New Zealand Veterinary Journal 2003 (in press) Responsiveness, behavioural arousal and awareness in fetal and newborn lambs: experimental, practical and therapeutic implications DJ Mellor*§ and NG Gregory† *Animal † § Welfare Science and Bioethics Centre, IFNHH, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand. South Australian Research and Development Institute, Flaxley, SA 5153, Australia. Author for correspondence. Email: D.J.Mellor@massey.ac.nz Abstract This review distinguishes between physical responsiveness, behavioural arousal and awareness in fetal and newborn lambs, and it summarises the physical and physiological factors which activate and suppress behavioural arousal. Important activators include rising blood oestrogen concentrations just before birth, physical stimuli during delivery, exposure to cold on delivery, and elevation in blood oxygen levels following the onset of pulmonary respiration. Suppressors of behavioural arousal and awareness are low oxygen levels and high concentrations of progesterone and its metabolites in the fetal circulation, exposure to a warm intrauterine environment and to a circulating placental factor that inhibits activity including breathing. In view of the levels of oxygen required to sustain awareness in adult animals, the low levels in the fetal circulation, and the actions of the other suppressors, it is not likely that awareness occurs in the fetus. Nevertheless, fetuses perform a range of physical acts that would be supported or initiated by brainstem activity. In addition they show physical responses to potentially painful stimuli during late gestation, but it has yet to be demonstrated that these are linked to perception of pain. It is postulated that perception of pain could only occur once there is a level of oxygenation that supports overall awareness, and under normal circumstances this would only occur once the newborn starts breathing air. The implications for the welfare of the fetal lambs and calves during experimental surgery, slaughter of the pregnant dam, collection of blood (serum) from fetuses at slaughter, and fetotomy are favourable, indicating that current practices, when carefully undertaken, are humane. Key words: animal welfare, behaviour, arousal, awareness, pain, fetus, birth changes, newborn, fetal surgery, slaughter, fetal blood (serum) collection, fetotomy. Abbreviations: ECoG, electrocorticogram; EEG, electroencephalogram; EOG, electro-oculogram; GABAA, aminobutyric acidA; PaCO2, arterial carbon dioxide partial pressure; PaO2, arterial oxygen partial pressure; REM, rapid eye movement. Key Points Responsiveness, behavioural arousal and awareness may be identified by reference to uncoordinated or coordinated skeletal muscle movements and to ECoG, EOG and breathing activity. 1 The fetal lamb responds to physical stimulation from early in pregnancy, behavioural arousal becomes evident during late pregnancy, and awareness may appear for the first time only after birth. Suppressors of behavioural arousal and awareness operate before birth and activators of them operate during and immediately after birth. Although the fetal lamb is not likely to be arousable to an aware state by noxious stimulation in utero, as a precaution fetal surgery should only be conducted when both the mother and fetus are under general anaesthetic. Slaughter of pregnant ewes stops placental O2 supply within seconds and, within 1 to 2 min at most, induces very severe fetal hypoxaemia and a flattening of the fetal ECoG. Such fetuses are not capable of behavioural arousal or awareness. Fetal lambs are therefore not likely to suffer during slaughter of their dams, and nor are fetal calves. Provided that fetal calf blood (serum) collection does not begin until cerebral hypoxia/anoxia has flattened the fetal ECoG and provided the calf is prevented from breathing air, blood (serum) collection will be humane. Allowing a safety margin, collection could begin 5 to 6 min after slaughter of the dam, but is normally delayed for at least 20 min in Australia and New Zealand. If the umbilical cord can be reached, it should be severed manually 5 to 6 min before fetotomy, if not, other precautions need to be taken to maximise the humaneness of fetotomy. Introduction At the moment of birth the newborn lamb has no prior experience of extrauterine life. All situations it encounters immediately afterwards are new and the intensity and character of the associated sensory inputs are obviously transformed markedly by the event of birth. For instance, the fetus is buoyant in amniotic fluid, cushioned by soft maternal tissues and fluids, cramped within a restricted space and its body temperature is kept just above that of the mother (Mellor 1969, 1980, 1984; Gluckman et al 1984; Fraser and Broom 1990), whereas after birth the newborn is exposed