Raptor Nest Decorations Are a Reliable Threat Against Conspecifics F. Sergio,1*† J. Blas,1† G. Blanco,2 A. Tanferna,1 L. López,1 J. A. Lemus,1 F. Hiraldo1 Individual quality is often signaled by phenotypic flags, such as bright plumage patches in birds. Extended phenotype signals can similarly show quality, but in these cases the signals are external to the individual, often taking the form of objects scavenged from the environment. Through multiple manipulative experiments, we showed that objects used for nest decoration by a territorial raptor, the black kite (Milvus migrans), act as reliable threats to conspecifics, revealing the viability, territory quality, and conflict dominance of the signaler. Our results suggest that animalbuilt structures may serve as signaling devices much more frequently than currently recognized. nimals transfer information through anatomical, physical, and behavioral traits, such as brightly colored body patches, extravagant ornaments, or size-revealing calls A (1–3). Some species go to further extents by collecting objects to build external signaling structures or “extended phenotype signals” (4). Such external signals may involve higher cognitive abilities (5) and be subject to cultural transmission (6). Further, our ability to address questions about such signals are facilitated by the facts that (i) they are often flexible and less constrained than bodily traits (4) and (ii) their constituent materials may be readily available for experimental manipulation (7, 8). Despite the broader appeal, extended phenotype signals have been examined in only a handful of passerine bird and fish species, and these were almost exclusively structures built solely for mate attraction 1 Department of Conservation Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñana, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas (CSIC), C/ Americo Vespucio, 41092 Seville, Spain. 2Department of Evolutionary Ecology, Museum of Natural Sciences, CSIC, C/ José Gutiérrez Abascal 2, 28006 Madrid, Spain. *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: fsergio@ebd.csic.es †These authors contributed equally to this work. B A Fig. 1. Decoration of nests by black kites (Milvus migrans). (A) Highly decorated nest and (B) selection of materials by kites relative to their availability in the environment. Kites show a preference for white (c2 = 270.0, P < 0.0001) plastic (c2 = 57.0, P < 0.0001), whereas all other materials and colors are used at rates equal to (other materials c2 = 0.1, P = 0.76) or less than (paper c2 = 68.1, P < 0.0001; cryptic colors c2 = 77.2, P < 0.0001; other colors c2 = 148.3, P < 0.0001) their availability in the environment. [Photo credit: F. Sergio] 1 and courtship, such as the bowers of bowerbirds (5–8). However, several hundreds of species erect nest structures in manners that could be compatible with extended phenotype signaling functions. Examples include the addition of man-made objects, stones, green plants, snake skins, dung, or carnivore scats by passerines, storks, swans, penguins, corvids, raptors, and fish and the building of multiple alternative nests by wrens, weavers, and raptors (4, 9–14). Therefore, although animals are thought to build structures for communication only rarely (12), nests could be signaling devices more frequently than currently recognized. We combined experimental manipulations and long-term observations of a long-lived raptor to show that nest decoration can act as an honest threat to conspecifics, revealing the viability, territory quality, and conflict dominance of the signaler. Our model species, the black kite (Milvus migrans), is a medium-sized migratory raptor. Pairs occupy breeding territories that may vary markedly in resource quality (15). Floating individuals are prevented from breeding through social dominance but may attempt violent territory takeovers, which sometimes succeed (16) (fig. S1). Trespassing can be frequent, especially in pre-incubation, and is also aimed at stealing food resources obtained by the residents. On return from migration after pair formation, both partners contribute to nest building. One to 2 weeks before laying, they may both place various man-made materials in the nest, typically white plastic objects (Fig. 1A). The objects seem placed so as to be highly visible and are collected during the peak period of territory trespassing and long after pair formation. Thus, we hypothesized that they serve as an extended phenotype signal of threat against intruders (table S1). We used a well-studied (15, 16) (fig. S2) population of marked, known-age (1 to 25 years old) kites in Doñana National Park. To investigate the signaling function of nest decorations (17), we (i) assessed decoration levels at 127 nests built by individuals of known age and body size, which were subsequently intensively monitored to estimate their viability (here defined by subsequent survival and offspring production); (ii) compared nest decorations with objects available in the environment; (iii) offered objects to marked individuals to examine their propensity to use decorations; (iv) estimated trespass rates and aggression of intruders for both nonexperimental and experimental pairs (i.e., those whose nest decorations had been augmented); (v) tested whether nest decorations predicted the efficiency of resource monopolization mediated by aggression within a Fig. 2. Change in mean (T 1 SE) nest decoration by black kites (black