 5-point Likert scale), 26% who felt the topic of suicidal thoughts “does not apply to me”, and 7% who reported that “I would discuss this, but it hasn’t come up.” This study did not capture how many of these respondents were experiencing SI, only their degree of honesty when it came up in psychotherapy. Respondents in the qualitative SI concealer sample (N = 66) are those who reported dishonesty and elected to answer in-depth questions about this topic. Forty respondents reported being either “not at all honest” (15 respondents) or only “a little honest” (25 respondents) when speaking about suicidal thoughts in therapy, while 26 respondents reported never having spoken of it due to deliberate avoidance of the topic. Three topics of dishonesty were notably more common among the 66 SI concealers than in the sample as a whole (N = 798). These were “past suicide attempts” (reported by 30% of SI concealers), “self-harm” (24%), and “whether therapy is helping me” (24%). Overall, there was no significant difference in the number of topics respondents reported speaking dishonestly about or deliberately avoiding between the SI concealer group (M = 5.4, SD = 4.9) and non-SI concealers (M = 4.4, SD = 4.3); t(79) = 1.57, p = .06. This suggests SI concealers were not more dishonest in general. Motives for Concealment Open-ended responses to the question. “What makes it hard to be honest about this?” ranged in length from four to 271 words, and contained between 1 and 18 separate message units, with the median number of message units being four. Once coded, 32% of the 66 SI concealers were found to have provided one motive for dishonesty, 26% provided two motives, 23% provided three motives, and the remaining 20% provided four or five separate motives for concealment, with one respondent offering eight separate motives. The following sections describe each of the four code groups: unwanted practical impacts; emotional experiences; beliefs about self or about suicide; and reasons specific to therapy. Table II shows the distribution of reported motives across these four code groups. Sample quotations drawn from text entered by respondents are also provided for each group. Unwanted practical impacts. A majority of SI concealers (70%) reported one of seven motives for dishonesty relating to the practical, real-world consequences of disclosing suicidal ideation. This group of themes includes “hospitalization”, “unwanted medication”, “others finding out”, “impacts on career or schooling”, “impacts on others” (such as children), and in a few cases, “loss of autonomy to attempt suicide”; as well as a category of “other or unspecified practical impacts”. All codes in this group reflect the fear that honest disclosure will cause their therapist to break confidentiality or make interventions seen as leading to practical consequences for their lives 6 M. Blanchard and B. A. Farber Table II. Code groups, or general categories of motivation, for concealing suicidal thoughts in psychotherapy (N = 66). Categories Concealment to avoid certain unwanted practical impacts (e.g., hospitalization, medication, career impacts, etc.) Number reporting Percent of total sample 46 70% 30 45% 26 39% 26 39% “I would say I wasn’t suicidal, even though I was, just to make sure I wasn’t hospitalized.” “Involuntary commitment is far more traumatic than just dealing with such feelings on my own.” “It would out me to my friends, family, and coworkers.” “I do not want to ever be sedated” “I am afraid to open up and lose everything I have.” Concealment to avoid certain emotional experiences (e.g., shame, guilt, or to continue denial of the problem, etc.) “I feel embarrassed that I want to attempt suicide.” “I know it’s supposed to protect me from myself, but it adds more shame and self-loathing that exasperates everything.” “It’s hard to be honest because it means admitting that the trauma of the sexual assault had such an impact on me.” Concealment due to beliefs about self or about suicide (e.g., belief that one is a low risk, that no one can help, or a preference to cope alone) “Since I wouldn’t really do it, I don’t want to talk about it.” “There are only a few moments a year when I feel suicidal.” “I feel I am fine coping on my own.” “Discussing it with people has never had any positive effect on my feelings.” Concealment for reasons specific to therapy or therapist (e.g., to control agenda of therapy, because of something therapist has done, etc.) “I think the topics we discuss would change greatly.” “Since it is my money and time, I want to choose what topics we will talk about … suicidal ideation is not one of them.” “It seemed he was concerned about his liability more than how I felt.” Note: Example quotations taken from survey responses, represent different codes within the code group. outside of therapy. Predicting that their disclosure would set off a chain of events beyond their control, they determined that sharing with their therapist was not worth the risk. Notably, 52% of these 66 respondents specifically mentioned a fear of being involuntarily admitted to a hospital psychiatric unit. This was by far the most