to gravity, air, hard surfaces, unlimited space and, usually, to cold challenge for the first time (Fraser and Broom 1990; Lynch et al 1992). To survive, the newborn lamb must, among other things, immediately start to breathe effectively (Mott 1961; Dawes et al 1972a) and, in cold conditions, it must also increase its heat production (Eales and Small 1980). From about 20 to 60 minutes after birth viable lambs of different breeds will stand, and then walk, locate the mother and her udder, drink colostrum and, in cold conditions, seek shelter (Mellor and Pearson 1977; Slee and Springbett 1986; Lynch et al 1992). Such behaviour is possible because, by the time of birth, the sensory apparatus of the lamb, along with numerous other neurological structures, have developed sufficiently for the lamb to use sight, hearing, smell, taste, touch, proprioception and thermal sensitivity effectively (Fraser and Broom 1990; Lynch et al 1992). Such behaviour presumably reflects the operation of innate drives initially, but learned reactions become evident subsequently (Fraser and Broom 1990; Lynch et al 1992). This suggests that the lamb may become more aware of its actions and surroundings as time passes, but it is not clear to what extent the lamb is aware immediately after birth, or indeed before birth. The lamb has been and continues to be used widely for biomedical and veterinary studies of fetal and neonatal physiology and pathophysiology (e.g. Meschia et al 1965; Mellor and Slater 1971; Liggins et al 1973; Mellor 1987; Harding et al 1981; Clewlow et al 1983; Berger et al 1990; Clapp et al 1988; Gluckman et al 1989; Richardson et al 1996; Johnston et al 1998; King and McCullagh 2001). This has greatly advanced knowledge of ovine prenatal 2 development and the practical or therapeutic application of that knowledge to enhancing the survival, health and well-being of the newborn lamb (Alexander 1980; Mellor 1983, 1988; Eales and Small 1995) and, by careful extrapolation, of other animals including the newborn human infant (e.g. Mellor and Cockburn 1986; de Haan et al 1996; Gunn et al 1998). Recent attention given to awareness in postnatal animals (e.g. Lehman 1998; Piggins and Phillips 1998; Sommerville and Broom 1998; Kirkwood et al 2001) raises the question of the extent to which our current knowledge of prenatal development and responsiveness permits conclusions to be drawn about awareness in the fetus and newborn. Accordingly, the behavioural and physiological bases of the potential for arousal and awareness in fetal and newborn lambs are considered in the early part of the present review. These matters are directly relevant to the welfare of fetal and newborn lambs. During surgical preparation of the fetal lamb for physiological and pathophysiological studies it is exposed to potentially noxious stimulation so that the extent to which it might be aware in utero is relevant to whether procedures should be conducted using epidural, paravertebral or general anaesthesia of the ewe. In addition, significant numbers of pregnant ewes, and other ruminants (Ladds et al 1975), are slaughtered annually and their fetuses die in utero from hypoxia and hypercapnia or, in some cases, from exsanguination during fetal blood (serum) collection. This raises the question of whether or not such fetuses might suffer before they die. Finally, the capacity of the fetus to be aware during birth has direct bearing on the humane management of fetotomy on those occasions when living fetuses need to be dismembered in utero in order to resolve intractable dystocia. The later part of present review therefore addresses the welfare implications of these practices for lambs and, where appropriate, for calves. Awareness Awareness is linked to wakefulness and, in general, implies that responses to stimuli involve higher brain centres and are not merely reflexes (Sommerville and Broom, 1998). Unaware but physically responsive In this situation, afferent and efferent neural pathways have developed but, following some sensory input which elicits a movement for example, complex cortical processing does not take place (Sommerville and Broom, 1998). In the fetal lamb at different gestational ages it is thought that physical movements might occur without cortical processing when impulse traffic does not ascend the spinal cord, or the required neural connections to the brain are not yet in place, or its higher neural structures are poorly developed. In the present review the word responsiveness means physical responsiveness, and when used without qualification relates only to unawareness. Perceptual awareness With awareness of this type, a perceived stimulus involves higher brain centres and causes a response which the individual may or may not be able to modify voluntarily (Sommerville and Broom, 1998). Non-modifiable responses in perceptually aware young lambs may include spinal reflex responses to pain, and blinking when an object passes close to the eye. Modifiable responses in such lambs may include scratching to relieve irritation. 