line) with age (n = 127). Placement of freely available items (gray) was highest for birds in prime age (7 to 12 years old) and lowest for young and old individuals (n = 49). competitive feeding environment away from the nest; and (vi) examined the egg predation rates of experimental dummy kite nests with and without decorations. Through this series of experiments, we specifically tested for the presence of an honest extended signal by addressing four hypotheses, as proposed by Searcy and Nowicki (3): (i) is the signal reliable; (ii) do receivers respond to the signal in a way that benefits signalers; (iii) is the signal costly, and is the cost higher for lowerquality individuals; and (iv) is there deceit (17) (table S1)? Nest decorations were observed in 77% of the nests (n = 127), appeared in the last 20 days before laying, and declined slightly during the incubation period and markedly thereafter (fig. S3). Both sexes were equally likely to carry decorations (males 15 times, females 12; binomial test, P = 0.70). When compared to availability in transects (Fig. 1B), kites overselected plastic materials (c2 = 57.0, P < 0.0001) and white color (as assessed by human vision; c2 = 270.0, P < 0.0001). All other materials and colors were avoided or used according to availability (Fig. 1B). When birds were offered a set of experimental objects in equal availability (17), 29 of 33 pairs that collected objects placed only white plastic items in their nest, two collected both white and transparent items, and another two collected both white and green items. White was overselected ( c2 = 100.7, P < 0.0001), and the other colors were avoided (green and transparent c2 = 24.5, P < 0.0001). Is the signal reliable (hypothesis i)? Nest decoration increased with territory quality and body condition and exhibited a quadratic (parabolic) relationship with age (table S2 and Figs. 2 and 3): The level of decoration increased up to ages 10 to 12 and declined thereafter with senescence (Figs. 2 and 3). Similarly, 37% of the pairs refrained from collecting any offered item despite its prolonged availability. These animals had lower-quality territories and differed in age from individuals that used the offered items (table S3): Individuals that refrained from signaling were very young or very old, whereas those that used the decorations to signal were predominantly in prime age (7 to 12 Fig. 3. Change in level of nest decoration in black kites. Level of decoration was (A) lowest for young birds, (B) increased up to ages 10 to 12 years old, and (C) declined thereafter with senescence. [Photo credit: F. Sergio] years old; Fig. 2). The rate and success of attacks on trespassers increased with decoration levels (table S4). Similarly, in feeding observations, the percent of time an individual spent eating and its success rate in food-related aggressive interactions were related to its nest decoration level (table S5). Do receivers respond to the signal and do they respond in a way that benefits signalers (hypothesis ii)? In pairs without experimental nest augmentation, territory intrusion rates were lower for highly decorated nests (table S4), consistent with the hypothesis that trespassers avoid decorated nests. However, intruder pressure actually increased when we experimentally augmented the decorations (table S6). This suggests that nonterritorial animals or “f loaters” may be stimulated to attack pairs that suddenly advertise a high-quality territory. Higher levels of nest decoration predicted higher subsequent breeding success and survival (table S7 and Fig. 4), suggesting that birds may benefit from lower rates of risky fights associated with higher-quality advertisement. Is the signal costly, and is the cost higher for lower quality individuals (hypothesis iii)? Predation rates of experimental eggs were higher for decorated dummy nests (81.3% predated, n = 16) than for nondecorated dummy nests (31.3%, n = 16, c2 = 8.54, P = 0.003), indicating that there Fig. 4. Nest decoration by black kites increased with the viability of the signaler, as estimated by breeding performance (number of fledglings produced, black circles, n = 127) and survival until spring (white squares, n = 76). Data are mean T 1 SE. Fig. 5. Experimental augmentation of nest decoration. Mean (T 1 SE) attack rates of black kites on trespassers were higher in prime age (7 to 12 years old; black circles, n = 25) individuals and lower in young or old (≤6 or ≥13 years old; white squares, n = 40) animals. is a cost to advertising quality through nest decoration. Similarly, trespassing rates increased for all individuals in the augmentation experiment, but only those in prime age managed to increase their attack rate in response (table S6 and Fig. 5). Very young or senescent individuals appeared incapable to counteract the cost of signaling (Fig. 5); thus, it appears this cost is higher for lower-quality individuals. Is there deceit (hypothesis iv)? In the equal availability experiment, lower-quality individuals almost systematically refrained from signaling (Fig. 2). One week after the augmentation experiment, 85% of the individuals in prime age had retained the plastic we placed in their nest, whereas 87% in the other age classes had removed it. Age and its quadratic effect were the only predictors of the likelihood that a pair would retain or expel the experimental plastic (table S8). Deceit, if present, seems rare. Our results show that nest decoration is a