COMPARISON OF INTENSIVE VERB INTERVENTIONS WITH AND WITHOUT THE REQUIREMENT OF SPEECH PRODUCTION IN THE TREATMENT OF FLUENT APHASIA Gretchen Amy Hess B.A., California State University, Sacramento, 2000 M.A., California State University, Sacramento, 2004 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE in SPEECH PATHOLOGY at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO FALL 2011 COMPARISON OF INTENSIVE VERB INTERVENTIONS WITH AND WITHOUT THE REQUIREMENT OF SPEECH PRODUCTION IN THE TREATMENT OF FLUENT APHASIA A Thesis by Gretchen Amy Hess Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Dr. Laureen O’Hanlon ___________________________________, Second Reader Dr. Robert Pieretti ____________________________ Date ii Student: Gretchen Amy Hess I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. _______________________, Department Chair Dr. Laureen O’Hanlon Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology iii ___________________ Date Abstract of COMPARISON OF INTENSIVE VERB INTERVENTIONS WITH AND WITHOUT THE REQUIREMENT OF SPEECH PRODUCTION IN THE TREATMENT OF FLUENT APHASIA by Gretchen Amy Hess Aphasia interventions that do not require verbal responses (implicit therapy approaches) can be beneficial when verbal errors are prevalent and frustrating for the individual. Intensive, error-reducing interventions utilizing computerized four-picture stimuli and requiring inhibition of close semantic competitors as well as activation of the target have also been shown to improve word retrieval. This study, which was counterbalanced to address order effects, compared an implicit, semantic decision-based verb intervention with an intervention identical in all respects except for the requirement of speech production in an individual with fluent aphasia. Clear response to both production-free and production-required interventions was observed, and training effects in both phases were maintained. Order effects were observed, with the first training method in each phase demonstrating a larger size effect. Findings support the use of semantic decisionbased, production-free training as a potentially valuable augmentation to conventional therapy for aphasia rehabilitation. In addition, verb training may have direct effects on spontaneous speech and fluency. _____________________________, Committee Chair Dr. Laureen O’Hanlon iv _____________________ Date ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Dr. Christine Davis made this research possible. She introduced me to and shared her knowledge about this topic during my first undergraduate year, encouraged and guided my participation in her research projects, ushered me into the greater community of aphasia researchers, provided countless additional learning opportunities, made me feel my ideas were valuable, and just generally served as role model and inspiration in every possible way. To whatever degree I have helped or will ever help to improve the communication skills of individuals with aphasia or further the cause of research conducted on their behalf, you are responsible. I can never thank you enough. My deepest thanks to my thesis chair, Dr. Laureen O'Hanlon, whose palpable enthusiasm for research and lifelong learning—and whose confidence in me, in particular—gave me, in turn, the confidence to believe that a mere English major could think like a scientist and researcher. It is because of you, ultimately, that I ever saw myself as someone who could succeed in this field, and that I was able to be open to surprising and counterintuitive new ideas (such as speech therapy that discourages the client from speaking!). Many thanks also to Dr. Kathleen Baynes, pioneer in the area of production-free treatment methods, for assisting in the compilation of the initial figures for this study, as well as by taking time out of her busy schedule to answer my questions. Thanks, too, to the ever-dependable Robert Pieretti, for stepping in as second reader, and to Jeanne Wielgus Marlatt, lifelong friend extraordinaire, for late-night, remote formatting assistance when I needed it most. Finally, to Bob and our children, I give thanks for all….now and always. v DEDICATION For I.T., whose curiosity, love of learning, and lively sense of humor made every session a delight. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................. v Dedication .......................................................................................................................... vi List of Tables ..................................................................................................................... ix List of Figures ..................................................................................................................... x Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION AND REVIEW OF RESEARCH ................................................... 1 Purpose of the Study ............................................................................................... 1 Traditional speech therapy for aphasia ................................................................... 4 Intensive treatment .................................................................................................. 5 Confrontation naming and verbal repetition ......................................................... 11 Lexical retrieval and semantic processing models ................................................ 13 Semantic-based intervention ................................................................................. 19 Errorless and error-reducing approaches .............................................................. 29 Implicit therapy ..................................................................................................... 35 Mental practice and inner speech .......................................................................... 37 Development of the implicit method for treatment of aphasia ............................. 41 The present study .................................................................................................. 48 2. METHODS .................................................................................................................. 54 Participant ............................................................................................................. 54 Procedure .............................................................................................................. 56 3. RESULTS .................................................................................................................... 69 Comparison of Implicit and Explicit Training Methods ....................................... 69 Error Patterns ........................................................................................................ 73 Change in Standardized and Non-Standardized Language Measures .................. 74 4. DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................. 77 Major Findings and Implications .......................................................................... 77 vii Limitations of the Present Study and Directions for Future Research.................. 82 Conclusions ........................................................................................................... 87 Appendix A. Trained and Untrained Verb Lists ............................................................... 89 Appendix B. Verb Use in Training Templates ................................................................. 91 Appendix C. Sample Training Templates ......................................................................... 93 References ....................................................................................................................... 109 viii LIST OF TABLES Page 1. Table 1. Classification of responses according to error type. ............................... 73 2. Table 2. Participant’s pre-and post-treatment scores ............................................ 76 3. Table A3. Phase One Trained and Untrained Verbs ............................................. 89 4. Table A4. Phase Two Trained and Untrained Verbs ............................................ 90 5. Table B5. Question Types with Sample Questions .............................................. 91 6. Table B6. Sample Typical and Atypical Exemplars ............................................. 92 ix LIST OF FIGURES Page 1. Figure 1. Outline of Levelt’s (1999) theory. ......................................................... 19 2. Figure 2. Illustration of treatment study design .................................................... 53 3. Figure 3. Participant’s MRI and CT scans showing area and extent of lesion. .... 54 4. Figure 4. Phase One: Comparison of implicit and explicit training effects. ......... 71 5. Figure 5. Phase Two: Comparison of explicit and implicit training effects. ........ 72 6. Figure 6. Percentage of responses in each phase classified by type. .................... 74 x 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION AND REVIEW OF RESEARCH Purpose of the Study Aphasia, an acquired communication impairment most frequently associated with stroke and traumatic brain injury (TBI), can have devastating effects on all aspects of a person’s life and work (National Aphasia Association, 2010). Among the most common and most distressing consequences of aphasia are profound word retrieval deficits (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Raymer, Kohen, & Saffell, 2006), and much clinical time is devoted to the remediation of these word retrieval or naming problems (Nickels, 2002). Although speech-language therapy can improve outcomes for individuals (Basso, Capitani, & Vignolo, 1979; Best & Nickels, 2000; Mazzoni et al., 1995), sometimes even when delivered to patients in “quite small amounts” (Howard, 2000), the most effective therapy for many patients may require anywhere from 9 to 20 or more hours of therapy per week (Basso & Caporali, 2001; Bhogal, Teasell, & Speechley, 2003; Hinckley & Craig, 1998). This level of treatment is unavailable to many patients due to financial considerations, health insurance limitations, the relative shortage of available speechlanguage pathologists, and logistical factors such as the time and travel required to transport clients to treatment facilities (Davis & Baynes, 2009). In addition, much therapy currently being conducted with individuals with aphasia can be classified as “errorful” in that it allows the client numerous opportunities for making—or, in effect, practicing—numerous speech production errors. Errorful therapy, in turn, can have the unintended consequence of increasing the likelihood of subsequent errors (Brookshire, 2 1972, 1975; Brookshire, Nicholas, Redmond, & Krueger, 1979), an effect which “may be even more harmful or pervasive in cases of neurological damage” such as that often sustained by stroke or TBI survivors with aphasia (Frattali, 2004). Aphasia interventions which require the client to make semantic decisions without producing explicit verbal responses have resulted in improvements in naming which last longer than the effects of interventions requiring articulation of the target (Howard, Patterson, Franklin, Orchard-Lisle, & Morton, 1985a). Therapy focusing on semantic decision-making in the absence of explicit naming can be especially beneficial for clients whose verbal errors tend to be prevalent, self-reinforcing, and frustrating (Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006, 2007; Davis, Farias, & Baynes, 2009). When applied intensively, such production-free interventions, sometimes referred to as “implicit therapy” or “error-reducing” approaches, have significantly improved naming and discourse, hypothetically through activation of the two neural networks—semantic and phonological—assumed to support naming ability and speech production (Davis & Baynes, 2009). The activation of these neural networks during implicit semantic processing tasks has been well-documented in fMRI studies (Ruff, Blumstein, Myers, & Hutchinson, 2008). Production-free interventions, by reducing error productions, are also thought to decrease the likelihood that the client will “practice” and therefore reinforce his or her own error productions during therapy (Davis & Baynes, 2009). An additional benefit of production-free interventions is that they can be individualized to match specific client needs and interests, and, via the use of computerized programs, can be included in home carryover programs for clients who need additional language practice, 3 or for whom continued or intensive treatment by a speech-language pathologist is not an option (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Although production-free treatment methods have recently begun to be compared experimentally with methods requiring explicit speech production (Davis, Baynes, & Hess, 2009; Davis, Farias, & Baynes, 2008), no previous experiment compared these two types of interventions in the treatment of a client with fluent aphasia, or used a crossover design to control for order effects. This single-subject, multiple baseline, counterbalanced study was conducted over the course of many months, between July, 2009 and September, 2010, at U.C. Davis Medical Center, and was presented at the international Academy of Aphasia (Davis, Hess, & Baynes, 2010). It compared a production-free, semantic decision-based verb intervention with an intervention identical in all respects except for the requirement of speech production in an individual with fluent aphasia secondary to a traumatic brain injury. Before beginning this study, the researchers hypothesized that both productionfree and conventional training would improve production of trained verbs, but asked the question whether such improvements would be essentially equal or would significantly differ due to the differing (speech production) response requirements in each condition. They also sought to determine whether training would generalize to untrained verbs, whether a measure of effect size would supplement observations from visual inspection of results, and whether order effects would be observed. 4 Traditional speech therapy for aphasia Although the consensus seems to be that aphasia therapy improves outcomes for individuals (Albert, 2003; Basso, 2005; Wertz, 2000), and numerous studies have demonstrated that treatment is generally efficacious and associated with statistically significant, long-term improvement in language skills (U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs & Department of Defense, 2003), there are still unanswered questions regarding which type of treatment may be the best match for a particular individual, or even for a particular profile of deficits. It has been observed that “in particular, it is still difficult to predict which therapeutic task or approach will be successful at remediating which particular disorders. We argue that this is particularly true in the rehabilitation of anomia” (Best & Nickels, 2000). Matching treatment and deficit is not always effective (Best & Nickels, 2000; Drew & Thompson, 1999; Wambaugh, Linebaugh, Doyle, Martinez, & Kalinyak-Fliszar, 2001). Moreover, treatment parameters and patient descriptions differ so much across studies that it is difficult to identify the individual factors that predict response to treatment (Davis & Baynes, 2009). The general approach most frequently used to treat anomia in aphasia is to explicitly train individuals in whole word naming (Maher & Raymer, 2004), or in other words, to have them practice production of target words in the hope that this exercise will increase their word-finding abilities in spontaneous speech (Nickels, 2002a, 2002b). Studies have shown that this method may improve naming performance for trained items but that generalization tends to be very limited, with “at best very modest improvement in naming performance with untrained words” (Kendall et al., 2008). In traditional speech 5 therapy, clinicians have long used prompts and cueing in naming tasks to elicit spoken words from patients “with the hope that the patients will subsequently be able to retrieve the names without help,” and yet this hope may be entirely without foundation (Howard, Patterson, et al., 1985a); after successfully naming a target picture, aphasic patients were shown to be no more likely to successfully name the target when presented with the same picture 30 minutes after training (Patterson, Purell, & Morton, 1983). Even when therapy does appear to have a positive effect, it is often difficult to determine precisely which variables were responsible for the improvement in performance, and thus “there is still no clear agreement on how to manage [the] deficits” of patients with anomia (Maher & Raymer, 2004). Poeck, Huber, and Willmes (1989) noted that although the efficacy of treatment had been generally demonstrated, “the mechanisms or factors that are related to the beneficial effects of treatment remain poorly understood.” Some authors have hypothesized that we know so little about the precise mechanisms of improvement in aphasia therapy that we may inadvertently be subjecting patients to therapies that have minimal effect, have no effect, or, even worse, may actually be harmful (Howard, Patterson, et al., 1985a). It is therefore of the greatest importance that investigators attempt to determine precisely which factors are associated with improvements in word retrieval for patients with aphasia. Intensive treatment Some studies have found that although factors such as age, duration, size and site of lesion, and impairment in intelligence were not related to improvement with therapy, one factor of crucial importance was the intensity or dosage at which therapy was 6 delivered (Basso, 2005; Basso & Caporali, 2001; Poeck, Huber, & Willmes, 1989). Some authors have suggested that intensity of treatment may be the single most important factor contributing to the inconsistency of treatment effects across studies, and that “the failure to identify a consistent benefit might have been due to the low intensity of speechlanguage therapy applied in the negative studies, whereas higher intensities of therapy were present in positive studies” (Bhogal, Teasell, & Speechley, 2003). Principles of neurobiological learning explored in animal studies (Squire, 1992) and in human research (Poldrack & Gabrieli, 2001) suggest that treatment intensity is a significant factor for learning (Off, Kavalier, & Rogers, 2008). Further research addressing neural plasticity involved in memory and learning indicates that a large number of trials are required to elicit change (Squire, 1992), but that such change can have long-lasting results. Hinckley (2002) reported that many patients continued to describe improved life satisfaction two years after receiving intensive therapy. Davis, Harrington and Baynes (2006), in a review of the evidence on intensive therapy, concluded that “intense, repetitive stimulation can have both behavioral and neural effects,” and that the neuropsychological effects of such intensive applications of speech and language therapy, once attained, can be both “significant and persistent.” Numerous studies have demonstrated that intensive intervention is effective for individuals with aphasia (Miceli, Amitrano, Capasso, & Caramazza, 1996) and may be the most potentially beneficial regimen of treatment (Bhogal, Teasell, & Speechley, 2003; Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006; Wertz et al., 1986), although authors have differed in their definition of “intensive” (Basso, 2005). Results of a meta-analysis of 7 studies showed that the total number of therapy sessions was an important factor in recovery, with indications that “when therapy is protracted for many months or even years with a very strict regimen” of two to four hours daily, individuals with aphasia “showed clear improvement in their daily use of language and communicative competence” (Basso, 2005). Some authors have noted improvements—even long-lasting improvements—with therapy delivered intensively over a much shorter period of time (Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). Bhogal, Teasell, and Speechley (2003), “building upon the idea that more is better,” conducted a review of previous studies to determine the relationship of intensity of aphasia therapy and aphasia recovery after stroke. The authors found that “intense aphasia therapy over a short period of time has greater impact on recovery than less intense therapy over a longer period of time.” Brindley, Copeland, Demain, and Martyn (1989) found that three months of intensive speech therapy in a residential setting resulted in significant speech improvements for patients with chronic Broca’s aphasia even when the patients were many years post-onset. Miceli, Amitrano, Capasso, and Caramazza (1996) found that participants receiving one hour of treatment five days a week for one week (for a total of only five sessions/five hours of therapy) showed marked improvement of treated words, and that one patient continued to display a difference between treated and untreated items 17 months post-treatment. Poeck, Huber and Willmes (1989) investigated whether intensive language treatment led to improvement beyond the rate of spontaneous recovery and whether such treatment was effective for patients in the chronic phase (over 12 months post-onset). They found that 8 with intensive treatment, defined as six to eight weeks of therapy consisting of five onehour individual sessions and four one-hour group sessions per week, improvement went beyond that expected with spontaneous recovery alone in up to 78% of patients, depending upon time since onset. With intensive treatment, aphasia improved even in the chronic stage for 68% of the patients, and these positive results appeared to be maintained. Furthermore, the authors felt that the actual percentages of patients who improved may have been much higher than these numbers reflected, due to the overly “strict” correction procedures used to account for spontaneous recovery. Only global aphasics failed to improve significantly when intensive treatment was administered beginning over one year post-onset (Poeck, Huber, & Willmes, 1989). Basso and Caporali (2001), noting that “it has never been our experience” that patients recover with therapy delivered at standard intensity levels, conducted a study to compare the relative effectiveness of intensive and less intensive therapy regimens. They found that intensive therapy resulted in more dramatic, sustained, and generalized improvements than did therapy delivered at less intensive treatment intervals, even for participants whose aphasia was chronic and whose previous (standard) therapy had been halted after further improvement was deemed unlikely. In their study, intensive therapy was defined as therapy received for two to three hours per day, seven days a week, over the course of many months. This level of intensity was achieved through use of a combination of one-to-one therapy with a speech pathologist for one hour per day, supplemented by intensive homework supported by family members and volunteers for an additional one to two hours per day. The authors noted that those participants who 9 received the more intensive therapy experienced improvements in word retrieval which improved their confidence in their language abilities and carried over to spontaneous speech in their everyday communication interactions. The authors concluded that the patients’ recovery, “and particularly their better use of language for communication purposes, was the result of intense work done with each patient, and that such a regimen could be successful in a number of patients for whom a less intensive regimen would be ineffective” (Basso & Caporali, 2001). Dronkers, Husted, Deutsch, Taylor, Saunders, and Merzenich (1999) reported similar results with therapy delivered to patients for one and a half hours per day, five days a week, for eight weeks. Although there is no consensus on what constitutes an ideal level of therapy intensity, the outcome data on aphasia treatment indicate that three sessions per week for five to six months (Basso, Capitani, & Vignolo, 1979) or eight to ten sessions per week