3 Fetal and neonatal behavioural arousal and awareness Fetal movements and what they mean Fetal movements begin early in pregnancy (Barcroft and Barron 1937, 1939; Fraser 1989; Fraser and Broom 1990; Berger et al 1997). Initially they are uncoordinated and involve local movements of the limbs, trunk or neck. As the fetus matures the movements become stronger and more coordinated (some of which appear to be directed towards changing the body’s orientation within the amniotic sac) and exhibit alternating periods of activity and inactivity (Ruckebusch et al 1977: Rigatto et al 1982; Clewlow et al 1983; Fraser 1989; Fraser and Broom 1990; Berger et al 1997). These changes reflect the way that neural and neuromuscular development progresses from sparsely connected rudimentary precursors of nerve tracts and brain structures very early in pregnancy to the well-defined, complex, sophisticated and operationally effective, yet still maturing, structures that are present at birth (Barlow 1969; Bernhard and Meyerson 1973; Persson 1973; Cook et al 1987; Nitsos and Rees 1993; Nitsos et al 1994; Rees et al 1994a, b; Fitzgerald 1996; Berger et al 1997). Apart from body stretching and other limb, trunk and neck movements, totalling about 4,000 to 6,000 movements per day during the last 14 days of pregnancy, the fetal lamb also exhibits gasps, sighs and shallow breathing, jaw, swallowing and tongue movements, eye movements and eyelid opening and closing. Of particular interest here, however, is what fetal movements – fetal behaviour – might suggest about the developmental interplay between physical responsiveness, behavioural arousal and awareness before birth and about the manifestation of behavioural arousal and awareness after birth. Some of the behaviours that are observed in the fetal lamb are undoubtedly rudimentary and can occur below the level of normal awareness. This is evident from comparable behaviours that occur in human subjects who are in a vegetative state intellectually. For example, the severely hydranencephalic infant has been observed to grimace in response to potentially painful stimuli, to keep its eyes open, swallow and breathe spontaneously (Pallis 1982). These are brainstem functions which do not signify higher levels of consciousness, but some of them demonstrate that the subject is not comatose. Fetal sleep states Distinct EEG, ECoG and EOG patterns are present in adult animals during wakefulness (awareness) and sleep (Empson 1993; Akerstedt et al 1998; Endo et al 1998; Baars 2001; Landolt et al 2001). The same patterns of slowwave, synchronised EEG activity observed during deep sleep, which is an arousable form of unconsciousness, are also evident in other states of global unconsciousness including general anaesthesia and coma (Baars 2001). States resembling slow-wave and REM sleep become established in fetal lambs at 110 to 125 days in the 147-day pregnancy in sheep (Dawes et al 1972b; Harding et al 1981; Clewlow et al 1983; Berger et al 1986; Dawes 1988), such that near birth the fetal lamb spends most of its time (all but about 5 minutes in every hour) in those states. Fetal behavioural arousal Behavioural arousal is distinguished from sleep in fetal lambs by low-voltage ECoG activity, together with increases in eye movements (EOG), postural muscle activity, breathing and responsiveness to somatic stimulation (Harding et al 1981; Rigatto et al 1982; Clewlow et al 1983). Thus, such arousal, defined physiologically and behaviourally, can be demonstrated and is distinct from fetal sleep states. 4 During labour the character of the fetal lamb’s ECoG changes, such that the time spent in low-voltage as opposed to high-voltage states declines significantly and a mixed high/low-voltage state appears for the first time (Berger et al 1986). Moreover, fetal motor systems in general, including the respiratory system, are largely quiescent during labour (Berger et al 1986; Fraser and Broom 1990; Hasan and Rigaux 1991), so that fetal behavioural arousal apparently does not increase markedly during labour. Newborn behavioural arousal and perceptual awareness Immediately after birth a viable lamb lies immobile on the ground. Soon it shakes its head and gasps several times before regular breathing begins. Within very few minutes it makes minor movements of the head, neck and limbs, movements which increase in intensity and co-ordination as the lamb first holds its head up and then tries to stand. Thus, behavioural arousal is usually evident in the newborn lamb within a few minutes of birth and accompanies the onset of pulmonary respiration (Mellor