Running head: READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM Reading on the ACT College Entrance Exam: What can we learn from students’ practice test performance? Rachel Matthews Vanderbilt University’s Peabody College of Education 1 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 2 Abstract This study investigated students’ strengths and weaknesses on the ACT reading subtest. The study focused on how the existing curriculum of a school might be enhanced to promote students’ performance on the assessment. The study addressed four domains of education: learners, learning context, curriculum, and assessment. The participants in the study were 21 African American, 12th grade students who lived in a small, rural Delta town and attended a high-performing charter school. Students in the study took a practice ACT examination, and item performance was analyzed to determine trends. Based on this analysis, curriculum suggestions included support for reading comprehension; instruction of question-answer relationships, questioning the author, and text structure; and intensive vocabulary support. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 3 Reading on the ACT College Entrance Exam I. Problem and Question Before leaving the classroom to attend graduate school for education, I taught English in a high school located in a rural town in southeast Arkansas. The population of the town is less than 10,000. The average per capita income at the time of the last census was $13,028, while the average per capita income for the U.S. was $21,587 (www.americantowns.com). 9.2% of the residents of the town live below the poverty line (www.americantowns.com). 62% of the residents hold a high school diploma, and 17% have a Bachelor’s degree (www.americantowns.com). Both of these figures are lower than the national average (www.americantowns.com). My students were predominantly African American; all of them receive federal free lunch; and they predominantly spoke African American Vernacular English. Within the context of this school and this town, I became familiar with the assets and “cultural funds” low-SES, minority students bring to school with them (Risko & Walker-Dalhouse, 2007, p. 98). For instance, my students were extremely verbal, expressing their thoughts in creative, rich language, and they each had various passionate interests that, when accessed, made them highly engaged learners (Fink, 1996, p. 268). My students’ parents continually demonstrated great care for them, and they placed much value on the opportunities that education would afford their children. However, I also became familiar with the challenges these learners may face in traditional school settings and standardized testing situations. Many of my eleventh and twelfth grade students felt daunted by the ACT, a widely-used college entrance exam. Upon their request, I began giving ACT English and reading practice items as bell ringers and met with them on READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 4 Saturdays to complete practice tests. To the joy of my students and myself, their scores on the English subtest rose, but to our dismay, their scores on the reading subtest improved only slightly, if at all, and remained below my students’ goals. Many students who earned A’s and B’s in English class continued to earn low scores on the ACT reading subtest. The mismatch between testing scores and class performance remained a conundrum. Because poor test performance thwarted the college aspirations of my hardworking students, I set out to pinpoint their strengths and weaknesses on the test by conducting an item by item analysis of test questions and students’ responses. I hoped my analysis of test items would indicate ways to infuse the curriculum with instruction that supports their success on future ACT reading tests. Critical theory, notably the work of Lisa Delpit, provides a foundation for my inquiry into why my students perform at certain levels on the ACT and how instruction can be modified to raise their scores. According to Delpit (1995) society operates by codes of power; in order to maximize opportunities for children from low-income and minority backgrounds, Delpit argues that teachers must make these codes of power visible and accessible. She wrote, I suggest that students must be taught the codes needed to participate fully in the mainstream of American life…that they must be allowed the resource of the teacher’s expert knowledge, while being helped to acknowledge their own ‘expertness’ as well; and that even while students are assisted in learning the culture of power, they must also be helped to learn about the arbitrariness of those codes and about the power relationships they represent. (p. 45) While Delpit’s work focuses mainly on students’ language and literacy, the overarching concept applies to students’ performance on the ACT. Assessment is always couched in language and culture, and this context causes test results to more accurately reflect the ability of test-takers whose home language and culture align to the writing of the test. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 5 Hillard (2002) illuminated issues of assessment reliability for speakers of nonstandard English: The results of standardized testing favor children who speak common American English simply because these children are able to respond to questions that are couched in a familiar language based upon familiar experiences. Since the ‘right children’—upper class, wealthy—tend to get the top scores, it is assumed that the I.Q., reading, speech, language acquisition, and other tests are valid. Test makers have no way of taking the achievement results of a privileged child and separating that part of the scores which is due to the student’s special skill and that part which is due simply to growing up in the common white American culture. (p. 98) Above and beyond understandings about the limits and biases of standardized assessment, the ACT is part of the code of power described by Delpit insofar as it acts as a gatekeeper for students’ ability to “participate fully in the mainstream