165 NOTES Differences in key habitat use between dominant and subordinate animals: intraterritorial dominance payoffs in Eurasian badgers? Eloy Revilla and Francisco Palomares Abstract: Group-living territorial animals such as the Eurasian badger, Meles meles L., face the problem of intragroup competition. Badgers are asymmetric in their access to reproduction (dominant individuals being the ones that reproduce), but little information exists about the extent of intragroup trophic competition. We studied badgers’ use of a key trophic resource (the European rabbit, Oryctolagus cuniculus), as well as the use of the habitat where this resource is located (key habitat, Mediterranean scrubland) by a low-density group-living population of badgers in Coto del Rey, Doñana, southwestern Spain. During 1995–1996, there was a 2.2-fold reduction of rabbit density, which was reflected in a significant diminution of rabbit use; despite this, rabbits continued to be the most used trophic resource. Notwithstanding the decrease in rabbit density, subordinate badgers reduced their use of the key habitat, while dominant badgers increased it. These results suggest that in Coto del Rey, badger groups exhibit a fully despotic system, with dominance by some individuals not only in access to reproduction, but also in access to food through unequal use of the key habitat that contains it. Résumé : Les animaux terrestres qui vivent en groupes, tels le Blaireau d’Eurasie, Meles meles L., doivent faire face à la compétition intra-groupe. Les blaireaux sont asymétriques quant à leur accès à la reproduction (les individus dominants sont les reproducteurs), mais il existe peu d’information sur l’importance de la compétition trophique intragroupe. Nous avons examiné l’utilisation d’une ressource trophique critique (le Lapin de garenne, Oryctolagus cuniculus) et l’utilisation de l’habitat où se trouve cette ressource (habitat optimal, maquis méditerrannéen), chez une population de petite densité de blaireaux vivant en groupes (Coto del Rey, Doñana, sud-ouest de l’Espagne). En 1995–1996, il s’est produit une réduction de la densité des lapins par un facteur de 2,2. La réduction de la disponibilité des lapins a causé une diminution significative de la prédation exercée sur eux, mais les lapins sont restés la ressource trophique de prédilection. Indépendamment de la diminution de la densité des lapins, les blaireaux subordonnés se sont mis à moins utiliser l’habitat optimal, alors que les dominants ont entrepris de les utiliser davantage. Ces résultats semblent indiquer qu’à Coto del Rey, les groupes de blaireaux sont organisés en un système despotique où quelques individus dominent l’accès à la reproduction et ont en plus l’avantage de l’accès à la nourriture à cause de l’utilisation inégale de l’habitat optimal. [Traduit par la Rédaction] Introduction Intraspecific territoriality is produced by asymmetric despotic competition for a given resource (Davies 1978; Gordon E. Revilla1 and F. Palomares. Department of Applied Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñana, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, Avenida Maria Luisa s/n, 41013 Sevilla, Spain. 1 Author to whom all correspondence should be sent at the following address: Department of Ecological Modelling, Umweltforschungszentrum (UFZ) (Centre for Environmental Research), Leipzig–Halle, P.O. Box 2, D-04301 Leipzig, Germany (e-mail: elsa@oesa.ufz.de). 1997). The main benefit to territory owners is a greater probability of holding this resource and, eventually, greater overall fitness (Stamps 1994). On the other hand, group living is favoured by such factors as high predation pressure and patchy resource distribution (Alexander 1974; Wrangham and Rubenstein 1986). Resource competition among group members has been reported in many social-living species (Barton and Whiten 1993; Hall and Fedigan 1997; Packer and Caro 1997; Schmidt and Mech 1997). Therefore, the role of intragroup competition for the resource that promotes or reinforces territoriality might be an important factor in the ecology of territorial species living in groups. Trophic resources have been proposed as one of the most important determinants of both territoriality and group living. In some cases, intragroup competition is reduced through the exploitation of renewable resources (Waser 1981). In others, it has been suggested that patchy resource distribution allows the primary owner to share the territory with other individuals at low cost (Macdonald 1983; Kruuk and Macdonald 1985; Carr and Macdonald 1986). Eurasian badgers, Meles meles L., are territorial, solitary foragers (Kruuk 1989; Woodroffe and Macdonald 1993). At high population density, they live in groups, sharing a communal territory but exhibiting little or no cooperation between individuals other than shared territory defence (Kruuk 1989; Woodroffe and Macdonald 1993). Under these circumstances, food competition within the group is presumed to be low, making living in a common territory inexpensive. Locally, group-living populations of badgers may rely on one food type, mostly earthworms (Lumbricus spp.) or European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus; for example, see Kruuk and Parish 1981; Martín et al. 1995). These trophic resources are concentrated in key habitats: deciduous woodlands and pastures in the case of earthworms (Kruuk 1978; Hofer 1988; Kruuk 1989) and Mediterranean scrubland in the case of rabbits (Palomares et al.1996; Palomares and Delibes 1997). Access to reproduction is asymmetric among group members, determining social status (Kruuk and Parish 1987; Cresswell et al. 1992; Woodroffe and Macdonald 1995). Female body condition, which depends on resource availability, is, in combination with age, an important factor determining reproduction probability (Cresswell et al. 1992; Woodroffe and Macdonald 1995). Under normal conditions, competition for trophic resources is thought to be negligible within groups (Woodroffe and Macdonald 1993). However, the question of whether intragroup competition occurs under trophic stress remains unanswered, despite its importance in understanding the regulation of the social system of badgers. Two alternative answers to this question are possible. If the territory is equally accessible to all individuals, there should be no asymmetry in access to the key trophic resource and, hence, they would be expected to suffer equally from food stress (scramble competition). In contrast, if badgers are unequal competitors, with a dominance hierarchy in access not only to reproduction but also to food when it is limiting, dominant animals should preferentially use the key habitat that contains the resource (contest competition). We studied the use of the main trophic resource, and the use of the key habitat holding that resource, by a groupliving population of badgers that uses rabbits as the staple food. After a decline in the rabbit population, badgers were still using this lagomorph as their main prey, but the use of the key habitat changed between individuals of different reproductive status. Methods Study area The study was carried out during 1995 and 1996 in an area called Coto del Rey at the northern border of Doñana National Park in southwestern Spain. The climate is Mediterranean subhumid with Atlantic influence. Winters are wet and mild, while summers are hot and dry; the normal annual rainfall is 500–600 mm/m2. During the study there was a shift from a drought (252 mm/m2 of rainfall during winter 1995) to a torrential rainfall period with frequent flooding events (1031 mm/m2 in winter 1996). Thus, we distinguish between a dry period (winter, spring, and summer 1995) and a wet period (autumn 1995 and all of 1996). We considered seasons to be as follows: winter, January to March; spring, April to June; summer, July to September; and autumn, October to December. We defined the study area (42 km2) as a minimum convex polygon enclosing all the radiolocations of marked badgers (see below). This area is covered by 49.0% marshland, 14.7% pine plantations, 12.1% Mediterranean scrubland, 11.6% pastureland, 8.2% Mediterranean savannah (“dehesa”), 2.6% ash stands, and 1.1% Eucalyptus spp. plantations (Revilla et al. 2000). Vegetation types define the habitat types (for a further description of the study area and vegetation types see Revilla et al. 2000). Badger population We trapped, measured, and weighed badgers, marked them with a microchip, and equipped them with a radio collar (A. Wagener, Telemetrieanlagen HF-NF Technik, Herwarthstraße 22, Köln, Germany). Marked animals belonged to three and four groups in 1995 and 1996, respectively. Territory size was, on average, 406 ha (defined as the 95% kernel isoline; Revilla 1998). Badger density was 0.83 and 0.75 individuals/km2 in 1995 and 1996, respectively (Revilla et al. 1999). The average number of animals per social group was 3.2 (excluding yearlings, estimated using sightings at setts and track censuses; for more details see Revilla et al. 1999). The number of animals present (for at least part of the time) in the population was 11 and 8, of which 9 and 7 were radio-marked (82–88%), during 1995 and 1996, respectively. Animals were radiolocated via triangulation (Revilla et al. 2000). We defined the social status of individuals in terms of their reproductive asymmetry: dominants were those that showed reproductive activity. In the case of females, pregnancy and lactation during any capture was sufficient for them to be considered dominant, since there was no case of double reproduction within any territory (Revilla et al. 1999). The oldest males, which also had the largest descended testes, were considered