et al 1972; Mellor and Pearson 1977; Berger et al 1990; Lynch et al 1992). Although perceptual awareness is evident by 20 to 60 minutes after birth when the lamb stands for the first time and starts teat seeking (Mellor and Pearson 1977; Slee and Springbett 1986; Fraser and Broom 1990), it is not clear whether or not the lamb at birth immediately becomes aware of its surroundings and, if it does not, when such awareness appears. After birth, behavioural arousal might appear before awareness, arousal and awareness might appear simultaneously, or awareness might appear before arousal. Whatever the case, both are required for survival of the newborn lamb. Without behavioural arousal and the associated onset of pulmonary respiration the lamb will die of hypoxaemia/hypercapnia very soon after birth. Without perceptual awareness the lamb will not be able to interact with its mother, a strong ewe-lamb bond will not be established and the lamb will not receive colostrum and will die later from starvation. It is still not clear whether the short periods of fetal behavioural arousal during late pregnancy simply reflect enhanced yet unconscious reflex responsiveness or indicate the earliest stage when short periods of fetal awareness might be possible. It is clear, however, that lambs rapidly show signs of behavioural arousal and perceptual awareness after birth, and that these bouts of arousal last longer than those in utero. This raises the question of whether birth removes suppressors of behavioural arousal and/or in some way triggers or activates it (Table 1). Suppressors and activators of fetal and neonatal behavioural arousal and awareness Fetal oxygen and carbon dioxide status Suppression The fetal lamb’s PaO2 is usually less than about 25 per cent and its PaCO2 is usually more than about 135 per cent of the respective values in the conscious ewe (Jones 1977; Jones et al 1977; Robinson et al 1979; Jensen et al 1991). The low PaO2 and high PaCO2 in the fetus exist because of the concentration gradients required for these gases to diffuse across the placenta (Meschia et al 1965; Comline and Silver 1970; Silver et al 1973). In mature animals including human beings, severe hypoxaemia (PaO2 below about 28 mm Hg) causes unconsciousness (Brierly et al 1980; West et al 1984; Hattingh et al 1986). As carotid arterial O 2 tensions are usually 20 to 27 mm Hg in normal 5 and 12 to 18 mm Hg in placentally deficient fetal lambs (Jones, 1977; Jones et al 1977; Robinson et al 1979, 1983; Harding et al 1985; Jensen et al 1991), the fetal brain is usually exposed to O 2 tensions that would cause unconsciousness during postnatal life. This suggests that fetal arousal and awareness may be suppressed by the low O2 status. This proposition is strongly supported by the observation that higher than normal fetal O 2 tensions in the presence of normal CO2 tensions apparently stimulate continuous breathing and behavioural arousal in fetal lambs (Baier et al 1990; Hasan and Rigaux 1991). Also, acute reductions in fetal PaO 2 decrease the incidence of behavioural arousal (Bocking and Harding, 1986). However, during chronic moderate hypoxaemia, an initially reduced incidence of fetal behavioural arousal returns to normal levels within about 16 hours (Bocking et al 1988). This recovery presumably results from circulatory adjustments during hypoxaemia that enhance blood flow to the fetal brain (Rudolph et al 1981; Rudolph, 1984), supported by the higher haematocrit in the fetus than in the mother (Meschia et al 1965) and the greater capacity of fetal haemoglobin to deliver O 2 to fetal tissues (including the brain) at lower than adult O2 tensions (Meschia et al 1961). In contrast, severe chronic hypoxaemia sufficient to cause a metabolic acidosis markedly reduces the signs of behavioural arousal (Richardson et al 1992), as do repeated short periods of severe hypoxaemia caused by reversible umbilical cord occlusion in utero (Gunn et al 1992). Several observations in newborn lambs and calves are also relevant. First, hypotension and hypoxia are potent stimuli that arouse normoxaemic lambs from sleep (Horne et al 1989; Johnston et al 1998), but they are less effective in arousing lambs exposed to continuous or repeated short bouts of moderate hypoxaemia (Fewell and Konduri 1988, 1989; Walker et al 1993; Johnston et al 1998). Note that the PaO 2 during hypoxaemia in these postnatal lambs (PaO2, 40-60 mm Hg) was greater than that in normoxaemic fetuses (20 to 27 mm Hg). Second, active, perceptually aware newborn lambs, immersed in water at maternal body temperature and exposed to 2.5 to 7.5 per cent O 2 in inspired air for a sufficient period to elicit a metabolic acidosis equivalent to that caused by severe hypoxaemia during birth, rapidly exhibit depressed behavioural arousal, awareness and respiratory