of American life” (Delpit, 1995, p. 45). Colleges and universities continue to use the test as criteria for admission as well as class placement. Thus, teachers, who believe as Delpit does that students deserve to call upon expertness in codes of power, must find ways to support their students’ achievement on the ACT. A general understanding of reading comprehension sets the backdrop for more specific understandings of students’ performance on the ACT reading assessment. According to Jennings, Caldwell, and Lerner, who view comprehension as an interactive process between text and reader, “When people read, they actually construct their own meaning of a text; in other words, people create their own mental version of what they read. Three factors contribute to the process of constructing meaning: (1) the reader, (2) the reading material, and (3) the reading situation” (2010, p. 13). First, the reader impacts the act of comprehension insofar as the reader brings various levels of background knowledge, engagement, purpose, and ability (Jennings, Caldwell, & Lerner, 2010). In addition, the simplicity or complexity of syntax and READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 6 diction in the text as well as the arrangement and structure of the text influence comprehension (Jennings, Caldwell, & Lerner, 2010). Issues here may be the appearance of low-frequency, esoteric vocabulary words, nominalization, or the telescoping effect (Alvermann, Phelps, & Gillis, 2010). Beyond word knowledge and sentence comprehension, readers use various strategies with different genres of text. Readers, for example, need a different set of strategies when approaching a novel than they need when approaching a chronologically-ordered social studies text (Alvermann, Phelps, & Gillis, 2010). Finally, Jennings, Caldwell, and Lerner (2010) explained that the reading situation contributes to comprehension, as purpose for reading and levels of anxiety differ by environment. A person, for instance, has very different purposes and anxiety levels while reading a book on the beach than while reading a science textbook in preparation for a final course examination. Thus, each of these factors—reader, text, and situation—contributes to students’ comprehension on the ACT reading assessment. For example, each student possesses varying comprehension abilities, including active reading, questioning, and fix-up strategies, vary. Depending on the course selection of the high school and the range of students’ personal reading and cultural experiences, students may possess or lack background knowledge and vocabulary for ACT reading passages. The content of each ACT reading passage may effect a student’s level of engagement, while the syntax and diction of the passage may influence the student’s anticipated level of success, perhaps impacting her motivation to try or not try. Finally students experience different levels of test anxiety. Looking at the specific data set of this study, broader components of READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 7 comprehension on the ACT reading assessment contextualize analysis of more specific trends, including strengths, weaknesses, and general test-taking habits. II. Context and Study Participants The students selected for the study live in the town in which I taught, or they live in the surrounding area. About 70% of the community is African American (www.americantowns.com). The percentage of people living below the poverty index exceeds the national average, and the percentage of residents who hold high school and Bachelor’s degrees falls below the national average (www.americantowns.com). The school where the study took place is a charter school that begins in the fifth grade and ends with twelfth grade. Initial admission to the school is based on a lottery, but anyone is eligible to enter the lottery. In high school grades, students are admitted to the school on a first come first serve basis in accordance with availability. All students, parents, and educators sign a form committing to excellence. Excellence is defined by attendance, behavior, and commitment to an extended school day, week, and year. The school focuses heavily on academic achievement and strongly promotes students’ goals of college attainment. In correspondence with town demographics, 85% of students at the school where the study took place qualified to receive federal free lunch during the 2009-2010 school year. 97% of students who attended the school from 2009-2010 were African American. They predominantly spoke African American Vernacular English. During the 2008-2009 year, students passed state end-of-course tests in English, biology, geometry, and algebra at rates that exceeded the traditional public school in the town as well as the statewide pass rate. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 8 The 21 students who participated in the study were twelfth graders at the time of data collection. 14 of the students were female, and 7 were male. They were all African American. Each of them was on track to graduate in the spring of 2010 in compliance with state education requirements. Each of them professed a desire to obtain admission to and attend a four-year college or university. English curriculum at the school was based on Arkansas state standards. These standards focus on the same general content from ninth to twelfth grade but scaffold students’ understanding from the knowledge level of Bloom’s Taxonomy through the evaluation level of Bloom’s taxonomy. At the eleventh grade level, students are expected to analyze literature and literary elements as well as nonfiction texts, and at the twelfth grade level, standards emphasize evaluation of texts. Within the Arkansas frameworks, the school gives teachers the autonomy to prioritize or de-prioritize standards. Teachers write the curriculum, including year-long plans, unit plans, and lesson plans. Curriculum at the school is literature-based. Teachers predominantly use novel-sets and may choose which texts they wish students to read; teachers do not use reading textbooks. Thus, students’ exposure