CHAPTER 6 What Has fMRI ThughtUs About Object Recognition? Kalanit Grill-Spector recogn researc t-ocust that ar recogr Int about tt'rpic u: ed I Fttr e: oblec i [1r\\ 6.L lntroduction Humanscaneffortlesslyrecognizeobjectsin a fractionof a seconddespitelargevariability in the appearanceof objects (Thorpe et al. 1996). What are the underlying representations and computationsthat enablethis remarkablehumanability? One wa1 to answerthesequestionsis to investigatethe neuralmechanisms of objectrecognition in the humanbrain.With the adventof functionalmagneticresonance imaging(fMRI I about 15 yearsago, neuroscientists and psychologistsbeganto examinethe neural basesof objectrecognitionin humans.Functionalmagneticresonance imaging(fMRI I is an attractivemethodbecauseit is a noninvasivetechniquethat allowsmultiple measurements of brain activationin the sameawakebehavinghuman.Among noninvasive techniques,it providesthe best spatialresolutioncurrently available,enablingus to localizecortical activationsin the spatialresolutionof millimeters(as fine as I mm I andat a reasonable time scale(in the orderof seconds). Before the adventof fMRI, knowledgeaboutthe function of the ventral streamwas basedon single-unitelectrophysiology measurements in monkeysandon lesionstudies. Thesestudiesshowedthat neuronsin the monkeyinferotemporal(IT) cortexrespondto shapes(Fujitaetal.1992)andcomplexobjectssuchasfaces(Desimoneet al. 1984),and thatlesionsto theventralstreamcanproducespecificdeficitsin objectrecognition,such (inability to recognize as agnosia(inability to recognizeobjects)and prosopagnosia faces,Farah 1995).However,interpretinglesiondatais complicatedbecauselesions aretypically diffuse(usuallymorethanoneregionis damaged),typically disruptboth a corticalregionandits connectivity,andarenot replicableacrosspatients.Therefore. the primary knowledge gained from fMRI researchwas which cortical sites in the normalhumanbrain are involvedin objectrecognition.The first set of fMRI studies of objectandfacerecognitionin humansidentifiedthe regionsin the humanbrainthat respondselectivityto objectsand faces(Malachet al. 1995;Kanwisheret al. 1997 Grill-Spectoret al. 1998b).Then a seriesof studiesdemonstrated that activationin object- and face-selective regionscorrelateswith successat recognizingobject and providingstrikingevidencefor the involvementof theseregionsin faces,respectively, 102 rrrtru: oh1e, rcco! .rf ttx rr her , lloP rmpl lr CO ..,fF lr :iur.\ of rI I I rrf :cl.: ::E cio :NU .-rf -rJ 3l -tl T1 .i JI wHAT nls flrnr TAUGHTus ABour oBJEcr REcocNrrron? tUs ion? rddespitelargevariare the underlying an ability?Oneway rf objectrecognition nceimaging(fMRI) examinethe neural nceimaging(fMRI) llowsmultiplemea\mong noninvasive ble. enablingus to ; (asfine as I mm) ventralstreamwas d on lesionstudies. ) cortexrespondto neetal. 1984),and t recognition,such rility to recognize d because lesions cally disruptboth rtients.Therefore, nical sitesin the t of fMRI studies humanbrainthat isheret al. 1997; :hatactivationin izing objectand 'theseregions in 103 recognition(Grill-Spectoret al. 2000;Bar et al.200l; Grill-Spectoret al. 2004).Once researchersfound which regionsin the cortex are involved in object recognition,the and computations focus of researchshifted to examiningthe natureof representations that are implementedin theseregionsto understandhow they enableefficient object recognitionin humans. "' In this chapter I will review fMRI researchthat provided important knowledge in the humanbrain. I choseto focus on this aboutthe natureof object representations topic becauseresults from theseexperimentsprovide important insights that can be usedby computerscientistswhen they designartificial object recognitionsystems. For example,one of the fundamentalproblemsin recognitionis how to recognizean (invariantobjectrecognition).Understanding objectacrossvariationsin its appearance how a biological systemhas solvedthis problem may give cluesfor how to build a robust artificial recognition system.Funher, fMRI is more adequatefor measuring than the temporal sequenceof computationsen route to object object representations is longerthan the time scale recognitionbecausethe time scaleof fMRI measurements of the recognitionprocess(the temporalresolutionof fMRI is in the orderof seconds, combiningpsywhereasobject recognitiontakesabout 100-250 ms). Nevertheless, chophysicswith fMRI may give us somecluesaboutwhat kind of visualprocessingis implementedin distinct cortical regions.For example,finding regionswhoseactivation is correlatedwith successat sometasks,but not others,may suggestthe involvement of particularcortical regionsin one computation,but not another. In discussinghow fMRI hasimpactedour currentunderstandingof objectrepresentations,I will focuson resultspertainingto two aspectsof objectrepresentation: . How do the underlyingrepresentations providefor invariantobjectrecognition? . How is categoryinformationrepresented in the ventralstream? I havechosenthesetopicsbecause(i) they are centraltopicsin objectrecognition for which fMRI has substantiallyadvancedour understanding.(ii) Some findings relatedto thesetopics stirred considerabledebate(see sect.6.7), and (iii) someof the fMRI findings in humansare surprisinggiven prior knowledgefrom single-unit electrophysiologyin monkeys.In organizingthe chapter,I will begin with a brief introductionof the functionalorganizationof the humanventralstreamanda definition of object-selectivecortex. Then I will describeresearchthat elucidatedthe properties of theseregionswith respectto basiccodingprinciples.I will continuewith findings relatedto invariant object recognition, and end with researchand theoriesregarding categoryrepresentationand specializationin the humanventral stream. 6.2 The Functional Organization of the Human Ventral Stream The first set of fMRI studieson object and face recognitionin humanswas devotedto Electrophysiology identifyingtheregionsin thebrainthatareobject-andface-selective. that neuronsin higher-levelregionsrespondto shapes researchin monkeyssuggested and objectsmore than simple stimuli such as lines, edges,and patterns(Desimone et al. 1984;Fujita et al. 1992;Logothetiset al. 1995).Basedon thesefindings,fMRI studiesmeasuredbrain activationwhen peopleviewedpicturesof objectscomparedto t04 KALANIT GRILL-SPECTOR I I ; ; I I I I (b) Eto t Ea x I lo E -..traa|raalrahlJla 6a r o *rftdrt f' +Ut I a I I Puonlrtlon Dwrlbo (mr) Figure6.1. Object-,face-,and place-selective cortex.(a) Data of one representive subjeo shownon her partiallyinflatedright hemisphere: lateralview (/eft);ventralview (right).Dark gray indicatessulci. Lightgray indicatesgyri. Black /inesdelineateretinotopicregions.Blue regionsdelineateobject-selective regions(objects> scrambledobjects),includingLO and pFus/OTSventrallyas well as dorsalfoci along the intraparietalsulcus(lPS).Redregions delineateface-selective regions(faces> non-facesobjects),includingthe FFA,a regionin LOS and a region in posteriorSTS.Magentaregionsdelineateoverlap betweenface- and regions(places> objectst, objects-selective regions.Creenregionsdelineateplace-selective includingthe PPAand a dorsalregionlateralto the lPS.Dark greenregionsdelineateoverlap betweenplace-and object-selective regions.All mapsthresholdedat P < 0.001,voxel level. (b)LO and pFus/OTS (butnot Vl) responses (Grillarecorrelatedwith recognitionperformance recognitionperformanceand fMRl signalson the same Spectoret al. 2000).To superimpose plot, all valueswere normalizedrelativeto the maximumresponsefor the 500-msduration s ti m u l u s . "hsignal(condition) ForfMRf signars(brue,red, and orarrge lines): For recognitionperforffi. o/ocorrect(condition). (seecolor plate6.1.) mance(bfack)"hcorrect(5O0ms) scrambled objects (have the same local information and statistics, but do not contain an object) or texture patterns (e.9., checkerboards, which are robust visual stimuli, but do not elicit a percept of a global form). These studies found a constellation of regions in the lateral occipital cortex (or the lateral occipital complex, LOC), beginning around the lateral occipital sulcus, posterior to MT, and extending ventrally into the occipitotemporal sulcus (OTS) and the fusiform gyrus (Fus), that respond more to objects than controls. The LOC is located lateral and anterior to early visual areas (VlV4), (Grill-Spector et al. t998a; Grill-Spector et al. 1998b) and is typically divided into two subregions: LO, a region in the lateral occipital cortex, adjacent and posterior to MT; and pFus/OTS; a ventral region overlapping the OTS and posterior fusiform gyrus (Fig. 6.1). The lateral occipital complex (LOC) responds similarly to many kinds of objects and object categories (including novel objects) and is thought to be in the intermediate- or high-level stages of the visual hierarchy. Importantly, LOC activations are correlated with subjects' object recognition perfonnance. High LOC responses correlate with I I I I I I I I I WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT ntive subject 'rishtt. Dark regions. B/ue ding LO and Red regions . a region in ln face- and s > objects\. teateoverlap , r , o x e ll e v e l . r t a n c er C r i l i on the same -ms duration t i : r c l np e n o r - not contatn ral stimulr. te llat iono i . b e ginn i n g ll 1 int o rh e td more tt-r areas( \'l ll r divide d d posterior ,r fusiform US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? 105 successfulobject recognition(hits), and low LOC responsescorrelatewith trials in which objectsare presentbut not recognized(misses)(Fig. 6.1(b)).Object-selective regionsare also found in the dorsal stream(Grill-Spector2003; Grill-Spectorand Malach2004),but activationof theseregionsdoesnot correlatewith objectrecognition performance(Fang and He 2005). Theseregionsmay be involved in computations relatedto visually guided actionstoward objects(Culhamet al. 2003). However,a comprehensive discussionof the role of the dorsal streamin object perceptionis beyondthe scopeof this chapter. In addition to the LOC, researchersfound severalventral regionsthat showpreferential responsesto specificobject categories.The searchfor regionswith categorical preferencewas motivatedby repons that suggestedthat lesionsto the ventral stream canproducevery specificdeficits,suchasthe inability to recognizefacesor the inability to readwords,while othervisual (andrecognition)facultiesremainpreserved.By contrastingactivationsto different kinds of objects,researchersfound ventral regions that show higher responsesto specificobject categories:including lateral fusiform regionsthat respondmore to animalsthan tools, and medial fusiform regionsthat respondto tools more than animals(Martin et al. 1996;Chao et al. 1999)),a region in the left OTS that respondsmore strongly to letters than textures(the visual word form area,or VWFA) (Cohenet al. 2000); severalfoci that respondmore strongly to facesthan to other objects(Kanwisheret al. 1997;Haxby et al. 2000; Hoffman and Haxby 2000; Grill-Spector et al. 2004), including the well known fusiform face area (FFA) (Kanwisheret al. 1997),regionsthatrespondmorestronglyto housesandplaces gyrus,the parahipthanfacesand objects(includinga regionin the parahippocampal pocampalplacearea(PPA)(Epsteinand Kanwisher1998);regionsthat respondmore stronglyto body partsthan facesand objects,including a regionnearMT called the extrastriate body area(EBA) (Downinget