Moving beyond the resource dispersion hypothesis Eloy Revilla Estació n Bioló gica de Doñ ana CSIC, Avenida de Marı́a Luisa s/n, E-41013 Seville, Spain In a recent article in TREE [1], Johnson et al. review the evidence showing that, when resources are patchily distributed, the economics of exploiting these patches enable individuals to share resources over a common area, satisfying their needs without imposing large costs on each other. This resource dispersion hypothesis (RDH) is presented as a causal mechanism of group living, especially when individuals obtain no apparent benefits from such living. There are two points on which I would like to comment. First, the authors only refer to the evidence in favour of the RDH. However, in no single case are all the assumptions and predictions satisfied (Table 1 in [1]). To test the RDH, all the assumptions must be fulfilled and, then, only a simultaneous positive test of all its predictions would validate the hypothesis [1]. However, we need evidence against only one of its assumptions and/or predictions for it to be invalid. This evidence exists [2]. For example, in badgers (a RDH model species), there is a high cost of living in groups [3 – 5], the territoriality and use of space by different individuals are affected by different resources [6 – 8], and decreases in food availability increase territory overlap [9]. These pieces of evidence invalidate the assumption of there being no cost to group living, that the determinant of space use is the same for all individuals, and that group ranges are exclusive, respectively. Neither was there a consistent relationship found between territory size and resource dispersion, nor was group size consistently related to resource richness in one site [10]. In another study, a decrease in food availability was followed by territory expansion rather than by the expected group size reduction [9]. Furthermore, in one group-living population, territory size was related to its richness and not to patch dispersion, whereas an adjacent population, suffering the same strong seasonal resource heterogeneity, was solitary living [8]. Second, from its conceptual and mathematical definition, the RDH provides an explanation for why there is a surplus of resources inside one territory. Although this is important, there is a large conceptual gap between this explanation and demonstrating that this surplus is the causal means of group formation. Otherwise, in our heterogeneous world, we would observe many more species living in groups. Corresponding author: Eloy Revilla (revilla@ebd.csic.es). Current evidence shows that resource patchiness alone is not enough to explain group living. In ecology, the adaptive process of understanding includes the first step of developing theory based on empirical information, followed by testing of theoretical predictions. When there are conflicts between empirical evidence and predictions, the theory has to be refined to accommodate the new knowledge. I think that it is now time to move beyond the RDH as a casual mechanism of group living. References 1 Johnson, D.D.P. et al. (2002) Does the resource dispersion hypothesis explain group living? Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, 563 – 570 2 Revilla, E. (2003) Does the resource dispersion hypothesis explain anything? Oikos 101, 428 – 432 3 Macdonald, D.W. et al. (2002) Density-dependence regulation of body mass and condition in badgers (Meles meles) from Wytham woods. Ecology 83, 2056 – 2061 4 Silva, J.D. et al. (1994) Net cost of group living in a solitary forager, the Eurasian badger (Meles meles). Behav. Ecol. 5, 151 – 158 5 Woodroffe, R. and Macdonald, D.W. (1995) Female/female competition in European badgers Meles meles: effects on breeding success. J. Anim. Ecol. 64, 12 – 20 6 Tuyttens, F.A.M. et al. (2000) Spatial perturbation caused by a badger (Meles meles) culling operation: implications for the function of territoriality and the control of bovine tuberculosis (Mycobacterium bovis). J. Anim. Ecol. 69, 815 – 828 7 Revilla, E. and Palomares, F. (2001) Differences in key habitat use between dominant and subordinate animals: intraterritorial dominance payoffs in Eurasian badgers? Can. J. Zool. 79, 165 – 170 8 Revilla, E. and Palomares, F. (2002) Spatial organization, group living and ecological correlates in low-density populations of Eurasian badgers, Meles meles. J. Anim. Ecol. 71, 497 – 512 9 Kruuk, H. and Parish, T. (1987) Changes in the size of groups and ranges of European Badger (Meles meles L.) in an area in Scotland. J. Anim. Ecol. 56, 351 – 364 10 Johnson, D.D.P. et al. (2001) Long-term resource variation and group size: a large sample field test of the Resource Dispersion Hypothesis. BMC Ecology 1, 2 (http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6785/1/2)