for twelve weeks (Wertz et al., 1986) is the minimum amount of therapy needed to demonstrate significantly more improvement in treated patients than in untreated patients. For therapy to be intensive enough to maximize recovery, then, some amount over and above that minimum is needed. However one chooses to define “intensive therapy,” it may be that if there is an upper limit to the ideal level of intensity, no one has found it yet; a meta-analysis of studies including patients with aphasia from all etiologies yielded the general rule of thumb that “the more intensive the therapy, the greater the improvement” (Bhogal, Teasell, Foley, & Speechley, 2003). In short, although the precise number of presentations of a given stimulus required to yield consistent improvement of naming is not known, and although questions remain regarding just how 10 intensively delivered a treatment should be in order to maximize an individual’s potential for recovery (Off, Kavalier, & Rogers, 2008), “there appears to be evidence that intensive treatment is useful in both the short and long term” (Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). Regardless of findings such as these, aphasic patients typically do not receive therapy approaching levels that could reasonably be called “intensive.” Patients with aphasia who reside in the United States are likely to receive speech-language therapy for one hour per day, one to three days per week, over the course of three to four weeks (Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). This is a discouraging statistic, especially in light of the fact that therapy delivered for two hours per week for 24 weeks or less—a level of intensity “representative of clinical practice”—has been shown to result in no statistical difference in improvement between treated and untreated patients, and therefore to be “ineffective for most” aphasic patients (Lincoln et al., 1984). It has been argued that therapy delivered at this low level of intensity—that is, over a short course of time, or in a limited number of sessions—may benefit some patients but not others, and some authors have questioned whether the latter should even be subjected to therapy that “may have no benefit” when delivered at such low intensities (Wertz, 2000). Bhogal, Teasell, and Speechley (2003) concluded that “intensive aphasia therapy delivered over two to three months is critical to maximizing aphasia recovery, and failure to provide it potentially compromises individual outcomes.” Some authors have gone so far as to suggest that if aphasia therapy cannot be administered with sufficient intensity, it may not result in any improvement for individuals (Brindley, Copeland, Demain, & Martyn, 1989), and therefore perhaps ought not to be attempted at all (Wertz, 2000). Brindle et al. 11 (1989) posited that “it is only by radically reorganizing current provision or increasing the time allocated to speech therapists that their expertise can be effective in the field of chronic aphasia.” Confrontation naming and verbal repetition Next to intensity of treatment, the factor thought to exert the greatest influence on the results of efficacy studies is the type of task used during intervention (Bhogal, Teasell, & Speechley, 2003). Naming skills, the expressive verbal skills most frequently targeted by speech-language pathologists (Roseberry-McKibbin & Hegde, 2006), are usually treated through the elicitation of spoken words using a variety of methods, including confrontation naming tasks and verbal repetition (Raymer, 2005). While picture-naming tasks do provide a valid means of assessment of lexical retrieval, such tasks may not be therapeutic (Herbert et al., 2008). Even when such treatments appear to result in improved naming, it is not always clear “that the improvement reported in these experiments is a specific consequence of the particular treatment regimens applied” (Howard et al., 1985a). The difficulty in interpreting many studies examining aphasia therapy “is that it is unclear which, if any, of the many techniques that were used actually helped the patients to improve their naming; some may have been beneficial, others useless, or even…actually harmful; we simply do not know” (Howard et al., 1985b). Howard et al. (1985a) emphasized a distinction between prompts and facilitators, a distinction crucial to understanding the difference between immediate and long-term effects of therapy. By facilitation, the authors explained, “we mean one application of a single technique with a view to assessing its specific effects at some later time. If the 12 patient is significantly more likely to be able to name the appropriate object some minutes, hours or days after a specific treatment has been applied to the target word, we would say that facilitation has been successful” (Howard et al., 1985a). The authors stated that while a variety of prompting techniques, most notably phonemic cueing, had been shown to effectively enable aphasic patients to verbally produce a picture name which they had been previously unable to produce, there was no evidence of any longterm effects from such techniques. Patterson, Purell, and Morton (1983) found that the naming facilitation effects of phonemic cueing lasted for less than 30 minutes. Howard et al. (1985a) found similar effects for the technique of word repetition. “When an aphasic patient could not find a picture name, repeating it was an effective naming prompt if the picture was presented for naming again immediately afterwards,” the researchers reported. After about five minutes, however, “all benefit from having repeated the word had disappeared; the aphasic subjects were then no more likely to name the picture than if they had had no opportunity to repeat its name” (Howard et al., 1985a). In other words, the authors argued, effective prompts may not be effective longterm facilitators; the evidence supported the conclusion that repetition, phonemic cues, rhyme cues and rhyme judgments all work as prompts, but “are not facilitators in any lasting sense” (Howard et al., 1985b). Other authors have reported similar findings. Investigating the possibility that “simply saying the words was important” in successful traditional lexical therapy requiring the individual to look at a picture and produce the word, Nickels and Best (1996) found that evidence for this claim was inconclusive. Patterson et al. (1983) found that the benefits of producing words in repetition or in 13 response to phonological cues (in picture naming) were very short lasting, although Miceli et al. (1994) subsequently demonstrated that such a task could have long-lasting benefits. Nickels and Best (1996) concluded that “a more complex explanation” than mere practice in word production might be necessary to account for such benefits, an explanation requiring an understanding of the complex processes underlying naming. Lexical retrieval and semantic processing models One factor limiting the effectiveness of standard confrontation naming and verbal repetition tasks may be that they treat only the surface of the problem (explicit production, or articulation of the target), ignoring the psycholinguistic/semantic processes that underlie the process of naming. In other words, although object naming may seem on the surface to be a simple task, the underlying process of retrieving and formulating a word is extraordinarily complex (Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999) and there may be benefits to therapy focusing on an earlier stage of the word production process (e.g., lexical selection or semantic processing) rather than on the last or end stage of articulation. Guidelines for future research consistently “stress the need for evidencebased practice that is based on clinical experience integrated with theoretical rationale” (Davis & Farias, 2010), and authors have noted that psycholinguistic models of word production “are increasingly specific and provide a theoretical foundation for treatment interventions of this type” (Davis & Baynes, 2009). The training method used in the present study is based on psycholinguistic models in which at least two interrelated networks—lexical selection (often referred to as the semantic network) and form encoding (often referred to as the phonological network)—underlie word production, and 14 in which naming involves spread of activation in these two distributed and interrelated networks (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). Current models of spoken word production emphasize the identification of processing levels necessary for lexical access and single word retrieval (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010). Although details of the lexical model are the subject of debate (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Raymer, 2005), it is widely agreed that at least two lexical stages or levels of representation, semantic and phonological, are critical to the process of word retrieval that occurs as part of normal language output (Caramazza, 2000; Raymer, 2005) and that speakers with aphasia make errors that arise at either or both of these levels (Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). The “semantic processing level governs operations involved in lexical selection of an intended concept” while the “phonological processing level governs operations involved in its phonological specification” (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010). Psycholinguistic stage models such as that proposed by Levelt (see Figure 1) postulate discrete movement from stage to stage, involving both lexical selection (semantic level) and form encoding (phonological level) (Davis et al., 2006). In this type of model, naming is primarily a serial or sequential process in which “feed-forward” patterns of spreading activation result in a “top-down” flow of information moving in one direction only (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999). The speaker begins with a conceptual preparation or communicative intention, and then proceeds to lexical selection (retrieving a word, or more specifically a lemma, or “package of syntactic information” about a word, from the mental lexicon) in a high-speed process that is “surprisingly robust” in normal speakers, 15 allowing for the retrieval of two to three words per second (Level et al., 1999). Only once the syntactic word or lemma has been selected can the speaker “cross the rift” between the lexical selection (conceptual/semantic/syntactic) domain to the form encoding (phonological/articulatory) domain, a crossing whose momentary failure (occurring when a lemma has been selected, but perhaps with insufficiently strong activation, leaving the speaker unable to retrieve the corresponding word form) is known as “tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon (Levelt et al., 1999). Levelt’s model is especially useful in helping to conceptualize the steps that must occur before a word is articulated, and how a speaker making a lexical selection without proceeding to the final step of articulation may, with the assistance of the self-monitoring process, or “internal monitoring loop” (Levelt, 1983,1993) that occurs during inner speech, obtain the semantic-level practice necessary to increase spread of activation while preventing speech errors. According to the author, “as long as the buffered internal speech has not been articulated,” the brief interruption in the process caused by the speaker checking the accuracy of his or her speech as it passes through the internal monitoring loop “can prevent the production of an error” (Levelt, 1993). Levelt’s model assumes “competition, but no inhibition” of competitors during lexical selection, although node selection is “subject to competition” from distractors or competitors (Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999) in what is sometimes referred to as the “semanticinhibition effect” (Levelt, 1999). When you are naming a picture of a sheep and you decide to go for the basic level term, you will activate the lexical concept sheep as your target and activation 16 spreads to the corresponding lemma. In the semantic network activation spreads to related concepts, such as goat and llama. They, in turn, spread activation to their lemmas. . . . In other words, there is competition between semanticallyrelated lemmas. Active alternatives slow down the selection process . . . If you present the semantically related word goat as a distracter, the already-activated lemma goat will receive an additional boost, thereby becoming a strong competitor to sheep. By contrast, if you present a semantically unrelated word, such as chair, as a distracter, there will be no convergence of activation and, correspondingly, competition will be relatively weak. That explains the semanticinhibition effect. (Levelt, 1999, p. 228) Other models (Dell, 1986; Dell & O’Seaghda, 1992) describe a similar process, but assume a more cascaded and continuous flow of information and “without any need for a particular meaning or lexical item to be selected before the next level initiates processing” (Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). Recent intensive production-free treatment investigations have concentrated on the semantic level of processing, assuming “an interactive account” of naming, with intra-level activation and bi-directionality of inhibition and excitation between levels (Davis et al., 2006). This model of naming assumes a continuous information transmission whereby activation (both excitation and inhibition) occurs throughout the network in a generalized top-down manner (Humphreys, Price, & Riddock, 1999). The process has been described this way: Each stage consists of processing the stored structural, semantic, and/or phonological aspects of the target. Structural characteristics are the perceptual 17 aspects of the picture or object, such as color, shape, and size; semantic characteristics are the associative and categorical aspects of the object; and phonological characteristics are the sound representations. At each level of the processing network, there are excitatory and inhibitory connections to related items that are co-activated by the object. The strength of the activation of the target must exceed its semantic competitors to successfully go to the next level and reach phonological encoding. If the target is not substantially stronger than its semantic or phonological alternatives, then a competitor will achieve activation and hence an error will be produced. (Davis et al., 2006, p. 62) The amount of activation depends on two factors: the strength of connections among units and the rate by which activation decays. After a period of time, the most activated word is selected (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010). Naming errors are thought to occur at either level, when a competitor achieves higher activation than the target and/or is insufficiently inhibited, and is therefore inappropriately selected for production (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Davis & Farias, 2010). Production-free, semanticlevel interventions are based on improvements of reaction time and accuracy during semantic and phonological priming, a benefit believed to arise from activation of neural networks and well-documented in fMRI studies (Ruff, Blumstein, Myers, & Hutchinson, 2008). The use of a lexical entry—even a use that precludes explicit articulation of the name of the target—is assumed to result in its being ‘primed’; that is, the activation threshold needed to produce the word will be temporarily lowered, increasing the 18 likelihood of an accurate verbal response during subsequent naming attempts (Howard et al., 1985a). Recent approaches to intervention rest on the assumption that a widely distributed, interactive semantic network supports naming, and that a neurological injury such as a stroke or TBI can cause interruptions in this network, which in turn disrupts naming (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Davis & Farias, 2010; Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006; Drew & Thompson, 1999; Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Raymer, 2005). According to Levelt, “activation spreading through a semantic network” is “the obvious explanation for semantic naming errors, the dominant speech error type” for individuals with aphasia (Levelt, 1999). It has been posited that an intervention that seeks to improve speech production by implementing semantic level treatment may have “the best chance of restoring or redirecting the flow of activation” (Davis et al., 2006). In addition, semantic-based treatment, when combined with a production-free approach, allows for exploitation of the normal process of inner speech monitoring, a necessary precursor to self-correction and self-monitoring of overt speech for increased accuracy of speech production and the avoidance of explicitly articulated naming errors. 19 L E X I C A L S E L E C T I O N F O R M E N C O D I N G Figure 1. Outline of Levelt’s (1999) theory. Preparing to produce a spoken word “proceeds through stages of conceptual preparation, lexical selection, morphological and phonological encoding, and phonetic encoding before articulation can be initiated” (Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999). Output monitoring, or self-monitoring of inner speech, occurs prior to overt articulation in a process which involves the speaker’s normal speech comprehension mechanism (Levelt, et al., 1999) and which can be exploited to allow for mental practice without the possibility of unintended error practice during implicit therapy interventions. Semantic-based intervention The semantic system (the store of meanings and information an individual has learned about words, objects, or actions) is known to play a common role in both word comprehension and word retrieval (Raymer, 2005). Numerous studies suggest that semantic-based interventions can result in improvements in naming ability (Coelho, 20 McHugh, & Boyle, 2000; Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006; Drew & Thompson, 1999; Howard, Patterson, Franklin, Orchard-Lisle, & Morton, 1985a, 1985b; Kiran, 2008; Pring et al., 1990; Scott, 1987), even in patients with good semantic processing (Nickels & Best, 1996) and patients whose deficits are partly or primarily phonological rather than semantic (Raymer, 2005). Dukette, Baynes, Redfern, Share, Ludy, and Dronkers (1998), after grouping participants into those whose errors were primarily phonological in nature and those whose errors were primarily semantic in nature and exposing both groups to both phonological and semantic therapy, found that while phonological training was of greatest benefit to participants with more phonological errors, semantic training was effective for both groups. The reasons for this may have to do with semantic processing and its role in the larger process underlying naming. Although there is no single agreedupon model of semantic processing (Behrmann & Lieberthal, 1989; Nickels & Best, 1996), it is helpful to recall that the process underlying object naming is thought to involve a continuous information transmission, wherein activation, both excitation and inhibition, occurs throughout the semantic network in a generalized top-down fashion (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Davis et al., 2006; Kiran, 2008). The fundamental assumption of semantic-based treatment has been “the spreading activation theory of semantic processing” which holds that “by activating the semantic network surrounding the target, the target itself may be activated above its ‘threshold’ level, increasing the probability that its name can be retrieved” (Coelho, McHugh, & Boyle, 2000). One reason semantic therapy may work to increase naming even for patients with primarily phonologic deficits may be “that strengthening access to semantic attributes results in facilitation of target 21 semantic nodes at the semantic level, which cascades downstream to the phonological representations, thereby strengthening phonological nodes as well” (Kiran, 2008). Also, as some authors have noted, semantic interventions are thought to be potentially advantageous based on the assumption that the neural network supporting semantic knowledge is more widely distributed than the one supporting phonological knowledge (Davis & Baynes, 2009). In contrast to confrontation naming or repetition tasks, wherein a patient may be asked simply to verbally produce the name of the target, semantic training refers to “techniques that require the patient to process the meaning” corresponding to the name (Howard et al., 1985b). Howard et al. (1985a), noting that semantic facilitation effects last longer than the effects of naming tasks providing phonemic cueing, argued that the longer-lasting effects of the semantic therapy were due to “priming of a word-specific lexical semantic representation”; in other words, access to semantics in the comprehension task primed a lemma representation, and this primed lemma was then more easily accessible when the picture was subsequently presented for naming” (Howard, 2000). The finding that “a single semantic task could improve the accessibility of a target word for at least 24 hours, in contrast to phonological techniques, which have only short-lasting effects, prompted the first systematic use of these tasks in treatment” (Howard, 2000). It has been posited that tasks which require a search in semantic memory have greater long-term effects than the relatively automatic production of a word in response to a cliché cue (Cohen et al., 1979; Howard et al., 1985a; 1985b). Nickels and Best (1996) hypothesized that semantic-based therapy may work either by facilitating 22 general semantic processes or through the use of a specific strategy (perhaps unconscious) of exploring the semantics of an item, which, when naming fails, may facilitate retrieval of that item. Many different techniques have been considered semantic interventions, including auditory or written word-to-picture matching, answering yes/no questions about the target, picture and spoken word categorization, and judging relatedness of target words to a series of pictures (Drew & Thompson, 1999; Raymer, 2005). One technique “that characterizes the general philosophy” behind many semantic-based aphasia treatments is semantic feature analysis, or SFA (Nickels, 2002). Although SFA applications differ across studies in terms of precise task requirements (Boyle, 2001), the general procedure is that the patient is first provided with a picture and asked to name it, and is then encouraged to describe the semantic features of the target, for example listing its group, use, action, properties, location and association (Coelho, McHugh, & Boyle, 2000; Nickels, 2002). A “matrix of printed cue words” regarding the object’s semantic features may also be presented along with the target picture, to assist in the process (Raymer, 2005). Boyle (2001), in a review of four studies employing SFA, reported that SFA training, when provided at sufficient intensity, led to improved naming of trained pictures and generalization to some untrained pictures. Coelho et al. (2000), using SFA in an intervention designed “to increase the activation in the semantic network,” posited that “by activating the semantic network surrounding the target word, that word may be activated above its threshold, thereby facilitating retrieval.” The authors reported that SFA resulted in improved confrontation naming on trained and untrained items, and was 23 associated with modest improvements in measures of connected speech, with naming accuracy improved at a two-month follow-up (Coelho, et al., 2000). Hillis (1991) used a similar technique, in which the participant was first asked to name a picture, and then, if the naming attempt resulted in a semantic error, a drawing of the error was made and compared to the target picture, after which semantic differences between the two were discussed. The treatment resulted in improvements in word comprehension as well as in both verbal and written naming (Hillis, 1991). Semantic interventions have differed as to whether the participants’ exposure was purely semantic or whether the target word form (either spoken