CURRENT TOPICS IN NUTRACEUTICAL RESEARCH Vol. 9, No. 1, pp. 53-59, 2011 ISSN 1540 -7535 print, Copyright © 2011 by New Century Health P ublishers, LLC w ww .n e w c e nt ur y h e al t h p u bl i s h e rs . c o m All rights of reproduction in any form reserved DEFINITIONS AND PERCEPTIONS OF SNACKING Katherine Chapli n and Andre w P. Smith Centre for Occupationa l and Health Ps ychology, Schoo l of Psychology, Cardif f University, P O Box 901, Cardiff, CF10 3AS, UK. [Receive d Apri l 4, 2011; Accepte d May 9, 2011] ABSTRA CT: The purpos e o f this study was to investigate definitions of snacking, perceptions of snack foods and snacking behavior. One hundred and thirty six participants (96 females, 40 males , mean age 37 years), wh o we re either students or member s of the genera l public took part in the study. The participants c o m p l e t ed a snacking questionnaire which measured their snacking behavior and beliefs. The majority of participants believed that snacking was best defined as food or drink eaten between main meals. The majority of participants consumed at least one snack per day (80%) and had on average 4.5 eating episodes per day. Participants were able to group snack foods depending on their differing qualities with sub groups of the sample consuming snacks from these groups. This study supports previous evidence that snacks are best defined relative t o meal s howeve r it highlight s a nee d fo r further research to be done examinin g the relationship between mea ls and snacks. The findings identify that not all snack foods provide extra calorie s and therefore snackin g is not necessaril y a predisposition to obesity. KEY WORDS : S nacking beliefs, Snacking Fr equenc y, Snacks Cor r esponding Autho r: Pro f. And rew P. Smith, Centre for Occupational and Health Ps y cholog y, School of Ps y cholog y, Cardiff U n i versit y, P.O. Bo x. 901, Cardiff CF11 3YG, UK; Fa x: +44 29 20874758; E- mail: S mithAP@Cardiff .ac.uk INT RODUCTION N o wadays eating habits are moving away fro m eating three substantial meals a day to eating smaller amounts of food more frequently (snacking). This pattern of eating has been observed in both childre n ( L i v i n g s t o n e , 1991 ) and adult s (British Nutritio n Foundation, 1984) . Frequency of eatin g episodes ranges fro m 6 per day in children (Livingstone, 1991) to 6.5 in adult s ( Britis h Nutritio n Foundation, 1984). Expected decreases in frequency of eating episodes in an elderly (6.02) and a very elderly (5.60 ) populatio n hav e been o b s e r v e d (G ate n b y, 1997). Ho we v er, it is cur rently difficult to accurately measure consumptio n of snacks due to a lack of an agreed definition o f snackin g a nd relativel y little quantitative information being provided. “Having a snack ” can referto either eating food between meals or eating a light meal. Consistent differences in the usage of the terms snac k, snackin g and snac k food s hav e been identified , although thes e differences were marginal (Chamontin et al., 2003). Snac ks have been define d with respect to caloric consumptio n (Bernstein et al., 1981), in relation to social interaction (Rotenberg, 1981) and based o n time of day of consumption (Summbell et al., 1995). These definitions fail to consider the influence of social patterns and/ or cultura l nor ms on timing and size of eatin g occasions ( Gatenby, 1997). A distinction has also been made with respect to motivation for eating (Marmonier et al, 2002) with snacks being defined as eatin g episodes not trigge red by hunge r. Snacks are frequently referred to relative to meals as smalle r, les s structu red eatin g episodes ( Gatenby, 1997). Although there is no clear definition, the majority of papers define a snack relati ve to a mea l as “food or drin k consume d other than during main meal times”. Regardles s of h o w snackin g is defined it contributes significantly to total food intake (Gregory et al, 1990; 1995). It is believe d tha t snac ks contribute 15- 20% o f our daily energy intake (Summerbell et al., 1995), 15-20% of our daily mineral intake and 13- 17% o f our daily vitamin intake. Snacks have been found to differ from meals in terms of size, nutritional content and hunger and thirs t s e n s ati on s before and after the event (Bellisl e et al., 2003) . Different foods are reported when referring to meals or snacks with sweets, cereal bars, biscuits and fizzy drinks being more likely to be reported as snacks. Differences have also been observed between males and females with males consuming more fruit, soft drinks and sandwiches and females eating more chocolate, biscuit, cakes and crisps as snacks (Drummond et al. cited in Gaten by, 1995). More research is needed to further examine individual s’ definitions of snacks, specificall y looking at the timing of the intakes and the types of foods eaten. 