al. 2001);anda regionin the fusiformgyrus (the fusiform body area,or FBA) (Schwarzloseet al. 2005)).Nevertheless, many of regionsrespondto more than one object theseobject-selective and category-selective categoryand also respondstronglyto object fragments(Grill-Spectoret al. 1998b; Lerneret al. 200I; Lerneret al. 2008).This suggeststhat one mustbe cautiouswhen interpretingthe natureof the selectiveresponses.It is possiblethat the underlying representationis perhapsof object parts,features,and/orfragmentsand not of whole objectsor objectcategories. regionsin the humanbrain initiateda fiercedebate Findingsof category-selective abouttheprinciplesof functional organizationin the ventralstream.Are thereregionsin the cortexthat are specializedfor any objectcategory?How abstractis the information in theseregions, represented in theseregions(e.g.,is categoryinformationrepresented I will addressthese or low-levelvisual featuresthat are associatedwith categories)? questionsin detailin section6.7. in the LOC 6.3 Cue-InvariantResponses lbjectsand rediate-or correlated e l a t e u'i th responsesin the humanbrain were suggestive Although findingsof object-selective of the involvementof theseregion in processingobjects,there are many differences betweenobjectsand scrambledobjects(or objectsandtexturepatterns).Objectshave 106 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR I Ob;c !r , I Figure responses visual 6.2.Selective to objects across multiple cuesacross theLOC.Statistical mapsof selective response to objectfrom luminance, stereoandmotioninformation in a representative subject. All mapswerethresholded atP < 0.005,voxellevel,andareshownon (a)Luminance the inflated righthemisphere of a representative > scrambled subject. objects (b)Objectsgenerated objects. fromrandomdot stereograms versus structureless randomdot (perceived streograms fromdot motionversus asa cloudof dots).(c)Objectsgenerated the same dotsmoving randomly. Visual meridians arerepresented byred(upper), blue(horizontal), andgreen(lower)lines.Whitecontourindicates region,or MT.Adapted a motion-selective fromVinberg andCrill-Spector 2008.(Seecolorplate6.2.) shapes,surfaces,and contours;they are associatedwith a meaning and semantic information;and they are generallymore interestingthan texturepatterns.Each of thesefactorsmay affectthe higherfMRI responseto objectsthancontrols.Differences in low-levelvisualpropertiesacrossobjectsandcontrolsmay be driving someof these effectsaswell. Convergingevidencefrom severalstudiesrevealedan importantaspectof coding in the LOC: it respondsto object shape,not low-levelvisual features.Severalstudies showedthatall LOC subregions(LO andpFus/OTS)areactivatedmorestronglywhen subjectsview objectsindependentof the type of visual informationthat definesthe object form (Grill-Spectoret al. 1998a;Kastneret al. 2000; Kourtzi and Kanwisher 2000, 2001; Gilaie-Dotanet al. 2002; Vinberg and Grill-Spector2008) (Fig. 6.2). The lateral occipital complex (LOC) respondsmore strongly to (1) objectsdefined by luminancethan to luminancetextures;(2) objects generatedfrom random dot (3) objectsgeneratedfrom stereograms than to structureless randomdot stereograms; structurefrom motionthanto random(structureless) motion;and(4) objectsgenerated from texturesthanto texturepatterns.Theresponse of theLOC to objectsis alsosimilar acrossobject formatl (gray-scale,line drawings,and silhouettes),and it respondsto objectsdelineatedby both real and illusory contours(Mendolaet al. 1999;Stanley and Rubin 2003).Kourtzi and Kanwisher(Kourtzi and Kanwisher2001)also showed thattherewasfMRl-adaptation(fMRI-A, indicatinga commonneuralsubstrate), when objectshadthe sameshapebut differentcontours,but that therewasno fMRI-A when the sharedcontourswere identicalbut the perceivedshapewas different,suggesting that the LOC respondsto global shaperatherthan to local contours(seealso Lerner etal.2002;Kourtzi etal.2003).Overall,thesestudiesprovidedfundamentalknowledge ! 1 , i t'Jf ::€ .'f a ! J t .t I Selectiveresponsesto faces and housesacross stimulus format (photographs,line drawings, and silhouettes) have also been shown for the FFA and PPA, respectively. WHAT HAS fMRI (a)object TAUGHT Hole US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? 107 Random Edges ffi (b) vl W trw v4 LO pFus/OTS :c i f; r.sl t: (.,r F: l' ,' i'*3: li rf e Ei 910 E os , 1 -9 onn - .s"- O H E S R Condition figure 6.3. Responses to shape,edges,and surfacesacrossthe ventralstream.(a)Schematic illustration from eithermotionor stereo of experimental conditions.Stimuliwere generated rnformation alone and had no luminanceedgesor surfaces(exceptfor the screenborder, n'hichwas presentduringthe entireexperiment, includingblank baselineblocks).Forillustrationpurposes,darkerregionsindicatefront surfaces.Fromleft to right Object on the front surfacein front of a flat backgroundplane.Hole on the front surfacein front of a flat backqround.Disconnected by scrambling edgesin frontof a flat background.Edgesweregenerated the shapecontours.Surfaces, flat surfacesat differentdepths.Random Two semitransparent stimuliwith no coherentstructure, or globalshape.Randomstimuli edges,globalsurfaces, had the samerelativedisparityor depthrangeas otherconditions.Seeexamplesof stimuli: (b) Responses http://www-psych.stanford.edu/-kalanit/jnpstim/. to objects,holes,edges,and mean+ SEMacross hierarchy. Responses: acrossthe visualventral-processing elobalsurfaces eightsubjects.O, object; H, hole; 5, surfaces;F, edges;R, random;diamonds,significantly differentthanrandomat P < 0.05.Adaptedfrom Vinbergand Crill-Spector 2008. by showing that activation of the LOC is driven by shape rather than by the low-level visual information that generatesform. In a recent study, we examined whether the LOC response to objects is driven by their global shape or their surface information and whether LOC subregions are sensitive to border ownership. One open question in object recognition is whether the region in the image that belongs to the object is first segmented from the rest of the image (figure-ground segmentation)and then recognized, or whether knowing the shapeof an object aids its segmentation(Petersonand Gibson 1994b,|994a;Nakayama et al. 1995). To addressthese questions,we scannedsubjectswhen they viewed stimuli that were matched for their low-level information and generated different percepts: ( I ) a percept of an object in front of a background object, (2) a shaped hole (same shape as the object) in front of a background, (3) two flat surfaces without shapes, (4) local edges (created by scrambling the object contour) in front of a background, or (5) random dot stimuli with no structure (Fig. 6.3(a)) (Vinberg and Grill-Spector 2008). We repeatedthe experiment twice, once with random dots that were presented stereoscopicallyand once with random dots that moved. We found that LOC responses (both LO and pFus/OTS) were higher for objects and shaped holes than for surfaces, local edges, or random stimuli (Fig. 6.3(b)). These results were observed for both 108 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR motion and stereocues.In contrast,LOC responseswere not higher for surfacesthan for randomstimuli and were not higher for local edgesthan for randomstimuli. Thus. adding either local edge information or global surfaceinformation doesnot increase LOC response.Adding a gl66al shape,however,doesproducea significant increase in LOC response.Theseresultsprovideclearevidencethat cue-invariantresponses in the LOC are driven by object shape,ratherthan by global surfaceinformation or local edgeinformation. Interestingly, recent evidence shows that the LOC is sensitive to border ownership/figure-groundsegmentation)(Appelbaumet al. 2006; Vinberg and GrillSpector2008).We found that the LO andpFus/OTSresponses werehigherfor objects (shapespresentedin the foreground)than for the sameshapeswhen they had defined holes in the foreground.The only differencebetweenthe objects and the holes was the assignmentof the figure region (or border ownershipof the contour defining the shape).This higherresponseto objectsthanto holeswasa uniquecharacteristic of LOC (Fig. subregionsand did not occur in other visual regions 6.3). This result suggests that the LOC prefers shapes(and contours)when they define the figure region. One implication of this result is that the samebrain machinerymay be involved in both recognizingobjectsand in determiningwhich region in the visual input containsthe figure region.Thus,oneconsiderationfor computerscientistsis that an effectiveobject recognitionalgorithmshoulddetermineboth what is the the objectin the sceneaswell aswhich regionin the scenecorresponds to the object. 6.4 Neural Bases of Invariant Object Recognition The literature reviewed so far provides evidencethat the LOC is involved in the recognitionandprocessingof objectform. Giventhe LOC's role in objectperception. onemay consider,how it dealswith variability in an object'sappearance. Many factors canaffectthe appearance of objects.Changesin an object'sappearance canoccurasa resultof the objectbeing at differentlocationsrelativeto the observer,which will affect it's retinalprojectionof objectsin termsof sizeandposition.Also, the 2-D projection of a 3-D object on the retina varies considerablyowing to changesin its rotation and viewpoint relative to the observer.Other changesin appearanceoccur because of differential illumination conditions, which affect an object's color, contrast,and shadowing.Nevertheless,humansare able to recognizeobjectsacrosslarge changes in their appearance, which is referredto as invariant object recognition. A central topic of researchin the study of object recognition is understanding how invariant recognition is accomplished.One view suggeststhat invariant object recognitionis accomplishedbecausetheunderlyingneuralrepresentations areinvariant to the appearance of objects.Thus,this view suggeststhat thereneuralresponses will remain similar even when the appearanceof an object changesconsiderably.One meansby which this can be achievedis by extractingfrom the visual input featuresor fundamentalelements(suchas geons(Biederman1987)that are relatively insensitive to changesin objects' appearance. According to one influential model (recognitionby components, or RBC; Biederman1987),objectsarerepresented by a library of geons, whichareeasyto detectin manyviewingconditions,andby theirspatialrelations.Other theoriessuggestthat invariancemay be generatedthrougha sequenceof computations WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? 109 aro\s a hierarchically organizedprocessingstreamin which the level of invariance n'reases from one level of the processingto the next. For example,at the lowestlevel ,r theprocessingstream,neuronscodelocal features;at higherlevelsof theprocessing rtrcam,neuronsrespondto more complexshapesand are ldsi sensitiveto changesin andPoggio 1999). axition and size(Riesenhuber \euroimaging studiesof invariantobjectrecognitionfound differentialsensitivity aross the ventralstreamto objecttransformations suchas size,position,illumination, aJ r iewpoint. Intermediateregions, such as LO, show higher sensitivity to image anstormationsthan higher-levelregions,suchas pFus/OTS.Notably,evidencefrom o.m) studiessuggeststhat at no point in the ventral streamare neuralrepresentations csctrelyinvariantto objecttransformations. Theseresultssupportan accountin which ryrariantrecognitionis supportedby a pooledresponseacrossneuralpopulationsthat r: One way in which this canbe accomplishedis "ensitiveto objecttransformations. Qt a neuralcodethat containsindependentsensitivityto object information and object firnrlormation (DiCarlo andCox 2007);for example,neuronsmay be sensitiveto both ro.!eL-t categoryand object position. As long as the categoricalpreferenceis retained rrLrsSobject transformations,invariantobject information can be extracted. 