activity (a state distinct from sleep), but return to an aware, active state when given room air to breathe (Eales and Small 1985). Third, the behaviour of newborn calves which breathed air (21% O 2) for the first 2-3 minutes after birth and then breathed a gas mixture (10.5% O2) designed to maintain their arterial O2 tensions at about 25 mm Hg for 24 hours (Tyler and Ramsey, 1991; HD Tyler, unpublished observations), is of interest. Apparently the calves were initially fairly active, but quickly became lethargic and then increasingly sleepy and unresponsive as the period of hypoxaemia progressed (HD Tyler, unpublished observations). The weight of evidence therefore suggests that the incidence of fetal behavioural arousal is a function of the arterial O2 tension, with greater than normal O2 tensions increasing it and lower than normal O2 tensions decreasing it, sometimes transiently. Thus, it appears that during fetal normoxaemia, the PaO 2 does have a suppressive effect on behavioural arousal in fetal lambs, but does not abolish it. In contrast, the somewhat elevated fetal blood CO2 tensions (40 to 50 mm Hg) may not be sufficiently high to suppress arousal and awareness because the arterial CO2 tensions required to induce anaesthesia in adult dogs (220 mm Hg; Eisele et al 1967) and monkeys (at least 115 mm Hg; Mattsson et al 1972), and comatose or semi-comatose states in human beings (130 mm Hg; Sieker and Hickam 1956), are much higher. Nevertheless the combination of low O2 and high CO2 tensions may synergistically enhance the suppressive effects of each gas alone (Mohan Raj et al 1992). 6 Activation Although low blood O2 and/or high blood CO2 tensions in the fetal lamb apparently inhibit breathing in utero, as already noted higher than usual fetal O2 tensions in the presence of normal CO2 tensions apparently stimulate continuous fetal breathing and behavioural arousal (Baier et al 1990; Hasan and Rigaux 1991), especially after about 135 days of gestation (Hasan and Rigaux 1991). This suggests that the marked increases in O 2 and decreases in CO2 tensions that accompany the onset of pulmonary respiration immediately after birth (Comline and Silver 1972; Mellor and Pearson 1977; Berger et al 1990) both remove an inhibition to continuous breathing and arousal (due to low O2 and high CO2), and actively stimulate continuous breathing and arousal (due to high O 2). The onset of breathing also rapidly increases the lamb’s PaO2 to levels well above the 28 mm Hg threshold for awareness in mature postnatal animals (Brierly et al 1980; West et al 1984; Hattingh et al 1986) and may thereby play a major role in promoting awareness in the lamb shortly after birth. Clearly the onset of breathing is critical for these changes to occur. Breathing is apparently initiated in the newborn lamb by the combined effects of several factors including umbilical cord occlusion (Adamson et al 1987; 1991) and low blood O2 and high blood CO2 tensions (Boddy et al 1974; Jansen et al 1982; Rigatto et al 1988) immediately after birth. Fetal progesterone and oestrogen status Suppression - progesterone Progesterone, its metabolites and/or synthetic analogues have potent sedative and anaesthetic actions in adult animals, including human beings (Paul and Purdy 1992). Progesterone metabolites can interact with GABA A receptors, enhancing GABA binding to them, thereby increasing the activity of GABA inhibitory pathways in the central nervous system (see Crossley et al 1997). In the pregnant ewe the placenta produces large quantities of progesterone throughout at least the last half of pregnancy, especially during the last 20 to 30 days (Bassett et al 1969), and thus exposes the fetus to high circulating concentrations of progesterone and its metabolites (Seamark et al 1970; Dolling and Seamark 1979). In fetal sheep GABAA receptors are apparently present in the brain from at least 56 days of gestation and reach adult levels by 120 days, about 27 days before birth (Villiger et al 1982). During the last 20 to 30 days of pregnancy, injections of progesterone or its metabolites into the fetal circulation or cerebral ventricles reduce fetal EEG, ECoG and EOG activity, breathing movements and behavioural arousal, and inhibition of placental progesterone production enhances them (Crenshaw et al, 1966; Crossley et al 1997: Nicol et al 1997, 1998; Hirst et al 2000). This strongly implicates progesterone and its metabolites as suppressors of fetal behavioural arousal. During the last 3 to 5 days before birth in sheep placental progesterone production declines as placental steroidogenesis increasingly favours oestrogen production under the action of the marked surge in fetal cortisol production that precedes birth (Liggins et al 1973; Thorburn and Challis 1979). An associated prenatal decline in progesterone (Power