2 Definition and perception o f Snacking It is a popular belief that people who snack frequently have a bad die t and they a re either ove r we ight or will become overweight. Some researchers believe that snacking may play an etiologic role in obesity (Basdevant et al., 1993; Takahashi et al. , 1999; Booth, 1988a,b). Snackin g is viewed as being food that is eaten in addition to 3 standard meals and therefore it is perceived as providing extra calories. Booth (1980) states that a small amount of food eaten approximatel y an hour before a meal is unlikely to decrease intake at the next meal. It is hypothesised that it is fattening to consume even a small amount of energy (from food or drinks) between main meals (Booth, 1988). However there is little evidence that frequent snackers are nutritionally disadvantaged or have a higher percentag e of body fat (Drummond, Kirk and de Looy, 1995). Snacks are pe rceived as being unhealthy foods, for example confectionery, which is believed to provid e “empty” calori es and therefore provide no other nutrients. It is also argued that these snacks are replacing foods with a greater nutritional content, which furt h er d i s a d v a n t a g es t he snacker. Ruxton et al. (1994) examined snackin g habit s o f 136 s ch o ol child re n . N o significant differences were found between frequent and nonfrequent snackers with respect to micro - and macronutrient intake. In addition Anderson (cited in Drummond et al, 1995) foun d no differenc e in energ y intake betwee n frequent and infrequent snackers. Drummond et al conclude that there is little evidence to suggest that increased consumption of snacks leads to an ex cess of “e m p t y” calories. Metzne r et al. (1977 ) compare d participants wh o ate 6 meals per day wit h those wh o ate 2 meals . No significant difference s were foun d with respect to Body Mas s Index (BMI). This contradicts the belief that increased snacking leads to an increas e in weight (Booth,1988). It is argued that i n c r e a s ed snackin g can be beneficial as long as an energy balance is maintained. The aims of the current study are to examine p a rticipant s’ definitions of snacking, their perceptions o f snacking and their snackin g habits . A n established definitio n of snacking is important in order for comparisons to be made across studies. This would also allow for other measuring instruments to be used . This is apparent when previous breakfas t research is conside red. B reakfast is defined as “that with which a person brea ks his fas t in the morning; the firs t mea l of the day” (Oxford Englis h Dictionary, 2nd Ed). Once a definition is derived it is possible to examine the length of the eating occasion, timing s betwee n tha t even t and othe r eating occasions and size of the occasion for example. If a definition of snackin g cou ld be agree d upo n this would allo w more specific measures of snacking to be taken. Any influence of snac k siz e a nd conten t on future mea ls cou ld b e easily examined including length of time between consumption and s ize of consumption. Th is would be pa rticularly beneficial when considering the effects of snacking on obesit y. It is of particular interestwhich foods participants perceive and eat as snacks. Factor analysis will be used to identify any s u b -scales of perceive d snac k foods and actual snack foods consumed by participants. METHODS Participants A sample of students was either contacted via a student participant database, and was sent the material in the post, or they were rec ruited fro m Cardiff Universit y through poster advertisements. A sa m ple of members fromthe general public was recruited fro m the genera l public participan t database. They were sent the materia l to complete throug h the post. Ta b le 1 d isp la