or written) was provided at any point during training. Although some authors have noted an advantage for purely semantic treatment over phonological treatment that provides the patient with information about the phonological form of a name (Howard et al., 1985b), evidence from other studies has indicated that semantic treatment that includes at least some aspects of phonologic treatment (e.g., provision of the spoken or written word form) may be most efficacious (Baynes, Share, & Redfern, 1995; Boyle & Coelho, 1996; Drew & Thompson, 1999; Howard et al., 1985a; Le Dorze, Boulay, Gaudreau, & Brassard, 1994; Thompson et al., 1986). Le Dorze et al. (1994) compared the effects of a purely semantic technique (a semantic comprehension task without the word form) with a “formalsemantic facilitation technique” in which information about the spoken or written word form of the target was provided. The researchers found that while there was no change in naming for items treated with the purely semantic technique, naming improved significantly for items treated with the formal-semantic technique. They posited that 24 “inclusion of word forms in the semantic tasks is a critical element” of naming facilitation techniques (Le Dorze et al., 1994). In a study conducted by Drew and Thompson (1999), semantic treatment for a group of four Broca’s aphasics with identified semantic-level deficits consisted of a sorting task, a yes/no judgment task, and a definition-to-picture matching task, with no explicit naming, no orthography, and “no phonological information made available to participants” during treatment. Results were mixed; two of the participants showed treatment effects and two did not. When additional semantic treatment including the word form was provided, the two participants who had not shown effects for the semantic treatment showed immediate improvement in picture naming, whereas the two who had already demonstrated semantic treatment effects showed a further increase in picture naming. The authors concluded that further research was necessary to “clarify the differences between pure semantic treatments and those that include the word form or their additive effects” (Drew & Thompson, 1999). Howard (2000), considering evidence showing that patients with the least semantic impairment made the most improvement, noted that if semantic therapies were effective at a purely semantic level, “the treatment should be primarily effective for subjects with a semantic impairment.” He put forth the hypothesis that so-called “semantic therapy” may work not on a semantic level as previously assumed, but instead by strengthening “the mapping from semantics to phonology,” or in other words, through simultaneous activation of semantics and phonology. He noted that this hypothesis (supported by “abundant” evidence that normal subjects automatically activate the phonological representations of words when comprehending the words in written form) 25 provides a simple explanation for the fact that semantic therapy treatment effects tend to be item-specific rather than generalizing to untreated items (Howard, 2000). Semantic treatments have generally yielded improvements, however (Howard et al., 1985b; Nickels, 2002), and in some cases have not only resulted in generalization to untrained items (Boyle & Coelho, 1985; Coelho, McHugh, & Boyle, 2000) but also in generalization to connected speech (Coelho et al., 2000; Davis, Harrington & Baynes, 2006). Boyle and Coelho (1995), applying semantic feature analysis treatment with a Broca’s aphasic, found that generalization to untreated items occurred when as few as seven exemplars were treated, although the authors noted that “whether such a small number of exemplars is sufficient for other subjects with aphasia is not clear.” In a replication of that study, Coelho et al. (2000) found that generalization to untreated items was noted to be more substantial when only a small number of exemplars were trained. In some cases, semantic treatments have resulted in impressive speech production improvements for patients who had previously failed to improve despite long exposure to therapy. Jones (1986) used a semantic intervention to improve sentence production in a patient with non-fluent aphasia whose single word output had remained unchanged for six years despite long-standing, intensive speech therapy. The author concluded that previous therapy may have been ineffective because it had focused on “the more superficial aspects” of the patient’s deficit, “ignoring the fundamental problem in mapping meaning relations between semantics and syntax” (Jones, 1986). The most important factor determining the success of a semantic intervention may be the precise type of task and its requirements. Howard, Patterson et al. (1985a) noted 26 that although many “schools of aphasia therapy assume that eliciting a response from an aphasic patient makes it more likely that the patient will subsequently be able to produce that response without the help of a therapist,” the evidence suggests that “the true picture is more complex; it is crucially important how responses are elicited.” Nickels and Best (1996) observed that “not every ‘semantic’ task can be expected to produce the same results,” and that semantic therapy “clearly cannot be considered a unitary treatment; slight changes in the nature of the task and its presentation may radically affect the effects of the therapy on naming.” The authors concluded that the “form of the semantic task influences the outcome; one semantic task may not be as good as any other” (Nickels & Best, 1996). Semantic interventions that use word-to-picture matching tasks or otherwise employ pictures during training may produce greater improvements in naming than other types of interventions (Nickels, 2002; Raymer & Ellsworth, 2002). Word-picture matching tasks have been demonstrated to produce effects lasting as long as a year in some studies, including small but significant improvement for untreated items that were semantically related to treated items (Pring, White-Thompson, Pounds, Marshall, & Davis, 1990). Word-picture matching tasks may have an advantage due to the changes in processing associated with the addition of picture stimuli to purely auditory or auditoryorthographic stimuli. When a patient is presented with a picture to name, “activation cascades through the lexical system, engaging visual, semantic, and phonogic information relevant to the picture” (Nickels, 2001). Some authors have suggested that there might be “specifically visual or pictorial semantics which is at least partially 27 independent from the verbal semantic system” (Howard et al., 1985a), which may help explain why the meaning of a word is more likely to be activated during a repetition task if the repetition is carried out in the presence of a picture (Whitworth, Webster, & Howard, 2005). Semantically-based tasks requiring word-to-picture matching are “often very effective,” and their use “is justified in that they stand a very good chance of working for the maximum number of people” (Best & Nickels, 2000). Raymer and Ellsworth (2002), in a comparison of semantic interventions with repetition treatments designed to improve verb retrieval, concluded that “the presence of the picture stimulus” used in all treatment conditions may have been the “key factor” that influenced treatment outcomes. The authors posited that their positive findings for all treatments may have been related to “semantic activation that occurs whenever a word is retrieved in the context of picture presentation, thereby fundamentally altering semantic activation patterns and making the word more easily accessible in subsequent retrieval attempts.” They concluded that different treatments could be equally effective in improving verb naming abilities in some individuals “as long as training takes place in the context of pictured targets” (Raymer & Ellsworth, 2002). Achieving significant improvements in naming ability may require more than just inclusion of picture stimuli, however. Howard et al. (1985a) found that the tasks which resulted in the most substantial and long-lasting improvements in naming ability were “comprehension tasks requiring the subject to access a semantic representation corresponding to a picture name,” although the authors hypothesized that the key factor affecting outcomes may have been the requirement of making semantic judgments, rather 28 than the influence of the visual stimuli itself. Along with semantic judgments, the type and/or number of pictures presented has been shown to be of potential importance in the success of anomia investigations incorporating picture stimuli. Nickels & Best (1996) found long-lasting improvement in the naming abilities of several patients with semantic therapy using written word-to-picture matching, with each training set consisting of one target picture and three other pictures representing semantic distractors. Using this technique, the authors found significant improvements in naming in treated items and some evidence of generalization to untreated items, although only the treated items showed long-lasting effects of therapy (Nickels & Best, 1996). The authors concluded that perhaps “it is the use of a task where four semantically related pictures are presented which is important” (Nickels & Best, 1996). Investigations into adult brain plasticity elicited by anomia treatment using picture stimuli have resulted in similar conclusions. Cornelissen et al. (2003) showed improved naming accompanied by increased lefthemisphere activation in all participants using a contextual priming technique wherein target items (e.g., a cow) were presented along with other semantically related items (e.g., a larger set of farm animals) rather than in isolation. According to the authors, “the semantic network representing domestic animals is activated because the picture of the cow is presented in the context of other semantically related items. This enhanced activity in the semantic network facilitates transfer of activity from the semantic to the phonological stage, and naming may succeed” (Cornelissen et al., 2003). One potential problem with word-picture matching tasks, although they are often classified as “error-reducing” (Davis & Baynes, 2009), is that they usually involve the 29 requirement of explicit verbal production of the target. This requirement is not unique to word-picture tasks, of course; most standard speech therapy approaches for aphasia require verbal responses from the individual (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Raymer, 2005), and repeated verbal practice is “inherent to nearly all anomia treatment protocols” (Off, Kavalier, & Rogers, 2008). For patients whose verbal errors are prevalent, this can result in the unintended consequence of the patient actually “practicing” his or her errors (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Nickels & Best, 1996), or, in effect, inadvertently “practicing a mistake” (Davis, Harrington & Baynes, 2006). As Turkstra and Bourgeois (2005) have speculated, the re-learning of speech and language in adults with acquired neurological disorders may engage communication processes known to be especially vulnerable to the effects of errors. The authors concluded that increased “attention to errors may benefit patients” with acquired neurological disorders (Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). Errorless and error-reducing approaches Errorless learning has a long history. Grounded in research on neural plasticity (Hebb, 1961), the success of errorless learning approaches has been linked to increases in synaptic strength (Frattali & Kang, 2004). Although errorless learning was first proven successful in animal research, a number of errorless learning approaches have since been shown to significantly improve learning in patients with amnesia and other memory impairments (Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). One reason for the success of errorless learning approaches may be that they reduce feelings of frustration or discouragement that might result from more errorful training, and correspondingly increase the individual’s feelings of success (Davis & Farias, 2010). As has often been noted, therapy 30 designed to ensure a certain level of success appears to help sustain patient motivation (Howard et al., 1985a; Webster & Gordon, 2009). Whatever the mechanisms behind the success of such therapy, recent evidence suggests that errorless learning approaches can benefit everyone, even young, non-neurologically impaired individuals (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994). Such approaches may therefore be effective with a wide variety of patients requiring speech and language services, “particularly when teaching changes in articulation, specific sentence structures, the use of compensatory strategies, or augmentative device use” (Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). Errorless learning approaches have been shown to help remediate deficits for individuals with aphasic disorders, particularly anomia (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; McKissock & Ward, 2007), and have been shown in neuroimaging studies to result in neural activation patterns more consistent with activation patterns observed in normal controls (Fillingham, Hodgson, Sage, & Lambon Ralph, 2003; Jackson, Lafleur, et al., 2003). In traditional therapy, patients who do not know the answer to a question are typically “encouraged to guess or learn via trial-and-error” (McKissock & Ward, 2007), a condition which researchers have described as “errorful” (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994). Errorless learning, by contrast, is based on the principle that “errors generate errors” (Brookshire, 1979)—in other words, that the mere production of an incorrect response may be self-reinforcing, making the incorrect response more likely to occur on subsequent occasions (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; McKissock & Ward, 2007; Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). Because of this self-reinforcing tendency of errant behavior, the probability of correct responses decreases with each error production (Brookshire, 1979). 31 The opposite also appears to be true; Frattali and Kang (2004) note that “if a stimulus elicits an errorless response, [errorless] learning will strengthen the tendency to activate the same pattern of response on subsequent occasions.” Treatment is therefore more likely to be successful if patients are not only assisted in providing correct responses but also prevented from reinforcing their own errors (Brookshire, 2007; Frattali, 2004). Although the definition for “errorless” (sometimes also called “error elimination”) methods varies depending on the source, the term is generally used to refer to any type of treatment or approach whereby the task is manipulated to eliminate or reduce errors (Fillingham et al., 2003; McKissock & Ward, 2007). Fillingham et al. (2003) noted that although the aphasic literature contained no examples of purely errorless learning methods, some studies had shown that anomia could be successfully treated using errorreducing techniques. Techniques described as “errorless” or “error-reducing” typically consist of the clinician verbally providing the participant with the correct response to an item before asking them to verbally provide the response themselves (Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005) during tasks such as confrontation naming (e.g., “I’m going to show you a picture, then I’m going to say the name of the object, then I want you to say the name of the object”), or by otherwise lowering the difficulty level of the task to the point that errors become very unlikely (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; Fillingham et al., 2003). Researchers have noted several problems with such techniques. Because such tasks “are unlike real language use” and because “errorless paradigms can seem inherently boring” to patient, clinician, or both (Fillingham et al., 2003), they may fail to pose sufficient challenge, and therefore fail to fully engage the patient’s focus. The best 32 condition for learning is one that prevents errors while also requiring effort on the part of the learner (Komatsu et al., 2000). A lack of sufficient task difficulty may have been a factor in studies demonstrating that errorless learning, though it improved naming in the short term, was shown to have limited long-term benefit (McKissock & Ward, 2007). An even more basic problem is that whether or not the target production is modeled/ provided by the clinician in advance, in most “errorless” or “error-reducing” interventions, the patient is still required to verbally produce the response (Fillingham et al., 2003; Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005); logically, then, despite claims that “by providing [the patient with] a correct response at the outset, [errors] can be avoided” (McKissock & Ward, 2007), the potential for errors of speech production still exists. Although some authors have classified tasks in which the patient is provided with a whole-word cue (in effect being given the answer to the question before being required to answer it) as “error eliminating” (McKissock & Ward, 2007), the reality is that for many patients, even simple word repetition tasks (“Repeat after me; say _____”) can be fraught with the potential for error (Davis & Baynes, 2009). The fact that “errorless” therapies have traditionally focused on eliminating errors of word retrieval rather than errors of speech production may help explain the surprising and counterintuitive results of “errorless-vs.errorful” treatment studies, wherein some authors have concluded that “there is little compelling evidence to suggest that errorless learning . . . outperforms errorful learning in the treatment of anomia” and that “it does not matter whether anomic patients are allowed to make errors” (McKissock & Ward, 2007). 33 The success of “errorless” interventions may depend upon “matching the type of training to the nature of patients’ errors” for maximum effect (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Because they only target errors at the level of word retrieval (rather than speech production), many “errorless” or “error-reducing” treatment methods ignore the possibility that for many patients, the most harmful, self-reinforcing errors (and therefore the errors arguably most in need of reduction or elimination) may be errors of speech production. It would appear that the way to eliminate or reduce speech production errors is to focus not on simplifying the word-retrieval task, but on changing the task so as to eliminate the requirement of speech production in patient responses. Errors of speech production, such as verbal and literal paraphasias and perseverations, “are pervasive in aphasia and can interfere with treatment” (Davis, Harrington & Baynes, 2006). When an individual with prevalent naming or speech production errors is required to give verbal responses, one consequence is “the unavoidable production of errors, which may inadvertently strengthen unwanted neural-motor pathways” (Davis, Farias, Baynes, Chand, & Orgun, 2007). According to Veterans Affairs and Department of Defense clinical practice guidelines, the goals of post-stroke rehabilitation include not only helping to maximize recovery of communication abilities, but also to help “prevent learning of ineffective or inappropriate compensatory behaviors” (U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs, 2003). Surely, in the case of a patient for whom verbal errors are prevalent and represent a significant component of the communication impairment, such behaviors might reasonably be supposed to include the production of self-reinforcing verbal errors, to whatever degree such production can be avoided during therapy. 34 Recent evidence suggests that performing semantic- based tasks which are free of the requirement of spoken responses (and which thereby eliminate many, if not most, potentially self-reinforcing verbal errors) can yield improvements for patients, especially those for whom verbal errors are especially prevalent or frustrating (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). Nickels and Best (1996) noted that “large and robust facilitating effects” of semantic processing therapy had been found even for tasks with no requirement of speech production of the target during training, and concluded that for some patients at least, explicit production of the target appeared to be unnecessary. Howard et al. (1985a) found that semantic-based therapy resulted in “dramatically improved accessibility of the name that lasts for at least 24 hours,” even though such techniques did not require patients to articulate the names of the pictures. In a type of target-production-free (or at least production-reducing) intervention described as “error-reducing” by some authors (McKissock & Ward, 2007), Frattali and Kang (2004) asked their anomic patients to discuss a picture without naming it, a task which was shown to be beneficial relative to an untreated set. Studies have shown that half an hour after successfully producing the picture name in response to a phonemic cue, an aphasic patient is no more likely to be able to name the same picture than if it had not been cued (Patterson, Purell, et al., 1983). If, on the other hand, the patient is simply asked to point to that picture out of a choice of four, he is “substantially more likely to be able to name the picture half an hour later (or, indeed, 24 hours later) than if he had not been given this treatment” (Howard et al., 1985a). 35 Implicit therapy Semantic interventions that rely on judgments and do not require explicit naming are thought to provide activation of the semantic networks that support naming “while minimizing the introduction of errors” (Davis, Harrington & Baynes, 2006). This type of speech therapy that does not require spoken responses is often referred to in the literature as “implicit therapy.” The approach is said to be ‘implicit’ in that “it requires no verbal response, in contrast to most traditional treatment methods” (Davis & Baynes, 2009) and in contrast to “standard techniques which require patients to speak as part of their treatment” (Davis & Farias, 2010). Implicit interventions are based on the same principles as errorless or error-reducing methods, but with the added benefit of specifically reducing or eliminating precisely those types of errors—namely, speech production errors—which are often most problematic and most self-reinforcing for aphasic clients (Davis & Baynes, 2009). While most error-reducing methods require explicit speech production (Fillingham et al. 2003), implicit methods use “semantic and phonological judgment tasks in the absence of explicit naming to produce learning with fewer errors than traditional interventions” (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Implicit interventions have been effective in patients with a wide range of disorders, and have been shown to improve naming in patients with aphasia (Baynes, Share, et al., 1995; Dukette, Baynes, et al., 1998; Davis, Harrington, et al., 2006). Researchers “have continued to develop this implicit method and to apply it to a broader range of aphasic disorders at the level of single case studies” (Davis & Baynes, 2009). 