dominants. Subordinates were animals born on the territory in previous years, but that showed no reproductive activity (despite being sexually mature; Revilla et al. 1999). In total, we radio-tracked 12 different individuals, of which 6 were considered dominants and 6 subordinates. Resource variation Rabbits are the main prey species for a large number of predators in Mediterranean ecosystems (Delibes and Hiraldo 1981), and badgers are no exception (Martín et al. 1995; Fedriani et al. 1998; Revilla 1998). The rabbit population of Coto del Rey has been the subject of long-term monitoring (Palomares et al. 1996, 2000; Palomares and Delibes 1997). Rabbit abundance was estimated by line-transect sampling (Buckland et al. 1993) carried out in randomly selected areas within the habitat types considered. Transects ranged in length from 1100 to 2040 m per habitat and were slowly (ca. 1.5–2.5 km/h) surveyed at dusk. We grouped observation distances of rabbits into 10-m intervals and truncated the distance at 50 m to improve the robustness of the estimator of density. The truncation distance and grouping options were chosen after three pilot samplings. Transects were always the same, did not follow any track or road in the study area, followed a straight line, and were always walked by the same observer. Rabbits were counted seasonally during the period of study. We estimated rabbit density (and 95% confidence intervals) with the program TRANSECT (Burnham et al. 1980). (For more information about differences between habitat types, characteristics of the rabbit population, and methodology see Palomares et al. 2000.) Resource use Diet To detect changes in the importance of rabbits as a trophic resource during the two periods, we studied diet through analysis of faeces collected at setts. We followed the general methodology used in other studies of badger feeding habits (see Kruuk and © 2001 NRC Canada Notes 167 Fig. 1. Use of Mediterranean scrubland habitat (key habitat) during the foraging period by dominant and subordinate Eurasian badgers (Meles meles), and rabbit abundance (number/ha) in Mediterranean scrubland, in Coto del Rey, Doñana, southwestern Spain. Data are shown as the mean and standard error (95% confidence intervals for rabbit abundance) for winter (WI), spring (SP), summer (SU), and autumn (AU) of 1995 (95) and 1996 (96). Parish 1981; Martín et al. 1995), except for estimating ingested biomass, for which we used correction factors calculated in a feeding trial (Revilla 1998). In total we analysed 624 and 382 scats from 1995 and 1996. Differences in the pattern of use (presence–absence of rabbit in every scat) between seasons and years were analysed with multiway analysis of frequencies (logit models, procedure CATMOD; SAS Institute Inc. 1990b). The significance level for posthoc comparisons was set with the Bonferroni correction. Habitat Mediterranean scrubland is the only habitat type significantly preferred by badgers in Coto del Rey. Badgers located their territories so as to include as much of this habitat as possible, while intraterritorial use was also biased towards Mediterranean scrubland during both foraging and resting (Revilla et al. 2000). This pattern of habitat selection and preferences is strongly correlated with rabbit abundance (Revilla et al. 2000). Within every territory the areas of available Mediterranean scrubland were similar, about 142 ± 28.63 ha (mean ± 1 SD, N = 5). Percent use of Mediterranean scrubland was calculated using radiolocations (N = 889), separated by at least 4 h for the same individual to ensure statistical independence (Revilla 1998), obtained during the period of activity of the animals. We analysed the use of this key habitat with a general linear model to detect any variation between periods (dry and wet), seasons, sexes, and reproductive status of the animals. The angular-transformed percentage of radiolocations in Mediterranean scrubland was the dependent variable. Independent variables were season, period, and class of reproductive dominance. We also included individuals as a nested independent variable to account for any variability between the animals. The initial model corresponded to a factorial complete design with type III sum of squares (procedure GLM in SAS; SAS Institute Inc. 1990a). From the initial complete model, we eliminated the nonsignificant factors (those with the lowest sum of squares) one by one, beginning with the most complex interactions, until we obtained a final model with only significant predictors. We did not standardise the percentage of locations into a selection ratio because Mediterranean scrubland was present in all territories considered and it was the preferred habitat type for every individual (Revilla et al. 2000), so percentages were comparable among territories. Results During the study period, maximum rabbit