et al 1982) and its metabolite levels within the fetal circulation would reduce the associated suppressive effects on behavioural arousal in the near-term fetal lamb and would presumably facilitate the appearance of arousal in the lamb after birth. Activation - oestrogen 7 Several observations suggest that fetal oestrogen status can affect behavioural arousal. First, inactive fetal lambs delivered under epidural anaesthesia of the ewe during mid-pregnancy “quickly” become active when injected with oestradiol-17 (DH Barron, unpublished observation). Second, inactive lambs, delivered prematurely very close to the time of normal birth, start breathing and become aroused behaviourally within 30 to 60 seconds of being injected with oestradiol-17 (Mellor et al 1972). Third, the switch in placental steroidogenesis away from progesterone which occurs just before birth (Liggins et al 1973; Thorburn and Challis 1979), leads to increasing secretion of oestrogens into fetal plasma (Challis and Patrick 1981) and, thus, exposes fetal neural tissue to rising concentrations of oestrogens. Although the mechanism by which oestrogens stimulate behavioural arousal and continuous respiration is not known, the preparturient surge in its circulating concentrations (Challis and Patrick 1981) has the clear potential to actively prepare the lamb to become behaviourally aroused and start breathing immediately after birth. Fetal thermal status Suppression - warmth There is evidence that the thermal status of the fetal lamb – in particular warmth – suppresses behavioural arousal. First, the fetus is slightly hyperthermic in relation to the mother because the heat it generates can only be dissipated down a thermal gradient across the placenta (Gluckman et al 1993). Second, mature fetal lambs cooled in utero such that their cutaneous thermoreceptors are exposed to cold (via cooling coils in amniotic fluid), exhibit behavioural arousal, shivering and increased respiratory activity, and rewarming such fetuses reverses these effects (Gunn et al 1985, 1986, 1991; Gluckman et al 1993). Third, aroused, physically active and perceptually aware newborn lambs immersed in water at maternal body temperature assume a sleep-like, non-aroused, unaware state, and cooling the water restores their prior aroused, aware and physically active state (Eales and Small 1980). Activation - cold At birth the newborn lamb commonly, but not invariably, enters air temperatures significantly below maternal body temperature. This would immediately remove the suppressive effect of prior warmth on behavioural arousal. Indeed, the above evidence suggests that cold-activation of cutaneous thermoreceptors is a strong stimulus for behavioural arousal after birth (Eales and Small 1980; Gunn et al 1985, 1986, 1991; Gluckman et al 1993). Fetal tactile stimulation Suppression and activation Amniotic fluid buffers the embryo/fetus against mechanical stimulation and presumably also reduces sensations associated with gravity, and thereby minimises tactile sensory input likely to stimulate behavioural arousal. Thus, transfer from complete immersion in amniotic fluid in utero into the postnatal air environment leading to marked increases in such sensory effects, may contribute to the usually rapid onset of arousal in the newborn. In addition, manual stimulation of the ears, nose and other areas of the head of inactive newborn lambs often elicits strong kicking and appears to aid the subsequent onset of continuous pulmonary respiration (DJ Mellor, unpublished observations). This suggests that the compression of these body parts by the cervix and vagina during birth, and by licking from the ewe shortly after birth, may actively help to initiate the behavioural arousal and breathing usually seen in the lamb straight after birth. 8 Placental inhibitor Suppression Occluding the umbilical cord (reversibly) in the presence of adequate oxygenation via a tracheal catheter induces behavioural arousal and continuous breathing in experimentally studied fetal lambs (Alvaro et al 1993). Infusion of a placental extract, or a sub-fraction of it, but not the vehicle, abolished this fetal arousal and respiration within two minutes. This suggests that a placental factor, probably a peptide, inhibits arousal and breathing during fetal life (Alvaro et al 1993). Clearly, loss of the placenta at birth would cause an immediate decrease in the circulating concentrations of this factor, thereby removing its inhibitory effects on behavioural arousal and respiration. Behavioural arousal and awareness in the fetal and newborn lamb – an integrated hypothesis The weight of evidence presented above suggests that although fetal lambs subjected to usual in utero sensory inputs exhibit brief periods of behavioural arousal during at least the last 20 to 30 days of pregnancy, perceptual