6.5 Object and PositionInformation in the LOC tfnc r ariation that the object recognitionsystemneedsto deal with is variation in the ruc andpositionof objects.Sizeandpositioninvariancearethoughtto be accomplished x Franby an increasein the size of neural receptivefields along the visual hierarchy : e..a-soneascends the visualhierarchy,neuronsrespondto stimuli acrossa largerpart ;t the visualfield). At the sametime, a more complexvisual stimulusis necessaryto ir*rt si,enificant in neurons(e.g.,shapesinsteadof orientedlines).Findings responses -'m electrophysiology suggestthat evenat the higheststagesof the visualhierarchy, Eurons retainsomesensitivityto objectlocationand sizealthoughelectrophysiology rn)rt\ r'arysignificantlyaboutthe degreeof positionsensitivityof IT neurons(Op De Bsclk and Vogels2000;Rolls 2000;DiCarlo and Maunsell2003).A relatedissueis rirether positionsensitivityof neuronsin highervisual areasmanifestsas an orderly, rrgrglnphic representation have examinedposition of the visual field. Researchers (such rize as PPA and FFA) using srJ sensitivityin the LOC and nearby cortex nr&\urementsof the mean responseacrossa region of interest;fMRI-A, in which trr measuredsensitivityto changesin objectsizeor position;andexaminationof the .fo'rnbutedresponseacrossthe ventral streamto the sameobject or object category rrr-r\\ sizesandpositions. Sereralstudiesdocumented sensitivityto botheccentricityandpolaranglein distinct ",rntral streamregions.Both object-selectiveand category-selective regions in the r<ntralstreamrespondto objectspresentedat multiple positionsand sizes.However, - amplitudeof responseto object variesacrossdifferentretinal positions.The LO regions(e.g.,FFA, PPA) respondmore rnt pFus/OTSas well as category-selective ist-'ngl) to objectspresentedin the contralateralversusipsilateralvisual field (Grill>,lcr'roret al. 1998b;Hemondet al. 2007;McKyton andZohary2007).Someregions LO andEBA) alsorespondmorestronglyto objectspresentedin the lower visualfield l+,rres and Grill-Spector2008; Schwarzloseet al. 2008).Responses also vary with 110 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR eccentricity:LO, FFA, and the VWFA respondmore stronglyto centrallypresented stimuli, and the PPA respondsmore stronglyto peripherallypresentedstimuli (Lerr et al., 2001;Hassonet al. 2002;Hassonet al. 2003;SayresandGrill-Spector2008). UsingfMRI-A, my colleaguesandl haveshownthatpFus/OTS,but not LO, exhibitr somedegreeof insensitivityto an object'ssizeandposition(Grill-Spectoret al.1999t. The fMRI-A methodallowsoneto characterizethe sensitivityof neuralrepresentation: to stimulustransformations at a subvoxelresolution.fMRI-A is basedon findingsfronr single-unitelectrophysiologythat showthat when objectsrepeat,thereis a stimulusspecificdecreasein the responseof IT cells to the repeatedimage but not to other object images(Miller et al. l99l; Sawamuraet al. 2006). Similarly, fMRI signals in higher visual regions show a stimulus-specificreduction(fMRI-A) in response to repetitionof identicalobject images(Grill-Spectoret al. 1999;Grill-Spectorand Malach 2001; Grill-Spectoret al. 2006a).We showedthat fMRI-A can be usedto test the sensitivityof neural responsesto object transformationby adaptingcortex with a repeatedpresentationof an identicalstimulusand then examiningadaptation effectswhenthe stimulusis changedalongan objecttransformation(e.g.,changingits position).If the responseremainsadapted,it indicatesthat neuronsare insensitiveto the change;however,if responses returnto the initial level (recoverfrom adaptation). it indicatessensitivityto the change(Grill-SpectorandMalach2001). Using fMRI-A we found that repeatedpresentationof the sameface or object at the sameposition and size producesreducedfMRI activationor fMRI-A. This is thoughtto reflect stimulus-specificneuraladaptation.Presentingthe sameface or object in different positionsin the visual field or at different sizes also produces fMRI-A in pFus/OTSand FEA, indicating insensitivityto object size and position (Grill-Spectoret aI. 1999;seealso Vuilleumieret al. 2002).This resultis consistent with electrophysiology findingsthat showedthat IT neuronsthat respondsimilarly to stimuli at differentpositionsin the visual field also show adaptationwhen the same objectis shownin differentpositions(Lueschowet al. 1994).Incontrast,LO recovers from fMRI-A to imagesof the samefaceor objectwhenpresentedat differentsizesor positions.This indicatesthat LO is sensitiveto objectpositionand size. Recently,severalgroupsexaminedthe sensitivityof the distributedresponseacross the visual streamto object categoryand object position (Sayresand Grill-Spector 2008; Schwarzloseet al. 2008) and also object identity and object position (Eger et al. 2008).Thesestudiesusedmulti-voxel patternanalysesand classifiermethods developedin machinelearningto examinewhatinformationis presentin thedistributed responses acrossvoxelsin a corticalregion.Thedistributedresponse cancarrydifferent informationfrom themeanresponse of a regionof interestwhenthereis variationacross voxels'responses. In order to examinesensitivityto position information,severalstudiesexamined whetherdistributedresponsepatternsto sameobject category(or object exemplar)is the same(or different) when the samestimulusis presentedin a different position in the visual field. In multi-voxelpatternanalysesresearchers typically split the data into two independentsetsand examinethe cross-correlation betweenthe distributed responses to the same(or different)stimulusin the same(or different)positionacross the two datasets.This gives a measureof the sensitivityof distributedresponsesto object informationand position. If responsesare position-invariant,there will be a \ - l WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? Etttttl 9o Left 4o Left Fovea 4'Right 4o Up 111 4o Down rllllll fflltlll 2-1 012 Amplitude versusmeanresponse % SignalChange Qure 5.4. LO distributedresponsepatternsto differentobject categoriesand stimuluspo;i tlns. Data are shown on the lateralaspectof the right hemispherecorticalsurfacefor a -Dreentativesubject.Eachpanelshowsthe distributedLO fMRl amplitudesaftersubtracting r-.-nreachvoxel its meanresponse.Redand yellow,Responses that are higherthan the voxel's -ean response.Blue and cyan, Responses that are lower than the voxel'smean response. r---:. Inflatedrightcorticalhemisphere, with red squareindicatingthe zoomedregion.Note :a: patternchangessignificantlyacrosscolumns(positions) and to a lesserextentacrossrows .a:eqories). Adaptedfrom Sayresand Crill-Spector 2008. (Seecolor plate6.4.) heh correlation between the distributed responses to the same object category (or <remplar) at different positions. If responsesare sensitive to position, there will be a Lur correlation between responsesto the same object category (or exemplar) at different poritions. Figure 6.4 illustrates distributed LO responsesto three categories: houses, limbs, uld faces at six visual locations (fovea; 4o up, right, down, or left from the fovea; 9 leti of fovea). Activation patterns for the same object category presented at diffcrent positions vary considerably (compare the response patterns in the same row LToss the different columns in Fig. 6.4). There is also variation (but to a lesser extent) \Toss different object categories when presented at the same retinal position (same .arlumn, different rows, in Fig. 6.4). Surprisingly, position effects in LO were larger dren category effects - that is, showing objects from the same category, but at a diff<rent position, reduced the correlation between activation patterns (Fig. 6.5, flrst vs. drrrd bars) more signiflcantly than changing the object category in the same position "Fig. 6.5, first vs. second bar). Importantly, position and category effects were inJcpendent, as there were no significant interactions between position and category 112 KALANIT WHAT GRILL-SPECTOR A related recen ysis more broadlt hierarchical organ zlose and colleae (r c .9 0.3- (faces, bodY Part' (e.g., PFus/OTS EBA and LOr' T (u o v 0.1 : Cl ((l:; o: = o: {}'1 ' : : 4.2' r 8$:tU,"ffio 8$[fr "ffio DifferentPosition SamePosition Figure LOdistributed 6.5. Meancross-correlations between responses across twoindependent halvesof the datafor the sameor different positionin the category at the sameor different visualfield.Positioneffects: patterns weresubstantially LO response to the samecategory (firstand moresimilarif theywerepresented positions at the samepositionversus different Themeancorrelation washigherfor same-category thirdbars,P < 10-7).Category effects: response patterns in the same thanfor different-category response patterns whenpresented (first retinotopic position twobars,P < 10-o).Error subjects. Adapted barsindicate SEMacross fromSayres andCrill-Spector 2008. (all F values< 1.02, all P values> 0.31).Thus,changingbothobjectcategoryandpo(Fig. 6.5, fourth sition producedmaximaldecorrelationbetweendistributedresponses bar). We also examinedwhetherposition sensitivityin LO is manifestedas an orderly topographicmap (similar to retinotopicorganizationin lower visual areas),by measuring retinotopic mapsin LO using standardtraveling wave paradigms(Wandell 1999; Sayresand Grill-Spector2008).We found a continuousmappingof the visualfield in LO both in terms of eccentricityand polar angle.This topographicmap containedan over-representation of the contralateraland lower visual field (more voxels preferred thesevisual field positionsthan the ipsilateraland uppervisual fields).Although we did not consistentlyfind a single visual field map (a singlehemifieldor quarterfield representation) in LO, this analysissuggeststhat thereis preservedretinotopicinformation in LO that may underliethe position effectsobservedin analysesof distributed LO responses. Overall, our data show that different object categoriesproduce relatively small changesto both the meanand distributedresponseacrossLO (categoricaleffectsare larger in the fusiform and parahippocampalgyri). In comparison,a modest4o change in an object'spositionproducessignalchangesin LO thatareaslargeor largerthanthe categorymodulation.This 4" displacement is well within the rangefor which humans can categonzeand detect objects (Thorpe et al. 2001). This indicates a difference betweenthe positionsensitivityof recognitionbehaviorandthat of neuralpopulations in LO. However,it is possiblethat performancein recognitiontasksthat requirefinegrain discriminationbetweenexemplarsis morepositionsensitive,and limited by the degreeof positionsensitivityin LO. s$dies suggest: which rePresent hierarchY. 6-5 It is imPofiant tt' tions of object' ' ' dependson thc' ral rePresentatl fullY invariant rt indePendentof ' the context trf to be a gradc'd1 degree of st'nt stimulus 'st'/t't1994; Roll: en growing litcrat tributed neural (Dill and Edclr maintaining { maintain infot man and Intra object-basedt and Grill-SPr structural L'nc robust u'ar ie model. oblec ttt PoPulation for fast deci' to deternrinc 2007)' Finall ization and n w h e r ei t i : t ' 6't Another :t-t change ucr WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? 