ys demographic informatio n abou t the pa rticipants. Pa rticipants w ere paid for taking part in the study. TABLE 1. Demographic informatio n of student and general public samples. Studen t s a m p l e Genera l public sample mean ( S E M ) mean ( S E M ) Sample size M ale/F emal e ratio Age White/Non white ratio 55 81 14/41 26/55 21.62 (0.35) 47.16 (1.52) 43/12 75/6 Procedure Par ticipant s from t he databases wer e sent a letter and informatio n sheet detailin g the stud y and a consent form. Participants w ere requested to complete the consent form and the snacking questionnaire and return them in the freepost envelope p rovided. Participants recruited through the poster campaig n came to the laboratory and were give n the same informatio n sheet detailin g the study and consent form. Pa rticipants completed the questionnaire in the laborato ry. Materials and measurement Data were collected from 3 sources: a recruitment booklet, snacking frequency items and a snacking questionnaire. Each of these measures is described belo w. Recruitment booklet Information was collected regardin g the age, gende r and ethnicit y of participants. In addition participant s were also asked about the past and present health status. This included whethe r p ar ticipants suffer fro m asthma/hay fev er, viral conditions, musculo -s k e l e t al conditions, digestive problems and chronic illnesses. Participants were also asked to record any medication they were currently taking. Snack food consumption This was measured for 20 different items using a 7 point like r t scale whe re 0 = never and 6 = 3 to 4 times a day. Consumption of each item was categorised into three groups: (1) The non-consumer – neverconsumed the snack. Definition and perception of Snacking 3 (2) The weekly consumer – c o n s u m e d the snack between once to four times a week. (3) The daily consumer – consumed the snack between once to four times per day. Snacking questionnaire The s n ac ki n g questionnai re w a s d e v e l o p e d to investigate pa r tic i pa n t s’ belie fs abou t snacking , their definitio n of snackin g a nd their snacking habits . T he firs t question provide d participants wit h 7 possibl e definition s of snacking . Pa r t i ci p a n ts w e re aske d t o selec t t he on e definitio n the y believe d bes t matche d the ir o wn opinion. A numbe r o f questions w e re u s ed to measure snacking habits. Information was c o l l e c t ed about how many times participants h ad eate n t h e d ay b ef o re an d h o w many meals and snacks it c o n s i s t e d o f. In addition participants were specificall y as ked t o indicat e how ofte n the y ate b r e a k f a st . A lis t of timings was provided a nd participants were requested to identify when they normally ate their snacks. The questionnaire included a list o f 14 food and drin k items and required participant s to c h o o se those they pe r cei v ed w er e snac k i tem s . The p r oces s wa s re p e a t ed with a different lis t of 11 items except the participants had to decid e whic h items w e r e h ealt hy snacks . To measu r e pa r t i c i p a n t s ’ beliefs about snac ki n g a number of statements we re included. Par ticipants used a 5 poin t like r t scale to indicat e how much they agreed or disagreed with eac h of the statement s (where 1 = strongl y a g r ee and 5 = strongly disagr ee). Pa r ticipants we re als o presented with a lis t of snack proper ties . P ar ticipants used a 5-poin t liker t scale to indicat e ho w importan t the y believe d each property wa s (wher e 1 = e xt r em e l y important and 5 = not important at all ). Furthe r d e t a i l s of the sta t e m e nts used can be found in the results section. Statistical analysis The data was analysed usin g SPS S for W i n d o w s v.11. Principle c o mp o n en ts facto r a n a l y s es o f correlatio n matrices were conducted in order to classify empirically deriv ed sets of subscales . The factor str uctu re was rotated usin g the direct oblimin method and was set to converge in 25 iterations. Factor loadings g reater than or equal to .4 0 w e r e c o n s i d e r e d s i g n i f i c a n t f o r i n c l u s i o n in t h e subscales. RESULTS Demogr aphics and definition s of snacking The majority of participants (71% ) agreed tha t the best definitio n of snackin g was , “food