commonly-endorsed motive for concealing suicidal ideation from a therapist. How did they come to have this fear? Several SI concealers described past experiences with hospitalization, and appeared to have knowledge of the reporting requirements for clinicians. One client remarked that despite some familiarity with the rules around hospitalization, she was made anxious by the uncertainty around how those rules will be applied: I am concerned they will force me into a hospital. I am less concerned about this than I first was as I learned that it [suicide] can be discussed to some degree if they realize you are not impulsive and at immediate risk. But still, I don’t know how much people know that, or what their view of what ‘immediate risk’ means. A week, a month, a year? Others believed that by merely mentioning suicidal thoughts they could be subject to forcible hospitalization. One respondent noted that she never came close to attempting suicide but was “afraid my therapist would commit me so I lied and said I didn’t have thoughts about suicide”. Another client wrote: “Talking about suicide … leads to actions that have to be taken. I feared having to go to a psychiatric hospital.” Some respondents appeared to believe that any discussion of suicidal thoughts is risky. They, therefore, concealed it from everyone in their lives. As one respondent noted: “I was scared of telling my therapist, or anyone, the truth. I was scared that would get me placed in some sort of intensive in-patient Psychotherapy Research 7 therapy.” Or as another respondent succinctly explained: “It is never okay to talk about suicide.” Hospitalization was often seen as the proximal impact that would lead to other impacts, such as “others finding out” cited by 11%, “unwanted medication or medication changes” (9%), “loss of autonomy to commit suicide” (6%), “harming or upsetting loved ones” (5%), and “impacts on career or schooling” (3%), as well as an unspecified category (e.g., “lose everything I have”) reported by 12%. Thus, hospitalization occupied a special place in the logic of the SI concealers in this sample. It was the primary feared outcome from which other feared outcomes were thought to flow. Feared emotional impacts. The second-most common group of motives for concealment of suicidal ideation in psychotherapy was the desire to avoid certain emotional experiences, reported by 45% of the 66 respondents providing qualitative data. This code group includes five codes, including “shame, stigma or embarrassment”, “avoidance or denial”, “guilt”, “sadness”, an “other or unspecified” category. Among these, the related elements of shame, embarrassment, or a fear of being judged constituted the most common reason for concealment, reported by 30%. While some authors distinguish between shame and embarrassment (e.g., Burton Denmark et al., 2012), our coding team found it was not possible to reliably distinguish “shame” from “embarrassment” or “stigma” or “judgment” in the responses analyzed here. Indeed, many respondents mentioned this theme in the briefest possible way, writing only “It’s embarrassing” or “the shame of it,” a brevity that itself may be motivated by embarrassment. Respondents who elaborated often acknowledged feeling embarrassed by the simple fact that they want to die by suicide. As unwell as they felt, they believed sharing the depth of their misery would only further compromise their dignity. As one client remarked: “It upsets me because I have no control over these thoughts or attempts, so I feel embarrassed that I want to attempt suicide.” Avoidance or denial was cited by 12% of respondents who said concealment in therapy was part of a larger effort to keep suicidal thoughts out of awareness. They felt talking about suicide would force them to realize how bad their condition is, to take action to address the problem, or in one case, to maybe even carry out a suicide attempt. Other, less common emotional experiences included unspecified emotional impacts (e,g., “It makes me emotional”) reported by 11%, and guilt and sadness, mentioned by 3% of the sample. Beliefs about self or suicide. A third code group for concealment, cited by 39% of SI concealers, consists of five motives stemming from some belief the respondent holds about themselves or about suicide. Codes in this category included “low risk”, “preference to cope alone”, “hard to speak about suicide”, “disclosure would not help”, and “suicide is morally wrong”. The most common among these beliefs was the perception that one is at low risk for actually attempting suicide, reported by 27% of the sample. These respondents felt their suicidal ideation was neither intense nor frequent enough to warrant addressing in therapy. Notably, the sense that they were “low risk” did not necessarily alleviate fears of serious consequences should they disclose. Indeed, 67% of those mentioning low risk also mentioned fear of unwanted practical impacts such as hospitalization and others finding out. For example, a female client reported “short-lived suicidal thoughts that I would never act on” happening only “a few moments in a year.” Nonetheless, she hid