113 A relatedrecentstudyexaminedpositionsensitivityusingmulti-voxelpatternanalvsis more broadly acrossthe ventral streamand providedadditionalevidencefor a hierarchicalorganizationacrossthe ventralstream(Schwarzlose et al. 2008).Schwarzloseand colleaguesfound that distributedresponsesto'ri particularobject category tfaces,body parts,or scenes)was similar acrosspositionsin ventrotemporalregions (e.9.,pFus/OTSand FBA) but changedacrosspositionsin occipital regions (e.9., EBA and LO). Thus, evidencefrom both fMRI-A and multi-voxel patternanalysis rtudiessuggestsa hierarchyof representations in the humanventral streamthrough r,rhichrepresentations thevisual becomelesssensitiveto objectpositionasoneascends hierarchy. 6.5.L lmplications for Theories of Object Recognition It is importantto relateimagingresultsto the conceptof position-invariant representationsof objectsandobjectcategories.What exactlyis implied by the term "invariance" dependson the scientificcontext.In someinstances,this term is takento reflecta neural representation that is abstractedso as to be independentof viewing conditions.A iullv invariantrepresentation, in this meaningof the term,is expectedto be completely rndependent of informationaboutretinal position (Biedermanand Cooper 1991).In the contextof studiesof visual cortex, however,the term is more often considered ttr [rs a gradedphenomenon,in which neuralpopulationsare expectedto retain some .lesreeof sensitivityto visual transformations(like position changes)but in which .timulus selectivityis preservedacrossthesetransformations(Kobatakeand Tanaka 199-l;Rolls and Milward 2000; DiCarlo and Cox 2007).In supportof this view, a rrowing literaturesuggeststhat maintaininglocal position informationwithin a distnbutedneuralrepresentation may actuallyaid invariantrecognitionin severalways ,Dill andEdelman2001;DiCarlo andCox 2007;SayresandGrill-Spector2008).First, maintainingseparableinformationaboutpositionand categorymay also allow on to naintain information about the structuralrelationshipsbetweenobject parts (Edelnan and Intrator2000).Indeedsomeexperimentssuggestthat LO may containboth .*rject-based(McKyton and Zohary 2007) and retinal-basedreferenceframes(Sayres end Grill-Spector2008). The object-basedreferenceframe may provide a basisfor .tructur&lencoding.Second,separablepositionandobjectinformationmay providea nrbustway for generatingpositioninvarianceby using a populationcode.Under this model,objectsarerepresented asmanifoldsin a high-dimensionalspacespannedby a gx-rpulation of neurons.The separabilityof position and object information may allow ior fast decisionsbasedon linear computations(e.g., linear discriminantfunctions) to determinethe object identity (or category)acrosspositionssee(DiCarlo and Cox lm7). Finally,separableobjectand positioninformationmay allow concurrentlocalrzationandrecognitionof objects(i.e.,recognizingwhat the objectis anddetermining u hereit is). 6.6 Evidencefor ViewpointSensitivityAcrossthe LOC Another source of change in object appearancethat merits separateconsideration is change across rotation in depth. In contrast to position or size changes, in which rt4 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR invariancemay be achievedby a linear transformation,the shapeof objectschange: with depthrotation.This is becausethe visual systemreceives2-D rettnalprojections of 3-D objects.Sometheoriessuggestthat view-invariantrecognitionacrossobject rotationsis accomplishedby largelyvfek-invariantrepresentations of objects(generalizedcylinders', Marr 1980;recognitionbycomponents', orRBC', Biederman19871: that is, the underlyingneuralrepresentations respondsimilarly to an objectacrossits views.Othertheories,however,suggestthatobjectrepresentations areview-dependent. thatis, theyconsistof several2-D viewsof an object(Ullman 1989;PoggioandEdelman 1990;Bulthoff and Edelman,1992;Edelmanand Bulthoff 1992;Bulthoff et al. 1995;Tarr and Bulthoff 1995).Invariantobjectrecognitionis accomplished by interpolationacrosstheseviews(Ullman 1989;PoggioandEdelman1990;Logothetis et al. 1995)or by a distributedneuralcodeacrossview-tunedneurons(Penettet al. r998). Single-unitelectrophysiology studiesin primatesindicatethat the majority of neurons in monkey inferotemporalcortex are view-dependent(Desimoneet al. 1984: Logothetiset al. 1995;Perrett1996:Wanget al. 1996;Vogelsand Biederrnan2002) with a small minority (5-l0%o)of neuronsshowingview-invariantresponses across (Logothetis objectrotations et al. 1995;BoothandRolls 1998). In humans,resultsvary considerably.Short-laggedfMRI-A experiments,in which the test stimulusis presentedimmediatelyafter the adaptingstimulus(Grill-Spector (Fang et al. 2006a),suggestthatobjectrepresentations in the LOC areview-dependent et al. 2007;Gauthieret al. 2002;Grill-Spectoret al.1999;but seeValyearet al. 2006). However,long-laggedfMRI-A experiments, in which manyinterveningstimuli occur betweenthe test and adaptingstimulus (Grill-Spectoret al. 2006a),have provided some evidencefor view-invariantrepresentations in ventral LOC, especiallyin the left hemisphere(Jameset al. 2002; Vuilleumier et al. 2002) and the PPA (Epstein et al. 2008).Also, a recentstudyshowedthat the distributedLOC responses to objects remainedstableacross60orotations(Egeret al. 2008).Presently,thereis no consensus acrossexperimentalfindingsin the degreeto which ventralstreamrepresentations are view-dependentor view-invariant.Thesevariableresultsmay reflect differencesin the neuralrepresentations dependingon object categoryand cortical region, and/or methodologicaldifferencesacrossstudies(e.g.,level of object rotation and fMRI-A paradigmused). We addressedthesedifferential findings in a recent study in which we used a parametricapproachto investigatesensitivityto object rotation and a computational modelto link putativeneuraltuning andresultantfI\4RI signals(Andresenet al. 2008, 2009).The parametricapproachallowsa richercharacterization of rotationsensitivity because,rather than characteizing representations as invariantor not invariant,it measures the degreeof sensitivityto rotations.Using fMRI-A we measuredviewpoint sensitivityas a function of the rotationlevel for two objectcategories- animalsand vehicles.Overall,we found sensitivityto object rotationin the LOC, but therewere differencesacrosscategoriesandregions.First, therewashighersensitivityto vehicle rotationthan to animalrotation.Rotationsof 60' produceda completerecoveryfrom adaptationfor vehicles,but rotationsof 120' werenecessary to producerecoveryfrom (Fig. adaptationfor animals 6.6). Second,we found evidencefor overrepresentation of the front view of animalsin the right pFus/OTS:its responsesto animalswere . 16rr ' , . I ; -r --i[ r-:' -i: l -j. -J wHAT HAs fMRr TAUGHT us ABour oBJECT REcocNruon? 8c' 115 (a) Vehicles }n! R-l ET. o P 1.3 au .tr o It. NI cid n- E tr .9 Right pFUS/OTS $,tr"*{ 0.8 o s o.g Adapt frontview Adapt backview 00 60"120"180' ;{;il;il;. Rotation from adapting view It. ri (b) Animals LO [t. t. o cr, 1.3 tr r! s o h f F E 0.8 .9 o s 0.3 k.4 Adapt frontview Adapt baeJ< view |-|-r-.._..-| 00 60" 120"180" 0' 60" 120' 190" Rotation from adapting view Figure6.6. LO responses Eachlinerepresents of rotationsensitivity. duringfMRl-Aexperiments response afteradaptingwith a front (dashedblack)or backview (solidgray)of an object.The ronadaptedresponseis indicatedby the diamonds(blackfor front view, and grayfor back r rerv).The open circlesindicatesignificantadaptation,lower than nonadapted, P < 0.05, oairedt-testacrosssubjects.(a)Vehicledata.(b) Animal data.Responses are plottedrelative :o a blank fixationbaseline.Errorbars indicateSEMacrosseight subjects.Adaptedfrom (2009). {ndreson,Vinberg,and Grill-Spector higher for the front view than the back view (compare black and gray circles in Fig. 6.6(b) righ|. In addition fMRI-A effects across rotation varied according to the adapting view (Fig. 6(b) righ|. When adapting with the back view of animals, we tound recovery from adaptation for rotations of 120o or larger, but when adapting with the front view of animals, there was no significant recovery from adaptation across rotations. One interpretation is that there is less sensitivity to rotation when adapting s'ith front views than back views of animals. However, the behavioral performance of rubjects in a discrimination task across object rotations showed that they are equally \ensitive to rotations (performance decreaseswith rotation level) whether rotations are relative to the front or back of an animal (Andresen et al. 2008), which suggeststhat this interpretation is unlikely. Alternatively, the apparent adaptation across a 180" rotation relative to a front animal view, may just reflect lower responses to a back animal view. To better characterize the underlying representations and examine which representations may lead to our observed results, we simulated putative neural responsesand predicted the resultant fMRI responsesin a voxel. In the model, each voxel contains a mixture of neural populations, each of which is tuned to a different object view 116 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR (a) View dependent- equal distribution Front o c g, Vlew Populatlon vlewtuning wldth (o) Front Vlew ?"a: * AdTtlmlt'b - Adftbd(lr|I T CD g tr OffectVlew a0 60 t20 180 Rotation from adapting view h voxel (b) Viewdependent- unequaldistribution Front g !g t tD c tr o c t I I f f i T I * I I I * -105'.05' 15' 75' 135' 195' OUgr;tWew II l l l l x LIIIII $- dlsttbufron in voxel Populatlon view tuning width (o) * ^frrffitu "9 o o J o @ 1.4 a0 6012018{t Rotation from adapting view Figure6.7. SimulationspredictingfMRl responses of putativevoxelscontaininga mixture of view-dependentneural populations.Left,Schematicillustrationof the view tuning ard distributionof neuralpopulationstunedto differentviews in a voxel.Forillustrationpurposes we show a putativevoxel with 4 neural populations.Right,Resultof model simulations illustratingthe predictedfMRl-A data. In all panels,the model includes6 Gaussians tuned to specificviews aroundthe viewing circle, separated60' apart.Acrosscolumns,the vierr tuning width varies.Acrossrows, the distributionof neuralpopulationspreferringspecific views varies.Diamond, Responses without adaptation(blackfor back view, and grayfor front view). Lines,Response afteradaptationwith a frontview (dashedgrayline)or backview (solid black line).