or drin k eate n betw een main meals.” Whe n the sample was considered based on ethnicit y (whit e compared to non -white) the patte rn was found to be very different. With in the whit e sub -group the vas t majorit y o f p articipants (77%) agreed that “food or drin k eate n bet w een ma in meals ” was the superlative definition. Howev er, in the non - whit e sub- gr oup there was an equa l split between thre e of the definitions (each wit h 25%). Table 2 sh o ws the pe r centag e of pa rticipants wh o agree d with the 9 different definitions of snacking. The sample is c onside r ed as a whole as wel l as split based on ethnicit y. T ABLE 2. Definitions of snacking based on demographic information in percentages. Definitions Whole White Non- white sample su b - g r o u p sub -group Food/drink eaten on the move 7 4 25 Food or drink eaten bet w een meals 71 77 25 Small amounts of food Food eaten more than once a day 3 3 4 3 0 0 Food that is quick to eat 9 7 25 Food that is easy to eat 4 3 13 Food that does n’t fillyou up 3 3 6 Food/drink eaten after evenin g meal 0 0 0 Food accompanying hot drink 0 0 0 No difference s in definitio n w er e foun d betwee n students and the genera l populatio n or based on gender or based on age group s (17- 25 year olds , 26-40 yea r olds, 41-55 year olds and 56years and over). Snacking perceptions and habits Par t i c i pa n t s gen eral l y b e li e v ed that snac k foods are less h ea lt h y f o o d s a n d t h a t i n c r e a s ed sn ack in g b y th e population , in general, is a majo r contributo r t o the c u r r e n t i n c r eas e in obesit y. Snackin g fre q u e n cy was si g ni fi ca n tl y associate d wit h t he belie f tha t a grazing (snac king) patte rn of eating is les s healthy (r = .203, p = .017). The positive r elationship bet ween thes e v a r i a bl e s implie s that those people wh o snac k mo re frequently d o not agre e wit h the statement tha t a snackin g pattern of eatin g is les s healthy. The s a me pattern was found when lookin g at whether snack foods a re less healthy (r = .237, p = .005). Participants di sag r e ed that t h ey depen d a lot on snack food because they ha v e a busy lifestyl e and don ’t have time to eat m e a l s . “B e i ng tasty” wa s rated as bein g a ver y important propert y of a snac k. “Being good for me,” “fills me up,” “eas y to e at on the go” and “eas y to carry” we re a ll c o n si d e r e d to be quit e i m p o rtant. Bein g “eas y t o shar e” a nd bein g “a recognised bra nd name” we re n o t considered to be important properties of snac k food. In total 80% of participants reported eating at leas t one snac k per day. O n av e r a ge par ticipant s ate 3 meals and 1.5 s n a c ks pe r day re sultin g in 4.5 eatin g e pi s ode s . Snacks we re reportedly eaten in the mid - afternoon (61%), mid mornin g (59%) and durin g the evening (52%). 4 Definition and perception o f Snacking Facto r analysis of snackin g questionnaire Facto r analysi s o f the 14 items measurin g pa rt i c i p a n t s ’ perception s of snacks revealed a 3 factor solution, based on Eigen v a l u es > 1. The factor l o a d i ng indicated a simple solutio n wit h n o items do ubl e loadin g an d t h e item loadin g h ad theo re t i c al co her en ce . F our items w e r e excluded fro m the analysis as they were not consistently loadin g on factors acros s the subscal e a nal y si s . They we re not included in the 3 s u b s c al es generated: convenience s n a cks ( 6 items), f ru it a n d nu t s n acks ( 2 items ) a n d carbohydrate snac ks (2 items). F igu re 1 sho ws the items and their loadings fo r each of the factors. In contrast to the firs t two fac tors the r e l a tio n s h i p betwee n the items in the carbohydrate snac ks subscal e was negative. the items in the sweet snacks subscal e was found to be negative. FIGURE 2. Snacks actually eaten by participants. FIGURE 1. Factor structure of perceived snacks. A facto r analysi s of the 20 ite ms measuring the food a nd drin k participants actuall y consumed reveale d a 6 facto r solution , base d on Eigen v alues > 1. T he factor loadin g indicated a simp le solutio n with no items double loadin g and the item loadin g had theoretica l coherence. Four items wer e e xcluded fro m the analysis as they we re n ot consistently loadin g on facto rs acros