awareness appears for the first time only after birth. The reasons for this are suggested to be as follows. A major factor is the fetal and neonatal O2 status. Normal fetal arterial O2 tensions (PaO2, 20-27 mm Hg) would cause unconsciousness postnatally in mature lambs and adult sheep, moderate hypoxaemia (PaO 2, 40-60 mm Hg) inhibits arousal from sleep in very young lambs, normal fetal O2 tensions are associated with relatively low levels of behavioural arousal (about 5 minutes in every hour) in fetal lambs, and higher than normal fetal O 2 tensions increase the incidence of fetal behavioural arousal. Moreover, progesterone and its metabolites, buoyancy, cushioned tactile stimulation, warmth and a placental inhibitor apparently also act to suppress fetal behavioural arousal (Table 1). However, the removal or reversal of their effects before, during and immediately after birth (Table 1), together with an acute worsening of fetal hypoxaemia/hypercapnia (Comline and Silver 1972; Mellor and Pearson 1977; Berger et al 1990) and umbilical cord occlusion (Adamson et al 1987; 1991), are suggested to have the primary function of arousing the newborn lamb sufficiently for it to initiate pulmonary respiration immediately after birth. Although the newborn is aroused at this stage, it is not yet likely to be aware. It is postulated that only after the lamb has begun to breathe effectively and its arterial O2 tensions have risen significantly above fetal levels would it display the first signs of perceptual awareness. If true, the absence of awareness until after birth would indicate that the fetal lamb exposed to usual in utero sensory inputs cannot suffer during pregnancy or during birth, and that animal welfare compromise (suffering) can only be experienced postnatally once the lamb has become perceptually aware. Implications for fetal lambs during surgery Surgical procedures with the fetal lamb and ewe under general anaesthetic have usually been conducted between about 80 and 135 days of the 147-day pregnancy (e.g. Mellor 1980, 1984; Harding et al 1981; Clewlow et al 1983; Berger et al 1986; Richardson et al 1996; Nichol et al 1998; King and McCullagh 2001), with some done as early as 40 days of gestation (e.g. Berger et al 1997). Such surgery has involved catheterisation, instrumentation and/or tissue ablations of fetal lambs to allow them to be studied in conscious, unstressed ewes once the effects of the anaesthesia and surgery have passed. Small body size and tissue fragility are major limitations to studying very 9 young fetuses, and difficulty in manipulating large fetuses and a greater chance that surgery will induce premature birth when conducted after about 135 days of gestation usually set the upper age limit. Experienced fetal surgeons report that such invasive procedures do not cause withdrawal or other movements by fetal lambs from about 60 days of gestation or later provided that the general anaesthetics given to the ewe have had sufficient time to act on the fetus (PJ Berger, JE Harding, R Harding, DJ Mellor, AM Walker, DW Walker and IR Young, independent unpublished observations). In contrast, surgery on unanaesthetised fetuses in ewes given epidural anaesthesia, an approach used rarely during the last 30 years, elicits strong leg, trunk and/or neck movements, especially after about 120 days of gestation. These observations show that general anaesthesia of the ewe is effective in virtually eliminating behavioural responses of the fetus to surgical stimulation. Such behavioural responses to noxious stimuli in newborn and young lambs are considered to indicate the generation and awareness of pain (Mellor and Murray 1989; Kent et al 1998), but the situation in the fetus is not as clear. The fetal responses to putatively painful stimuli appear to be somewhat exaggerated, and a simple interpretation is that the fetal lamb is more sensitive to painful stimuli. However, this may not be the case. Instead, based on findings in rat pups, it is just as likely that the strong behavioural responses in the fetus occur because they have not yet been entrained under central inhibitory control (Fitzgerald, 1999). Thus, the exaggerated responses are probably a feature of immaturity of the central nervous system rather than enhanced pain perception. The capacity to feel pain can be inferred by the presence and activation of appropriate functional nerve pathways in combination with behavioural effects. Unfortunately knowledge in that area does not extend to quantitative assessment of pain, but the inference is that as the newborn lamb is capable of responding to undoubtedly painful stimuli (Mellor and Murray 1989), the mature fetal lamb would have the capacity for neurotransmission in pain pathways. This raises the question of whether or not the increased behavioural