(a) Mixtureof view-dependent neuralpopulationsthat are equallydistributedin a voxel. Narrowertuning (left)showsrecoveryfrom fMRl-A for smallerrotationsthan wider view tuning (right).This model predictsthe samepatternof recoveryfrom adaptationwhen neuralpopulationsin a adaptingwith the front or backview. (b) Mixtureof view-dependent voxelwith a higherproportionof neuronsthat preferthe front view The numberon the right indicatesthe ratio betweenthe percentagesneuronstuned to the front versusthe back view. Top row, ratio - 1.2. Bottomrow, ratio : 1.4. Becausethereare more neuronstunedto the frontview in thismodel,it predictshigherBOLDresponses to frontalviewswithoutadaptation (grayvs. blackdiamonds)and a flatterprofileof fMRl-Aacrossrotationswhen adaptingwith the front view.Adaptedfrom Andreson,Vinberg,and Crill-Spector(in press). (Fig. 6.7 and Andresen et al. 2008, in press). fMRI responses were modeled to be proportional to the sum of responses across all neural populations in a voxel. We simulated the fMRI responses in fMRI-A experiments for a set of putative voxels that varied in the view-tuning width of neural populations, the preferred view of different neural populations, the number of different neural populations, and the distribution of populations tuned to different views within a voxel. Results of the simulations indicate that two main parameters affected the pattern of fMRI data: (l) the view-tuning width of the neural population, and (2) the proportion of neurons in a voxel that prefer a specific object view. WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? Lt7 Figure6.7(a) showsthe responsecharacteristicsof a model of a putativevoxel that containsa mixture of view-dependentneural populationsthat are tuned to different object views, in which the distributionof neuronstunEdto different views is uniform. ln this model,niurowertuning (left) showsrecoveryf";fi fMRI-A for smallerrotations than wider view tuning (right). Responsesto front and back views are identical when thereis no adaptation(Fig. 6.7(a),diamonds),andthe patternof adaptationasa function of rotation is similar when adaptingwith the front or back views (Fig. 6.7(a)). Such e model provides an accountof responsesto vehicles acrossobject-selectivecortex r.s measuredwith fMRI) and for animalsin LO. Thus, this model suggeststhat the differencebetweenthe representationof animals and vehiclesin LO is likely due to e smaller population view tuning for vehicles than for animals (a tuning width of o < 40o producescompleterecovery from adaptationfor rotations larger than 60o, rs observedfor vehicles).Figure 6.7(b) showsthe effects of neural distributionson fllRl signals.Simulationspredictthat when thereare more neuronsin a voxel that are uned to the front view, therewill be higher BOLD responsesto frontal views without daptation (gray vs. black diamonds)and a flatter profile of fMRI-A acrossrotations rben adaptingwith the front view, as observedin pFus/OTSfor animals. 6.6.1Implications for Theories of Object Recognition Orerall, our resultsprovide empirical evidencefor view-dependentobject representson acrosshumanobject-selectivecortexthat is evidentwith standardfMRI aswell as fIIRI-A measurements. Thesedataprovideimportantempiricalconstraintsfor theories d object recognition and highlight the importanceof parametricmanipulationsfor qturing neuralselectivityto any type of stimulustransformation. Given the evidencefor neuralsensitivityto objectview, how is view-invariantobject reognition accomplished?One appealingmodel for view-invariantobjectrecognition rr that objectsare representedby a populationcode in which single neuronsmay be r{cctive to a particularview, but the distributedrepresentationacrossthe entireneural gopulationis robustto changesin objectview (Perrettet al. 1998). Doesthe view-specificapproachnecessitatea downstreamview-invariantneuron? ftc possibilityis thatperceptualdecisionsmay be performedby neuronsoutsidevisual cutter and theseneuronsare indeedview-invariant.Examplesof such view-invariant -urons havebeenfound in the hippocampus,perirhinal cortex,and prefrontalcortex tfi,aedmanet al. 200I,2003; Quirogaet al. 2005; Quirogaet al. 2008). Alternatively, ogalationsbasedon the population code (or a distributed code) acrossview-tuned :urons may be sufficientfor view-invariantdecisionsbasedon view-sensitiveneural Ipresentations. 6.7 DebatesAbout the Nature of Functional Organization in the Human Ventral Stream So far we haveconsideredgeneralcomputationalprinciples that are requiredby any Sycct recognition system. Nevertheless,it is possible that some object classesor &srains requirespecializedcomputations.The restof this chapterexaminesfunctional 118 KALANIT GRILL-SPECTOR specializationin the ventral stream that may be linked to these putative "domainspecific"computations. As illustratedin Figure6.1, severalregionsin the ventralstreamexhibit higherresponsesto particularobjectcategories,.such asplaces,faces,andbody parts,compared to otherobjectcategories. The discoveryof category-selective regionsinitiateda fierce debateaboutthe principles of functional organizationin the ventral stream.Are there regionsin the cortex that are specializedfor any object category?Is there something specialaboutcomputationsrelevantto specificcategoriesthat generatesspecialized cortical regions for thesecomputations?In other words, perhapssome generalprocessingis appliedto all objects,but somecomputationsmay be specificto certain domainsandmay requireadditionalbrainresources. We may alsoaskabouthow these category-selective regionscome about:Are they innate,or do they requireexperience to develop? Four prominentviews haveemergedto explainingthe patternof functional selectivity in the ventral stream.The main debatecenterson the questionof whetherregions that elicit maximal responsefor a categoryarea module for the representationof that category,or whetherthey are part of a more generalobject recognitionsystem. 6.7.1 Limited Category-Specific Modules and a General Area for All Other Objects Kanwisherandcoworkers(Kanwisher2000;Op de Beecket al. 2008) suggested that ventraltemporalcortexcontainsa limited numberof modulesspecializedfor the recognition of specialobjectcategoriessuchas faces(in the FFA), places(in the PPA),and bodyparts(in the EBA andFBA). The remainingobject-selective cortex(LOC), which showslittle selectivityfor particularobject categories,is a general-purpose mechanism for perceivingany kind of visually presentedobject or shape.The underlying hypothesisis that there are few domain-specificmodulesthat perform computations that are specific to theseclassesof stimuli beyond what would be required from a generalobjectrecognitionsystem.For example,faces,like other objects,needto be (a domain-generalprocess).However, recognizedacrossvariationsin their appearance giventhe importanceof faceprocessingfor socialinteractions,thereareaspectsof face processingthat areunique.Specializedfaceprocessingmay includeidentifyingfaces at the individual level (e.9.,John vs. Harry), extractinggenderinformation,evaluating gazeand expression,and so on. Theseunique, face-relatedcomputationsmay be implementedin face-selective regions. 6.7.2 ProcessMaps Tarr and Gauthier(2000) proposedthat object representations are clusteredaccording to the type of processingthat is required,ratherthanaccordingto their visual attributes. It is possiblethat differentlevelsof processingmay requirededicatedcomputations that are performedin localized cortical regions.For example,faces are usually recognized at the individual level (e.9., "That is Bob Jacobs"),but many objectsare typically recognizedat the categorylevel (e.9.,"That is a horse").Followingthis reasoning,andevidencethat objectsof expertiseactivatethe FFA morethan otherobjects (Gauthi sugges ordinat et al. l( Haxbl cipitott attribut across Haxbl were n distritr that it region exclud reprev On' binato Given (Biedt a largr consi as mu Mala nizat COTTC and t ities hous fove to fc et al bias ana recc tion alsc fielr wit I fun wHATHAsfMRrTAUGHTus ABour oBJEcr REcocNlrrox? r.e "domainrit higher ret-s.compared iated a fierce m. Are there e something ; specialized general proic to ceftain ut how these e experience rnal selectiv:ther regions ation of that stem. nea ggested that )r the recogre PPA), and .OC), which ose mechaunderlying rmputations ired from a need to be ). However, ects of face fying faces rn. evaluatrns may be according attributes. nputations ;ually recrbjects are _ethis rearer objects 119 (Gauthieret al. 1999;Gauthieret al. 2000), Gauthier,Tarr, and their colleagueshave suggestedthat the FFA is not a region for facerecognition,but rathera region for subordinateidentificationof any object categorythat is automatedby expertise(Gauthier et al. 1999:Gauthieret al. 2000: Tarr ahtl Gauthier2000). 6.7.3 Distributed Object-Form Topography Haxby and colleagues(2001)positedan "object-formtopography,"in which the occipitotemporal cortex contains a topographicallyorganizedrepresentationof shape attributes.The representationof an object is reflectedby a distinct patternof response acrossall ventralcortex,and this distributedactivationproducesthe visual perception. Haxby and colleaguesshowedthat the activationpatternsfor eight object categories were replicableand that the responseto a given categorycould be determinedby the distributedpatternof activationacrossall ventrotemporalcortex.Further,they showed that it is possibleto predict what object categorythe subjectsviewed, even when regions that show maximal activation to a particular category (e.9., the FFA) were thatthe ventrotemporal cortex excluded(Haxbyet al. 2001).Thus,this modelsuggests representsobject categoryinformation in an overlappingand distributedfashion. One of the reasonsthat this view is appealingis that a distributedcodeis a combinatorial code that allows representationof a large number of object categories. Given Biederman'srough estimatethat humanscan recognizeabout30,000categories (Biederman1987),this providesa neuralsubstratethathasa capacityto representsuch a largenumberof categories.Second,this model positeda provocativeview that when consideringinformation in the ventral stream,one needsto considerthe weak signals as much asthe strongsignals,becauseboth conveyuseful information. 6.7.4 Topographic Representation Malach and colleagues(2002) suggestedthat eccentricity biasesunderlie the organization of ventral and dorsal streamobject-selectiveregions becausethey found a correlationbetweencategorypreference(higherresponseto one categoryover others) andeccentricitybias (higherresponseto a specificeccentricitythan to othereccentricities (Levy et al. 200I; Hassonet al. 2002; Hassonet al. 2003). Regionsthat prefer housesto other objects also respondmore strongly to peripheralstimulation than to foveal stimulation.In contrast,regionsthatpreferfacesor lettersrespondmore strongly to foveal stimulation than to peripheralstimulation.Malach and colleagues(Malach et al. 2002)proposedthat the correlationbetweencategoryselectivityand eccentricity needs.Thus,objectswhoserecognitiondependson biasis drivenby spatial-resolution and objectswhose analysisof fine detailsare associatedwith foveal representations, peripheral representawith recognitionrequireslarge-scaleintegrationare associated tions.To date,however,thereis no clearevidencethateccentricitybiasesin the FFA are also coupledwith better representationof high spatialfrequencyor smallerreceptive fields or, conversely,that the PPA preferslow spatialfrequenciesor containsneurons with largerreceptivefields. Presently,there is no consensusin the field about which accountbest explainsthe functional organizationof the ventralstream.Much of the debatecenterson the degree r20 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR to which object processingis constrainedto discretemodulesor involves distributed computationsacrosslarge stretchesof the ventral stream(Op de Beeck et al. 2008). The debateis about the spatial scale on which computationsfor object recognition occur as well as the fundamenthlprinciples that underlie specializationin the ventral stream. On the one hand,domain-specifictheoriesneedto addressfindingsof multiple foci that show selectivity.For example,multiple foci in the ventral streamrespondmore strongly to faces than to objects; thus, a strong modular accountof a single "face module" for face recognition is unlikely. Also, the spatial extent of these putative modulesis undetermined,and it is unclear whether eachof thesecategory-selective regionscorrespondsto a visual area.Further,high-resolutionfMRI (1-2 mm on a side) showsthat the spatialextentof category-selective regionsis smallerthanthat estimated with standardfMRI (34 mm on a side) and that theseregions appearmore patchy (Schwarzlose et aL.2005;Grill-Spectoret al. 2006b). On the otherhand,a potentialproblemwith very distributedandoverlappingaccount of object representationin the ventral stream is that, in order to resolve category information, the brain may need to read out information present acrossthe entire ventral stream(which is inefficient). Further,the fact that there is information in the distributedresponsedoesnot meanthat the brain usesthe information in the sameway that an independentclassifier does.It is possiblethat activationin localized regions is more informative for perceptualdecisionsthan the information availableacrossthe entire ventral stream(Grill-Spector et al. 2004; Williams et al. 2007). For example, FFA responsespredictwhen subjectsrecognizefacesandbirds but do not predictwhen subjectsrecognizehouses,guitars,or flowers (Grill-Spectoret aI.2004). 