s th e s u b s c a l e an al y si s . Thes e w er e n ot include d in t he 6 s u b s c a l e s generated : crisps, chocolat e and canne d drin ks (3 items), fruit and c e r e al s n ac k s (3 items ), s a v o u r y s n a c ks (4 items), teatime sna c k s ( 2 items), nuts and yoghu rt (2 items ) and sweet snacks (2 items). F igu re 2 sh o ws the items and their l o a d i n g s for each of the factors . The rela ti o n s hi p bet ween Factor analysi s was conducted with the 11 items used to examin e which foods par ticipants believ ed we re healthy. A forced 2-factor solution was accepted as examination of the score plo t revealed the poin t of inflectio n was at 2 factors although an additional factor with an Eigen v a l u e greater than 1 was present. The factor loading indicated a simple solution wit h no items doub le loading and the ite m loading had theoretica l cohe r ence. Three items we re exclude d from the analysis as they were not consistently loading on factors across the subscale analysis. These items w ere not included in the 2 subscales generated: traditionally healthy snacks (5 items) and other snacks (3 items). Figu re 3 sh ows the items and their Definition and perception of Snacking 5 loadings for each of the factors. The first factor contains the traditiona l healthy food items whereas the second factor contained those foods where particular types can be healthy. FIGURE 3. Factor s tructu re of snacks seen as being healthy. Age groups difference in snack consumption The sample was split into thre e age groups; 17- 25 year olds (group 1), 26- 40 yea r olds (grou p 2), 41- 55 year olds (group 3) and 56 year s and over (group 4). Differences in consumptio n wer e found between the ag e groups for 7 items out of 20. Thes e items were fru it juice, canned drinks , hot drinks , b iscu i ts , yoghurt , f ru it a n d c r i s p s. Grou p 1 consume d more fruit juic e (45% ) and canned drinks (37%) on a weekly basi s than the othe r 3 groups (35% , 30% , 8% an d 19% , 20% , 17% r e s p e c t i v e l y ) . In contras t th ey consume d les s hot drinks on a daily bas is (32%) compared to the other age groups (58%) . Group 2 ate more yoghurt on a weekly basi s (45% ) c o m p a r e d with the other ag e groups (37% , 33% and 38%). They als o ate mo re fr u it on a daily basi s (50% ) than the othe r groups. Group 3 were foun d to eat more biscuit s on weekl y basi s (65% ) c o m p a r ed to groups 1 (49%) , 2 (42% ) and 4 (45%). Half of the eldes t ag e group reported eatin g crisps on a weekly bas is and half reported never eatin g crisps. Gender differences in snack fo o d consumption A similar proces s was used in orde r to e x a m i n e whether there we re any differences between males and females in terms of s n acki n g patterns. Only four items wer e foun d to differ b e t w e en males and females . T h e s e result s s h o w ed that f e m a l es ate mo re yoghurt and fruit and drank mo re fruit juice than males . In contras t m a l es dran k more hot drinks than females. Health status and snacking No significant results w ere found bet ween past health status and snacking behavior and current health status and snacking behavio r. DISCUSSION The results obtained demonstrate participants believe snacks are “food or drink which is eaten between main meals.” This support s the notio n tha t the majorit y of papers define snacks relati v e to meals. This definition, h o w ev er, is dependent on the definition individuals have of a main meal. This is also problematic as meals are influenced by socia l pattern s and cultural norms. It would appear that definitions o f snacks and meals are circula r, snacks are defined as not being meals and vic e vers a a nd no specifi c definition s c an be made independently. Some d i s a g r e e m e nt relatin g to definitio n has occurre d in the literature with respect to breakfas t research. B reakfast is defined as “that with which a person breaks his fast in the morning; the first meal of the da y” (Oxford English D ictiona ry, 2n d Ed). H o we v e r, some studies include a condition which provides participants with a mid - morning snack (having missed breakfast) and therefore by definition the snack is also their breakfast (Benton et al., 2001; Smith and Wilds, 2009). This makes it difficult to differentiate breakfast