to literature varies from year to year. Vocabulary words are drawn from literature texts, and for grammar instruction, a grammar and writing textbook is used. III. Overview of ACT Assessment and Reading Subtest The ACT, originally American College Testing, was founded by E.F. Lindquist in 1959; Lindquist also developed the widely-used Iowa Test of Basic Skills (Lemann, 2000). The ACT contains five sections: math, English, reading, science reasoning, and an optional writing section. Test takers receive a score for each section, from 13 to 36, as READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 9 well as a composite score (ACT, 2008). The reading subtest, which is the focus of this study, includes four types of passages—prose fiction, humanities, social sciences, and natural sciences (ACT, 2008). Each passage is 750 words in length and has ten corresponding questions (ACT, 2008). Students receive a total of 35 minutes for the reading section (ACT, 2008). A sample passage and test items are presented in Appendix A. The ACT, unlike its competitor SAT, is a criterion-referenced test. This means that ACT questions align to a set of descriptive statements, or standards, about the skills and knowledge of test takers. Alternately, the SAT is a norm-referenced test, meaning that it projects a tester’s aptitude in comparison to other test takers. Because of its classification as a criterion-referenced test, ACT purports to assess whether or not high school students have acquired a set of skills and knowledge “essential to college and workforce training readiness” (ACT, 2009, p. 2). Based on the outline of essential skills and knowledge, the ACT prescribes what score students must earn in each section to demonstrate they are prepared for college; a score of 21 on the reading test is the benchmark for college readiness (ACT, 2009). As colleges have different levels of selectivity and different qualifications for admission, some deny admission to students who score below 21 on the reading test, while others require that students who score below 21 enroll in remedial English. ACT outlines the skills and knowledge required to earn a 21 in its College Readiness Standards for reading. ACT presents the College Readiness Standards in downloadable PDF format on their website at www.act.org. These standards are presented in chart format; in order to make them easier to manipulate, I arranged them in READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 10 list format, using general principles of unpacking standards informed by Wiggin’s and McTighe’s (2005) Understanding by Design. (See Appendix B.) These College Readiness Standards contain five categories: 1) main ideas and author’s approach; 2) supporting details; 3) sequential, comparative, and cause-effect relationships; 4) meanings of words; and 5) generalizations and conclusions. The wording of the College Readiness Standards increases the rigor of these knowledge and skills sets across texts of increasing difficulty. Additionally, skills range from the identification level of Bloom’s Taxonomy to the analysis level of Bloom’s Taxonomy. IV. Analysis and Results For the purposes of this study, I analyzed a data set containing students’ item by item performance on the reading subtest of a practice full-length ACT administered in the fall of 2009 to seniors attending a charter school in the rural Delta town (see description of the setting and students provided above). The test was selected from The Real ACT Prep Guide (Peterson’s, 2008). All student-identifying information was removed from the report by school administrators prior to giving it to me for analysis. I analyzed items from the reading section of the test to identify trends of understanding and misunderstanding. First, I looked at trends within each type of passage; then, I analyzed trends of standard mastery and non-mastery salient across all four passages; finally, I focused on specific analysis of items that seemed to be outliers in students’ performance. In order to analyze the trends, I paired test items with the respective ACT College Readiness Standard for reading. For example, the first questions for the prose sections reads, “Fran would most likely agree with which of the following statements about her relationship with Linda Rose?” This question matches with the College Readiness READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 11 Standard that requires students to make generalizations about characters and character relationships. As described above, the ACT reading subtest is divided into four passages, and each passage is a particular genre. Every reading subtest contains a prose fiction passage, a social science passage, a humanities passage, and a natural science passage. The order and arrangement of these passages may vary, but on this particular practice test the order was first prose, then social science, then humanities, and finally natural science. Table 1 below shows students’ rates of accuracy by test item. Items are listed in the order they appeared on the practice ACT reading test, and they are grouped by passage type. The raw number column shows the number of students out of the sample of 21 who answered that item correctly, while the percentage column shows the percentage of students who answered that item correctly. Generally, the students responded with greater accuracy on questions about characterization, demonstrating their ability to make generalizations about characters and characters’ attitudes, as well as drawing conclusions and inferences about the relationships between characters. On the prose passage, students showed higher mastery on questions about specific details and main idea than on subsequent content passages. Across all passages, student struggled with identifying and analyzing cause-effect; in addition, students performed poorly on questions that asked them to use context clues to infer the meanings of words and the meaning of figurative language. Table 2 presents another representation of trends, illustrating students’ percent accuracy by skill. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 12 TABLE 1 ACCURACY OF RESPONSES ON READING ASSESSMENT BY ITEM* (n=21) Passage type Item Raw number Percentage Prose Fiction 1. Character Generalization 2. Character Generalization 3. Supporting Detail 4. Main Idea 5. Main Idea 6. Supporting Detail 8. Cause-Effect 9. Generalization 10. Character Generalization 10 14 13 8 15 16 9 13 9 48 67 62 38 71 76 43 62 43 Social Science 11. Main Idea 12. Supporting Detail 13. Supporting Detail 14. Word Meaning 15. Supporting Detail 16. Word Meaning 17. Generalization 18. Cause-Effect 19. Word Meaning 20. Cause-Effect 9 3 10 6 8 13 8 6 10 11 43 14 48 29 38 62 38 29 48 52 Humanities 21. Main Idea 22. Supporting Detail 23. Supporting Detail 24. Supporting Detail 25. Word Meaning 26. Word Meaning 27. Cause-Effect 28. Supporting Detail 29. Word Meaning 30. Word Meaning 2 3 8 3 9 4 2 4 5 5 10 14 38 14 43 19 10 19 24 24 Natural Science 31. Word Meaning 32. Cause-Effect 33. Supporting Detail 34. Word Meaning 35. Supporting Detail 36. Word Meaning 37. Supporting Detail 38. Main Idea 39. Supporting Detail 40. Word Meaning 5 4 4 5 1 2 5 3 6 5 24 19 19 24 5 10 24 14 29 24 *Note: Items are identified broadly by topic rather than a specific ACT College Readiness Standard. For instance, “main idea” is a more general label than the actual ACT College Readiness Standard. For an exact list of College Readiness Standards, see Appendix B or visit www.act.org. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 13 TABLE 2 ACCURACY OF RESPONSES ON ACT READING ASSESSMENT BY SKILL* Skill Percent accuracy Character Generalization Generalization Main Idea Supporting Details Cause-Effect Word Meaning 53 50 35 30 30 30 *Note: Skills are identified broadly by topic rather than a specific ACT College Readiness Standard. For instance, “main idea” is a more general label than the actual ACT College Readiness Standard. For an exact list of College Readiness Standards, see Appendix B or visit www.act.org. In examination of Tables 1 and 2, test items 4 and 16 act as outliers from more general trends. Item 4 is a main idea question within the prose fiction section on which students showed noticeably less accuracy, and at a rate of 62% accuracy, item 16 is the only test question about word meanings on which students responded with greater than 50% accuracy. There are a few possible explanations about why these items deviate from overall trends. Item 4 refers to a paragraph that contains an extended metaphor about honing pigeons, a topic about which students may lack background knowledge. Lack of background knowledge could have contributed to a lower rate of accuracy. Meanwhile, item 16 arguably is the most straightforward test item about word meaning, as it directly asks the meaning of a single vocabulary word and the passage provides many context clues. Finally, Table 3 below illustrates the students’ accuracy by subject area and genre. This view of data roughly illustrates that students performed better on prose fiction than on content passages. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 14 TABLE 3 STUDENTS’ ACCURACY BY PASSAGE TYPE Passage Overall percent correct Prose Fiction Social Science Humanities Natural Science 57% 40% 22% 19% An additional factor that may have influenced performance is the sequence of passages and the length of the test. The class averaged 57% accuracy on questions corresponding to the first passage of the practice assessment, 40% on the second passage, 22% on the third passage, and 19% on the fourth passage. It is unclear whether this decline in performance can be explained entirely by the fact that students find content texts more challenging than fiction texts. Poor pacing or students’ decreasing stamina may cause some of the decline in accuracy at the end of the test. More students left items blank on the last section of the test. V. Instructional Suggestions Looking across the data, students exhibited greater success on the prose fiction passage. This trend is supported by the literature-based curriculum of the school. Particularly trends within the prose section show the students are generally more successful on questions about characterization, answering them with 53% accuracy. In the language of ACT’s College Readiness Standards, the sample of students demonstrated a beginning proficiency in “[drawing]…generalizations and conclusions about the main characters in…literary narratives” and “[combining] several pieces of information to make a reasonable generalization about a specific character” (ACT, 2008, READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 15 p.9). The language of this particular College Readiness Standard approximately aligns to Arkansas state standards for tenth and eleventh grade English. Thus, the Arkansas curriculum guidelines and the instruction at the school provide time for students practice these skills with literature they read in class. As students continue to practice these skills, their mastery of aligned ACT reading questions should continue to grow. In addition, students performed with a greater rate of success on questions about supporting details and main ideas within the prose fiction section. On the prose fiction section, students answered questions about supporting details with a rate of 69% accuracy as compared to an overall rate of 30% accuracy; meanwhile, students’ rate of accuracy on main idea questions within the prose fiction section was 55%, compared to 35% overall. In the midst of this trend of student success with prose, item 4 acted as an outlier. Question 4 from the prose fiction section addressed main idea, and students’ accuracy was only 38% as compared with 71% accuracy on question 5, which was also a main idea question. Two theories may explain why item 4 diverges from the trend. First, item 4 addresses the opening paragraph of the passage, while question 5 addresses the last paragraph. At the beginning of the passage, students would have developed less knowledge of the passage, and their lack of schema for the content of the paragraph may have made this question seem more difficult. A second explanation is that understanding of the first paragraph relies on students’ prior knowledge of honing pigeons, as the passage builds on a metaphor comparing a character in the passage to a honing pigeon. Regardless of outliers, analysis of the salient trends of greater student accuracy on the prose fiction section points to the rigor of the literature-based curriculum at the school. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 16 While prose is a demonstrated area of strength, declining success on content passages indicates that students need more support in content area literacy. The sample group of students revealed a few particular areas of weakness on content texts. As mentioned above, they showed less mastery on questions about identifying the main idea and supporting details on social science, humanities, and natural science passages. In addition, they struggled with inferring the author’s approach. A few additions may be made to direct comprehension instruction that will yield higher mastery of these skills. First, students can be asked to use questioning while they read. Alfred Tatum, whose work focuses on African American learners and struggling readers, suggested the following objective for during reading activities: “Students will identify the key characteristics of each paragraph and create a question that is answered by each paragraph (self-questioning strategy)” (2005, p. 62). Explicitly modeling this strategy for students and then asking them to use it independently promotes an awareness of main idea and author’s purpose. In addition, it may be helpful to teach students question-answer relationships, also known as QARs (Alvermann, Phelps, & Gillis, 2010). Developing students’ awareness of the relationship between questions and answers fosters metacognition about test items, giving them a greater sense of how to answer questions about main idea, author’s approach, and how details function within a passage. Research by Taffy Raphael demonstrated, “Students can be taught about QARs and that this knowledge of where answers come from can actually improve students’ ability to answer questions” (Alvermann et al., 2010, p. 208). Pearson and Johnson (1978) defined three types of QARs: textually explicit, textually implicit, and scriptally implicit. Textually explicit READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 17 answers are stated directly in the passage, while textually implicit answers are implied and require an inference. ACT asks readers to answer both types of question; however, ACT does not ask readers to answer scriptally implicit questions, as scriptally implicit questions require the reader to access personal beliefs. In order to instruct students on QARs, research recommends a scaffolded approach, beginning with the teacher’s explanation and modeling and continuing with time for students to practice reading passages and labeling questions. Class discussion of students’ work reinforces students’ understanding of QARs (Alvermann et al., 2010). A technique called Questioning the Author (QtA) provides a final instructional method for supporting students’ ability to identify main ideas, author’s approach, and how details function (Beck, McKeown, & Kucan, 1997). QtA makes transparent to students that a text’s “content is simply someone’s ideas written down, and that this person may not have always expressed things in the clearest or easiest way for readers to understand” (Beck et al., 1997, p. 18). This strategy encourages students to evaluate texts. Evaluation of texts enhances students’ comprehension of both expository and narrative texts, and Arkansas twelfth grade standards for English emphasize such evaluation. Beyond these benefits, QtA supports students’ ability to “understand the overall approach taken by an author or narrator” and to “discern which details…support important points” in passages (ACT, 2008, pp. 10-12). Thus QtA may yield benefits for students who struggle to identify and analyze author’s approach. Another noticeable area for growth in students’ scores is their ability to recognize cause-effect relationships. Implementing instruction on common text structures may support students’ comprehension across the curriculum as well as support their ability to READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 18 identify cause-effect relationships in ACT reading passages. Alvermann, Phelps, and Gillis reported, “Teachers can aid student comprehension both by teaching students about text structure and by using the structures inherent in text to help students organize the information that is presented” (2010, p. 223). Common text structures include lists, descriptions, definition/example, chronological/sequential order, compare and contrast, cause-effect, and problem-solution (Alvermann et al., 2010). In addition, knowledge of signal words proves helpful to students because signal words often indicate text structure patterns. For instance, signal words for cause-effect include: because, since, therefore, if…then, due to, hence, thus, as a result, consequently, subsequently, accordingly, eventually, initiated, precipitated, the outcome, the aftermath (Alverman et al., 2010). Teacher modeling and think-alouds, once again, may be used to support students’ understanding of how to identify and use knowledge of text structure and signal words to deepen comprehension. Along with teacher modeling and think-alouds about text structure patterns, graphic organizers may be useful to help students comprehend how awareness of text structure supports their comprehension, as well as their recognition of cause-effect relationships. Perhaps students’ greatest area of demonstrated weakness on the ACT practice test was vocabulary. As mentioned in the section above, students generally answered with less than 50% accuracy on questions regarding vocabulary. Only text item 16 acted as an outlier from this trend, and upon further examination, question 16 was the most straightforward word meaning question on the test, as it asked students to identify a synonym for circumscribed using context clues about the overall meaning of the passage. While item 16 shows an area of