these experiences from her therapist believing that “I would be committed to a psych ward and my family would know.” Most respondents who felt they were not at risk for suicide nonetheless worried that clinicians would intervene. Smaller numbers of respondents cited other reasons such as that it is better to cope privately (8%), that suicide is hard to talk about (6%), that disclosure will not be helpful (5%), and that suicide should not be discussed because it is morally wrong (2%). Reasons specific to therapy or therapist. The fourth and final group of motives for concealment consists of four codes relating to events inside therapy, as distinct from the practical impacts on the respondent’s life outside therapy discussed above. These codes include “to control the agenda of therapy”, “therapist is blamed for concealment”, “fear that therapist will feel bad at their job”, and an “other or unspecified therapy-related reason”. Taken together, 29% of SI concealers endorsed one of these motives. The most common motive in this category was a desire to control the agenda of what is discussed (20%), essentially a concern that acknowledging suicidal thoughts would distract the therapist from the “real reason” the respondent entered therapy. These respondents believed their therapist’s focus on suicide would prevent work on problems such as anxiety or relationships. As one respondent noted: “Since I only have 45 minutes a week I unfortunately have to be very selective when it comes to the topics I discuss.” It was also common to blame therapists for discouraging disclosure (17%), either because they failed to ask, did not seem to care enough, or seemed unsympathetic. Those noting this motive 8 M. Blanchard and B. A. Farber implied that they would have disclosed their suicidal ideation had it not been for something about their therapist. By contrast, one respondent reported concealing in order to “save” the therapist from feeling like she had failed to help. Finally, an unspecified category was included for responses in which some event in therapy was mentioned as a reason for suicide concealment, but its precise nature was either vaguely worded or not explained (9%). In addition to the open-text item querying respondent’s motives for concealment, the survey later asked respondents to complete a multiple-choice item assessing their motivation for non-disclosure of suicidality: “Which of these describes your reason for not being honest?” Both methods of inquiry show that practical consequences such as hospitalization were the most common motive for concealment of suicidal ideation; indeed, the same percentage of respondents (70%) endorsed this motive in both question formats. Similarly, while 20% reported a desire to control the agenda of therapy in the open-text, 24% endorsed a similar question choice, “I don’t want this to distract from other topics”, in the multiple-choice format. By contrast, SI concealers were more likely to endorse embarrassment or shame as a motive in the multiple-choice format (where 58% did so) than in the open-text format (where only 30% did so). Fostering Greater Honesty Essay responses to the question “How could your therapist make you feel more comfortable being honest about your suicidal thoughts?” ranged in length from two to 95 words, and contained between one and four separate message units, with the median number of message units being one. Once coded, 45 SI concealers (70%) were found to have provided one idea for how their therapist could help them be more honest about suicidal thoughts, 11 (17%) provided two ideas, and 10 (15%) provided no ideas, saying they did not know. Across 66 respondents, 84 message units were coded into one of eight thematic categories, with the number of respondents endorsing each provided in Table III. One theme, “provide assurances about reporting my suicidal ideation,” was found to have three clear subthemes, described below. The structure of the data for research question 2 did not require the creation of higher-order code groups. Ideas about how clinicians could foster honesty were heavily weighted toward practical fears. Close to half of SI Concealers (48%) reported that they would feel more comfortable being honest about suicidal thoughts if they received some form of assurance, explanation, or control over whether the therapist would report their disclosure to others, as well as assurances about the subsequent consequences of that reporting. Many mentioned hospitalization directly, while others mentioned downstream impacts such as being taken out of school or work. Three sub-themes were identified among those seeking assurances about reporting. The most common was the belief that concealment would no longer be necessary if the therapist could simply “promise not to report” the respondent’s suicidal ideation. As one respondent wrote: “If he reassured me that it would stay between us.” Several acknowledged that this would probably be impossible. A second sub-theme involved asking that clinicians “explain the triggers for reporting” so that patients can know precisely where the line is and decide for themselves whether to