36 Investigators employing implicit methods acknowledge that their ongoing use of the term “implicit” is somewhat problematic, and that it is ongoing mainly due to lack of a more precise (yet concise) alternative. Although the term “implicit therapy” is known to its primary researchers and practitioners to refer to training which “involves a mental practice that uses the prearticulatory ‘inner speech’ mechanism and a spread of activation into other related verb/noun/tactile event nodes,” there are “no one or two words” sufficiently descriptive to do it justice (C. Davis, personal communication, March 18, 2010). Referring to implicit interventions as “interventions requiring non-verbal responses” lacks elegance as well as specificity regarding type of task (e.g., semantic decision-based), and although “error-reducing” might be a partial solution to the question of what to call this type of therapy, “errorless” is, strictly speaking, inaccurate (K. Baynes, personal communication, March 15, 2010). Acronyms such as NoVERT (Non-Verbal Error Reducing Therapy), REST (Reduced Error Semantic Therapy), and REPT (Reduced Error Phonological Therapy) have been proposed and used as alternative terms in some studies (K. Baynes, personal communication, July 25, 2010). However, since these acronyms do not specify the type of error (verbal) that is being reduced, and since the term “implicit” has received increasingly widespread use in the research community to refer to interventions that do not require explicit articulation of target responses (Wambaugh, 2010), the terms “production-free” and “implicit” are used interchangeably in the present study to refer to therapy tasks free of the requirement of speech production. 37 Part of the problem with the “implicit therapy” label is that it can be confused with the term “implicit learning,” which refers to learning that occurs outside conscious awareness (Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). Implicit therapy, by contrast, occurs with the patient’s full conscious awareness, explicit consent, and active participation. The importance of the patient’s understanding both the particular task requirements and the overall goals of therapy is undisputed in the implicit therapy literature. This may have contributed to the success of many implicit interventions conducted to date, since evidence suggests that greater transparency between therapy task and goals of improved speech production may increase the efficacy of intervention (Webster & Gordon, 2009). Implicit therapy does share one important characteristic of implicit learning, however. A key feature of implicit learning is that it is “probabilistic,” meaning that “the probability of learning a new association is dependent on the relative frequency of correct responses versus errors,” and therefore, for optimal learning, tasks should be designed to minimize errors (Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). This focus on error reduction is also a key feature, and in fact forms the foundation, of implicit therapy methods (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Mental practice and inner speech Implicit therapy interventions reduce production errors by discouraging speech while requiring intensive “mental practice” during rehabilitation (Davis, Farias, Baynes, Chand, & Orgun, 2007). Mental Practice, mental imagery, and implicit practice are used to describe techniques in which an individual thinks about performing a task but does not actually execute the task. Implicit techniques have been used widely in physical rehabilitation, and have demonstrated that the neural substrates that underlie motor tasks 38 are activated during implicit practice and imagery (Braun, Beurskens, Borm, Schack, & Wade, 2006; Harrington, Farias, & Davis, 2009; Jackson, Lafleur, Malouin, Richards, & Doyon, 2003). During mental practice, “an internal representation of the movement is activated and the execution of the movement is repeatedly simulated, without physical activity” (Braun et al., 2006). Recent evidence suggests that motor imagery training may help access the motor network independently of recovery, increasing motor recovery after stroke (Sharma, Pomeroy, & Baron, 2006), even in patients with complete spinal cord injury (Cramer, Orr, Cohen, & Lacourse, 2007). Authors have noted that although these techniques have received much more attention in the fields of sports psychology and physical and occupational therapy (Davis, Farias et al., 2007), mental practice is increasingly being acknowledged as a technique that “may need to be considered” as a complementary treatment technique in the field of speech pathology (Van der Merwe, 2007). Recent studies have shown that both aphasia and apraxia of speech may be well suited to this type of intervention, for reasons related to the process of pre-articulatory editing of inner speech that occurs prior to overt speech. Pre-articulatory editing, the process of detecting and suppressing speech errors prior to overt speech production, “may be viewed as a form of self-monitoring of overt speech” using the inner speech loop (Davis, Farias, & Baynes, 2009). Levelt, Roelofs, and Meyer (1999) hypothesized that “the person to whom we listen most is ourself” and that “just as we can detect trouble in our interlocutor’s speech, we can discover errors, dysfluencies, or other problems of delivery” in our own inner speech, as well as in our own overt speech. Levelt et al. (1999) claimed that inner speech is produced in essentially 39 the same way that we speak, but without the final stage of overt articulation. Selfmonitoring of inner speech occurs after speech preparation but prior to articulation, as the inner speech is “heard” by the speaker through an inner loop that transmits the speech plan at the phonological level (Levelt, 1983, 1989). This inner loop feeds the constructed phonological word back into the speech comprehension system for monitoring. The process of pre-articulatory editing allows the speaker to mentally practice speech production while internally monitoring his or her planned utterances for errors, thereby “allowing a correction prior to speech production” instead of executing a speech error, which could then make the error much more likely to occur on subsequent attempts (Davis, Farias, & Baynes, 2009). It is possible that resource allocation also plays a part in the success of implicit interventions, since the practice these interventions afford is solely mental, and therefore free of the additional effort required for physical articulation. Davis and Farias (2010) suggested that “aphasic individuals can benefit from practice in semantic-lexical or phonological decision-making when they are free from the additional burden of actual word production.” Apraxia of speech (AOS) is generally agreed to be a breakdown in the motor programming or motor planning process of speech production. Even so, “traditional approaches to treatment of AOS have incorporated motor execution into therapeutic activities despite the consensus that the breakdown probably occurs prior to speech execution” (Davis, Farias, Baynes, Chand, & Orgun, 2007). In a review of apraxia treatment studies conducted through 2003, the Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences (ANCDS) found that one commonality observed across all 40 investigations was motoric practice of speech targets, or in other words, “verbal production was requisite” in all studies (Wambaugh, Duffy, McNeil, Robin, & Rogers, 2006). New studies, however, have provided evidence that phonological awareness training can yield positive results in the absence of overt production, and that implicit therapy targeting phoneme manipulation can result in improved speech production for patients with AOS (Davis, Farias, Bord, & Baynes, 2006). Farias, Davis, and Whittmann (2009) used an implicit intervention requiring intensive mental practice with phoneme manipulation tasks to improve consonant blend production in an individual with a primary diagnosis of AOS. The authors, who posited that the patient’s concurrent improvements in auditory discrimination may have been “a byproduct of mental practice,” concluded that their findings supported “the use of mental practice as a complementary technique to overt practice.” Treatment for AOS, the authors noted, “is well suited to mental practice, as it requires the individual to focus on inner speech, a necessary precursor to self-correction and self-monitoring for accurate overt speech” (Farias, Davis, et al., 2009). In another case study, Davis, Farias, and Baynes (2009) found that intensive therapy requiring implicit phoneme manipulation improved speech sound production and resulted in significant generalization to untreated words for a patient with mild aphasia whose primary speech disturbance was AOS. Neuroimaging studies have supported these findings; phoneme manipulation tasks performed mentally, without speech production, have been shown to activate regions of the brain known to be activated during motor planning and programming (Davis, Farias, & Wilson, 2011). New ANCDS AOS Treatment Guidelines, currently being updated based on evidence obtained 41 from studies conducted since the last review in 2006, will include implicit therapy evidence (Shuster, 2011; Wambaugh, 2010). Implicit therapy interventions incorporating intensive mental practice have been used as supplementary techniques in the rehabilitation of patients with a wide variety of disorders. Baynes, Truong, Jonathan, Farias, and Davis (2008) employed an implicit method in combination with explicit therapy requiring verbal responses to improve the reading of grammatical morphemes in a patient with phonological alexia. The patient, a mildly anomic aphasic with moderate alexia, was highly motivated to improve his impaired reading ability so that he could read to his child. He began each session by explicitly reading aloud a set of probe words, but also practiced reading using a computerized implicit intervention. During implicit training, each trained word was presented visually on the computer screen for 500 ms, followed by an auditory presentation of a word that either matched the presented word or not; the participant’s task was to respond nonverbally by pressing a button indicating “match” or “no match.” Foils were unrelated, phonologically related, or related in both sound and word class (e.g., for target him, foils were cash, hem, and his). The participant took the computer home in between sessions for additional unsupervised practice. Results indicated that the combined explicit and implicit training increased accuracy of word retrieval, although improvement appeared to be limited to reading fluency (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Development of the implicit method for treatment of aphasia Implicit investigations to date have provided compelling evidence to suggest the potential usefulness for this method in the treatment of individuals with aphasia. Howard 42 et al. (1985a, 1985b) demonstrated that naming improvements were greatest and persisted longest when patients with semantic deficits were given semantic-based therapy tasks that did not require explicit naming. Training requiring pointing responses was more effective and the effects of training persisted longer than the effects of more traditional speech rehabilitation methods, such as confrontation naming, phonemic cueing, or repetition. Baynes, Share, et al. (1995) designed a study modeled on the semantic decision-making approach used by Howard et al. (1985a). During treatment, patients saw the training pictures embedded in a set of three distractor pictures on a computer screen. A verbal question required categorical, featural, or associative knowledge for a correct response. Pointing responses were requested and naming of the items was discouraged. Results strongly supported the efficacy of implicit training, both for trained items and in generalization to untrained items as demonstrated by improved naming on the Boston Naming Test (BNT). The authors found these results especially encouraging due to the fact that the training was not intensive; sessions were relatively brief and were only provided two days per week (Baynes, Share, et al., 1995) The impetus for the development of current implicit methods also grew out of the observation made by Baynes, Wessinger, Fendrich, and Gazzaniga (1993), that the previously mute right hemisphere of a callosotomy patient gained the ability to produce verbal responses to words and pictures after several months of steady participation in experiments based on semantic decisions and semantic priming. This raised the question of whether activation of semantic networks in the right hemisphere might provide a basis for speech production, at least at the single word level. Dukette, Baynes, Redfern, Share, 43 Ludy, and Dronkers (1998) designed a study to test this hypothesis. The researchers tested eight patients who had suffered left CVAs at least one year prior to intervention using a confrontation naming task, and grouped them according to their predominant error type (semantic or phonological). All patients received three weeks of implicit semantic training, three weeks of implicit phonological training, and three weeks of no training. Phonological training had the largest effect on participants whose errors were primarily phonological in nature; semantic training was effective for both groups. Most encouraging, the results generalized to improved scores on the BNT, which was “a striking result given the modest length of time spent in each training method . . . and the fact that there was no explicit naming practice” (Davis & Baynes, 2009). In the implicit phonological treatment investigation conducted by Davis, Farias, Bord, and Baynes (2006), the participant, a mildly anomic aphasic with apraxia as his primary complaint and speech disturbance, performed several types of covert (responsefree) phoneme manipulation tasks including rhyming, alliteration, insertion, and deletion tasks. During training sessions, he was instructed to make choices based solely on the mental manipulation of sounds, with no accompanying verbal output. Training resulted in improved overt speech production and significant generalization to non-treatment words. Although the authors concluded that questions remained regarding whether different tasks exerted differential effects on treatment, they noted that all of the tasks encouraged semantic access (in addition to lexical, phonological, and motor planning) and required inhibition of closely related competitors as well as activation of the target, which may have played a role in the success of the intervention (Davis, Farias, & Baynes, 2009). 44 Baynes, Shenaut, Ober, Davis, D’Angelo, and Teague (2010), noting that “the intensive practice and decreased opportunity for error” provided by production-free semantic training “may provide improved word retrieval equivalent to that of conventional methods,” compared the results of a hierarchical cueing intervention requiring overt speech with results of a nonverbal intervention using REST (Reduced Error Semantic Therapy). The REST intervention required the participant to touch the correct picture in response to a categorical or associative level question about a target natural or manmade item, without verbally producing the name of the item. The order of presentation of the two types of questions (categorical vs. associative) was random in the REST intervention, but was hierarchical in the hierarchical cueing condition, with categorical templates for each training item preceding the associative level templates (Baynes et al., 2010). Despite the pre-treatment limitations of the participant, a conduction aphasic with perseveration tendencies and working memory impairment, the authors found a modest improvement in retrieval of words used in REST training, as well as in post-intervention naming ability as measured by the BNT. The type of semantic training employed by Dukette, Baynes, et al. (1998) was applied intensively in an implicit, semantic-based, single subject aphasia intervention designed by Davis, Harrington, and Baynes (2006). Stimuli were designed to use “the structural and semantic aspects of the target object to help the patient practice selection of the object under competition,” and consisted of four-picture displays in which each of the three foils chosen was a close semantic competitor of the target (Davis et al., 2006). For example, for target “binoculars,” selected foils in one four-image display included a 45 microscope, a telescope, and bifocals, all objects related to, and therefore activated by (and in competition with) the target, due to their shared perceptual, semantic, and/or phonological characteristics. The participant, a patient with Wernicke’s aphasia, was asked to perform tasks requiring judgments about the categorical, associative or perceptual characteristics of the target object by pointing to the target in each field of four closely semantically related objects. Overt naming was discouraged. The researchers felt that “selection under these conditions provides practice that strengthens activation, both excitatory and inhibitory patterns, necessary to perform accurate word selection and ultimately production” (Davis et al., 2006). According to the authors, if selection of the target “requires activation of the target as well as inhibition of competitors, particularly close competitors,” it was expected that this type of task would provide practice in inhibition of competing items in much the same way that such inhibition occurs during overt speech production. The authors hypothesized that since production of semantic paraphasias constitutes a common error type for speakers with aphasia, and since semantic paraphasias can reasonably be supposed to occur precisely because they are close semantic competitors for the target, then practice with inhibition of such competitors would be beneficial, especially if it occurred without inadvertent simultaneous “practicing” of error productions (Davis et al., 2006). Results demonstrated improvement on trained items, with generalization to untrained items from the same category. The patient also demonstrated improvements in narrative speech (specifically, increased use of nouns) consistent with his improvements in word retrieval, along with improved communication at home per family report. Pre- and post-training fMRI 46 scanning demonstrated changes in neural activation consistent with the behavioral results (Davis et al., 2006). The researchers concluded that their results supported the effectiveness of intensive intervention methods that require semantic judgments rather than explicit naming (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Davis et al., 2006). Davis, Farias, and Baynes (2008) conducted the first study in which implicit treatment was compared experimentally with an explicit treatment method identical in all respects except for the requirement of verbal responses. The patient presented with nonfluent (mixed type) aphasia secondary to a stroke suffered four years earlier. His speech was characterized by agrammatism with a mean length of utterance of two words, almost entirely nouns and often paraphasic. Twelve sessions of implicit verb training were conducted first, followed by twelve sessions of explicit (production-required) training. The treatment stimuli consisted of computerized DynaVox® templates, each with a single written question at the top concerning a trained verb, and four pictures below, one the target and the other three foils (chosen for their categorical, associative or perceptual similarity to the target). An auditory presentation of the question was given with a button press, at the patient’s discretion. His task was to answer the question by touching one of the four pictures. Templates advanced automatically, but only when the correct picture was selected. In order to strictly adhere to the implicit treatment, and to avoid the possible confound of the necessity of speech production during probes testing, no probes were collected. The patient did not show significant gains in naming accuracy after the implicit training, but increased his percentage of correct responses after the explicit training. However, error analysis showed that implicit training had the effect of 47 decreasing the number of “no response” errors and increasing the number of semantically-related or “smart” errors. The researchers felt that this pattern suggested that implicit treatment may have helped the participant to overcome a reluctance to respond, which in turn may have contributed to his excellent response to the second (explicit) phase of treatment (Davis, Farias, et al., 2008). The authors concluded that although it was clear that implicit training “did change the nature of the patient’s responses,” further research was necessary to determine “if patient variables, lack of probe stimuli, or word class of the targets” might have lessened the apparent effectiveness of the implicit intervention (Davis, Farias, et al., 2008). In an attempt to answer some of the questions posed by Davis, Farias, and Baynes (2008), Davis, Baynes, and Hess (2009) compared the results of an intensive, semantic decision based, implicit verb training with those of a method identical in all respects except for the requirement of speech production in a patient with non-fluent (Broca’s type) aphasia secondary to a pedestrian-versus-automobile accident. Just as in the previous study, stimuli consisted largely of templates featuring icons selected from the available images in a DynaVox® program. This time, however, despite the potential confound of explicit naming during probe testing, probes were conducted every other session during both explicit and implicit phases of training to assess the participant’s response to training. In addition, stimuli were more individualized and more challenging, in that not only were the foils varied in semantic distance to the targets, but the participant’s error productions in her attempts to name the targets during pre-testing were incorporated into the templates as foils for those targets. For example, because 48 “combing” was the participant’s frequent error substitution for the target brushing, a picture of a comb or of the action of combing hair was incorporated as a foil into all templates used to train the verb brushing. The authors hypothesized that this practice required increased activation of the target, as well as inhibition of its competitors. The authors found that the implicit intervention resulted in more rapid acquisition of trained verbs than that which occurred during the explicit (production required) training. However, due to the fact that the explicit training had been conducted first, they could not be certain as to whether implicit results reflected the cumulative effects of previous training. Therefore, the determination was made to embark upon the present study, using the same type of interventions with a cross-over design to address order effects. The present study Purpose, research questions, and hypotheses The purpose of the present study was to compare the results of two interventions, identical in all respects except for the requirement of speech production during training, in a patient with fluent aphasia. Both interventions were intensive, semantic decisionbased, and highly individualized to match the needs and interests of the participant. The intent was to determine what effects each type of training would have on the participant’s word retrieval abilities, and whether the order of treatments would be seen to have an effect on treatment results. In terms of potential benefit to the participant, a 24-year-old anomic aphasic with profound word retrieval deficits whose progress appeared to have plateaued prior to this intervention, the aim of intervention was to potentially facilitate naming of a larger scope of verbs through intensive, highly challenging semantic training 49 of a smaller set of verbs. Verbs, rather than nouns, were chosen as treatment targets due to the potential for verb training to increase spread of activation (Damasio & Tranel, 1993; Perani et al., 1999; Tyler, Bright, Fletcher, & Stamatakis, 2004), lead to greater improvements in word retrieval and sentence production (Raymer & Kohen, 2006), and result in greater generalization to spontaneous speech (Conroy, Sage, & Lambon Ralph, 2006). All lists of trained and untrained verbs in each phase were matched for frequency and baseline performance. Computer-based stimuli were used exclusively in all training sessions, in keeping with findings from previous studies suggesting the potential benefits of computerized interventions to client motivation and engagement (Petheram, 2004; Raymer, Kohen, et al., 2006; Wertz, 2000). In the implicit condition, the participant responded by pointing to the correct picture or pictures in a field of four images on a computerized template in response to a semantic question about the categorical, associative or perceptual qualities of the target verb. Verbal responses were discouraged, and templates moved forward only when the correct target was chosen. The explicit condition was identical, except that the participant was required to verbally produce the target verb before advancing to the next question. Training templates, which incorporated multiple-target questions, multiple-verb questions, and embedded negatives, were sufficiently challenging to require intense focus. Templates were also individualized so that the foils frequently comprised substitutions produced by the participant in attempts to name the action, thereby requiring increased inhibition of competitors as well as activation of the target. 