6.8 Differential Developmentof CategorySelectivity from Childhood to Adulthood One researchdirection that can shedlight on thesedebatesis an examinationof the developmentof ventral streamfunctional organization.What is the role of experience in shapingcategoryselectivityin the ventral stream? 6.8.L fMRI Measurements of the Development of the Ventral Stream To addressthesequestions,our lab (Golarai et al. 2007) identified face-, place-, and object-selective regionswithin individualchildren(7-11yearsold), adolescents (12-14 years old), and adult subjects(18-35 years old) while subjectsfixated and reported infrequent eventswhen two consecutiveimageswere identical (one-backtask). We found a prolonged developmentof the right FFA (rFFA) and left PPA (IPPA) that manifestedas an expansionof the spatialextent of theseregionsacrossdevelopment from age7 to adulthood(Fig. 6.8). The rFFA and IPPA were significantly larger in adults than in children, with an intermediatesize of these regions in adolescents. Notably, the rFFA of children was about a third of the adult size, but it was still evidentin 85Voof children. Thesedevelopmentalchangescould not be explainedby smaller anatomicalcortical volumes of the fusiform gyrus or the parahippocampal L2l SHAT H^4sfunr TAUGHTus ABour oBJEcr REcocNlrrox? f'olvesdistributed :eck et al. 200gt. bject recognition ion in the ventral ; of multiplefoci m respondmore ,f a single ..face ,f theseputative fiegory-selective ? mm on a side) n rhatestimated ar more patchy 2,500 ;. r.600 3 ' -8oo I ! I :4m ffi Ll Match€d PPA size Left Right € ,,* Right ni'iJ-'d'"#t$ Ll 2,500 2,000 1,500 1,000 500 0 Matched . € r,soo E r,ooo € soo 0 LOC size Right Left ffi z-t t vear-old I f o,ooo1 to1-J,9t"*-o'o . , 6.OOO g;.qffi 4,500 3,000 appingaccount solve category ross the entire )rmationin the t the sameway 'alized regions tble acrossthe For example, t predictwhen ty narionof the rf experience itream place-,and e n B( 1 2 _ 1 4 nd reported c task). We IIPPA)that :r'elopment y' larger in lolescents. t was sdll llained by pocampal 1,500 0 andadults. adolescents, children, STS,andLOCacross Figure6.8. Volumeof therFFA,IPPA, 10adoles20 children, whichinclude Fiiiedbarsindicate all subjects, volumeacross average that of subjects for thesubset volumes cents,and15 adults.Openbarsindicatethe average g adolescents, and13 andinilude10 children, nerematched confounds for BOlD-related than different indicatesignificantly Asterisks adults.ErrorbarsindicateSEMacrosssubjects. from on the y-axis.Adapted scales panelshavedifferent adult,P < 0.05.Notethatdifferent Colarai et al.2007. gyrus (Golarai et al. 2007),which were similar acrosschildren and adults,or higher BOlD-related confoundsin children (i.e., larger fMRl-related noise or larger subject motion;seeGolaraiet al. Z}}7;Grill-Spectoret al. 2008)becauseresultsremainedthe sarnefor a subsetof subjectsthat were matchedfor fMRl-related confoundsacross ages.Thesedevelopmentalchangeswere specificto the rFFA and IPPA, becausewe found no differencesacrossagesin the size of the LOC or the size of the pSTS faceselectiveregion. Finally, within the functionally defined FFA, PPA, and LOC, there were no differencesacrossagesin the level of responseamplitudesto faces,objects, andplaces. We alsomeasuredrecognitionmemoryoutsidethe scannerandfound that that faceand place-recognitionmemory increasedfrom childhood to adulthood(Golarai et al. 2007).Further,face-recognitionmemory was significantly correlatedwith rFFA size (but not the sizeof otherregions)in childrenandadolescents(butnot adults),andplacerecognitionmemorywassignificantlycorrelatedwith IPPAsize(but not the sizeof other regions)in eachof the agegroups(Fig. 6.9). Thesedatasuggestthat improvementsin face-and place-recognitionmemory during childhood and adolescenceare correlated *'ith increasesin the sizeof the rFFA and IPPA,respectively. Another recent study (Scherf et al. 2007) examinedthe developmentof the venrral stream in children (5-8 years old), adolescents(ll-14 years old), and adults usingmovieclips that containedfaces,objects,buildings,andnavigation.Using group analysismethods,they reportedthe absenceof face-selectiveactivations(vs. objects, buildings,and navigation)in 5- to 8-year-oldchildren in both the fusiform gyrus and rhe pSTS.The lack of FFA in young children in the group analysismay be due to the smallerand more variableFFA location in children, which would affect its detection 122 KALANIT (a) Face recognition memory vs. rFFA size z" 1.0 o E o c .9 ?_ It GRILL.SPECTOR (b) Place recognition memory vs. IPPA size (c) Object recognition memory vs. LOC size 0.8 0.7 1.0 0.7 0.6 0.8 0.7 .c inrplcrtlcl: .lJ\C\ 1g$ tr 0.4 0.6 j\[Cn\l\ C r'\f 0.3 0.4 0.2 o.2 0.1 0.0 0.6 0.4 c o) o 0.2 o o 0.0 E. 4.2 0 1000 3000 5000 RightFFA Size [mmst O t-tl years old n=20 :hc lir:t h t,' 'rr Jtthn:rrll; 1.2 0.8 :cgton:. Tht' r tcs ing t'l l-r Thc rcl"' \n\r\\ n. lrrrp 0 I Left PPA Size [mmst lz-t6 years old n=10 2000 6000 10000 RightLOC size [mmgt # 18-35years old n=15 Figure6.9. Recognition memoryversussizeof rFFA,iPPA,and LOC.(a)Recognitionmemory accuracyfor differentcategories acrossagegroups.Recognitionmemoryfor faceswassignific a n t l y b e t t e r i n a d u l t s t h a n c h i l d r e n<(0*.P0 0 0 1 ) o r a d o l e s c e n t s ( *<*0P. 0 3 ) . A d o l e s c e n t s ' (**P < 0.03).Recognition memoryfor faceswasbetterthanchildren's memoryfor placeswas betterin adultsthanin children(tP . 0.0001).Adolescents' memoryfor placeswasbetterthan children's(+P < 0.01).Recognition accuracyfor objectswas not differentacrossagegroups. Errorbars indicateSEM.(b) Recognitionmemoryfor facesversusFFAsize.Correlations are (r > O.49,P < 0.03),but not adults.(c) Recogwithin childrenand adolescents significant nition memoryof placesversusPPAsize.Correlations within eachagegroup are significant (r > 0.59, P < 0.03).(d) Recognition memoryfor objectsversusLOC size.No correlations (P > O.4).Adaptedfrom Colaraiet al. 2OO7. weresignificant in a group analysis. Indeed when Scherf and colleagues performed individual subject analysis, they found face-selective activations in 807o of their child subjects, but the extent of activations was smaller and more variable in location compared to those in adults. Like Golarai and colleagues, they found no difference in the spatial extent or level of response amplitudes to objects in the LOC. Unlike Golorai and colleagues, they reported no developmental changes in the PPA. The variant results may be due to differences in stimuli (pictures vs. movies), task (one-back task vs. passive viewing), and analysis methods (single subject vs. group analysis) across the two studies. For example, Golarai and colleagues instructed subjects to fixate and perforrn a one-back task. In this task, children had the same accuracy as adults but were overall slower in their responses,with no differences across categories.In the Scherf study, subjects watched movies, and perfonnance was not measuredduring the scan.It is possible that the children had differential different eye movements or levels of attention than the adults, and this affected their findings. 6.8.2 Implications of Differential Development of Visual Cortex Overall,fMRI findingssuggestdifferentialdevelopmentaltrajectoriesacrossthehuman ventral visual stream.Surprisingly,thesedata suggestthat more than a decadeis necessaryfor the developmentof an adult-like rFFA and IPPA. This suggeststhat experienceovera prolongedtime may be necessaryfor thenormaldevelopmentof these lf trlt;'tlll lllCJ r''th urc' llkc : C p f L ' \ CI l t J l I \ ' 3 \ [ ' r . - f l r " ' 0 s Cl r :'lJtUrC .!l il lla:tlr-ttr [:Ii.\ rat rc-[r'n' ]rx x tr. [:t,ufi i r f f c ' r J I ' lr \ ' ' ! l : wHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT us ABour ognition OC size '0@0 stze old I memory a ss ignifi llescents' laceswas etterthan e groups. r t i o nsare l, Recog8e group 're lat ion s subject but the those in llent or eagues. : due to eu'ing). es. For te-back slower ubjects rle that ran the luman ade is ts that f these oBJEcr REcocNrrrox? L23 regions.This resultis surprising,especiallybecausethereis evidencefor preferential viewing of face-like in newbornbabiesand evidencefor face-selectiveERPs within the first 6 to 12 months(for a review,seeJohnson2001).One possibility suggested by Johnsonand colleaguesis that face processinghas an innatecomponentthat may be implementedin subcorticalpathways(e.g.,throughthe superiorcolliculus),which biasesnewbornsto look at faces.However,cortical processingof facesmay require extensiveexperience(GauthierandNelson2001;Nelson2001)andmay developlater. The reasonsfor differential developmentacross ventral stream regions are unknown.Importantly,it is difficult to disentanglematurationalcomponents(genetically programmeddevelopmentalchanges)from experience-related components,because both are likely to play a role during development.One possibility is that the type of representationsand computationsin the rFFA and IPPA may require more time and experienceto maturethan thosein the LOC. Second,differentcortical regionsmay mature at different rates owing to genetic factors. Third, the FFA may retain more plasticity (evenin adulthood)than the LOC, as suggestedby studiesthat show that FFA responsesare modulatedby expertise(Gauthier et al. 1999; Tarr and Gauthier 2000).Fourth,the neuralmechanisms underlyingexperience-dependent changesmay differ amongLOC, FFA, and PPA. 6.9 Conclusion In sum,neuroimagingresearchin the pastdecadehasadvancedour understanding of objectrepresentations in the humanbrain.Thesestudieshaveidentifiedthe functional organizationof the human ventral stream,shown the involvementof ventral stream regionsin objectrecognition,and laid fundamentalsteppingstonesin understanding the neuralmechanisms that underlieinvariantobjectrecognition. Many questionsremain,however.First,whatis the relationshipbetweenneuralsensitivity to object transformationsand behavioralsensitivityto object transformations? producebiasesin