densities occurred in Mediterranean scrubland habitat (41.5 rabbits/ha in June 1995) and minimum densities in pine plantations (<0.1 rabbits/ha in October 1996). The difference in rabbit density between Mediterranean scrubland and other habitat types varied between 1.5- and 23-fold. After the beginning of the rains in autumn 1995, there was a 2.2-fold reduction in rabbit density in Mediterranean scrubland (Fig. 1). This diminution was even greater in other habitat types such as ash stands or pasturelands, where rabbit density was between 5.4 and 11.1 times lower than during the previous year. Resource use: diet Rabbits were the most important prey during both dry and wet periods, accounting for 65.4 and 44.7% of ingested biomass, respectively. As expected, there was less use of rabbits in 1996 than in 1995 (67.8 and 62.5% occurrence and 58.9 and 49.2% biomass ingested during 1995 and 1996, respectively; Fig. 2). Multiway analysis of frequencies showed a significant effect of year and season (χ2 = 7.75, df = 1, P = 0.0054, and χ2 = 118.57, df = 3, P < 0.0001), but not of their interaction (χ2 = 5.18, df = 3, P = 0.1590). Differences between years were due to the lower percent occurrence during 1996 (Fig. 2). All seasons differed from one another (posthoc comparisons, χ2 > 13.27, df = 1, P = 0.0003 in all cases) except for winter and spring (χ2 = 0.81, df = 1, P = 0.3689), when badgers most frequently consumed rabbits (Fig. 2). Resource holding: key-habitat use Mediterranean scrubland was the most used habitat yearround. Its use was lowest during summer, with 62% of active radiolocations, and greatest in winter, with 87%. In the final model of the use of Mediterranean scrubland (r2 = 0.67, F = 3.45, df = 16, 27, P = 0.0023), dominance status, individuals, © 2001 NRC Canada Fig. 2. Use of rabbits as a trophic resource by badgers in Coto del Rey, represented as percent occurrence and ingested biomass, for winter (WI), spring (SP), summer (SU), and autumn (AU) of 1995 (95) and 1996 (96). The numbers above the bars show the number of scats analysed per season. Table 1. Results of the final general linear model of the use of Mediterranean scrubland by Eurasian badgers (Meles meles) in Coto del Rey, Doñana, Spain. Variable df Type III sum of squares F P Dominance status Period × dominance status Individual 1 2 13 0.1491 1.0013 1.1501 4.43 14.86 2.63 0.0449 0.0001 0.0166 and the interaction between dominance status and period were significant (Table 1). The strong effect of the interaction between status and period was due to a change in the use of Mediterranean scrubland between the dry and wet periods, with subordinates decreasing and dominants increasing their use of this habitat in a complementary way (post-hoc t test on the least-squares means, t > 3.02, P < 0.005; Fig. 1). Discussion Badgers are generalists, but locally may depend on one trophic resource, such as rabbits (Martín et al. 1995; Fedriani et al. 1998). Mediterranean scrubland is the key habitat for rabbits (Palomares et al. 1996; Palomares and Delibes 1997), hence it contains the key trophic resource for badgers in the Coto del Rey area (Revilla et al. 2000). During the study period, there was a 2.2-fold reduction of rabbit density in Mediterranean scrubland habitat. This diminution was even greater in other types of habitat such as dehesa or ash stands (Palomares et al. 2000). Therefore, Mediterranean scrubland probably played an increasing role as the most important habitat for badgers during the wet period. After the decline in the rabbit population, badgers reduced their use of this food; however, rabbits were still the main trophic resource in winter 1995 and spring 1996. There was a clear shift in habitat use between the two status classes of badgers. Dominants increased their use of Mediterranean scrubland at the beginning of the rains and kept using it throughout the period of low rabbit density. Subordinates followed the opposite pattern (Fig. 1), reducing their use of the areas that still contained the main resource. The most reasonable interpretation of these results is that during the period of low rabbit density, dominants were excluding subordinates from rabbit-rich areas. If this interpretation is correct, badgers would be asymmetric not only in access to reproduction (Revilla et al. 1999) but also in access to the key trophic resource when the carrying capacity of the territory is reached. Suppression of reproduction is one of the mechanisms by which group size in Eurasian badgers is adjusted to the local availability of resources (Cresswell et al. 1992; da Silva et al. 1994; Woodroffe and Macdonald 1995). For females, good body condition is a necessary precondition for reproduction (Woodroffe and Macdonald 1995). Our findings suggest that dominant females may use habitat exclusion as a means of suppressing reproduction of