50 The current research study posed four questions: 1.) To what degree would implicit and explicit training improve production of targeted verbs as measured by accuracy of probe verb production? 2.) Would these training effects generalize to untrained verbs in either condition, or in both conditions, as measured by production accuracy of non-targeted verbs? 3.) How would the effects of these training methods compare upon visual inspection and as measured by d statistic (Beeson & Robey, 2006) size effects? 4.) Would order effects be observed? Prior to the start of this experiment, the researchers hypothesized that both production-free and production-required training would improve naming of trained verbs, but asked the question whether such improvements would be essentially equal or would significantly differ due to the differing (speech production) response requirements in each condition. We also sought to determine whether the effects of training would generalize to untrained items, whether a measure of effect size (Beeson & Robey, 2006) would supplement observations from visual inspection of results, and whether order effects would be observed. Design A single subject, multiple baseline intervention with a crossover design (ABCA / ACBA) to address order effects was used in the current study (see Figure 2 for an illustration of the design). The participant’s speech and language were assessed prior to intervention using standardized measures including the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) (Kertesz, 1982), the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (P&PTT) (Howard and Patterson, 1992), and the BNT. Baseline naming performance on a large set of verbs was 51 established over a period of baseline testing prior to the start of Phase One of the intervention. Baseline performance was used to help select and match verbs for trained and untrained lists. A period of implicit treatment (B) was then initiated, with treatment probes every other session to monitor progress for trained and untrained stimuli. After a three-week break to help ensure lack of carryover from the previous treatment, a period of explicit treatment (C) was initiated, with treatment probes administered every other session. Following Phase One and prior to the start of Phase Two, generalization to verbs on the set of 96 verbs was assessed. Generalization to a larger corpus of 125 additional untrained verbs was also assessed, to monitor improvement in untrained items and to help determine new treatment targets. The participant was also re-tested on the WAB and BNT to monitor any generalization to naming of nouns and spontaneous speech that may have occurred during Phase One. A three-week break between Phase One and Phase Two was observed, and new treatment targets, matched for frequency and for accuracy of production during baseline testing, were selected. At the start of Phase Two, baseline performance (A) was again established over a period of baseline testing. A new period of explicit treatment (C) was initiated, followed by a three-week break, and then the initiation of a new period of implicit treatment (B). During both periods of treatment in Phase Two, treatment probes were administered following the same every-other-session schedule as in Phase One. Following cessation of treatment, maintenance (A) of verb naming for trained and untrained stimuli was tested. Maintenance testing (an essential component in studies of treatment efficacy) was conducted again at one month, three months, and eight months post-training, to measure the sustainability of treatment gains 52 over time. Post-testing on the WAB, BNT, P&PTT, and both larger sets of verbs was conducted following Phase Two. To summarize, all four periods of training were identical except for the independent variable of speech production, which was discouraged during the two implicit training (B) periods, and was required during the two explicit training (C) periods. Similarly, each phase of treatment was identical except for the order of the two treatments. It is anticipated that the findings of this study will inform future research on the potential use of implicit therapy as a potentially valuable adjunct to traditional speech-language therapy for individuals with aphasia. 53 Figure 2. Illustration of treatment study design PHASE ONE BASELINE MIDTESTING Implicit Tx Explicit Tx (3 wks later) WAB List 1A Training List 2B Training PPT BNT Three baselines of 96 Verbs WAB PHASE TWO MAINTENANCE Explicit Tx Implicit Tx (3 wks later) List 3A Training List 4B Training BNT Lists 1A, 2B*, and Phase One Untrained* probed every other session Lists 1A*, 2B, and Phase One Untrained probed every other session (*Probes = continued baseline testing) (*Probes = maintenance testing) WAB PPT Post-treatment testing of 96 Verbs Maintenance testing of Lists 1A and 2B Three Baselines on 125 verbs Lists 3A, 4B*, and Phase Two Untrained probed every other session Lists 3A*, 4B, and Phase Two Untrained probed every other session (*Probes = continued baseline testing) (*Probes = maintenance testing) Baselines on lists 3A, 4B, and Phase Two Untrained verbs established Maintenance testing of all trained and untrained lists: BNT 1 month posttreatment Maintenance testing: all trained and untrained verbs 3 months posttreatment 8 months posttreatment Posttreatment testing of 96 Verbs Posttreatment testing of 125 verbs TIME 53 54 Chapter 2 METHODS Participant At the time of his recruitment to participate in this study, I.T. was a 24-year-old, right-handed, native English-speaking male who presented with fluent aphasia secondary to a traumatic brain injury from a gunshot wound sustained eight months earlier, in November, 2008. Hospital records indicate that the damage was mostly to his left temporal lobe, specifically to the inferior, superior, and middle temporal gyri (see Figure 3). Although no bullet fragments had directly entered the brain, they had penetrated bone and caused severe swelling and a 5 mm left-to-right midline shift. Figure 3. Participant’s MRI and CT scans showing area and extent of lesion. Left and center MRI scans were obtained through the cranial vault with axial T2weighted images six weeks post-injury. Lesion included region of encephalomalacia involving the superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri on the left. Extensive abnormalities also included residuals of left frontoparietal craniotomy and ex vacuo enlargement of anterior tip of the temporal horn. Right image is chronic comparison CT scan taken two years post-injury. The area of encephalomalacia involving the left temporal lobe was unchanged from previous images. For all images, note that the left side of the picture shows the right side of the brain, and vice versa. Prior to his injury, I.T. had attained a high school education and had been enrolled in community college for several semesters. He was also employed as a telemarketer, a 55 job which required excellent verbal communication skills and at which he excelled per self-report. He had no reported pre-morbid history of psychiatric, neurological, cognitive, or speech-language deficits. His medical history was significant for diabetes diagnosed at age 16. In the year following his injury, I.T. underwent numerous surgeries including craniotomy, left ear canal reconstruction (including canaloplasty and tympanoplasty), medial and lateral facial nerve decompression, and mandibular and lower facial reconstruction. He had residual left facial paralysis but had developed good compensatory strategies resulting in clear articulation. He demonstrated no verbal or nonverbal oral apraxia and no articulatory weakness. Pure tone audiometric testing performed six months post-injury showed that he had normal pure tone thresholds in the right ear and a profound mixed hearing loss in the left ear with a 30 to 40 dB air-bone gap. A hearing aid for the left ear had been recommended but was not yet being worn at the time of this intervention. Due to this hearing issue, the clinician sat on I.T.’s right side during all training, probe and testing sessions. I.T.’s vision was within normal limits with corrective lenses, which he consistently wore. He had no identified spatial neglect or visual field deficits. I.T. received standard speech and language therapy consistently from the time of his hospitalization until his recruitment for this study. At the time he was recruited and consented to participate in this study, he was receiving services twice weekly for one hour per session as a speech therapy outpatient at the U.C. Davis Medical Center Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 56 Although he tested as an anomic aphasic according to the WAB, and presented with spontaneous speech consistent with that aphasia profile, I.T. exhibited some verbal behaviors similar to those often demonstrated by patients with Wernicke’s aphasia: he demonstrated frequent topic shifts, tended to become distracted and tangential while telling a story, and occasionally related a narrative without realizing he had left out a crucial or salient detail necessary for listener comprehension. In conversation, he demonstrated frequent instances of word-finding difficulty (for nouns and verbs approximately equally), although he was fairly skilled at using alternative modalities (circumlocution, gestures, etc.) to effectively communicate his message. His speech, which was typically grammatically correct, was characterized by frequent hesitations and attempted self-corrections, whole-word phonemic and semantic paraphasias, occasional neologisms, and revisions and explicit expressions of frustration typical of an individual with significant word retrieval difficulties (“I know that word, it’s….wait, no, it’s not that, it’s…oh, what is the word for that, when you’re going, no, not going but when you’re doing the thing where you go like this?”). Procedure U.C. Davis Medical Center Institutional Review Board approval was obtained prior to I.T.’s recruitment. I.T. signed informed consent prior to participation, and his parents also gave their informed consent prior to the start of the intervention. Language testing pre- and post-intervention. The participant’s language and word retrieval skills were assessed prior to intervention using standardized measures including the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) 57 (Kertesz, 1982), the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (P&PTT) (Howard & Patterson, 1992), and the Boston Naming Test (BNT) (Goodglass & Kaplan, 2000). Standardized instruments were administered according to clinical protocol, and results were used to characterize in detail the deficits that hindered I.T.’s verbal communication and to aid in focusing the intervention. Non-standardized measures were also used, and consisted of large verb lists with corresponding picture stimuli selected from the Picture This® program. Pre-intervention scores were obtained two weeks prior to the intervention and post-intervention scores were obtained three weeks after the conclusion of the six month intervention. In addition, standardized testing and assessment of non-standardized verb naming performance measures were conducted in between Phase One and Phase Two of the intervention, to determine effects of treatment (see Table 2). I.T.’s pre-intervention classification on the WAB was anomic aphasia. This standardized test was also used to obtain an aphasia quotient (A.Q.), a composite severity score determined by the participant’s performance on picture description, auditory comprehension, repetition, and naming tasks. I.T.’s pre-intervention A.Q. was 72.4, according to the WAB. He demonstrated relatively spared auditory comprehension (auditory comprehension score of 4.0 or higher) and slightly impaired repetition (repetition score below 7.9) per normative data on performance of participants with aphasia (Kertesz, 1982). The proportion of nouns and the proportion of verbs used in a connected speech task were also computed using the participant’s picnic picture descriptions from the WAB. This was done in order to help determine the suitability of a verb intervention for this participant, as well as to provide data for post-intervention 58 comparison. During pre-intervention testing, 9% of the total words produced by the participant in response to the picnic picture were verbs, and 20% were nouns. The BNT, which assesses naming responses on 60 black and white line drawings of high- to low-frequency English words, was also administered. I.T.’s pre-intervention score of 18 was more than five standard deviations below the norm, which equated to a profound naming deficit. I.T.’s errors on the BNT were classified as a combination of semantically related and unrelated verbal paraphasias, neologisms, and non-word phonemically based paraphasias. It is also worth noting that a significant number of the participant’s errors constituted perseverations. Out of 21 phonemic cues given after incorrectly named items, I.T. gave nine correct responses following the cue. The Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (P&PTT) (Howard & Patterson, 1992) was also administered pre- and post-intervention. The P&PTT is thought to be particularly useful for theoretically-motivated testing of picture and word comprehension in participants with aphasia, because it makes semantic demands (determining the degree to which a subject can access meaning from pictures and words) but without requiring verbal expression. Information from the P&PTT enables clinicians to establish whether the patient’s difficulty in naming or pointing to a named picture is due to a difficulty in retrieving semantic information from pictures, a difficulty in retrieving semantic information from words, or a naming failure due to a difficulty in retrieving the appropriate spoken form of the word. P&PTT performance can help determine if a patient’s naming errors are due to a breakdown at the semantic-lexical level or to postlexical phonological disorders. The picture condition of the P&PTT was administered to 59 determine if the participant was able to draw inferences about pictorial material. In the picture version, the target picture is presented above two pictures, and the individual is asked to indicate which picture is most closely associated to the target. The word condition requires the individual to access semantic information by deciding which of two written concepts (e.g., pine tree or palm tree) is most related to the target concept (e.g., pyramid). Results from the two versions of the test were analyzed to compare the participant’s performance on tasks that require access to the object semantic system (three-picture version) and the lexical semantic system (three-word version). I.T.’s scores on both the word and picture versions of the P&PTT were approximately equal (45.5 and 45, respectively) and were both within normal limits, providing evidence that his object and lexical semantics were intact, or in other words, that the semantic system was intact between object and meaning. This led to the conclusion that his naming failure was due to poor representation of the word in the lexicon, and/or to poor lexical access due to insufficient semantic activation of the target and/or insufficient inhibition of competitors. Thus, the use of an intervention designed to increase spread of activation through semantic-level tasks (tasks believed to cause wider spread of activation than other types of tasks) was thought to be potentially beneficial to the participant. In addition to standardized testing, naming performance on a large verb list was assessed to confirm the presence of a verb retrieval deficit (operationally defined as an accuracy score below 75% on this measure) and establish the items needed for training sets. Responses were elicited using images from the Picture This® program. Error types during pre- and post-tests were recorded for later analysis. All the testing and 60 experimental stimuli were one- to two-syllable verbs. The present progressive form of the verb was elicited (e.g., “What is he doing?”) for all verbs in both pre- and post-tests and in training sets. Responses were not timed, and independently self-corrected responses were accepted. In all, 96 verbs were tested during pre-intervention and during the three sessions of pre-intervention baseline testing. Development of test and treatment stimuli. Three lists of eleven verbs each—one Trained Implicit list, one Trained Explicit list, and one Untrained list—were created for each phase of the experiment (see Appendix A). The lists were matched according to verb frequency (Francis & Kucera, 1982) and according to the participant’s baseline performance during assessment (using a compilation of 96 verbs paired with images from the Picture This® program). Imageability was also a consideration; only verbs judged to be unambiguously representable in picture form were selected for inclusion. For each trained list, at least 179 templates were created (with an average of 222 templates per trained list, or 20 templates per trained verb), using a Microsoft PowerPoint® program and images selected from Google Images® readily available on the internet. This method proved to be an immense improvement over previous implicit training methods (e.g., creation of templates using images from the limited pool in a prepackaged DynaVox® program) in many respects. Perhaps the most important of these was that it permitted creation of unique computer templates that were matched to the participant’s areas of interests. The participant, a lively, curious young man with a keen sense of humor, was especially interested in small furry animals, cute babies, food, sports 61 (particularly football), slapstick humor, cartoons, popular culture, attractive young women, and pets. Because Google Images® enables searching of any terms (i.e., any verb, paired with any noun that was anticipated to provide a theme of interest to the participant), there were virtually no limitations in terms of possible templates. This method of stimuli creation also allowed for a strong element of visual appeal and balance in the training templates; if, for instance, a blue sky with puffy white clouds was visible in the top left image, it was possible to ensure that there was also a blue and white image in the lower right, or that a blue sky with puffy white clouds (or just four blue and white images) appeared in all four quadrants of the template. It was also possible to include dynamic (moving) images, by including the words “animated” or “gif” (graphics interchange format) in any image search. Although most images used were static (photos or illustrations), the occasional inclusion of moving images was thought to be especially helpful in the training of action verbs (McCann & Greig, 2010). These features of the stimuli creation method all apparently helped ensure that the participant’s interest in and motivation for the tasks remained high; he always appeared to enjoy the sessions greatly, and often expressed eagerness to see what new templates had been created since his last session. This method of stimuli creation also allowed images to be matched according to visual/perceptual as well as categorical and associative features, which was thought to increase the level of challenge offered by the tasks. For example, when training the verbs yawning, drying, digging, and balancing, four semantically unrelated verbs, four images could be easily located and selected for their similar features (e.g., a puppy yawning, a 62 puppy being dried with a towel, a puppy digging in a yard, a puppy balancing on a ball), which were all related to one another (in this example, through the common theme of a puppy). This ensured that the four pictures were all close competitors, whether or not the common theme itself (e.g., puppies, hamsters, black and white line drawings, cartoon characters, sea creatures, gorillas, panda bears, red objects, metal objects, babies, kittens, football, attractive young women, or food) was semantically related to the target verb. In other words, the four pictures might be semantically related to the target verb (e.g., Which one splashes? accompanied by pictures of a starfish, an octopus, an anemone, and a dolphin, all creatures that live in water, which is semantically related to the verb splash), but might only be related to each other visually or featurally in some way unrelated to the target verb (e.g., Which two are not swinging or sailing? with pictures of an owl and a pussycat swinging on a playground, sailing in a boat, eating a meal, and playing a board game). Another way in which the stimuli selection ensured that tasks required inhibition of close semantic competitors for the target was that the participant’s own error productions (e.g., mowing for harvesting) were included as foils during training as often as possible. This was done to ensure that the task required inhibition of the very semantic competitors which had previously resulted in verbal errors. The stimuli creation method also allowed for the training of multiple verb meanings using atypical exemplars (see Appendix B, Table B6), which was thought to have benefit in terms of increasing spread of activation (Kiran, 2001, 2008; Kiran & Thompson, 2003). For any atypical meaning or usage of the verb thought to be imageable (e.g., pouring rain, versus simply pouring liquid into a container), searching the term 63 (e.g., “pouring rain”) yielded countless image choices for inclusion in training templates. Whenever a question arose regarding precise or allowable definitions (e.g., whether carving could legitimately be used to refer to the action of cutting a pattern or design into a vegetable), this was resolved through consulting the definitions offered by the Oxford English Dictionary. Approximately 50% of all templates included an orthographically presented question at the top of the four-picture display. Those that did not feature a printed question were often versatile enough to be used for more than one question, and more than one question type. Question types were varied to allow for increased grammatical complexity and the inclusion of plural markers, multiple verbs, and embedded negatives, in order to offer greater challenge and require more intense focus. Embedded negatives in particular (teaching not only the concept, but also what the concept is not) were thought to help ensure deeper understanding of the concept. Question types took eight basic forms (see Appendix B, Table B5): Single-target, single-verb questions (the form used most often during the first day or two of training of a new list, such as Which one is skating?); multiple-target, single-verb questions (Which two are sniffing?); single-target, single-verb questions with embedded negatives (Which one is not drying?); multiple-target, single-verb questions with embedded negatives (Which two are not reaching?); single-target, multiple-verb questions (Which one goes with both painting and drying?); multiple-target, multiple-verb questions (Which three are rolling and floating?); single-target, multiple-verb questions with embedded negatives (Which one goes with baking but not carving?); and multiple-target, multiple-verb 64 questions with embedded negatives (Which two are jumping but not splashing?; Which ones are not rocking or swinging?). Approximately the same number of questions of each type was included in each training phase. There were minor variations among questions of any given type according to the specific focus of the question. Questions primarily used the present participle form of the verb (e.g., slicing), to differentiate the word clearly as an action and avoid confusion with usage of the root form in other grammatical categories, such as noun usage (e.g., [a] slice). An auxiliary form of the verb was often used along with the main infinitive form (e.g., Which one can jump?) to encourage more intense concentration than that required by relatively simple identification tasks (e.g., Which one is jumping?). Occasionally, the past participle form of the verb was used (e.g., Which one was baked? Which one is closed?) to encourage focus on the verb as a past event. Sometimes the main form of the infinitive verb was used, such as when use of future tense was needed in order to encourage consideration of the verb in the context of consequences of a future event (e.g., Which one will splash?). Verbs from Phase One Untrained and Phase Two Untrained lists were never included as foils on templates during either phase of training. The Untrained list from Phase Two did not include any verbs ever used as foils during Phase One. The Untrained list from Phase One remained untrained during Phase Two. Treatment protocol. The participant received three to four sessions per week, for one to one and a half hours per session. All training sessions were conducted in the offices of the U.C. Davis Medical Center Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation by the author, with 65 direct supervision by a licensed and ASHA-certified speech-language pathologist. During the intervention, I.T. did not participate in any group or individual speech-language therapy. Probe sessions were administered at the start of every other session. The criteria for cessation of training during any phase of the experiment were when the participant met one of two conditions: 1.) demonstrated accuracy of 90% (correct naming of 10 out of 11 verbs) on three consecutive treatment probes, or 2) reached the end of 12 training sessions for that particular training set. During every treatment session, including probe sessions, I.T. was presented with the training templates for the verb list then being trained. Templates were presented on a laptop computer with a 17 ½ inch screen placed at a distance comfortable to the participant. For each template, I.T. was asked at least one question requiring him to make a semantic decision regarding the target verb. Question types on the templates varied (see Appendix C for examples) and were generally evenly distributed in each training set as previously described, with the exception that the first one or two training sessions with each new set featured more questions of the simpler types (e.g., single-target, single-verb questions) and fewer of the more grammatically complex question types (e.g., multipletarget, multiple-verb questions with embedded negatives). During implicit training, I.T. either read the question silently or listened as the clinician presented the question verbally. After considering the question and visually inspecting the four options, he responded nonverbally, selecting an image by touching it with a rubber-tipped pointing stick. All tasks were untimed; the participant was permitted to take as much time as he needed or wanted to make his decision. 