performance? For example,emDo biasesin neuralrepresentations pirical evidenceshowsoverrepresentation of the lower visual field in LO. Does this lead to betterrecognitionin the lower visual field than in the upper visual field? A secondquestionis relatedto the developmentof the ventral stream:To what extent is experience(vs. genes)necessaryfor shapingfunctional selectivityin the ventral remainplasticin adulthood?If so,what is the stream?Third, do objectrepresentations changeslong-lasting?Fourth, temporalscaleof plasticity,andareexperience-induced what computationsareimplementedin the distinctcorticalregionsthat areinvolvedin in objectrecognition?Doesthe"aha"momentin recognitioninvolvea speciflcresponse a particularbrain region,or doesit involve a distributedresponseacrossa largecortical expanse? CombiningexperimentalmethodssuchasfMRI andMEG will provideboth this question.Fifth, high spatialandtemporalresolution,which is criticalto addressing what is the patternof connectivitybetweenventralstreamvisual regions?Although the connectivityin monkeyvisual cortex has beenexploredextensively(Van Essen et al. 1990;Moelleret al. 2008),we know little aboutthe connectivitybetweencortical visual areasin the human ventral stream.This knowledgeis necessaryfor building a model of hierarchicalprocessingin humansand any neuralnetwork model of lu KALANIT GRILL-SPECTOR object recognition.Approachesthat combine methodologies,such as psychophysics with fMRI, MEG with fMRI, or DTI with fMRI, will be instrumentalin addressing thesefundamentalquestions. I I I Acknowledgments I thank David Andresen,Golijeh Golarai, Rory Sayres,and Joakim Vinberg for their contributionsto the researchsummarizedin this chapter.David Andresen,for his contributions to understandingviewpoint-sensitiverepresentationsin the ventral stream. Golijeh Golarai,for spearheading the researchon the developmentof the ventralstudy. Rory Sayres,for his contributionsto researchon position invarianceand distributed analysesof the ventral stream.Joakim Vinberg, for his dedicationto researchingcueinvariant and figure-groundprocessingin the ventral stream.I thank David Remus, Kevin Weiner, Nathan Witthotft, and two anonymousreviewersfor commentson this manuscript.This work was supportedby National Eye Institute Grants 5 R21 EY016199-02andWhitehallFoundationGrant2005-05-III-RES. Bibliography Andresen DR, Vinberg J, Grill-Spector K. 20[19.The representationof object viewpoint in the human visual cortex. Neuroimage 45(2):522-536. E pub Nov, 25, 2008. Appelbaum LG, Wade AR, Vildavski VY, Pettet MW, Norcia AM. 200f, Cue-invariant networks for figure and background processing in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 26:11,695-1 1,708. Bar M, Tootell RB, Schacter DL, Greve DN, Fischl B, Mendola JD, Rosen BR, Dale AM. 2001. Cortical mechanismsspecific to explicit visual object recognition. Neuron29:529-535. Berl MM, Vaidya CJ, Gaillard WD.2006. Functional imaging of developmentaland adaptivechanges in neurocognitton. N euroitnage 30:67 949 l. Biederman I. 1987. Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. Psychol Rev 94:115-147. Biederman I, Cooper EE. 1991. Evidence for complete translational and reflectional invariance in visual object priming. Perception 20 :585-593. Booth MC, Rolls ET. 1998. View-invariant representationsof familiar objects by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 8:510-523. Bulthoff HH, Edelman S. 1992. Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:60{4. Bulthoff HH, Edelman SY, Tarr MJ. 1995. How are three-dimensional objects representedin the brain? Cereb Conex 5:247-2ffi. Chao LL, Haxby JV, Martin A. 1999. Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat Neurosci2:913-919. Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, lrhericy S, Dehaene-LambertzG, Henaff MA, Michel F. 2000. The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characteization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123 (Pt 2):291-307. Culham JC, Danckert SL, DeSouzaJF, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA. 2003. Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp Brain Res 153:18f189. Desimone R, Albright TD, Gross CG, Bruce C. 1984. Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque.J Neurosci 4:2051-2M2. WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? L25 DiCarlo JJ, Cox DD.2007. Untangling invariant object recognition. TrendsCogn Sci ll:333-341. DiCarlo JJ, Maunsell JH. 2003. Anterior inferotemporal neurons of monkeys engaged in object recognition can be highly sensitive to object retinal position. J Neurophysiol S9:3264- 3278. Dill M, Edelman S. 2001. Imperfect invarianceto object translati6tiin the discrimination of complex shapes.Perception 3O:7O7-7 24. Downing PE, Jiang Y, ShumanM, Kanwisher N. 2001. A cortical areaselectivefor visual processing of the human body. Science 293:247V2473. Fielman S, Bulthoff HH. 1992. Orientation dependence in the recognition of familiar and novel views of three-dimensional objects. Vision Res32:2385-24ffi. Edelman S, Intrator N. 2000. (Coarse coding of shape fragments) * (retinotopy) approximately : representationof structure. Spat Vis 13:255-264. Eger E, Ashburner J, Haynes JD, Dolan RI, Rees G. 2008. fMRI activity patternsin human LOC carry information about object exemplars within category.J Cogn Neurosci 20:35G370. Epstein R, Kanwisher N. 1998. A cortical representationof the local visual environment.Nature 392:598-601. EpsteinRA, ParkerWE, Feiler AM. 2008. Two kinds of fMRI repetition suppression?Evidencefor di ssociable neural mechanisms. J N europ hysio I 99(6) :2877 -2886. FangF, He S. 2005. Cortical responsesto invisible objectsin the human dorsal and ventral pathways. Nat Neurosci8: 1380-1385. FangF, Murray SO, He S. 2007. Duration-dependentFMRI adaptationand distributed viewer-centered face representationin human visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 17:14O2-1411. FarahMJ. 1995. Visualagnosia.Cambridge:MIT Press. FreedmanDJ, RiesenhuberM, PoggioT, Miller EK. 2001. Categoricalrepresentationof visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science291312-316. FreedmanDJ, RiesenhuberM, Poggio T, Miller EK. 2003. A comparison of primate prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices during visual categoization. J Neurosci 23:5235-5246. Fujita I, Tanaka K, Ito M, Cheng K. 1992. Columns for visual features of objects in monkey i nferotemporal cortex. N ature 360:343-346. Gauthier I, Hayward WG, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC.2002. BOLD activity during mental rotation and viewpoint-dependentobject recognition. Neuron 34:16l-17l. GauthierI, Nelson CA. 2001. The developmentof face expertise.Curr Opin Neurobiol lI:219-224Gauthier I, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW. 2000. Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areasinvolved in face recognition.Nat Neurosci 3:l9l-197 . Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC. 1999. Activation of the middle fusiform 'face area' increaseswith expertisein recognizingnovel objects.Nat Neurosci 2:568-573. Gilaie-Dotan S, Ullman S, Kushnir T, Malach R. 2002. Shape-selectivestereoprocessingin human object-related visual areas.Hum Brain Mapp 15:67--79. Golarai G, Ghahremani DG, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Reiss A, Eberhardt JL, Gabrieli JD, Grill-Spector K. 2001. Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory. Nat Neurosci lO:512-522. Grill-Spector K. 2003. The neural basisof object perception.Curr Opin Neurobiol l3:159-166. Grill-Spector K, Golarai G, Gabrieli J. 2008. Developmental neuroimaging of the human ventral visualcortex.TrendsCogn Sci 12:152-162. Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A. 2006a. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulusspecific effects. Trends Cogn Sci 10:14-23. Grill-Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N. 2ffi4. The fusiform face area subservesface perception, not genericwithin-categoryidentification. N at N eurosci 7 :555-562. Grill-SpectorK, Kushnir I EdelmanS, Avidan G, Itzchak Y, Malach R. 1999.Differential processing of objectsunder variousviewing conditionsin the humanlateraloccipital complex.Neuron24:187203. 126 KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Itzchak X Malach R. 1998a. Cue-invariant activation in object-related areasof the human occipital lobe. Neuron 2l:l9l-202. Grill-SpectorK, Kushnir T, Hendler T, EdelmanS, Itzchak Y Malach R. 1998b.A sequenceof objectprocessing stagesrevealed by fMRI'iri the human occipital lobe. Hum Brain Mapp 6:316-328. Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Malach R. 2000. The dynamics of object-selectiveactivation correlate with recognition performance in humans. Nat Neurosci 3:837-843. Grill-Spector K, Malach R. 2001. fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons.Acta Psychol (Amst) 107:293-321. Grill-Spector K, Malach R. 2fiX. The human visual cortex.Annu RevNeurosci 27:649477. Grill-Spector K, SayresR, RessD. 2006b.High-resolutionimaging revealshighly selectivenonface clustersin the fusiform face area.Nat Neurosci 9:1177-1185. Hasson U, Harel M, Levy I, Malach R. 2003. Large-scale mirror-symmetry organization of human occipito-temporalobject areas.N euron 3l :1027-1041. Hasson U, Levy I, Behrmann M, Hendler T, Malach R. 2002. Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas.Neuron 34:479490. Haxby JV Gobbini MI, Furey ML, Ishai A, SchoutenJL, Pietrini P. 2001. Distributed and overlapping representationsof faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293:2425-2430. Haxby JV Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. 2000. The distributed human neural systemfor face perception. TrendsCogn Sci4:223-233. Hemond CC, Kanwisher NG, Op de Beeck HP. 2007. A preferencefor contralateral stimuli in human object- and face-selectivecortex. PlnS ONE 2:e574. Hoffman EA, Haxby JV. 2000. Distinct representationsof eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat Neurosci 3:8G-84. JamesTW, Humphrey GK, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA. 2002. Differential effects of viewpoint on object-drivenactivationin dorsal and ventral streams.Neuron 35:793-801. JohnsonMH. 2001. Functional brain developmentin humans.Nat Rev Neurosci 2:475483. Kanwisher N. 2000. Domain specificity in face perception.Nat Neurosci 3:759-763. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. 1997.The fusiform face area:a module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception.J Neurosci 17:43024311. Kastner S, De Weerd P, Ungerleider LG. 2000. Texture segregation in the human visual cortex: a functional MRI study. J N europhysiol 83:2453-2457. Kobatake E, Tanaka K. 1994. Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the venffal visual pathway of the macaquecerebralcortex. J NeurophysiolTl:85G867. Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. 2000. Cortical regions involved in perceiving object shape. J Neurosci 20:3310-3318. Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. 