graduated signal that conveys complex information on territory quality, individual viability, and dominance in social interactions. We found no support for alternative hypotheses (tables S1 and S9). Reliability of the signal was reinforced because lower-quality individuals refrained from dishonest signaling, even though such cheating would have been possible. Theoretical studies predict signaling abstention by lower-quality individuals in the presence of agonistic retaliatory costs imposed by receivers (18). Such social costs were evident in this study: Advertisement of highquality resources was met with aggressive challenges. The costs of such challenges could be disproportionately high for cheating individuals, because advertising high-quality resources may attract strong contestants (19) that are able to displace dishonest signalers through aggressive, and physically damaging, conflicts (16) (fig. S1). Consistent with such disproportional costs, only individuals in prime age were able to respond to the increase in challenges stimulated by experimental nest augmentation. Lowerquality individuals were not able to match their defense rates to the cheating decoration levels that we imposed. Social punishment has been recurrently shown as a major theoretical determinant of communication honesty, especially for signals involving minimum production costs [e.g., (20, 21), review in (2)], such as many extended phenotype signals. This may explain why social punishment and cheating abstention seem common in such communication systems [e.g., (7, 22–26)]. Extended phenotype signals can be reliable indicators of both individual and territory quality. Previous analyses have shown that levels of external signals may increase from juvenile to adult age classes [e.g., (23, 27)] or be individually repeatable through time [e.g., (28, 29)]. Our findings confirm such earlier studies and expand them by showing a clear senescent decline in signaling strength. This reinforces the idea of individuals signaling their physical prowess, which increases and then declines with age. Further, our findings indicate that external signals can be used in the context of resource defense and do not solely signal reproductive quality in a sexual selection context, although reinforcement of pair bonding may have contributed to our results (10). In line with these ideas, many characteristics typical of the objects used to create extended phenotype signaling structures make them optimal as signals of territorial defense: high visibility, effectiveness in the absence of the signaler, encoding of social dominance and motivation, ease of deconstruction, and reliability enforced by punishment of cheaters [reviews in (1–4)]. References and Notes 1. J. W. Bradbury, S. L. Vehrencamp, Principles of Animal Communication (Sinauer, Sunderland, MA, 1998). 2. J. Maynard Smith, D. Harper, Animal Signals (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2003). 3. W. A. Searcy, S. Nowicki, The Evolution of Animal Communication (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ, 2005). 4. F. C. Schaedelin, M. Taborsky, Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 84, 293 (2009). 5. J. R. Madden, Proc. Biol. Sci. 268, 833 (2001). 6. J. R. Madden, Anim. Cogn. 11, 1 (2008). 7. J. R. Madden, Proc. Biol. Sci. 269, 1347 (2002). 8. S. W. Coleman, G. L. Patricelli, G. Borgia, Nature 428, 742 (2004). 9. P. H. Wimberger, Auk 101, 615 (1984). 3 10. J. Moreno, M. Soler, A. P. Møller, M. Linden, Anim. Behav. 47, 1297 (1994). 11. M. Hansell, Bird Nests and Construction Behaviour (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 2000). 12. M. Hansell, Animal Architecture (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2005). 13. D. J. Levey, R. S. Duncan, C. F. Levins, Nature 431, 39 (2004). 14. J. G. Schuetz, Behav. Ecol. 16, 133 (2005). 15. F. Sergio et al., Oecologia 160, 507 (2009). 16. F. Sergio, J. Blas, F. Hiraldo, J. Anim. Ecol. 78, 109 (2009). 17. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 18. E. S. Adams, M. Mesterton-Gibbons, J. Theor. Biol. 175, 405 (1995). 19. A. Berglund, A. Bisazza, A. Pilastro, Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 58, 385 (1996). 20. R. A. Johnstone, K. Norris, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32, 127 (1993). 21. M. Enquist, S. Ghirlanda, P. L. Hurd, Anim. Behav. 56, 749 (1998). 22. J. Diamond, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 83, 3042 (1986). 23. G. Borgia, Am. Nat. 141, 729 (1993). 24. S. Östlund-Nilsson, M. Holmlund, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 53, 214 (2003). 25. J. M. Wojcieszek, J. A. Nicholls, A. W. Goldizen, Behav. Ecol. 18, 689 (2007). 26. N. R. Doerr, Anim. Behav. 79, 747 (2010). 27. E. C. Collias, N. E. Collias, Auk 81, 42 (1964). 28. B. J. Rushbrook, N. J. Dingemanse, I. Barber, Anim. Behav. 75, 547 (2008). 29. P. T. Walsh, M. Hansell, W. D. Borello, S. D. Healy, Biol. Lett. 6, 149 (2010). 30. We thank F. J. Chicano, J. Giralt, and F. G. Vilches for help in the field. S. N. Vignieri and three anonymous reviewers greatly improved a previous draft of the manuscript. The study was funded by the research projects CGL2008-01781, JA-58, RNM 1790, and RNM 03822 granted by the Junta de Andalucía, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación, and Natural Research Limited. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/331/6015/327/DC1 Materials and Methods Figs. S1 to S3 Tables S1 to S9 1a 1b 5 6 7 10