66 Sometimes, undoubtedly because I.T. enjoyed talking and because he was accustomed to “thinking out loud,” he attempted to repeat the question verbally while deliberating and considering the available choices. Because this tendency in effect equated to practicing explicit production of the target verb, it was discouraged with a nonverbal signal (finger held to clinician’s lips) or an explicit verbal reminder (“Remember, try to do this without talking”) whenever it occurred. I.T.’s accuracy and errors were tallied during each session, partly to ensure that tasks were not too difficult and that his accuracy was maintained at greater than 90% on total semantic decisions per session, and partly to ensure that any errors could be incorporated as foils in the creation of future templates. Whenever I.T. selected an incorrect target in response to a question, the question was repeated verbally and/or key features of the printed question (plural s, or key words such as not, best or most often) were pointed out. Whenever the correct target or targets had been selected, the clinician or the participant pressed a computer key to advance to the next template. The treatment was intensive in that it required intense concentration on the part of the participant, who reported that he often found himself thinking about the verbs throughout the day, even on days when he did not have a treatment session. I.T. approached each session with apparent eagerness, and referred to the training sessions as a “fun learning game.” At no time did he appear discouraged or overwhelmed, and on those few occasions when he demonstrated fatigue and began making more errors instead of fewer errors as a training session progressed, he was encouraged to take a short break, 67 drink some water, and chat about his current activities for a few minutes before returning to the training set. During the explicit condition, the same treatment protocol was followed, except that I.T. was asked to verbally produce each target verb before advancing to the next template. If he did not remember this step on his own, he was reminded by the clinician. For example, if the question at the top of the template asked “Which two are jumping?” and I.T. merely pointed to the images illustrating the concept of jumping before attempting to advance to the next template, the clinician pointed to each target and said, “So, s/he is doing what?” to prompt him to verbally produce the target verb. On those rare occasions when I.T. did not remember the target verb when asked to produce it, he was given a phonemic cue. If this did not result in a correct production, the full form of the target was modeled and he was asked to repeat it before advancing to the next target. No incorrect productions were accepted; advancement to the next template was always contingent upon a correct production (one per target) from the participant. If there were three targets on a template, I.T. was required to say the target word three times correctly, pointing to each target image in turn (e.g., “jumping, jumping, jumping”), before advancing to the next template. During both conditions, images were switched to different positions or replaced with new images, questions were altered, and/or new templates were added before each training session, so that no two training sessions ever featured exactly the same series of templates, and often two back-to-back training sessions featured entirely unique templates with minimal or no overlap of previously viewed stimuli. This was made 68 possible by creating such a large number of templates (between 179 and 261 templates per training set) that the same exact series never had to be viewed twice. The decision to create sufficient templates to ensure a unique presentation during each training session was made in response to an incident that occurred on the second day of training, when it became clear that the participant had memorized, after only one exposure, the positions of at least some of the target images on the templates. When I.T. was observed to point to the upper right corner of the screen in anticipation of the advancement of the current template (preparing to point to the correct target on a template not yet visible to him, to correctly answer a question he had not yet been asked), the determination was made that template images had to be altered, questions changed, and/or new templates added prior to each day’s training. 69 Chapter 3 RESULTS Comparison of Implicit and Explicit Training Methods Clear response to both production-free and conventional interventions was verified by medium to large size effects in both phases of training (see Figures 4 and 5). Training effects in both phases were maintained three months after cessation of training. Eight months after the end of Phase Two of the intervention, training effects for verbs trained explicitly in both phases were maintained, although training effects for both sets of verbs trained implicitly had declined somewhat. Order effects were observed. The first training method in each phase had a larger size effect. In Phase One, a larger size effect was observed for the implicit (d = 9.9) than for the explicit condition (d = 6.9). In Phase Two, a larger size effect was observed for the explicit (d= 18.1) compared to the implicit condition (d= 8.7). No significant generalization to untrained verbs occurred in either condition in either phase of training. Visual inspection of Phase One indicated that both types of training were effective (see Figure 4). However, visual inspection of Phase One revealed greater generalization to untrained items during implicit training than during explicit training. Visual inspection of Phase Two revealed a steeper learning curve, along with solid retention of training effects, for verbs trained explicitly (see Figure 5). In Phase Two, clear effects of training, and good maintenance of improvements, were observed for verbs trained implicitly. In Phase Two, very little generalization of either list into 70 untrained items was observed upon visual inspection; there was some generalization during implicit training, and none during explicit training. There appeared to be tight experimental control with explicit training; in other words, explicit training only affected the items that were trained. There was some generalization to untrained items during implicit training in both phases, although the increases in accuracy for untrained items were not significant. Explicit training was observed to yield greater, more consistent retention of trained items in both phases. Maintenance Baseline Implicit Tx < 3 mo Explicit Tx 3 mo 6 mo >8 mo Number of accurate probe responses 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 Probe Sessions List 1A Trained Implicit List 2B Trained Explicit Untrained/Never Trained Figure 4. Phase One: Comparison of implicit and explicit training effects. 71 Baseline Explicit Tx Maintenance <3 mo 3 mo Implicit Tx 8 mo Number of accurate responses 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 Probe Sessions List 3A Trained Explicit List 4B Trained Implicit Untrained Figure 5. Phase Two: Comparison of explicit and implicit training effects. 72 73 Error Patterns Error analysis was performed on the original 96-verb list used to establish baseline performance. The 96-verb list was tested three additional times during the intervention (pre-, mid-, and post-intervention). Treatment generalization on the list was observed with results as shown in Table 2. Error analysis was performed (see Table 1) to determine an increase in correct responses and a shift in error response types. Along with the expected increase in the number of correct responses, there was a clear decrease in the number of non-semantically-related responses, as well as in the number of neologisms and no response/“I don’t know” responses (see Figure 6). Table 1. Classification of responses according to error type (expressed as a percentage of total responses). NonNo Correct Semantically- semantically- Neologisms response related related (IDK) 61 13 19 3 4 Baseline 72 15 11 1 1 Post-Phase One 90 9 1 0 0 Post-Phase Two 74 100 90 80 70 Correct 60 Semantically-related 50 Non-semantically-related 40 Neologisms 30 No response 20 10 0 Baseline Post-Phase One Post-Phase Two Figure 6. Percentage of responses in each phase of assessment classified by type. Change in Standardized and Non-Standardized Language Measures Training resulted in improvements on the Western Aphasia Battery, with increased scores in spontaneous speech, including information content, fluency and grammatical competence, along with improved object naming (see Table 2). The participant’s A.Q. as measured by the WAB increased from 72.4 to 90.9. The proportion of verbs in spontaneous speech increased from 9% to 13% of total words produced, while the proportion of nouns as a percentage of total words remained approximately the same. Auditory comprehension scores on the WAB increased from 8.5 to 9.9. Scores on the BNT increased slightly, from 18 to 22. Although this score remained over five standard deviations below the norm, the participant’s errors consisted solely of semantically related verbal paraphasias (sweeper/broom; chair/bench), with no perseveration or neologisms, in contrast to his pre-intervention error profile. Scores on the P&PTT 75 remained essentially unchanged. Improvements were noted on naming of the larger set of 96 verbs (binomial p < 0.04, z = 5.0). Anecdotally, it is worth noting that I.T.’s regular conversational partners reported that he appeared to be expressing himself verbally with more precise word choice postintervention, and that I.T. reported subjective improvements in his word retrieval abilities in conversation. 76 Table 2. Participant’s pre-and post-treatment scores Pre-Intervention Post- Phase One Western Aphasia Battery Spontaneous Speech: Information Content* 8 9 Fluency & Grammatical 6 8 Competence* Verbs**/total words produced in picture 6/66 (9%) 10/103 (10%) description Nouns/total words in 13/66 (20%) 18/103 (17%) picture description Post-Phase Two 10 10 16/125 (13%) 21/125 (17%) Auditory Verbal Comprehension* 8.5 8.9 9.9 Naming & Word Finding* 5.9 6.9 7.4 Aphasia Quotient 72.4 82.4 90.9 Boston Naming Test 18 20 22 Pyramids and Palm Trees Test Pictures Written Words 45 45.5 N/T N/T 48 44 96 Verbs 67 (70%) 70 (73%) 86 (90%) 50 (40%) 79 (63%) N/T 125 Verbs *WAB subtest scores out of a possible 10 points **Excluding auxiliary verb forms of be and have 77 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION Major Findings and Implications The purpose of the present study was to compare the results of two interventions, identical in all respects except for the requirement of speech production during training. The intent was to determine what effects each type of training would have on the participant’s word retrieval abilities, as well as whether the order of treatments would be seen to have an effect on treatment results. The first finding was that both implicit and explicit training methods were effective in training of verbs. This finding supports the use of implicit therapy as a potentially valuable adjunct to traditional therapy in the treatment of fluent aphasia. The fact that both methods were efficacious, and that post-intervention improvements were noted in the participant’s spontaneous speech, also suggests that intensive, semantic decision-based verb interventions can have direct effects on spontaneous speech and fluency. One factor unrelated to the presence or absence of verbal production requirements may have helped increase the effectiveness of training overall for this participant. Training effects in both conditions may have been maximized by the fact that training tasks were made as enjoyable as possible for I.T., partly through incorporation of images specifically chosen to match his interests and appeal to his sense of humor, which in turn resulted in a great deal of laughter and positive emotion during each session. Humor has been found to enhance the learning process (Dormann & Biddle, 2006; Potter & Goodman, 1983) and has been referred to as an important (though often overlooked) 78 therapy tool (Simmons-Mackie & Schultz, 2003). The fact that the stimuli were specifically chosen to engage the client by employing images reflecting his interests undoubtedly enhanced his learning of the target verbs. The use of stimuli with a high degree of emotional salience can serve as a potent aid to later recall of the stimuli (Janata, 2009), and tasks for which a participant expresses enthusiasm have been shown to be most beneficial in naming interventions (Nickels & Best, 1996). In addition to the overall efficacy of the intervention, a second finding was that order effects were observed; the first training method in each phase had a larger size effect, regardless of whether or not it required speech production. This finding helps answer questions raised by previous research (Davis, Baynes, & Hess, 2009) regarding whether apparent gains from implicit therapy may have been due to the cumulative effects of previous training. It would appear that, for this participant at least, the effects of implicit therapy were not due to the cumulative effects of previous training. Another finding was that no significant generalization to untrained verbs occurred in either condition. As has been often noted, generalization beyond the trained items, though very limited in most studies of verb training (Raymer & Ellsworth, 2002), is crucial for improving the participant’s communication in daily life, not just in the restricted environment of therapy (Basso & Caporali, 2001; Davis, Harrington, & Baynes, 2006). However, although treatments that generalize are preferable to those with effects limited to a small set of items, “item-specific effects can be worthwhile, particularly when they are on items that are functionally useful and when the effects maintain” (Nickels & Best, 1996). Also, the intervention was shown to have effects 79 beyond generalization to specific verbs, in that it improved the participant’s spontaneous speech and fluency as demonstrated by pre- and post-intervention standardized test scores (particularly an increase in A.Q. on the WAB) and narrative speech samples, and per patient report. Although there was little generalization to untrained items overall, visual inspection revealed more generalization to untrained items during the implicit interventions regardless of order effects. This could be due to resource allocation; it may be that language resources used for retrieval of the word for articulation during explicit production were used in the implicit intervention (when the participant did not need to focus on explicit articulation) to mentally process aspects of the action, and the resulting increased spread of activation resulted in generalization to other untrained verbs. Another finding was that although both implicit and explicit training yielded medium to large size effects, explicit training yielded greater, more consistent retention of trained items. The first possibility to consider is that for some participants at least, some degree of verbal production practice may be necessary to achieving the greatest possible maintenance of treatment gains. Although Nickels and Best (1996) argued that if output of the item were a key part of therapy, then only those items which had been produced would improve and there would be no generalization untrained items, other authors have differed on this point. As some authors have noted, it is possible that production-free therapy may be appropriate for some individuals, but not for others, depending on the nature of the deficit (Nickels & Best, 1996). 80 A second possibility is that feedback regarding production, rather than production practice itself, is an important component for some patients. Although significant improvements in naming have resulted from repeated presentation of stimuli even when no feedback or error correction was provided (Howard, 1985a, 1985b; Nickels, 2002; Nickels & Best, 1996; Pring, White-Thompson, Pounds, Marshall, & Davis, 1990), it may be that “although overt naming seems unlikely to be essential, internal activation of the phonological (or orthographic) representation could be necessary to produce the effect” (Nickels & Best, 1996). Perhaps this internal activation is not fully achieved without at least occasional feedback regarding correct articulation of the target. This could help explain why explicit training yielded more consistent retention than implicit training. I.T. demonstrated numerous mispronunciations and error productions during explicit training (e.g., “suing”/sewing), receiving feedback and eventually correctly producing the target after each mispronunciation. It is reasonable to assume that he would have made just as many production errors with verbs trained implicitly, if he had been given the opportunity to produce the word and receive feedback regarding his productions, and that this feedback might have been beneficial. Another possibility to consider is that although the verb lists appeared to be balanced, they may have actually differed in some way in terms of difficulty. It was noted that one reason for the relatively poor maintenance of the implicitly trained verb list in Phase Two (List 4B) may have been the fact that I.T. did not achieve mastery of those verbs during training. Because he never achieved 90% criteria on that list, his learning of 81 those verbs never had the advantage of consolidation during training, an advantage seen during the other implicit training and both explicit trainings. This raises the question of why the participant demonstrated such relative difficulty with learning the verbs in List 4B. One possibility that needs to be considered is that although the verb lists were thought to be evenly matched due to having been matched for frequency and for baseline performance, they may have varied in difficulty due to factors that were not taken into account. Recent evidence suggests that matching training sets of verbs according to frequency and baseline performance may be inadequate, and that other criteria may need to be considered (Graham, 2009). Semantic weight (Barde, Schwartz, & Boronat, 2006), semantic verb category (Kemmerer, Castillo, Talavage, Patterson, & Wiley, 2008), semantic relatedness (Cornelissen et al., 2003), and overlap of semantic features between training verbs (Graham, 2009; Faroqi-Shah, Wood, & Gassert, 2010) are all factors shown to potentially affect the naming performance of anomic patients. In particular, for List 4B, there may have been issues related to semantic weight and/or overlapping semantic features. During List 4B training, I.T. was observed frequently attempting to use his body to simulate and differentiate the actions of rocking, swinging, reaching and crouching as he heard or read each target verb. The distinctions between these verbs often appeared to confuse him, particularly the differences between rocking and swinging. The fact that there was significant semantic feature overlap for List 4B, with seven of the 11 verbs (reaching, rocking, crouching, rolling, leaping, swinging, and floating) potentially involving full-body actions, may have resulted, in 82 effect, in that list presenting a higher level of difficulty, especially in light of the fact that only two to three verbs in any other list involved full-body actions. It has been demonstrated that body part features are activated by verbs and by pictures of actions, and that listeners mentally simulate actions denoted by verbs, even when the verbs are presented as decontextualized single words (Faroqi-Shah, Wood, & Gassert, 2010). This results in an increase in the difficulty of verb retrieval tasks when two verbs with overlapping semantic features (especially somatotopically congruent verbs, or verbs with overlapping body part features) are presented together (Faroqi-Shah, Gassert, & Wood, 2009). Future implicit therapy investigations should incorporate consideration of semantic weight and semantic feature overlap during the selection of training items, to ensure that training sets present an equal level of difficulty in each condition. Limitations of the Present Study and Directions for Future Research There were several confounds in the present study. One was the need for overt production during probe sessions, as has been noted by previous researchers using implicit training methods. Also, simple exposure to the pictures shown to the participant during probes testing could have had an effect. Another potential confound was the necessary inclusion of non-target, unrelated verbs--any untrained verbs at all, even if they appeared nowhere in the 96 verbs or 125 verb assessments administered during pre-and post-testing—as foils on training templates, because one of the research questions we asked was whether there would be generalization to untrained items, and if either the participant or the clinician ever produced