strength within the standard of identifying word READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 19 meanings, it seems clear from students’ performance on other word meaning questions that they need more comprehensive vocabulary instruction. The language of the College Readiness Standards asks that students demonstrate a number of skills in the area of word meanings, including understanding connotations of words and using context clues to define words and understand figurative language. Current vocabulary instruction at the school is based in the literature that students read; while this curriculum choice is a supported method for selecting words to introduce to students, research suggests many ways to enhance and build upon this foundation. After words are selected for study, students need to be provided with rich instructional experiences in order to develop a deep understanding of meanings and integrate vocabulary into their language and writing. Beck, McKeown, and Kucan wrote, “Multiple encounters over time are called for if the goal is more than a temporary surface-level understanding and if new word are to become permanently and flexibly represented in students’ vocabulary repertoires” (2002, p. 32). Consequently, teachers must carefully introduce words. When introducing words, research proves dictionary definitions insufficient (Beck et al., 2002). In order to provide more student-friendly explanations, Beck, McKeown, and Kucan suggested that teachers “characterize the word and how it is typically used” and “explain the meaning in everyday language” (2002, p. 33). Because the College Readiness Standards emphasize connotation as well as denotation, teachers should keep in mind the multidimensionality and polysemy of words when introducing them to students (Pearson, Heibert, Kamil, 2007). Beyond these broad guidelines, many instructional techniques can be used to help students integrate new words into their existing schema. Alvermann, Phelps, and Gillis (2010) suggested READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 20 concept maps, visual associations, and frayer models among other strategies. After initial interactions, students need opportunities to experience the words in texts and use the words in their own language and writing. In addition to providing instruction on specific vocabulary words, strategy-based instruction on use of context clues and use of morphemic analysis fosters students’ independence. Bromley (2007) reported that 60% of meaning of English words could be derived from analysis of word parts. Meanwhile, teachers almost unanimously suggest context clues as a means for identifying the meaning of unfamiliar vocabulary (Beers, 2003). Students’ ability to independently identify meanings of unfamiliar words in reading is essential to their success on the ACT. As with other instructional strategies, teacher think-alouds and modeling are best practice in teaching students the skill of identifying word meaning from context. Kylene Beers (2003) suggested that teachers focus on four types of context clues: definition or explanation clues, restatement or synonym clues, contrast or antonym clues, and gist clues. Definition clues provide the most specific explanation of word meanings, while restatement puts word meaning in simpler terms; contrast clues often provide an antonym in a following sentence, and gist clues are the “most subtle type of clue” (Beers, 2003, pp. 186-187). Because gist clues are the most subtle, they may not lead to a full understanding of the word, but they enable students to understand the gist of the passage or sentence. Therefore gist clues may deserve more time during instruction to ensure students master their use. Another powerful word identification strategy that fosters students’ independence is morphology or morphemic analysis. Morphemic analysis provides students with READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 21 knowledge of word parts, such as roots, prefixes, and suffixes; in turn, students may use these word parts to infer meanings of unfamiliar words. (Alvermann et al., 2010). Kieffer and Lesaux (2007) found that teaching morphology to urban students positively influenced their vocabulary ability and comprehension. Morphology instruction is most effective in the context of “rich, explicit vocabulary instruction,“ but in order to help students transfer knowledge of morphology from class work to independent settings like the ACT reading exam, teachers need to model how to use morphology to infer word meanings in context and provide students time to practice this skill (Kieffer & Lesaux, 2007, pp. 138-140). A final way the school might support vocabulary growth is by adding a technology component to the curriculum. Digital reading environments provide support for vocabulary through hypertext links to dictionary and thesaurus entries. Proctor, Dalton, and Grisham (2007) found that using tools like digital reading environments support both students’ vocabulary growth and reading comprehension. Finally, multiple factors might cause students’ declining rate of accuracy from the first passage to the last passage of the test. One possible cause is lack of familiarity with the test and therefore a lack of awareness about pacing. Hughes explained, “If any aspect of a test is unfamiliar to candidates, they are likely to perform less well than they would otherwise…For this reason, every effort must be made to ensure that all candidates have the opportunity to learn just what will be required of them” (2002, pp. 47-48). Thus, students’ accuracy may increase if they understand the format of the test, number of questions per passages, and time limits. In the testing environment, reminding students of time limits may positively influence students’ performance. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 22 VI. Concluding Thoughts Last year, my classroom environment featured an inspiration wall, or a wall of quotes and pictures collected by my students to inspire our hard work and tenacity in pursuit of academic excellence. One of my girls added the following quote, from rapper T.I. (2008), to our wall: “Make impossible possible. Even when winning’s illogical, loosing’s still far from optional.” Looking at the words of this quote, I think about my students’ educational journeys. My students’ success is far from impossible, but they need great teachers who build relationships upon respect and trust and who work hard to continually implement and modify the curriculum to support every student’s achievement. I argue a great curriculum begins with literacy. The strategies and support described in this paper have potential benefits beyond success on the ACT reading subtest; the strategies and support described in this paper will makes students better readers across the curriculum. The journey out of generational poverty, the same journey T.I. speaks of, is a difficult one. I agree that at first glance it might seem illogical to teach students how to speak Standard English or to modify curriculum explicitly to support students’ success on culturally biased assessments like the ACT. However, my reasoning always returns to the arguments of Delpit (1995) and the idea of empowering students to succeed in multiple social spheres by giving them access to different cultural codes. To borrow from the ambiguous advice of the narrator’s grandfather in Ralph Ellison’s Invisible Man (1952, 1995), we will “overcome ‘em with yeses, undermine em’ with grins, agree ‘em to death and destruction” (p. 16). Like the unnamed protagonist, when my students are READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 23 empowered with the ability to assimilate, deviate, and advocate in any sphere of society, then the world, indeed, will be a better place. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM Appendix A QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 24 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 25 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 26 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 27 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 28 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 29 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 30 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 31 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM QuickTime™ and a TIFF (LZW) decompressor are needed to see this picture. 32 READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 33 Appendix B ACT COLLEGE READINESS STANDARDS: READING (WORDING FROM ACT, REORGANIZED BY BLOOM’S LEVEL AND TEXT DIFFICULTY) Strand 1: Main Ideas and Author’s Approach (a.k.a. Author’s Purpose) MI.13.15.1 Recognize a clear intent of an author or narrator in uncomplicated literary narratives. MI.16.19.1 Identify a clear main idea or purpose of Straightforward paragraphs in uncomplicated literary narratives (16-19) Identify a clear main idea or purpose of any paragraph or paragraphs in uncomplicated passages (24-27) Identify clear main ideas or purposes of complex passages or their paragraphs (33-36) MI.20.23.1 Infer the main idea or purpose of Straightforward paragraphs in uncomplicated literary narratives (20-23) Straightforward paragraphs in more challenging passages (24-27) More challenging passages or their paragraphs (28-32) MI.20.23.2 Understand the overall approach taken by an author or narrator (e.g., point of view, kinds of evidence used) in Uncomplicated passages (20-23) More challenging passages (24-27) Virtually any passage (28-32) MI.24.27.3 Summarize basic events and ideas in More challenging passages (24-27) Virtually any passage (28-32) Strand 2: Supporting Details SD.13.15.1 Locate within the text basic facts (e.g. names, dates, events) that are clearly stated (13-15) simple details at the sentence and paragraph level in uncomplicated passages (16-19) important details in uncomplicated passages (20-23) important details in more challenging passages (24-27) SD.16.19.2 Recognize a clear function of a part of an uncomplicated passage o How I would say it: Identify function of a part of a passage And understand the function of a part of a passage when the function is subtle or complex (33-36) And make simple inferences about how details are used in passages (20-23) o How I would say it: Infer function of a part of a passage And discern which details, though they may appear in different sections throughout a passage, support important points in more challenging passages (2427) READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 34 o How I would say it: Analyze how details are used to support the main idea in more challenging passages And use details from different sections of some complex informational passages to support a specific point or argument (28-32) o How I would say it: Synthesize details from different parts of the passage to support specific points and arguments SD.24.27.2 Locate and interpret minor or subtly stated details in uncomplicated passages (24-27) more challenging passages (28-32) in complex passages (33-36) Strand 3: Sequential, Comparative, and Cause-Effect Relationships SCCE.13.15.1 Determine when (e.g., first, last, before, after) or if an event occurred in uncomplicated passages And order simple sequences of events in uncomplicated literary narratives (2023) And order sequences of events in uncomplicated passages (24-27) And order sequences of events in more challenging passages (28-32) And order sequences of events in complex passages (33-36) SCCE.13.15.2 Recognize clear cause-effect relationships described within a single sentence in a passage (13-15) a single paragraph in uncomplicated literary narratives (16-19) uncomplicated passages (20-23) more challenging passages, and (24-27) understand implied or subtly stated cause-effect relationships in uncomplicated passages, and within (24-27) more challenging passages, and (28-32) understand implied, subtle, or complex cause-effect relationships in virtually any passage (33-36) SCCE.16.19.1 Identify relationships between main characters in uncomplicated literary narratives and within more challenging literary narratives (24-27) SCCE.20.23.2/SCCE.24.27.2 Identify and understand clear relationships between