disclose suicidal thoughts. As one respondent put it, “They could explain upon asking about the topic that only very serious thoughts or active attempts would be considered grounds for involuntary hospitalization.” These respondents were seeking transparency from their clinician in order to reduce uncertainty about the likely response to different levels of suicidal ideation. Before they disclose, they would want to understand the law, and also how their therapist interprets that law. One respondent asked that his therapist prove that she understands “the difference between ideations and actual intentions”. Another wrote: “Inform me what the protocol is when I am having these feelings, before it occurs, so I can decide how comfortable I am sharing.” The third sub-theme present in this category was a desire by clients to be included in the decision of whether to report. Such a solution would have therapists share decision-making power with clients about the best response when suicide is being discussed. As one respondent wrote, therapists could engender honesty by “allowing me to decide if I needed to be hospitalized”. Another respondent went so far as to propose that her therapist “contract” with her not to over-react to her suicide-related disclosures. These respondents suggested that they would be more honest about their suicidal ideation if they could gain some control, or at least participate in a thorough discussion of next steps or options. As one respondent wrote: “Promise to listen to everything I say and take into consideration my emotional state at this time, and his/her opinion about my overall emotional state. Then see admitting to a hospital as a LAST resort.” Other ideas about fostering honesty provided by respondents included asking direct questions about suicide, normalizing and validating their experience, along with unspecified changes in clinical technique. A few imagined that if they had more time in therapy, or a more trusting relationship with the therapist, Psychotherapy Research 9 Table III. Themes in open-text responses to “How could your therapist make you feel more comfortable being honest about your suicidal thoughts?” (N = 66). Themes Provide assurances about reporting my suicidal ideation Individuals reporting Percent of sample 32 48% 13 20% 10 15% 6 9% 5 8% 1 2% 1 2% 10 15% “If hospitalization wasn’t a consequence of talking about suicide” “Saying they won’t take me out of school” “Explain what would happen if I talked about my suicidal thoughts” “Allow me to decide if I needed to be hospitalized” “Offering to work with me through my suicidal thoughts without inpatient care.” There is nothing my therapist can do “I honestly would not share it with a therapist. As much help that they may be giving me, I trust my friends and family a whole lot more.” Ask me direct questions about my suicidal thoughts “If my therapist asked me frankly about it, I think that could make me finally open up about it.” “If she asked about specific time frames for example, I would probably tell her directly.” Normalize my suicidal thoughts or validate my experience “If he assured me very clearly … that I am normal for having these feelings, and that they can co-exist with healthier feelings, then I might discuss them.” If my therapist and I had a closer, more trusting relationship “I would have to be more comfortable with them.” “I guess maybe just over time as I observe if she understands things I say and my general thoughts more.” If I had more time in therapy “Considering the costs involved … Maybe if it wasn’t so expensive and I had more time to work with.” Unspecified change of technique “If his technique/approach was different.” Don’t know/no response “I honestly don’t know.” Note. Example quotations taken from survey responses. Sample percentages refer to proportion of 66 SI concealers who reported each theme. then disclosure might become possible. Finally, a significant subset expressed very little hope of ever being honest, either having no idea what could change (15%) or believing there was really nothing their therapist could do (20%). Respondents in these categories cited a wide range of motives for concealment (shame, hospitalization fear, sense of being low risk, desire to deny the problem, etc.) with no clear pattern emerging. After providing the open-text responses discussed above, respondents provided multiple-choice responses to the question “Under what circumstances would you be more honest about this topic?”. Notably, the most commonly-endorsed item choice, “If I knew my therapist would not over-react” (selected by 52%), appears to corroborate the open-text finding that 48% sought assurances about how their therapist would react to their disclosure. Other commonly-selected choices included “If my therapist asked me about it directly” (38%), “If I felt like this was blocking my progress in therapy” (29%), and “If I trusted my therapist more” (26%). 