subordinates via body condition. In accordance with this interpretation, subordinate badgers in Doñana had poorer body condition than dominants (Revilla 1998). The badger population in Coto del Rey is characterised by low density and the importance of rabbits in the diet (Revilla et al. 1999). However, in areas of Central Europe and the British Isles, badgers feed on earthworms, and reach much higher densities. Earthworms are spatially and temporally less predictable than rabbits. Predictability is probably important in the competition for food, as dominants should be able to evict subordinates from resource-rich patches. If resourcerich habitat patches were less predictable, this exclusion would be more difficult, or even impossible. Doñana badgers feed © 2001 NRC Canada mainly on rabbit kittens inside warrens, where they are accessible through digging (Martín et al. 1995). Areas of high rabbit density within Mediterranean scrubland habitat follow a gradient of increasing density towards the ecotones with pastureland and marshland (Palomares et al. 1996; Palomares and Delibes 1997). This spatial distribution makes the availability of rabbits very predictable between years, as clusters of warrens do not change substantially in the short term. The breeding season of rabbits also follows a very predictable temporal pattern (Rogers et al. 1994). In contrast, earthworm availability depends on day-to-day factors such as temperature and humidity, local factors such as grass height, and landscape factors such as habitat type (Kruuk 1989). Therefore, we might expect that in areas where badgers have an earthworm-based diet, it would be difficult for dominants to control the use of patches with low predictability. If this holds true, in these high-density populations, food stress will produce a pattern closer to scramble competition than to contest competition, which should be reflected in poorer body condition for all individuals (including dominants) during periods of prey scarcity. This has been shown to some extent by Rogers et al. (1997) in Gloucestershire, England, and by Kruuk and Parish (1985) in northern Scotland. However, if badgers shift to more predictable resources (such as cereal crops) during periods of scarcity, there may be a change from scramble to contest competition, where dominant individuals would be at an advantage, as is shown by data on reproduction during a poor year and a good year in England (Woodroffe and Macdonald 1995). Models of badger social organisation suggest that group living has evolved as a response to the low or absent competition between group members, owing to the patchy distribution of resources and (or) high rates of prey renewal (Woodroffe and Macdonald 1993; da Silva et al. 1994). If carrying capacity is reached or reduced, our results suggest that different levels of competition might be expected, depending on the predictability of the patches of key resource, following a gradient between scramble and contest competition, in which dominants would be at an advantage. Acknowledgements This research was founded by the Dirección General de Investigación Científica y Técnica (projects PB94-0480 and PB97-1163) and sponsored by Rover España. E.R. was supported by a predoctoral grant from the Spanish Ministry of Education and Culture. J. Ayala, J. Calzada, and numerous students helped with field and laboratory work. The comments of J. Calzada, M. Delibes, and two anonymous reviewers greatly improved the manuscript. References Alexander, R.D. 1974. The evolution of social behavior. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5: 325–383. Barton, R.A., and Whiten, A. 1993. Feeding competition among female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Anim. Behav. 46: 777–789. Buckland, S.T., Aanderson, D.R., Burnham, K.P., and Laake., J.L. 1993. Distance sampling. Chapman and Hall, London. Burnham, K.P., Anderson, D.R., and Laake., J.L. 1980. Estimation of density from line transect sampling of biological populations. Wildl. Monogr. No. 72. Carr, G.M., and Macdonald, D.W. 1986. The sociality of solitary foragers: a model based on resource dispersion. Anim. Behav. 34: 1540–1549. Cresswell, W.J., Harris, S., Cheeseman, C.L., and Mallinson, P.J. 1992. To breed or not to breed: an analysis of the social and density-dependent constraints on the fecundity of female badgers (Meles meles). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 338: 393–407. da Silva, J., Macdonald, D. W., and Evans, P. G. H. 1994. Net cost of group living in a solitary forager, the Eurasian badger (Meles meles). Behav. Ecol. 5: 151–158. Davies, N.B. 1978. Ecological questions about territorial behaviour. In Behavioral ecology: an evolutionary approach. Edited by J.R. Krebs and N.B. Davies. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford. pp. 148–169. Delibes M., and Hiraldo, F. 1981. The rabbit as prey in the Iberian Mediterranean ecosystem. In Proceedings of the World Lagomorph Conference, Guelph, Ont., August 1979. Edited by K. Myers and C.D. Innes. University of Guelph, Guelph, Ont., IUCN Species Survival Commission, and World Wildlife Fund Canada. pp. 614–622. Fedriani, J.M., Ferreras, P., and Delibes, M. 1998. Dietary response of the Eurasian badger, Meles meles, to a decline of its main prey in the Doñana National Park. J. Zool. (Lond.), 245: 214–218. Gordon, D.M. 1997. The populational consequences of territorial behavior. Trends Ecol. Evol. 12: 63–66. Hall, C.L., and Fedigan, L.M. 1997. Spatial benefits afforded by high rank in white-faced capuchins. Anim. Behav. 53: 1069–1082. Hofer, H. 1988. Variation in resource presence, utilization and reproduction success within population of European badgers (Meles meles). Mammal Rev. 18: 25–36. Kruuk, H. 1978. Foraging and spatial organisation of the European badger, Meles meles L. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 4: 75–89. Kruuk, H. 1989. The social badger: ecology and behaviour of a group-living carnivore (Meles meles). Oxford University Press, Oxford. Kruuk, H., and Macdonald, D.W. 1985. Group territories of carnivores: empires and enclaves. In Behavioural ecology: ecological consequences of adaptative behaviour. Edited by R.M. Sibly and R.H. Smith. Blackwell Scientific Publications, London. pp. 521–536. Kruuk, H., and Parish, T. 1981. Feeding specialization of the European badger Meles meles in Scotland. J. Anim. Ecol. 50: 773–788. Kruuk, H., and Parish, T. 1985. Food, food availability and weight of badgers (Meles meles) in relation to agricultural changes. J. Appl. Ecol. 22: 705–715. Kruuk, H., and Parish, T. 1987. Changes in the size of groups and ranges of European badger (Meles meles L.) in an area in Scotland. J. Anim. Ecol. 56: 351–364. Macdonald, D.W. 1983. The ecology of carnivore social behaviour. Nature (Lond.), 301: 379–384. Martín, R., Rodríguez, A., and Delibes, M. 1995. Local feeding specialization by badgers (Meles meles) in a Mediterranean environment. Oecologia, 101: 45–50. Packer, C., and Caro, T.M. 1997. Foraging costs in social carnivores. Anim. Behav. 54: 1317–1318. Palomares, F., and Delibes, M. 1997. Predation upon European rabbits and their use of open and closed patches in Mediterranean habitats. Oikos, 80: 407–410. Palomares, F., Calzada, J., and Revilla, E. 1996. El manejo del hábitat y la abundancia de conejos: diferencias entre dos áreas potencialmente idénticas. Rev. Florest. 9: 201–210. Palomares F., Delibes, M., Revilla, E., Calzada, J., and Fedriani, J.M. 2000. Spatial ecology of the Iberian lynx and abundance of its primary prey, the European rabbit, in a heterogeneous environment. Wildl. Monogr. In press. Revilla, E. 1998. Organización social del tejón en Doñana. Ph.D. thesis, University of León, León, Spain. Revilla, E., Delibes, D., Travaini, A., and Palomares, F. 1999. Physical and population parameters of Eurasian badgers, Meles meles, from Mediterranean Spain. Z. Saeugetierkd. 64: 269–276. Revilla, E., Palomares, F., and Delibes, M. 2000. Defining key habitats for low density populations of Eurasian badgers in Mediterranean environments. Biol. Conserv. 95: 269–277. Rogers, L.M., Cheeseman, C.L., and Langton, S. 1997. Body weight as indication of density-dependent population regulation in badgers (Meles meles) at Woodchester Park, Gloucestershire. J. Zool. (Lond.), 242: 597–604. Rogers, P.M., Arthur, C.P., and Soriguer, R. 1994. The rabbit in continental Europe. In The European rabbit: the history of a successful colonizer. Edited by H.V. Thomson and C.M. King. Oxford University Press, Oxford. pp. 22–63. SAS Institute Inc. 1990a. SAS/STAT user’s guide. Vol. 2, version 6 ed. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, N.C. SAS Institute Inc. 1990b. SAS/STAT user’s guide. Vol. 1, version 6 ed. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, N.C. Schmidt, P.A., and Mech, L.D. 1997. Wolf pack size and food acquisition. Am. Nat. 150: 513–517. Stamps, J. 1994. Territorial behavior: testing the assumptions. Adv. Stud. Behav. 23: 173–231. Waser, P.M. 1981. Sociality or territorial defence? The influence of resource renewal. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 8: 231–237. Woodroffe, R., and Macdonald, D.W. 1993. Badger sociality— models of spatial grouping. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. No. 65. pp. 145–169. Woodroffe, R., and Macdonald, D.W. 1995. Female/female competition in European badgers Meles meles: effects on breeding success. J. Anim. Ecol. 64: 12–20. Wrangham, R.W., and Rubenstein, D.I. 1986. Social evolution in birds and mammals. In Ecological aspects of social evolution. Edited by D.I. Rubenstein and R.W. Wrangham. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. pp. 452–470. © 2001 NRC Canada