effects from snack effects. This suggests that previous eating episodes may als o contribute to defining both snacks and meals . It is important that more research is done to further examine definitions of both snacks and meals in order for more detailed research to be conducted. The result s sugges t that the tim e of consumptio n is an important component in defining snacking. In relatio n to timing, snac ks are commonly consume d durin g the mid morning, the mid -a f te r noon and the evening. This re-enforces the finding that snacks are food or drink consumed between main meals. Frequency of eating episodes was found to be 4.5 a day on average. This consisted of 3 meals and 1.5 snacks. The current findings are lower than would be expected based on previous resea r ch (Livingstone, 1991; Britis h Nutritio n Foundation, 1984). This average was the same regardless of age. This was not expected based on p revious findings (Gatenb y, 1997). On e important consideration when comparing the current results wit h previous findings relates bac k to which definitio n of snackin g was used . S pe c i f i c a l ly it re l a t es t o whether consumption of drinks without food is included as snacks. In the current study drinks were not included when reporting what snacks had been eaten and this could explain why snack consumption is lowerthan previous findings suggest. The current study found that 80% of people ate at least one snack per day. This supports the notion that w e are moving away fro m a pattern of eatin g thre e traditiona l meals a day and are following a more grazing pattern of eating. In contrast the majority of people agreed that snack foods were generally les s healthy and we re a majo r contributo r to the current 6 Definition and perception o f Snacking increase in obes it y. It would appear that the individuals in the curr ent study perceiv e snackin g as bein g unhealthy. This would support the belief that snacks pr ovide “e m p t y” calories and therefor e play a role in obesit y. H o w ever the results show that the items consumed varied greatl y. Consumption of 20 snack items was best accounted for with 6 factors. These were labelled crisps/chocolate and a can of drink, fruit and cereal snacks , teatim e snacks, nuts and yoghurt snacks, savour y snacks and s weet snacks. The results s how that both foods that are traditionally seen as unhealthy snack foods, and are believed to provide empty calories, and other foods and drinks which offer nutritional value were consumed as snacks. Differing opinions wer e identified with respect to perceived snacks. These were best accounted for with 3 factors labelled convenienc e snacks , fruit and nut snacks and carbohydrate snacks . Foods such as fruit which are commonly v i ew ed as being healthy were perceived to be snack foods. In addition distinctions were made between traditionally healthy snacks and those snacks which have different varieties with differing nutritiona l content. So me participants were found to snack on the traditionally healthy items , however in genera l they stil l perceived snacks to be unhealthy and believed they contribute to obesity. In contras t frequent snacker s do not perceive snacking to be a cause for the current risein obesity and they do not think snack foods are unhealthy. It would be of interest to examine the perceptions individuals have of their snacking behavior and the consequences o f this behavio r. Althoug h some difference s were foun d between the four different age groups in terms of snack food consumption the majority of food items sh o we d a similar distribution of results. It has bee n shown tha t average consumptio n o f snacks was 1.5 per day across all age groups. It would also appear that the same types of snacks are generall y eaten acros s all age groups . Thre e ex ception s we re found between the youngest age group and the oldest group. These w ere the consumption of canned drinks , hot drinks and f ruit juice. The youngest group drank more fruit juice and canned drinks and the eldest group consume d more hot drinks . This would be expected based on social norms and previous experiences. It is generally thought that females snack on more chocolate/ biscuits and males on sandwiches/fruit (Drummond et al. cited in Gatenb y, 1995). The results of the current