10 M. Blanchard and B. A. Farber Discussion Since suicide assessment relies almost entirely on the client’s willingness to disclose, understanding why clients make the decision to conceal is key to improving such assessment. Finding ways to foster greater honesty from suicidal ideation concealers is likely to improve the quality and usefulness of psychotherapy for clients who experience suicidal ideation. This study directly queried concealers to learn how psychotherapists can better elicit honest disclosure of suicidal material. The findings illuminate how clashing desires for certainty and control by both therapist and patient can result in patients resisting efforts to accurately report their suicide risk. Practical Concerns Often Motivate Concealment The most common motivation for concealing suicidal ideation in psychotherapy is a fear of practical, realworld consequences of disclosure. Most often, this fear focuses on the possibility of hospitalization, of being involuntarily admitted to an inpatient psychiatric unit. Expressions of this fear were not limited to clients experiencing severe or imminent suicidality; a substantial proportion of those saying they were a low risk for actually making an attempt reported a fear of being hospitalized. These findings regarding fears of hospitalization as the primary factor in client non-disclosure of suicidal ideation are consistent with the results of Ganzini et al. (2013) in their study of veterans and of Hom et al. (2017) in their study of college students. Consistent too with observations made by Hom et al. (2015), many clients in the current sample appeared to lack clarity about the triggers for hospitalization. Without a clear sense for where the line was, they described staying well back of the border by concealing even mild suicidal symptoms. A related finding is that a large percentage of SIconcealing clients said they would be more honest if the threat of hospitalization was somehow reduced or controlled. These respondents wanted some form of assurance, explanation, or certainty about the chances of being hospitalized as a result of disclosure. Some wanted a frank promise not to report the ideation. Others wanted to be educated about the triggers for hospitalization so as to control their disclosure strategically. And still others wanted to be included in a collaborative decision about hospitalization and other treatment interventions. These requests resonate with recent models of suicidality that strongly differentiate between suicidal ideation and suicidal action, in which the two are seen as “separate processes that come with separate sets of explanations and risk factors” (Klonsky & May, 2015; see also O’Connor, 2011). Some respondents worried their clinicians would not recognize this separateness, treating all ideation as prelude to action. Taken together, these findings strongly suggest that if clients with suicidal thoughts are calculating their chances of triggering unwanted interventions, it is imperative for clinicians to take seriously their fears about hospitalization and their uncertainty about the limits of confidentiality. Surprisingly, though, this dynamic is rarely or only cursorily addressed in the literature. Many texts about managing suicidal risk (e.g., Jobes, 2006; Maltsberger, 1986) start from the point in time after a patient has been identified as a suicide risk, for example, by showing up in an emergency room after a suicide attempt. Texts that address the earlier challenge of how to encourage new disclosures rarely mention the practical concerns raised by clients in this study. For example, Shea’s (1999) Practical Art of Suicide Assessment contains three insightful chapters about uncovering suicidal ideation, yet includes only one brief mention that clients may have fears about being “locked up” for revealing suicidal ideation to a clinician (p. 112). Hospitalization fear may be considered too obvious to mention and yet the results of this study and recent others suggest that it is decisive for many clients. Shame as a Motive for Concealment Strategies for interviewing potentially-suicidal clients are often based on the assumption that such individuals are highly sensitive to shame; interviewers are urged to move gradually into the topic and normalize the experience (e.g., the “hierarchical approach”; Bryan & Rudd, 2006; Shea’s “shame attenuation”, 1999). However, in the present study, when given an opportunity to provide reasons for their concealment, less than 1/3 of the sample mentioned embarrassment, shame, stigma, or fear of being judged. Again, they were more likely to mention fear of hospitalization, and almost as likely to cite a desire to set their agenda for their therapy. Why was shame not mentioned more often? It may be that some amount of embarrassment is assumed by clients, such that they did not see fit to mention it. Alternately, respondents may have shied away from volunteering shame as a motive because, after all, acknowledging that one is experiencing shame can feel shameful. It is notable that shame was more commonly endorsed when provided in a list of multiplechoice options. Whatever the case, it does not appear that reducing shame was seen by SI concealers as a way to foster Psychotherapy Research greater honesty. When asked what could help them be more honest, few respondents