200 1. Representationof perceivedobject shapeby the human lateral occipital complex. Science 293: I 506-1 509. Kourtzi Z, Tolias AS, Altmann CF, Augath M, Logothetis NK. 2W3. Integration of local features into global shapes:monkey and human FMRI studies.Neuron3T:333-346. Lerner I Epshtein B, Ullman S, Malach R. 2008. Class information predicts activation by object fragments in human object areas.J Cogn Neurosci 20:1189-1206. Lerner Y HendlerT, Ben-BashatD, Harel M, Malach R. 2001.A hierarchicalaxis of objectprocessing stagesin the human visual cortex. Cereb Cortex II:287-297. Lerner Y, Hendler T, Malach R. 2002. Object-completion effects in the human lateral occipital complex.CerebCortex 12:163-177. Levy I, Hasson U, Avidan G, Hendler I Malach R. 2001. Center-periphery organization of human object areas.Nat Neurosci4:533-539. Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Poggio T. 1995. Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys.Curc Biol 5:552-563. WHAT HAS fMRI TAUGHT US ABOUT OBJECT RECOGNITION? nant activation in cquenceofobjecf tpp 6:316-328. electiveactivation onal propertiesof ':619477. selectivenonface lrzationof human as an organizing 127 Lueschow A, Miller EK, Desimone R. 1994. Inferior temporal mechanismsfor invariant object recognition. Cereb Cortex 4:523-53I. Malach R, Levy I, HassonU.2002. The topographyof high-order human object areas.TrendsCogn Sci 6: l7Gl84. Malach R, ReppasJB, BensonRR, Kwong KK, Jiang H, KennedyWA, Ledden PJ, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB. 1995.Object-relatedactivity revealedby functional magneticresonanceimaging in human occipital cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:8135-8139. Man D. 1980. Visual information processing: the structure and creation of visual representations. Philos TransR Soc Innd B Biol Sci 29O:199-218. Martin A, Wiggs CL, UngerleiderLG, Haxby JV. 1996.Neural correlatesof category-specificknowledge.Nature 379:649452. McKyton A, Zohary 8. 2007. Beyond retinotopic mapping: the spatial representationof objects in the human lateral occipital complex. Cereb Cortex l7:1164-1112. Mendola JD, Dale AM, Fischl B, Liu AK, Tootell RB. 1999. The representationof illusory and real contours in human cortical visual areasrevealed by functional magnetic resonanceimaging. d and overlapping -i-t-t30. rr lace perception. J Neurosci I 9:8560-8572. Miller EK, Li L, Desimone R. 1991. A neural mechanismfor working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. Science 254:1377-1379. Moeller S, Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. 2008. Patcheswith links: a unified systemfor processingfacesin stimuli in human the macaquetemporal lobe. Science320:7355-1359. NakayamaK, He ZJ, Shimojo S. 1995.Visual surfacerepresentation:a critical link betweenlow-level and high-level vision. ln An invitation to cognitive sciences:visual cognition, ed. SM Kosslyn and DN Osherson.Cambridge:MIT Press. Nelson CA. 2001. The development and neural bases of face recognition. Infant Child Develop rn rhe distributed iectsof viewpoint l0:3-18. {7-5-.183. Op de Beeck HP, Haushofer J, Kanwisher NG. 2008. Interpreting fMRI data: maps, modules and dimensions.Nat RevNeurosci 9:123-135. u*un extrastriate Op De Beeck H, Vogels R. 2000. Spatial sensitivity of macaqueinferior temporal neurons. J Comp Neurol426:505-518. r risual cortex:a Perrett DI. 1996. View-dependent coding in the ventral stream and its consequencefor recognition. ln Vision and movement mechanisms in the cerebral cortex, ed. R Camaniti, KP Hoffmann, and :he \ entral visual AJ Lacquaniti, 142-151. Strasbourg:HFSP. Perrett DI, Oram MW Ashbridge E. 1998. Evidence accumulationin cell populations responsive to faces: an account of generalisation of recognition without mental transformations. Cognition 67:llI-145. tape. ./ Neurosci n lateraloccipital PetersonMA, Gibson BS. 1994a.Must shape recognition follow figure-ground organization?An assumptionin peril. Psychol Sci5:253-259. of local features Peterson MA, Gibson BS. 1994b. Object recognition contributions to figure-ground organization: operationson outlines and subjectivecontours.PerceptPsychophys56:551-564. Poggio T, Edelman S. 1990. A network that learns to recognize three-dimensional objects. Nature ration by object 343:263-266. ,bjectprocessing lateral occipital .ation of human rporal cortex of Quiroga RQ, Mukamel R, Isham EA, Malach R, Fried I. 2008. Human single-neuronresponsesat the thresholdof consciousrecognition.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:3599-3604. Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. 2005. Invariant visual representationby single neuronsin the human brain. Nature 435:1102-1107. RiesenhuberM, Poggio T. 1999. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nat Neurosci 2:1019-1025. Rolls ET. 2000. Functions of the primate temporal lobe cortical visual areasin invariant visual object and face recognition. Neuron 27:205-218. KALANIT GRILL.SPECTOR Rolls ET, Milward T. 2000. A model of invariant object recognition in the visual system: learning rules, activation functions, lateral inhibition, and information-based performance measures.Neural Comput 12:2547-2572. SawamuraH, Orban GA, Vogels R. 2006. Selectivity ofhburonal adaptationdoes not match response selectivity: a single-cell study of the FMRI adaptation paradigm. Neuron 49:307-318. SayresR, Grill-Spector K. 2008. Relating retinotopic and object-selectiveresponsesin human lateral Re occipital cortex. J Neurophysiol 100(1):249-267,Epub May 7,2008. Scherf KS, BehrmannM, Humphreys K, Luna 8.2N7. Visual category-selectivityfor faces,places J- and objects emergesalong different developmental trajectories. Dev Sci l0:F15-30. SchwarzloseRF, Baker CI, Kanwisher NK. 2005. Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus.J Neurosci25:11055-11059. Kt SchwarzloseRF, Swisher JD, Dang S, Kanwisher N. 2008. The distribution of category and location information across object-selective regions in human visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA lO5:4447 4452. Stanley DA, Rubin N. 2003. fMRI activation in responseto illusory contours and salient regions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron3T:323-331. Tarr MJ, BulthoffHH. 1995. Is human object recognition better describedby geon structural descriptions orby multiple views?Comment on Biedermanand Gerhardstein(1993).J Exp PsycholHum Percept Perfo rm 2l : | 49Ll 505. Tarr MJ, Gauthier I. 2000. FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise.Nat Neurosci 3:764-769. Thorpe S, Fize D, Marlot C. 1996.Speedof processingin the human visual system.Nature 381:520- MinskY t I $ necessit\ of 522. ...it i: nt Thorpe SJ, Gegenfurtner KR, Fabre-Thorpe M, Bulthoff HH. 2001. Detection of animals in natural images using far peripheral vision. Eur J Neurosci 14:869-876. structurc' an intcntt for onc Pu account tt Ullman S. 1989.Aligning pictorial descriptions:an approachto object recognition. Cognition32:193254. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M, Macko KA. 1983. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical path- (and idca achierc. I ways. TrendsNeurosci 6:414417. Valyear KF, Culham JC, Sharif N, Westwood D, Goodale MA. 2006. A double dissociation between sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation in the ventral and dorsal visual sitting rnif ther .hi we nc'cd internal i' streams:a human fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44:218-228. Van Essen DC, Felleman DJ, DeYoe EA, Olavarria J, Knierim J. 1990. Modular and hierarchical organization of extrastriate visual cortex in the macaque monkey. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol55:679496. Vinberg J, Grill-Spector K. 2008. Representationof shapes,edges,and surfacesacrossmultiple cues in the human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol99:1380-1393. The earl structurt'()1 functions t' Vogels R, Biederman l. 2ffi2. Effects of illumination intensity and direction on object coding in macaqueinferior temporal cortex. Cereb Cortex l2:75G766. been rePre reasonins ; Vuilleumier P, Henson RN, Driver J, Dolan RI. 2002. Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-relatedfMRI of repetition priming. Nat Neurosci 5:491499. Wandell BA. 1999. Computational neuroimaging of human visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 22; with thc' ai quote illu: depicts a ' 145-173. Wang G, Tanaka K, Tanifuji M. 1996. Optical imaging of functional organization in the monkey inferotemporal cortex. Science 272: 1665-1668. Williams MA, Dang S, Kanwisher NG. 2m7. Only some spatial patterns of fMRI responseare read out in task performance.Nat Neurosci 10:685-686. * t.,. I research ir Using Ft'r point that I how that o persp€cti (b) Eto J Eo T* E go I, 0 10m Prtrntrllon Dundm{mr} Plate 6.1. Object-, face-, and place-selectivecortex. (a) Data of one representivesubject shown on her partially inflated right hemisphere:lateralview (/eft);ventral view (right).Dark gray indicates sulci. Light gray indicates gyri. Black /ines delineate retinotopic regions. Blue regions delineate object-selectiveregions (objects > scrambled objects), including LO and pFus/OTSventrally as well as dorsal foci along the intraparietalsulcus (lPS). Red regions delineate face-selectiveregions (faces > non-facesobjects), including the FFA, a region in LOS and a region in posterior STS. Magenta regions delineate overlap between face- and objects-selective regions.Creen regionsdelineate place-selectiveregions(places> objects), including the PPAand a dorsal region lateralto the lPS.Dark green regionsdelineateoverlap between place- and object-selectiveregions.All maps thresholdedat P < 0.001, voxel level. (b) LO and pFus/OTS(but not Vl) responsesare correlatedwith recognitionperformance(CrillSpectoret al. 2000). To superimposerecognition performanceand fMRl signalson the same plot, all values were normalized relativeto the maximum responsefor the 500-ms duration stimulus. %sien*#!ti:ll. For fMRl signals(blue, red, and orange ltnes) : -o/orignal(Soo ms) "h correct(condition) (black) : mance For recognition perfor- ms) "/"correct(S00 Pfr Plate6.2. Selective responses to objectsacrossmultiplevisualcuesacrossthe LOC.Statistical maps of selectiveresponseto object from luminance,stereoand motion informationin a representative subject.All mapswerethresholded at P < 0.005,voxellevel,and areshownon the inflatedrighthemisphere of a representative subject.(a)Luminance objects> scrambled objects.(b) Objectsgeneratedfrom randomdot stereograms versusstructureless randomdot (perceivedas a cloud of dots).(c) Objectsgeneratedfrom dot motion versusthe streograms samedotsmovingrandomly.Visualmeridiansarerepresented by red (upper),blue(horizontal), andgreen(lower)lines.Whitecontour indicatesa motion-selective region,or MT. Adapted fromVinbergand Crill-Spector 2008. ;ubject ,. Dark s. Blue O and egions ;ion in e- and rjects), rverlap I level. ,Crilll same lration rerlor- rltltlt 9o Left 4'Lefr Fovea 4o Right 4oup 4o Down rllllll ffllllll -2-1012 Amplitude versusmeanrosponse % SignalChange Plate 6.4. LO distributedresponsepatternsto differentobject categoriesand stimuluspositions.Data are shown on the lateralaspectof the right hemispherecorticalsurfacefor a representative subject.Eachpanelshowsthe distributedLO fMRl amplitudesaftersubtracting from eachvoxelits meanresponse.Redand yellow,Responses that are higherthanthe voxel's mean response.Blue and cyan, Responses that are lower than the voxel'smean response. lnset,Inflated with red squareindicatingthe zoomedregion.Note rightcorticalhemisphere, that patternchangessignificantlyacrosscolumns(positions) and to a lesserextentacrossrows (categories). Adaptedfrom Sayresand Grill-Spector 2008.