study suggest that both males and females snack on very similar items with no differences bein g found for consumption of chocolate, biscuits or crisps. It is apparent that considerabl y more researc h need s to be u n der ta k en to further examin e perceptions of snack s along with individual s’ healt h beliefs and attitudes to wa rd s snacking . Now that the issu e o f definition has bee n examine d research i s needed t o in v estigat e whethe r p a rt ic i pa n t s ’ snackin g habit s a re correlated wit h an y othe r aspect s o f physica l or mental behavio r. A CKNOWLEDGEMENTS The research was supported by a BBSRC Ph D studentship and industrial sponsorship f rom K ellogg ’s Company Ltd. REFERENCES Basdevant A, Craplet C and Guy- Grand B (1993) Snacking patterns in obese French women. A ppetite, 21, 17-23. B ellisle F, Dali x AM , Mennen L, Gala n P, Hercbe rg S, de Castro JM and Gausseres N (2003) Contribution of snacks and meals in the die t of Frenc h adults : a diet-dia r y study. Physiolog y and Behavior 79, 183 - 189. Benton D, Slater O and Donohoe RT (2001) The influence o f breakfas t an d a snac k o n psychological functioning. Physiolog y and Behavior 74, 559- 571. Bernstein IL, Zimmerman JC, C zeisler C A and Weitzman ED (1981) M eal pattern s in “fre e- runnin g ” humans . P hysiolog y and Behavior 27, 621- 623. Booth DA (1980) Acquire d behaviou r controllin g energy intake and output. In Obesit y, ed . A.J. Stunka rd , pp. 101143. Philadelphia,PA : W.B. Saunders. Booth DA (1988a) A simulatio n model of psychobiosocia l theory of human food-intake controls. Internationa l Journal of Vitami n and Nutrition Research 58, 55-69. Booth DA (1988b) Mechanisms from models – actual effects fro m rea l life : the zero -calori e drink-break option . Appetite 11(Suppl.) , 94- 102. Britis h Nutritio n Foundatio n (1984) . Eating in the earl y 1980s. Attitudes and Behaviour : Main Findings . London: British Nutrition Foundation. Chamontin A, Pretzer G and Booth DA (2003) Ambiguity of “snac k” in British usage. Appetite, 41, 21- 29. Dr ummond S, Kirk T a nd de Looy A (1995) S n a c k i n g : Implications in body composition and energy balance. British Food Journal 97, 12- 15. Gatenby S (1997 ) Eating frequency : methodologica l and dietary aspects. B ritis h Jo ur na l of Nutrition 77 (Suppl.1) , S7-S20. G r ego ry J, Collins DL, Davies PSW, Hughes JM and Clar ke PC (1995) National Diet and Nutrition Su r vey: Childr en Aged 1½ to 4½ Years. Vol 1, Repo rt of the Diet and Nutrition Sur v ey. London: H. M. Stationery Office. Gregory J , Foster K, Tyle r H and Wisema n M (1990) The Dietar y and Nutritional Su r vey of British Adults. London: H. M. Stationery Office. Livingstone MBE (1995) Assessment of food intakes: are we measurin g what peop le eat? British Journa l of Bi o me dic al Sciences 52, 58-67. Definition and perception of Snacking 7 Marmonier C, Chapelot D, Fantino M and Louis -S y l vestre J (2002) Snacks consumed in a nonhungry state have poor satiatin g efficiency : influence o f snac k compositio n on substrate utilization and hunger. A merican Journal of Clinical Nutrition 76, 518-528. Metzne r H L, Lamphiear DE, Wheele r NC and La rkin FA (1977) The relationship between the frequency of eating and adiposit y in adul t men a nd wome n in t h e Tecumseh Community Healt h S tudy. A m e ric a n Jou r n a l of Clinical Nutrition 30, 712-715. Rotenberg R (1981) The impact of industrialisation on meal patterns in Vienna, Austria. E colog y of F ood and Nutrition 11, 25- 35. Ruxton C, Kirk TR, Betton NR and Holmes MAM (1994) Does snacking affect overall daily nutrient intake in children? Pr oceedings of Nutrition Societ y 53, 253.A. Smith AP and Wilds A (2009) The effects of cereal bars for breakfas t and mid- mornin g snacks on mood and memo ry. In t er nationa l Jo u r na l o f F o od Scienc e and Nutritio n, 60, s4, 63- 69. Summerbell CD, Moody RC, Shanks J, Stock M J and Geissler C (1995) Sources of energy from meals vs snacks in 220 people in four age groups. E u r opea n Jo u r nal o f Clinical Nutrition 49, 33- 41. Takahashi E, Yoshida K, S ugimori H, M iyakawa M, Izuno T, Yama ga m i T and Kagamimori S (1999) Influence factors on the development of obesity in 3 -year - old children based on the Toyama study. Pr eventive Medicine 28, 109- 114. 8