mentioned shame-reducing interventions such as normalizing or validating. For SI concealers in this sample, at least, it appears that efforts to de-stigmatize suicidal ideation would have little effect on their willingness to disclose to a clinician. Clinical Implications Clients who believe—correctly or not—that they will be involuntarily hospitalized or experience other unwanted interventions if they disclose suicidal thoughts may feel strongly motivated to conceal no matter what method of assessment is used, and no matter how much clinicians attempt to reduce shame. For this reason, achieving the goal of honest disclosure may require finding ways for therapists to better educate about the basic rules of confidentiality, reporting, and hospitalization, in such a way that clients feel some measure of control or predictability. Ethical and professional responsibilities to respond when patients are at imminent risk are not negotiable; however, nothing is lost by, for example, clarifying circumstances under which one would certainly not be hospitalized. In the majority of cases, doing so will likely alleviate unrealistic fears held by the client, and potentially remove or mitigate this major barrier to disclosure. In fact, our findings provide some clues for what this conversation should look like. Respondents suggest that clinicians can: (a) explain the rules about mandated reporting, (b) explain the level of suicide risk that might trigger hospitalization at the facility where the client is being seen, (c) express their personal awareness of the difference between severe and non-severe suicidal ideation, (d) describe what typically happens when suicide risk rises to the level of being reported, and (e) acknowledge and validate the anxiety that clients often have about unwanted interventions. Due to the significant number of our respondents who doubted whether fleeting or low-intensity SI was worth discussing, clinicians might also (f) educate clients about the evolving understanding of SI as often involving rapid fluctuations in intensity (see Kleiman & Nock, 2018). But doesn’t every therapy start with an explanation of the limits of confidentiality? While clinics often require clients to sign consent documents, it is not clear how often clients receive detailed explanations about the triggers for hospitalization in a style and format that they will remember. Indeed, there is evidence that many clinicians are inclined to gloss over this material. A recent observational study of British 11 psychiatrists interviewing patients about suicidal ideation found that they tended to ask patients to confirm that they are not suicidal (“You don’t have thoughts of harming yourself?”), to which patients were more likely to deny being suicidal (McCabe, Sterno, Priebe, Barnes, & Byng, 2017). Avoidance of in-depth suicide assessment has been observed in psychiatric nurse practitioners (O’Reilly, Kiyimba, & Karim, 2016) and primary care physicians (Stoppe, Sandholzer, Huppertz, Duwe, & Staedt, 1999). Among psychotherapists, there is evidence (e.g., Farber, 1983) that suicidal statements are felt to be the most stress-inducing client behaviors, more so even than aggression and hostility. Anxious therapists may shy away from providing more than cursory explanations of the rules around confidentiality. Explanations may also be avoided by clinicians who do not want to alarm new patients, or who are themselves unsure of the specifics, or who perhaps worry that “giving away” the precise triggers for hospitalization might make it easy for suicidal clients to escape detection. A fuller approach to discussing suicide, confidentiality, and interventions is in keeping with the collaborative approach to suicide risk management favored by recent scholarship in the field. Jobes (2006), for example, urges clinicians to ensure that the “patient – who is the expert of his or her own experience – is engaged as an active collaborator in clinical care” (p. 41). Similarly, our findings can be seen as an endorsement of some of the 24 “core competencies” in suicide assessment promulgated by the Suicide Prevention Resource Center (2006). Specifically, the third competency calls for clinicians to maintain a collaborative, non-adversarial stance, including “obtaining informed consent to protect client rights and promote client participation in making decisions regarding care and treatment options”. Our findings suggest that mastery of this competency might be especially valuable for those clients concealing suicidal thoughts out of fear of practical repercussions. Limitations and Future Directions It is important to note that the original study from which this data set was drawn was not primarily about suicide. Responses make it evident that the sample includes a wide range of frequency and severity of client suicidal ideation; however, the study did not include measures that could have captured this information. Thus, our sample includes an unknown proportion of mild versus serious ideators, and we cannot conclude that this sample is representative of the symptom severity among the general 12 M. Blanchard and B. A. Farber