these extraneous verbs, we were in effect partially training them. 83 An additional confound was the fact that although speech production during the implicit condition was discouraged, it was neither absolutely prohibited nor entirely prevented. I. T. often attempted/began to read the orthographically-presented questions aloud, and/or to verbally repeat the verbally-presented questions (containing the target word) , and/or to verbally repeat, if only subvocally/sotto voce, the target word prior to making a decision and pointing to one of the four pictures. This may be a common occurrence for individuals with effortless (fluent) speech who enjoy talking and are enthusiastic about the task, even when they are instructed (and even frequently reminded, as I.T. was) to complete a task silently. Nickels and Best (1996) observed that, in the absence of explicit instructions regarding production, one participant “always attempted to read the words aloud (not always successfully) prior to matching the picture(s).” As has been previously mentioned, lists were matched only according to frequency and baseline performance, which, although standard practice in verb naming studies, may be insufficient to prevent differences in difficulty level between lists of training items. Future research may need to control for semantic distance and degree of overlap of semantic features in trained and untrained verbs. This should be done not because tasks with a high degree of overlap between training items are too difficult— effortful tasks have been shown to be beneficial, as demonstrated by Frattali and Kang (2004)—but because lists of training items should be matched as precisely as possible in terms of task difficulty to prevent unintended variables. Another factor that could be taken into account in future studies is the degree to which trained verbs are meaningful to the participant. The present study could potentially 84 have incorporated the participant’s specific needs and interests as selection criteria not only for picture stimuli but also for the trained verbs themselves. The training verb selection criteria employed in the present study resulted in the inclusion of numerous verbs related to food preparation and cooking (harvesting, peeling, slicing, grating, stirring, baking, roasting, and grilling), activities the participant admitted that he rarely or never engaged in, and which he (perhaps as a consequence) demonstrated relative difficulty with learning and differentiating during training sessions. It has been observed that in treating naming, it is important to select words that are both client specific and functional (Nickels & Best, 1996; Roseberry-McKibbin & Hegde, 2006). Future research may also need to control more carefully for factors related to the semantic questions asked in each training set. Counting and matching the number of question types used for training each list, to ensure that no one list contains more challenging questions overall than another list, could help eliminate the possibility of unintended variables related to training set difficulty. Related to this issue, the present study could have eliminated questions potentially requiring increased word retrieval which were used in explicit training but not in implicit training (e.g., What is each one doing?). Although the difference between this type of training question and other types of questions used was thought to be inconsequential, it is possible that providing no information about the word form made this type of question more difficult for the participant, and therefore, could have influenced the results to some degree. Future researchers may also wish to manipulate such factors deliberately, to help determine the 85 precise effects of grammatical complexity of the question (e.g., Which one is sneezing, versus Which ones are not sneezing) on retrieval of verbs. Another way in which training sets could perhaps be more carefully balanced would be to count and match the number of dynamic and static images used for each training corpus, to ensure that no training set contains more dynamic images than another training set. Dynamic images may provide increased benefit over static images when training action verbs (McCann & Greig, 2010). The present study could also have potentially ensured closer matching of the lists according to how many verbs per list had multiple meanings and atypical exemplars being trained. For example, although numerous multiple meanings and atypical exemplars were used to train the verbs popping and rolling, many verbs had fewer meanings or exemplars trained, and for some verbs (e.g., sneezing, yawning, hopping), there was only one imageable meaning. The present study could also have employed a hierarchy, as did Baynes, Shenaut, Ober, Davis, D’Angelo, and Teague (2010), in presentation of templates, rather than presenting them essentially in random order. In the comprehensive group study planned by Davis and Baynes (2009) to rigorously investigate the behavioral and neural treatment effects of semantic and phonological training on a heterogeneous group of aphasia patients, the authors designed a set of computerized semantic tasks with two to three difficulty levels for each task type, with difficulty level manipulated through altering display and response times, varying the frequency of items in a category, and varying the semantic distance between the training items and the foils, using semantic analysis procedures. Participants in this study would not move on to the next level until they 86 reached criterion at the present level (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Future research may also wish to incorporate pre-intervention assessment to determine any individual variance on, or hierarchy of difficulty regarding, categorical, associative or perceptual levels. Although we do not know of any norms for which levels are generally found by patients to be more difficult, if there is a difference for the participant, then this should be taken into account so that training templates (and the number of templates of each level included in training sets) can be matched more precisely in terms of difficulty. In each condition, some templates contained orthographically presented questions and some did not. Questions for those that did not were presented verbally by the clinician. It is possible that this may have affected the results, since the percentages of templates in each condition that contained orthographically presented questions were not calculated precisely and therefore may have differed slightly between the two conditions. Nickels and Best (1996) found that “the modality of input can affect whether or not the therapy influences output, and this may be related to differences in comprehension in the two modalities.” Our therapy could have been more intensive, which might have helped consolidate learning (of List 4B, in particular): While the present study defined intensity partly in terms of intense focus on a small number of training items, and involved scheduling the maximum number of sessions per week thought possible for this participant due to transportation issues and other considerations, it might have been possible to train a volunteer or family member to provide additional practice on a home or laptop computer, as other researchers have done (Davis & Baynes, 2009). Some 87 authors have urged further investigation to determine both the ideal length of therapy and the number of hours of therapy per week that would be most likely to “allow for maximum recovery” (Bhogal, Teasell, & Speechley, 2003). In this as in any other study, a critical appraisal can suggest numerous ways to increase rigor and control for extraneous factors. While no study can control for every potential variable or confound, Nickels and Best (1996) noted that by varying aspects of tasks used in semantic therapy, it is possible to tease apart the differential treatment effects and identify which aspects of therapy may be crucial to success. “Although we are not recommending this ‘reductionist’ approach for clinical practice, it is nonetheless effective for determining why treatments may be differentially effective” (Nickels & Best, 1996). Conclusions Despite limitations, this comparison of two intensive, semantic decision-based, computerized verb interventions, one free of the requirement of speech production and one requiring spoken responses, was accompanied by improvements in naming of trained verbs in both conditions and improvements in spontaneous speech for a patient with fluent anomic aphasia. Findings support the use of semantic decision-based, productionfree training as valuable augmentation to conventional therapy for aphasia rehabilitation. In addition, verb training may have direct effects on spontaneous speech and fluency. Studies such as the present investigation suggest that implicit treatments have the potential to become an important part of an integrated approach to treatment that would allow clinicians to provide patients with intensive therapy that would otherwise be cost 88 prohibitive. Interventions which do not require explicit speech production may present the opportunity to develop treatment programs that can be adapted for use at home to offer extended practice with minimal supervision between speech therapy sessions, or to offer a means of allowing patients to continue improving their word retrieval skills long after their speech therapy sessions have ended. In the face of declining insurance coverage for therapy, interventions amenable to use on a home computer may provide an ideal method for individuals requiring or desiring additional treatment (Davis & Baynes, 2009; Petheram, 2004, Raymer, Kohen, et al., 2006). As one author noted, with clinicians everywhere increasingly required to “do more with less,” it may be “essential to explore alternative methods of service delivery,” for example, using clinicians “as consultants to patient and family rather than as the primary treatment provider” (Wertz, 2000). Further research is needed to determine if results of this study were directly related to the presence or absence of the requirement of speech production, and if so, whether results would replicate in other patients, in other aphasic syndromes, and in combination with other types of therapy approaches. In addition, future investigations may wish to consider which specific factors may have contributed to improvements in verb naming and spontaneous speech. 89 APPENDIX A Trained and Untrained Verb Lists Table A3. Phase One Trained and Untrained Verbs List List 1A: Trained Implicit List 2B: Trained Explicit Untrained List Verb Peeling Slicing Skating Biking Shoveling Jumping Splashing Chewing Pouring Closing paddling K-F Frequency 3 13 1 5 5 24 3 6 9 234 1 Baseline Performance (# of times missed) 3 3 1 1 1 0 3 3 3 1 3 Sneezing Sniffing Hiking Roasting Painting Balancing Feeding Digging Grilling Drying Yawning Total: 304 Average: 27.64 3 2 4 10 37 32 123 10 3 72 2 Total: 22 Average: 2.0 3 3 3 3 1 3 1 1 3 2 0 Tying Whispering Dipping Diving Plowing Climbing Falling Undressing Crawling Riding Juggling Total: 298 Average: 27.09 23 12 6 23 16 12 147 1 11 49 2 Total: 23 Average: 2.09 2 1 3 1 3 1 1 3 1 1 3 Total: 302 Average: 27.45 Total: 20 Average: 1.81 90 Table A4. Phase Two Trained and Untrained Verbs List List 3A Trained Explicit Verb Marching Dressing Filling Rescuing Squirting Popping Harvesting Grating Fixing Sewing Hopping K-F Frequency 120 67 50 15 1 8 12 3 14 6 2 Total: 298 Average: 27.09 List 4B Trained Implicit Untrained List Baseline Performance (# of times missed) 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 2 3 Reaching Rocking Baking Stirring Sailing Crouching Rolling Leaping Carving Swinging Floating 106 75 12 7 12 7 35 14 3 24 3 Total: 31 Average: 2.82 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 Resting Milking Towing Delivering Orbiting Scooping Punching Scrubbing Serenading Drilling Parachuting Total: 298 Average: 27.09 163 49 1 18 16 5 5 9 1 33 1 Total: 32 Average: 2.90 2 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 1 Total: 301 Average: 27.36 Total: 29 Average: 2.63 91 APPENDIX B Verb Use in Training Templates Table B5. Question Types with Sample Questions Single Target Multiple Targets Single Target with (Single Verb) (Single Verb) Embedded Negative (Single Verb) Which one is peeling? Which one goes with biking? Who can paint? Who paddles? Where can you skate? What do you chew with? What can you balance on? Which one will float? Which one do you sail? Who splashed? What goes best with baking? Which one was roasted? Which one swings? Which one would you slice? Who is crouching? Which one will splash? Where would you bake? Which one is about to roll? Which one is rocking? Multiple Targets with Embedded Negative (Single Verb) Which ones are leaping? Which ones go with painting? Which ones can yawn ? Which ones are hiking? Which two are balancing? Which three are feeding? Which ones would you feed? Which three are drying? Which two are digging? Which ones are harvesting? Which ones do you use for sewing? Which ones are rescuing? Which three can you close? Which two will splash? Which ones would you sniff? Which ones go with sneezing? Which three are filling? Which three go with dressing? Which one is not leaping? Which one is not grilling? Which one does not go with shoveling? What does not go with biking? Who is not jumping? Which one does not paddle? Which one is not for pouring? Which one does not hike? Who does not reach? Who is not dressing? Which one is not for fixing? Where can you not skate? Where can you not bike? Where should you not leap? What never closes? Who never marches? Which one was not rescued? Which one has not been filled? Which ones cannot float? Which ones do not go with grilling? Which ones should you not roast? Which ones can you not close? Which two cannot hike? Which three cannot bake? Which ones will not roll? Which ones cannot rock? Which ones can you not stir? Which three are not reaching? Which two were not baked? Which ones do not go with biking? Which ones are not stirring? Which two are not for baking? Which three do not go with dressing? Single Target (Multiple Verbs) Multiple Targets (Multiple Verbs) Single Target with Embedded Negative (Multiple Verbs) Multiple Targets with Embedded Negatives (Multiple Verbs) Which one is both floating and sailing? Which one will pop and squirt? What can crouch and leap? Which one is reaching and swinging? Which one is squirting and filling? Which one is rocking while floating? Which one is marching while dressing? Which one is feeding and grilling? Which one is both drying and yawning? Which one goes with stirring and baking? Which one floats and sails? Who is harvesting and filling? Which one can roll and float? Which one goes with both fixing and sewing? Which one goes with both leaping and carving? Which ones are both stirring and baking? Which ones go with either rocking or swinging? Which ones go with both fixing and sewing? Which ones are hopping and popping? Which ones are crouching or leaping? Which two are marching or hopping? Which three go with grating or harvesting? Which two are jumping and splashing? Which ones would you peel or slice? Which three go with carving or stirring? Which two are rocking, rolling or swinging? Where can you skate or bike? Where can you jump, splash or paddle? Who is reaching and carving? Which one is pouring but not splashing? What goes with grilling but not roasting? Which one is not sneezing or yawning? Which one is not sneezing, sniffing, or yawning? Which one is not filling or squirting? Which one is filling but not squirting? Which one is reaching but not crouching? Which one is not biking or skating? Which one does not go with peeling or slicing? Which one can you not roast or grill? Which one goes with sewing, but not dressing? Where can you skate but not bike? Where can you grate but not harvest? Which ones are not rocking, rolling, or swinging? Which ones cannot crouch or leap? Which three are popping but not squirting? Which ones go with stirring but not with baking? Which ones float but do not sail? Which ones do not go with grilling or roasting? Which two are crouching but not reaching? Which two are jumping but not splashing? Which three are paddling but not splashing? Which two would you not grate or harvest? Which two rock, but cannot roll? Which ones are for carving but not for stirring? Which ones cannot hop or pop? 92 Table B6. Sample Typical and Atypical Exemplars Verb Typical Exemplar Peeling Peeling a banana Closing Closing container (box) lid Pouring Balancing Pouring liquid into glass Standing on one leg Drying Drying with a towel Dressing Person getting dressed Filling Filling a glass with water at faucet Squirting Squirt gun squirting water Popping Popping balloon with a pin Stirring Stirring food or beverage with a spoon Rolling Ball rolling on ground Carving Etching design into wood or soap Swinging Child on playground swing Atypical Exemplar(s) Peeling a hard-boiled egg Peeling paint Peeling sunburned skin Peeling off adhesive backing Closing a shop Closing a gate Pouring rain Using balancing scales Balancing objects on head Air drying on clothesline Drying hair with blow dryer Air drying fingernails or hands Drying on a drying rack Drying meat or fish Dressing a doll or mannequin Putting a sweater on a dog Filling gas tank at gas pump Filling muffin cups with batter Filling swimming pool using hose Santa filling Christmas stocking Grapefruit squirting juice in eye Elephant squirting from trunk Eyes popping out Corn popping Children’s Popper toy popping Pop-up book “popping” open Pop-gun popping Fingers popping bubble wrap Champagne cork popping Popping soap bubbles Stirring paint with a stick Stirring witch’s cauldron Stirring chemicals in a lab Rolling dice Rolling dough with rolling pin Child rolling down a hill Rolling dough into a ball in hand Rolling object with wheels Rolling a tire or inner tube Log rolling on water Carving a turkey Carving a jack-o-lantern Carving Mount Rushmore Pendulum swinging to and fro Sign swinging on hinges Swinging bed or hammock Tarzan swinging on vine Swinging from chandelier 93 APPENDIX C Sample Training Templates . Templates with visual and featural similarities between images to offer greater challenge. 94 Which one is shoveling? Templates featuring single-target, single-verb questions were the primary template type used in the intervention, especially for early training of each new list. 95 What goes best with jumping? Images selected to match the participant’s areas of interest (cute babies and sports). 96 What goes with chewing? Templates featuring four images with strong categorical, associative, or perceptual similarities. 97 Who is biking? Templates with a strong unifying theme were thought to offer greater challenge. 98 Multiple-target, single-verb questions training multiple meanings/atypical exemplars of target verbs. 99 Template with multiple-target, single-verb question used in explicit training of feeding. Single-target, single-verb question with embedded negative. 100 As often as possible, images were selected to appeal to the participant’s sense of humor. 101 Templates were designed to be aesthetically pleasing, and to require intense focus. 102 Whenever possible, the participant’s own most frequent error productions (e.g., mowing/harvesting) were incorporated as foils. Images on templates were visually balanced for maximum visual appeal. 103 Multiple-target, single-verb question with embedded negative. Single-target, multiple verb question: Which one rocks and floats? 104 Multiple-target, multiple verb question (Which two are both reaching and leaping?”) incorporating football, one of the participant’s primary areas of interest. Primate-themed template featuring single-target, multiple-verb question with embedded negative. 105 Single-target, multiple-verb question with embedded negative (and cute hamsters). . Versatile template suitable for single target, multiple verb question with embedded negative (Which one rocks, but does not swing or float?) 106 Multiple-target, multiple-verb question with embedded negative (and cute pandas). Versatile bride-themed template used for training hiking, balancing, feeding, and grilling. 107 Versatile template.used for a variety of question types, e.g., single-target, single-verb questions (Which one goes best with peeling?), multiple-target, single-verb questions (Which two can you peel with?), Multiple-target, single-verb questions with embedded negatives (Which three can you not slice with?), Single-target, multiple-verb questions (Which one can you peel or slice with?), Single-target, multiple-verb questions with embedded negatives (Which one can peel but not slice?), and multiple-target, multipleverb questions with embedded negatives (Which two cannot peel or slice?). Versatile template used for single-target, single-verb question (Which one can roll?), multiple-target, single-verb question (Which two rock?), or single-target, multiple-verb question with embedded negative (Which one does not roll or rock?). 108 A Name-the-Verb template, one of the only template types occasionally used during one condition (explicit training) but not in the other condition (implicit training), due to the requirement of explicit naming. The implicit therapy counterpart to the explicit therapy Name-the-Verb template type (see Figure 29, above). For this template, the clinician asked “Which one is grilling/balancing/yawning/drying” questions. 109 REFERENCES Albert, M. L. (2003). Aphasia Therapy Works! [Editorial]. Stroke, 34, 992. Aphasia Advocacy Foundation (2010). What is aphasia? Retrieved from http://home.comcast.net/~aafnh/Aphasia.htm Ashenfelter, K.T., & Eberhard, K.M. (2007). Effects of verb complexity on speech errors. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1527-1541. Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 53-58. Barde, L. H. F., Schwartz, M. F., Boronat, C. B. (2006). Semantic weight and verb retrieval in aphasia. Brain and Language, 97, 266-278. Barry, C., & McHattie, J. (1991, September). Depth of semantic processing in picture naming facilitation in aphasic patients. Paper presented at the British Aphasiology Society Conference, Sheffield. Basso, A. (2005). How intensive/prolonged should an intensive/prolonged treatment be? Aphasiology, 19, 975-984. Basso, A., Capitani, E., & Vignolo, L. (1979). Influence of rehabilitation of language skills in aphasic patients: a controlled study. Archives of Neurology, 36, 190-196. Basso, A., & Caporali, A. (2001). Aphasia therapy or the importance of being earnest. Aphasiology, 15, 307-332. doi: 10.1080/02687040042000304 Baynes, K., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Callosal disconnection. In M. J. Farah & T. E. Feinberg (Eds.), Patient-based approaches to cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 349-357). Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press. 110 Baynes, K., Share, L. J., & Redfern, B. B. (1995). Dual access to the lexicon in production: A targeted rehabilitation study. Brain and Language, 51, 26-29. Baynes, K., Shenaut, G. K., Ober, B.A., Davis, C.H., D’Angelo, E., & Teague, A. (2010). Use of semantic intervention in rehabilitation of naming in fluent aphasia. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 6, 227-228. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.114 Baynes, K. Truong, E., Jonathan, D., Farias, D., & Davis, C. (2008, October). Two levels of treatment for phonological alexia: A case study. Poster session presented at the Academy of Aphasia, Turku, Finland. Baynes, K., Wessinger, C.M., Fendrich, R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1993). Emergence of access to speech in a disconnected right hemisphere. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19, 1809. Baynes, K., Wessinger, C.M., Fendrich, R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). The emergence of the capacity to name left visual field stimuli in a callosotomy patient: implications for functional plasticity. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1225-1242. Beeson, P.M., & Robey, R.R. (2006). Evaluating single-subject treatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasia literature. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 161169. Behrmann, M., & Lieberthal, T. (1989). Category-specific treatment of a lexicalsemantic deficit: a single case study of global aphasia. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 281-299. 111 Best, W., & Nickels, L. (2000). From theory to therapy in aphasia: Where are we now and where to next? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10, 231-247. Bhogal, S.K., Teasell, R. W., Foley, N. C., & Speechley, M. R. (2003). Rehabilitation of aphasia: More is better. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 10, 66-76. Bhogal, S. K., Teasell, R., & Speechley, M. (2003). Intensity of aphasia therapy, impact on recovery. Stroke, 34, 987-993. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000062343.64383.D0 Boyle, M. (2001). Semantic feature analysis: The evidence for treating lexical impairments in aphasia. ASHA Special Interest Division 2: Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders, 11, 23-28. Boyle, M., & Coelho, C. A. (1995). Application of semantic feature analysis as a treatment for aphasic dysnomia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 4, 94-98. Braun, S. M., Beurskens, A. J., Borm, P. J., Schack, T., & Wade, D. T. (2006). The effects of mental practice in stroke rehabilitation: A systematic review. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 87, 842-852. Brindley, P., Copeland, M., Demain, C., & Martyn, P. (1989). A comparison of the speech of ten chronic Broca’s aphasics following intensive and non-intensive periods of therapy. Aphasiology, 3, 695-707. Brookshire, R.H. (1972). Effects of task difficulty on naming performance of aphasic subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 551-558. 112 Brookshire, R.H. (1976). Effects of task difficulty on sentence comprehension performance of aphasic subjects. Journal of Communication Disorders, 9, 167173. Brookshire, R., Nicholas, L., Redmond, K., & Krueger, K.. (1979). Effects of clinician behaviors on acceptability of patients’ responses in aphasia treatment sessions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 12, 369-384. Buckingham, H.W., & Christman, S. S. (2004). Perseveration in neurogenic communication disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 25, 291-293. Cano, A., Hernandez, M., Ivanova, I., Juncadella, M., Gascon-Bayarri, J., Rene, R., et al. (2010). When one can write SALTO as noun but not as verb: A grammatical category-specific, modality-specific deficit. Brain and Language, 114, 26-42. Caramazza, A. (2000). Aspects of lexical access: Evidence from aphasia. In Y. Grodinsky, L. Shapiro, & D. Swinney (Eds.), Language and the Brain (pp. 203228). San Diego: Academic Press. Coelho, C., McHugh, R., & Boyle, M. (2000). Semantic feature analysis as a treatment for aphasic dysnomia: A replication. Aphasiology, 14, 133-142. Cohen, R., Engel, D., Kelter, S., & List, G. (1979). Kurz- und Langzeiteffekte von Benennenhilfen bei Aphatikern. In G. Peuser (Ed.), Studien zur Sprachtherapie. Munich: Wilhelm Fink. Cornelissen, K., Laine, M., Tarkiainen, A., Jarvensivu, T., Martin, N., & Salmelin, R. (2003). Adult brain plasticity elicited by anomia treatment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 444-461. 113 Cramer, S.C., Orr, E.L., Cohen, M. J., & Lacourse, M. G. (2007). Effects of motor imagery training after chronic, complete spinal cord injury. Experimental Brain Research, 177, 233-242. Damasio, A. R., & Tranel, D. (1993), “Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proceedigs of the National Academy of Science USA, 90, 4957-4960. Davis, C., & Baynes, K. (2009). [Examining effectiveness of intensive implicit semantic and phonological treatment after stroke: Using information about the nature of patients’ deficits and lesions to develop improved guidelines for treatment recommendations]. Unpublished grant proposal. Davis, C., Baynes, K., & Hess, G. (2009, May). Comparison of implicit with standard (explicit) treatment of aphasia. Poster session presented at the annual Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Keystone, Colorado. Davis, C. H., & Farias, D. (2010, March). Implicit therapy: Semantic-based therapy that discourages verbal production. Presented at annual convention of the California Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Monterey, CA. Davis, C. H., Farias, D., & Baynes, K. (2005). Understanding the effects of cuing strategies through error analysis. Brain and Language, 95, 183-184. Davis, C., Farias, D., & Baynes, K. (2008, May). Comparison of implicit and explicit methods for treatment of aphasia: a case study. Poster session presented at the Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Jackson Hole, WY. Abstract retrieved from http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00001914/ 114 Davis, C., Farias, D., & Baynes, K. (2009). Implicit phoneme manipulation for the treatment of apraxia of speech and co-occurring aphasia. Aphasiology, 23, 503528. Davis, C., Farias, D., Baynes, K., Chand, V., & Orgun, O. (2007). Is there a role for mental practice in speech rehabilitation? Brain Impairment, 8, 198. Davis, C., Farias, D., Bord, A., & Baynes, K. (2006). An implicit phonological intervention in an individual with apraxia of speech: A preliminary analysis. Brain and Language, 99, 16-17. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.018 Davis, C., Farias, D., & Wilson, S. (2011, October). Imaging the effects of implicit treatment for apraxia of speech. Poster session presented at the meeting of Academy of Aphasia, Montreal, Canada. Davis, C., Harrington, G., & Baynes, K. (2003, October). Effects of error-free, intensive semantic therapy: An fMRI study. Poster session presented at the Aphasia Therapy Workshop, Austrian Academy of Sciences, Vienna, Austria. Davis, C., Harrington, G., & Baynes, K. (2006). Case study: Intensive semantic intervention in fluent aphasia: A pilot study with fMRI. Aphasiology, 20, 59-83. Davis, C., Hess, G., & Baynes, K. (2010). Comparison of production-free with conventional treatment of aphasia. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 6, 223-224. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.112 De Bleser, R. Negative optic aphasia: How much semantics does a name need? Wolff’s re-examination of Voit. Aphasiology, 23, 1427-1437. 115 Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 92, 283-321. Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801-838. Dormann, C., & Biddle, R. (2006). Humor in game-based learning. Learning, Media and Technology, 31, 411-424. Drew, R. L., & Thompson, C. K. (1999). Model-based semantic treatment for naming deficits in aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 972989. Dronkers, N. F., Husted, D. A., Deutsch, G., Taylor, K., Saunders, G., & Merzenich, M. (1999). Lesion site as a predictor of improvement after “Fast ForWord” treatment in adult aphasic patients. Brain and Language, 69, 450-452. Dukette, D., Baynes, K., Redfern, L., Share, L., Ludy, C., & Dronkers, N. (1998). Lexical access in production: A targeted rehabilitation study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 68-69. Farias, D., Davis, C., & Harrington, G. (2006). Drawing: Its contribution to naming in aphasia. Brain and Language, 97, 53-63. Farias, D., Davis, C. H., Lundstrom, S., & Osecheck, M. (2002). Drawing: Its contribution to naming in aphasia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 780. 116 Farias, D., Davis, C., & Whittmann, R. (2009, October). The treatment effects of phonological complexity on apraxia of speech: An implicit alternative. Poster session presented at the annual conference of the Academy of Aphasia, Boston, Massachusetts. Faroqi-Shah, Y., Gassert, J., & Wood, E. J. (2009, May). Kissing slows licking: An investigation of body part overlap in verb priming. Poster session presented at the annual Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Keystone, CO. Abstract retrieved from http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00001985/ Faroqi-Shah, Y., Wood, E., & Gassert, J. (2010). Verb impairment in aphasia: A priming study of body part overlap. Aphasiology, doi:10.1080/02687030903515362 Fillingham, J. K., Hodgson, C., Sage, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2003). The application of errorless learning to aphasic disorders: A review of theory and practice. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13, 337-363. Fillingham, J. K., Sage, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). The treatment of anomia using errorless learning. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 16, 129-154. Francis, W. N., & Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Co. Frattali, C. (2004). An errorless learning approach to treating dysnomia in frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 12(3), 11-14. Frattali, C., & Kang, Y. K. (2004). An errorless learning approach to treating dysnomia. Brain and Language, 91, 177-178. 117 Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. Academic Press: San Diego, CA. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (2000). Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: Boston Naming Test. New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Goodglass, H. Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders (3rd ed.). New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Graham, L.E. (2009). Verb naming treatment for individuals with agrammatic aphasia: Efficacy data. (master’s thesis). Retrieved from http://drum.lib.umd.edu/bitstream/1903/9341/1/Graham_umd_0117N_10384.pdf Harrington, G., Farias, D., & Davis, C. (2009). The neural basis for simulated drawing and the semantic implications. Cortex, 45, 386-393. Hashimoto, N., & Thompson, C. K. (2010). The use of picture-word interference paradigm to examine naming abilities in aphasic individuals. Aphasiology, 24, 580-611. Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In J.F. Delafresnaye (Ed.), Brain Mechanisms and learning (pp. 37-46). Oxford: Blackwell. Herbert, R., Hickin, J., Howard, D., Osborne, F., & Best, W. (2008). Do picture-naming tests provide a valid assessment of lexical retrieval in conversation in aphasia? Aphasiology, 22, 184-203. Hillis, A.E. (1989). Efficacy and generalization of treatment for aphasic naming errors. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 70, 632-636. 118 Hillis, A. E. (1991). Effects of separate treatments for distinct impairments within the naming process. Clinical Aphasiology, 19, 255-265. Retrieved from http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00000120/01/19-24.pdf Hinckley, J. J. (2002). Vocational and social outcomes of adults with chronic aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 543-560. Howard, D. (1992). Cognitive neuropsychology and rehabilitation. In von Cramon, D., Poeppel, E., & von Steinbuechel, N. (Eds.), Brain Damage and Rehabilitation: A Neuropsychological Approach (pp. 146-154). Berlin: Springer Verlag. Howard D. (2000). Cognitive neuropsychology and aphasia therapy: The case of word retrieval. In Papathanasiou, I. (Ed.), Acquired Neurogenic Communication Disorders (pp. 76-99). London: Whurr Publishers Ltd. Howard, D., & Hatfield, F.M. (1985). Competing approaches to treatment: Aphasia therapy in modern times. In J. Oxbury, M. Coltheart, R. Whurr, & M. Wyke (Eds.), Aphasia. London: Butterworth. Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1992). The Pyramids and Palm Trees Test. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. Howard, D., Patterson, K.E., Franklin, S., Orchard-Lisle, V., & Morton, J. (1985a). The facilitation of picture naming in aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 49-80. Howard, D., Patterson, K., Franklin, S., Orchard-Lisle, V., & Morton, J. (1985b). The treatment of word retrieval deficits in aphasia: A comparison of two therapy methods. Brain, 108, 817-829. 119 Humphreys, G. W., Price, C. J., & Riddoch, M. J. (1999). From objects to names: A cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Research, 62, 118-130. Jackson, P. L., Lafleur, M. F., Malouin, F., Richards, C. L., & Doyon, J. (2003). Functional cerebral reorganization following motor sequence learning through mental practice with motor imagery. NeuroImage, 20, 1171-1180. Janata, P. (2009, December). Music, Memories and the Brain. Paper presented at OMNI meeting, UC Davis Center for Mind and Brain, Davis, CA. Janata, P. (2009). The Neural Architecture of Music-Evoked Autobiographical Memories. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2579-2594. doi:10.1093./cercor/bhp008 Jones, E. V. (1986). Building the foundations for sentence production in a non-fluent aphasic. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 21, 6382. Kambanaros, M. (2008). The trouble with nouns and verbs in Greek fluent aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 1-19. Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001). Boston Naming Test (second edition). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins. Katz, R. T., & Wertz, R. T. (1997). The efficacy of computer-provided reading treatment for chronic aphasic adults. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 493-507. Kendall, D. L., Rosenbek, J. C., Heilman, K. M., Conway, T., Klenberg, K., Rothi, L. J. G., et al. (2008). Phoneme-based rehabilitation of anomia in aphasia. Brain and Language, 105, 1-17. 120 Kemmerer, D., Castillo, J. G., Talavage, T., Patterson, S., & Wiley, C. (2008). Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: Evidence from fMRI. Brain and Language, 107, 16-43. Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune & Stratton. Kiran, S. (2008). Typicality of inanimate category exemplars in aphasia treatment: Further evidence for semantic complexity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1550-1568. Kiran, S. D. (2001). Effect of exemplar typicality on naming deficits in fluent aphasia. Dissertation Abstracts International Section B the Sciences Engineering, 62, 5076. Kiran, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2001, May). Effect of exemplar typicality on naming deficits in fluent aphasia. Paper presented at the Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Santa Fe, NM. Kiran, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2003). The role of semantic complexity in treatment of naming deficits: Training semantic categories in fluent aphasia by controlling exemplar typicality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 773-787. Komatsu, S., Mimura, M., Kato, M., Wakamatsu, N., & Kashima, H. (2000). Errorless and effortful processes involved in the learning of face-name associations by patients with alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10, 113-132. 121 LaPointe, L. (Ed.) (2005). Aphasia and related neurogenic disorders. New York: Thieme. Le Dorze, G., Boulay, N., Gaudreau, J., & Brassard, C. (1994). The contrasting effects of a formal-semantic technique for the facilitation of naming in a case of anomia. Aphasiology, 8, 127-141. Levelt, W. J. M. (1993). The Architecture of Normal Spoken Language Use. In G. Blanken, J. Dittman, H. Grimm, J. C. Marshall, & C. W. Wallesch (Eds.), Linguistic Disorders and Pathologies: An International Handbook (pp. 1-15). New York: Walter de Gruyter. Levelt, W.J.M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1-75. Lincoln, N. B., McGuirk, E., Mulley, G.P., Lendrem, W., Jones, A. C., & Mitchell, J. R. A. (1984). Effectiveness of speech therapy for aphasic stroke patients: a randomized controlled trial. Lancet, 1, 1197-200. Maher, L. M., & Raymer, A. M. (2004). Management of anomia. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 11, 10-21. Martin, N., Dell, G.S., Saffran, E.M., & Schwartz, M.F. (1994). Origins of paraphasias in deep dysphasia: Testing the consequences of a decay impairment to an interactive spreading activation model of lexical retrieval. Brain and Language, 47, 609-660. 122 Mazzoni, M., Vista, M., Geri, E,. Avila, L., Bianchi, F., & Moretti, P. (1995). Comparison of language recovery in rehabilitated and matched, non-rehabilitated aphasic patients. Aphasiology, 9, 553-563. McCann, C., & Greig, L. (2010). Moving on from single words: The use of dynamic video stimuli for verb therapy. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 6, 260261. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.130 McKissock, S., & Ward, J. (2007). Do errors matter? Errorless and errorful learning in anomic picture naming. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 17, 355-373. Miceli, G., Amitrano, A., Capasso, R., & Caramazza, A. (1996). The treatment of anomia resulting from output lexical damage: Analysis of two cases. Brain and Language, 52, 150-174. National Aphasia Association (2010). Aphasia Frequently Asked Questions. Retrieved from http://www.aphasia.org/index.html Nickels, L. (2001). Spoken word production. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 291-320). New York: Psychology Press. Nickels, L. (2002a). Improving word finding: Practice makes (closer to) perfect? Aphasiology, 16, 1047-1060. Nickels, L. (2002b). Therapy for naming disorders: Revisiting, revising, and reviewing. Aphasiology, 16, 935-979. Nickels, L. (Ed.) (2003). Rehabilitation of Spoken Word Production in Aphasia. New York: Psychology Press. 123 Nickels, L., & Best, W. (1996). Therapy for naming disorders (Part II): Specifics, surprises, and suggestions. Aphasiology, 10, 109-136. Nooteboom, S. (2004, June). Self-monitoring of inner speech and overt speech. Poster session abstract included in the conference proceedings of From Sound to Sense: 50+ Years of Discoveries in Speech Communication, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Off, C., Kavalier, H., & Rogers, M. (2008). Repetition priming and anomia: An investigation of stimulus dosage. Presented at the Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Jackson Hole, Wy. Retrieved from http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00001951/ Oppenheim, G. M., & Dell, G. S. (2008). Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition, 106, 528-537. Patterson, K. E., Purell, C., & Morton, J. (1983). Facilitation of word retrieval in aphasia. In C. Code and D. J. Muller (Eds.), Aphasia therapy. London: Arnold. Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Collina, S., Rosa, M. M., et al. (1999). The neural correlates of verb and noun processing: A PET study. Brain, 122, 2337-2344. Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Tettamanti, M., Rosa, M., Scifo, P., Miozzo, A…Fazio, F. (2003). A fMRI study of word retrieval in aphasia. Brain and Language, 85, 357368. Petheram, B. (2004). Computers and aphasia: A means of delivery and a delivery of means. Aphasiology, 18, 187-191. 124 Poeck, K., Huber, W., & Willmes, K. (1989). Outcome of intensive language treatment in aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 471-479. Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Characterizing the neural mechanisms of skill learning and repetition priming: Evidence from mirror reading. Brain, 124, 67-82. Potter, R.E., & Goodman, N. J. (1983). The implementation of laughter as a therapy facilitator with adult aphasics. Journal of Communication Disorders, 16, 41-46. Pring, T., White-Thompson, M., Pound, C., Marshall, J., & Davis, A. (1990). Pictureword matching tasks and word retrieval: Some follow-up data and second thoughts. Aphasiology, 4, 479-483. Raymer, A. M. (2005). Naming and word-retrieval problems. In LaPointe, L. (Ed.), Aphasia and related language disorders (pp. 68-82). New York: Thieme. Raymer, A. M., & Ellsworth, T. A. (2002). Response to contrasting verb retrieval treatments: A case study. Aphasiology, 16, 1031-1045. Raymer, A., & Kohen, F. (2006). Word-retrieval treatment in aphasia: Effects of sentence context. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 43, 367-378. Raymer, A., Kohen, F. P., & Saffell, D. (2006). Computerised training for impairments of word comprehension and retrieval in aphasia. Aphasiology, 20, 257-268. Roelofs, A. (2004). Error bias in spoken word planning and monitoring by aphasic and nonaphasic speakers: Comment on Rapp and Goldrick (2000). Psychological Review, 111, 561-572. 125 Rose, M., Douglas, J., & Matyas, T. (2002). The comparative effectiveness of gesture and verbal treatments for a specific phonological impairment. Aphasiology, 16, 10011030. Roseberry-McKibbin, C., & Hegde, M. N. (2006). An Advanced Review of SpeechLanguage Pathology (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-ed. Ruff, I., Blumstein, S. E., Myers, E. B., & Hutchinson, E. (2008). Recruitment of anterior and posterior structures in lexical-semantic processing: An fMRI study comparing implicit and explicit tasks. Brain and Language, 105, 41-49. Scott, C. (1987). Cognitive neuropsychological remediation of acquired language disorders. Unpublished master’s thesis, Birkbeck College, University of London. Sharma, N., Pomeroy, V. M., & Baron, J. C. (2006). Motor imagery: A backdoor to the motor system after stroke? Stroke, 37, 1941-1952. Shuster, L. (2011). ACNDS Annual Meeting: Philadelphia, PA. Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders & Sciences Newsletter, 9(1). Retrieved from http://www.ancds.org/pdf/newsletter/ANCDS_Spring_2011_1.pdf. Simmons-Mackie, N., & Schultz, M. (2003). The role of humor in therapy for aphasia. Aphasiology, 17, 751-766. Stadler, M. A. (1993). Implicit serial learning: Questions inspired by Hebb (1961). Memory and Cognition, 21, 819-827. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195-231. 126 Turkstra, L., & Bourgeois, M. (2005). Intervention for a modern-day HM: Errorless learning of practical goals. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 205-212. Tyler, L. K., Bright, P., Fletcher, P., & Stamatakis, E. A. (2004). Neural processing of nouns and verbs: The role of inflectional morphology. Neuropsychologia, 42, 512-523. Tyler, L.K., Randall, B., & Stamatakis, E. A. (2008). Cortical differentiation for nouns and verbs depends on grammatical markers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1381-1389. U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs & Department of Defense (2003). VA/DoD clinical practice guideline for the management of stroke rehabilitation in the primary care setting (Version 1.1). Retrieved from http://www.healthquality.va.gov/stroke/stroke_fulltext.pdf Van der Merwe, A. (2007). Self-correction in apraxia of speech: The effect of treatment. Aphasiology, 21, 658-669. Wade, J., Mortley, J., Enderby, P. (2003). Talk about IT: Views of people with aphasia and their partners on receiving remotely monitored computer-based word finding therapy. Aphasiology, 17, 1031-1056. Wambaugh, J. (2010). Treatment of Acquired Apraxia of Speech: Updates and Current Issues. Presentation at the meeting of the Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences. Retrieved from http://www.ancds.org/documents/2010_Meeting/Wambaugh.pdf 127 Wambaugh, J. L., Duffy, J. R., McNeil, M. R., Robin, D. A., & Rogers, M. A. (2006). Treatment guidelines for acquired apraxia of speech: Treatment descriptions and recommendations. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 14, pp. xxxvlxvii. Wambaugh, J. L., Linebaugh, C. W., Doyle, P. J., Martinez, A. L., & Kalinyak-Fliszar, M. M. (2001). Effects of two cueing treatments on lexical retrieval in aphasic speakers with different levels of deficit. Aphasiology, 15, 933-950. Webster, J., & Gordon, B. (2009). Contrasting therapy effects for verb and sentence processing difficulties: A discussion of what worked and why. Aphasiology, 23, 1231-1251. Wertz, R. T. (2000). Aphasia therapy: A clinical framework. In Papathanasiou, I. (Ed.), Acquired Neurogenic Communication Disorders (pp. 3-27). London: Whurr Publishers Ltd. Wertz, R. T., Weiss, D. G., Aten, J.L., Brookshire, R.H., Garcia-Bunuel, L., Holland, A.L., . . . Goodman, R. (1986). Comparison of clinic, home, and deferred language treatment for aphasia: A Veterans Administration cooperative study. Archives of Neurology, 43, 653-658. Whitworth, A., Webster, J., & Howard, D. (2005). A cognitive neuropsychological approach to assessment and intervention in aphasia: A clinician’s guide. New York: Psychology Press.