people, ideas, and so on in uncomplicated passages and within more challenging passages (28-32) virtually any passage (33-36) Strand 4: Meanings of Words W.13.15.1 Understand the implication of a familiar word or phrase and of simple descriptive language W.16.19.1 Use context to understand basic figurative language W.20.23.1 Use context to determine the appropriate meaning of some figurative and nonfigurative words, phrases, and statements in uncomplicated passages W.24.27.1 Use context to determine the appropriate meaning of virtually any word, phrase, or statement in uncomplicated passages READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 35 W.24.27.2 Use context to determine the appropriate meaning of some figurative and nonfigurative words, phrases, and statements in more challenging passages W.28.32.1 Determine the appropriate meaning of words, phrases, or statements from figurative or somewhat figurative language W.33.36.1 Determine, even when the language is richly figurative and the vocabulary is difficult, the appropriate meaning of context-dependent words, phrases, or statements in virtually any passage Strand 5: Generalizations and Conclusions GC.13.15.1 Draw simple generalizations and conclusions about the main characters in uncomplicated literary narratives, and draw subtle generalizations and conclusions about characters, ideas, and so on in uncomplicated literary narratives (24-27) understand and generalize about portions of a complex literary narratives (33-36) GC.16.19.1/GC.20.23.1 Draw simple generalizations and conclusions about people, ideas, and so on in uncomplicated passages, and using details that support the main points of more challenging passages, and (2023) in more challenging passages, and (24-27) use information from one or more sections of a more challenging passage to draw generalizations and conclusions about people, ideas, and so on, and (28-32) draw complex or subtle generalizations and conclusions about people, ideas, and so on, often by synthesizing information from different portions of the passage (33-36) How I think about the Generalizations and Conclusions Standards: Passages Literary Narratives Students need to be able to make simple, Students need to be able to make simple, subtle and complex generalizations subtle and complex generalizations about… about… 1. people 1. main characters and their relationships 2. ideas 2. ideas 3. main points 4. aspects of the passage using details Students need to be able to synthesize information and details from different parts of the passage. *Reading level/difficulty of the text is also scaffolded for both non-literary and literary passages. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 36 References ACT. (2008). Connecting college readiness standards to the classroom: For language arts teachers/reading. Retrieved from www.act.org ACT. (2009). Measuring college and career readiness: The class of 2009, Arkansas. Retrieved from www.act.org Alvermann, D., Phelps, S., & Gillis, V. (2010). Content area reading and literacy: Succeeding in today’s diverse classrooms (6th edition.). New York: Allyn & Bacon. Beck, I., McKeown, M., Hamilton, R., & Kucan, L. (1997). Questioning the author: An approach for enhancing student engagement with text. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Beck, L., McKeown, M. & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life. New York: The Gulliford Press. Beers, K. (2003). When kids can’t read: What teachers can do. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Bromley, K. (2007). Nine things every teacher should know about words and vocabulary instruction. Journal of adolescent and adult literacy, 50 (7), 528-537. Delpit, L. (1995). Other people’s children: Cultural conflict in the classroom. New York, New York: The New Press. Ellison, R. (1995). Invisible man, (2nd edition.). New York: Vintage International. Fink, R. P. (1996). Successful dyslexics: A constructivist study of passionate interest reading. Journal of adolescent and adult literacy, 39 (4), 268-280. Hillard, A. (2002). Language, culture, and the assessment of African American children. Delpit, Lisa & Dowdy, Joanne Kilgour. (Eds.), The skin that we speak: Thoughts on language and culture in the classroom (pp. 88-105). New York, New York: The New Press. Hughes, A. (2002). Testing for language teachers. New York: Cambridge University Press. Jennings, J., Caldwell, J., & Lerner, J. (2010). Reading problems: Assessment and teaching strategies (6th edition.). New York: Allyn & Bacon. READING ON THE ACT COLLEGE ENTRANCE EXAM 37 Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2007). Breaking down words to build meaning: morphology, vocabulary, and reading comprehension in the urban classroom. The reading teacher, 61 (2), 134-144. Lemann, N. (2000). The big test: The secret history of the American meritocracy. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. NA. (2008). The real ACT prep guide (2nd ed.). Iowa City, Iowa: Peterson’s. NA. (2010). Helena, Arkansas census and community profile [On-line]. Available: http://www.americantowns.com/ar/helena-information Pearson, P.D., & Hiebert, E.H., & Kamil, M.L. (2007). Vocabulary assessment: What we know and what we need to learn. Reading research quarterly, 42 (2), 282-296. Pearson, P.D., & Johnson, D. (1978). Teaching reading comprehension. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Proctor, C.P, & Dalton, B., & Grisham, D.L. (2007). Scaffolding English language learners and struggling readers in a universal literacy environment with embedded strategy instruction and vocabulary support. Journal of literacy research, 39 (1), 71-93. Risko, V. & Walker-Dalhouse, D. (2007). Tapping students’ cultural funds of knowledge to address the achievement gap. The Reading Teacher, 61 (1), 98-100. T.I. (2008). “No matter what.” On Paper Trail [CD]. Atlanta, Georgia: Grand Hustle/Atlantic. Tatum, A. (2005). Teaching reading to Black adolescent males: Closing the achievement gap. Portland, Maine: Stenhouse Publishers. Wiggins, G. & McTighe, J. (2005). Understanding by design (2nd edition.). Columbus, Ohio: Pearson.