population of suicidal ideators, or one that a clinician might encounter in a given clinical setting. Furthermore, the relatively small sample size, the absence of clients from countries outside the United States, and the absence of random sampling methods means we cannot claim that this sample is representative of the therapy-using population, nor of the population experiencing suicidal thoughts. In addition, while the sample has an extensive age range, it is heavily skewed toward female and white clients; men and minorities are underrepresented in the sample. If there are unique features of the male and/or minority experience around disclosing suicidality, these may not be fully represented by the present analysis. In fact, the small size sample here precluded analyses of motives for concealment as a function of multiple demographic (e.g., age, relationship status) and diagnostic (e.g., type of disorder, chronicity of disorder, previous suicide attempts) variables. Further, the practice of content analysis, like all qualitative methods, involves fallible coders. It is possible that different groups of coders would have arrived at different conclusions using the same data set. A final limitation arises from the respondents’ own insight into their motives and behaviors. There is no guarantee that what our respondents believe will help them disclose SI would, in actual practice, foster greater honesty. Important avenues of future research are suggested by the prevalence of hospitalization fear in this sample. What are clients actually told about the triggers for hospitalization? How much of this information do they retain? What do they believe about the types of disclosures that would mandate a therapist to break confidentiality? Research with psychotherapists could gauge the diversity of attitudes and practices regarding hospitalization across the profession, and potentially reveal the need to standardize or otherwise advance practice on this matter. Finally, we agree with the general point made by Hom et al. (2017), that “further research is needed to better understand how to enhance accuracy of reporting when individuals are probed about thoughts of suicide” (p. 2). We hypothesize that clarifying triggers for hospitalization empowers clients to make more informed decisions about whether to disclose suicidal ideation in therapy. Would this actually increase rates of disclosure? SI concealers in the present study certainly believed it would for them, but controlled experiments directly studying suicide-related disclosure may be impossible due to ethical concerns. Future research to test this proposition could instead employ proxy measures to study risk-taking under conditions of greater or lesser uncertainty (e.g., Balloon-Analogue Risk Task; Lejuez et al., 2002), which may prove useful in modeling the disclosure dilemma regarding SI in conditions of varying uncertainty about the clinical response. This study has demonstrated that clients make risk-benefit calculations about whether disclosing suicidal ideation will have serious impacts on their daily life. Our ability to help as psychotherapists may depend, in part, on helping them making that calculation more accurately. Acknowledgements Our thanks to Melanie N. Love for her help shaping and conducting this research. References Baumann, E. C., & Hill, C. E. (2016). Client concealment and disclosure of secrets in outpatient psychotherapy. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 29, 53–75. Blanchard, M., & Farber, B. A. (2016). Lying in psychotherapy: Why and what clients don’t tell their therapist about therapy and their relationship. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 29, 90– 112. Bryan, C. J., & Rudd, M. D. (2006). Advances in the assessment of suicide risk. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(2), 185–200. Burton Denmark, A., Hess, E., & Becker, M. S. (2012). College students’ reasons for concealing suicidal ideation. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 26(2), 83–98. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). Facts at a Glance: Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS) [Online]. National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/injury/ wisqars/inde.html. Drum, D. J., Brownson, C., Burton Denmark, A., & Smith, S. E. (2009). New data on the nature of suicidal crises in college students: Shifting the paradigm. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(3), 213–222. Farber, B. A. (2006). Self-disclosure in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press. Farber, B. A. (1983). Psychotherapists’ perceptions of stressful patient behavior. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 14(5), 697–705. Farber, B. A., Blanchard, M., & Love, M. (in press). Secrets and lies in psychotherapy. Washington, DC: APA Publications. Farber, B. A., & Hall, D. (2002). Disclosure to therapists: What is and is not discussed in psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58(4), 359–370. Farber, B. A., & Sohn, A. E. (2007). Patterns of self-disclosure in psychotherapy and marriage. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 44(2), 226–231. Franklin, J. C., Ribeiro, J. D., Fox, K. R., Bentley, K. H., Kleiman, E. M., Huang, X., … Nock, M. K. (2017). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 187–232. Ganzini, L., Denneson, L. M., Press, N., Bair, M. J., Helmer, D. A., Poat, J., & Dobscha, S. K. (2013). Trust is the basis for effective suicide risk screening and assessment in veterans. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 28(9), 1215–1221. Hill, C. E., Thompson, B. J., Cogar, M. C., & Denman, D. W. (1993). Beneath the surface of long-term therapy: Therapist and client report of their own and each other’s covert processes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 278–287. Psychotherapy Research Hom, M. A., Stanley, I. H., & Joiner, T. E. (2015). Evaluating factors and interventions that influence help-seeking and mental health service utilization among suicidal individuals: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 28–39. Hom, M. A., Stanley, I. H., Podlogar, M. C., & Joiner, T. E. (2017). “Are you having thoughts of suicide?” Examining experiences with disclosing and denying suicidal ideation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73(10), 1382–1392. Jobes, D. A. (2006). Managing suicidal risk: A collaborative approach. New York: Guilford Press. Jobes, D. A., & Ballard, E. (2011). The therapist and the suicidal patient. In K. Michel, & D. A. Jobes (Eds.), Building a therapeutic alliance with the suicidal patient (pp. 51–61). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Joiner, T. E., Jr, Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., & Rudd, M. D. (2009). The interpersonal theory of suicide: Guidance for working with suicidal clients. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. Kelly, A. E. (1998). Clients’ secret keeping in outpatient therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45(1), 50–57. Kleiman, E. M., & Nock, M. K. (2018). Real-time assessment of suicidal thoughts and behaviors. Current Opinion in Psychology, 22, 33–37. Klonsky, E. D., & May, A. M. (2014). Differentiating suicide attempters from suicide ideators: A critical frontier for suicidology research. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 44, 1–5. Klonsky, E. D., & May, A. M. (2015). The three-step theory (3ST): A new theory of suicide rooted in the “ideation-toaction” framework. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 8(2), 114–129. Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L. … Strong, D. R. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8 (2), 75. Maltsberger, J. T. (1986). Suicide risk: The formulation of clinical judgment. New York: New York University Press. Martin, L. E. (2006). Lying in psychotherapy: Results of an exploratory study (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest dissertations and Theses database. (UMI No. 3253137). 13 Mays, D. (2004). Structured assessment methods may improve suicide prevention. Psychiatric Annals, 34(5), 366–372. McCabe, R., Sterno, I., Priebe, S., Barnes, R., & Byng, R. (2017). How do healthcare professionals interview patients to assess suicide risk? BMC Psychiatry, 17(1), 122. Morrison, L. L., & Downey, D. L. (2000). Racial differences in self-disclosure of suicidal ideation and reasons for living: Implications for training. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 6(4), 374–386. Murphy, G. E. (1984). The prediction of suicide: Why is it so difficult? American Journal of Psychotherapy, 38(3), 341–349. Neuendorf, K. A. (2002). The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Nock, M. K., Borges, G., Bromet, E. J., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M., Beautrais, A., … Williams, D. (2008). Cross-national prevalence and risk factors for suicidal ideation, plans and attempts. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(2), 98–105. O’Connor, R. C. (2011). The integrated motivational-volitional model of suicidal behavior. Crisis: The Journal of Crisis Intervention and Suicide Prevention, 32, 295–298. O’Reilly, M., Kiyimba, N., & Karim, K. (2016). “This is a question we have to ask everyone”: asking young people about self-harm and suicide. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 23 (8), 479–488. Saldaña, J. (2009). The coding manual for qualitative researchers. London: Sage. Shea, S. C. (1999). The practical art of suicide assessment: A guide for mental health professionals and substance abuse counselors. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Stiles, W. B. (1995). Disclosure as a speech act: Is it psychotherapeutic to disclose? In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 71–91). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Stoppe, G., Sandholzer, H., Huppertz, C., Duwe, H., & Staedt, J. (1999). Family physicians and the risk of suicide in the depressed elderly. Journal of Affective Disorders, 54(1), 193–198. Suicide Prevention Resource Center. (2006). Core competencies in the assessment and management of suicidality. Newton, MA: SPRC. Welton, R. S. (2007). The management of suicidality: Assessment and intervention. Psychiatry, 4(5), 24–34.