activity of unanaesthetised fetuses due to surgical stimulation indicates responsiveness without arousal, arousal or both arousal and awareness. In part, this will depend on the age of the fetus and its associated neurological development. If noxious surgical stimulation, which is not usually encountered in utero, were to induce a state of awareness, the fetus would presumably feel pain. It seems likely that fetal surgery applied to the hind- and fore-quarters, but not to the head, of the unanaesthetised fetal lamb before about 105 days, and possibly as late as about 120 days of gestation, may simply induce responsiveness without arousal. After about 120 days, however, arousal might be elicited. The reasons for proposing this are as follows. First, stimulation by light touch or stretch of hind limb receptors in fetal lambs elicits electrical discharges in dorsal root ganglia and/or dorsal horn cells of the spinal cord from about 75 days of gestation (Rees et al 1994a, b), and fibres projecting from dorsal horn cells, and presumably entering the spinothalamic tract, can carry signals at least as far as the thoracic spinal cord from about 105 days (Rees et al 1994a). Second, although it is not known at what fetal age sensory signals from the hind limb might reach the cerebral cortex (Rees et al 1994a), electrical activity in the somatosensory cortex can be elicited by sensory input from the forelimb from about 125 day of gestation (Cook et al 1987). Third, behavioural arousal in chronically instrumented, non-stimulated fetal lambs, well after recovery from the associated surgery, becomes evident only from about 120 days of gestation (Ruckebusch et al 1977; Clewlow et al 1987). In contrast to surgical stimulation of the body, stimulation of the face of unanaesthetised fetal lambs has the potential to elicit behavioural arousal earlier than 120 days of gestation, as electrical activity can be evoked in the somatosensory cortex by input from the nose and lips of the fetal lamb from about 70 days (exteriorised fetus; 10 Persson 1973) or 97 days (chronically instrumented fetus; Cook et al 1987). However, this could occur only if neurophysiological development within the brain prior to about 120 days were such that the noxious stimulation of surgery, not usually encountered in utero, could override any inhibition to behavioural arousal which might exist before it appears naturally in the non-stimulated fetus. Even if mature, unanaesthetised fetuses were to become both responsive and behaviourally aroused during surgery, it is not likely that they would become aware. Direct fetal surgery usually involves partial exposure of the uterus through an abdominal incision (with the mother anaesthetised), removal of some portion of the fetal fluids (returned later) and partial or complete exposure of the fetus through an incision in the uterine wall. Such exposure and manipulation of the uterus and fetus usually compromises uterine and/or umbilical blood flow and placental gas exchange and leads to increased fetal hypoxaemia and hypercapnia (Barcroft et al 1940; Barron and Meschia 1954; Meschia et al 1965; Comline and Silver 1970, 1972). This fetal hypoxaemia/hypercapnia becomes progressively worse the nearer to birth the uterine and fetal exposure occurs (Barcroft et al 1940; Barron and Meschia 1957), so any associated suppressive effects on fetal arousal and awareness (Table 1) would thereby be increased in older, more mature and otherwise potentially more arousable fetuses. It is also noteworthy that the bovine placenta and amniotic fluid contain a substance which promotes analgesia (Pinheiro Machado et al 1997). Its effect depends on a simultaneous elevation of opioid levels, and the substance, known as placental opioid enhancing factor, has not yet been characterised. Its actions have been examined in the mother, but not in the fetus and newborn where they may be more relevant. Notwithstanding this analysis, it is recommended that, in accord with common practice during the last 30 years, general anaesthesia of the mother and fetus be used both to minimise fetal movements (responsiveness) during surgery and to make sure that the fetus remains unaroused and unaware throughout. Note, however, that even while it is completely anaesthetised the fetus will exhibit physiological stress responses due to direct surgical stimulation, to cooling and to increased hypoxaemia/hypercapnia through compromised placental gas exchange (Jones 1977; Jones et al 1977; Gunn et al 1985, 1986). Such stress responses also occur in adult ewes during surgery conducted under general anaesthesia (Pearson and Mellor 1975) and do not indicate awareness. Implications for fetuses during slaughter of pregnant ewes Sheep are usually slaughtered by a neck cut that severs both carotid arteries and jugular veins. Although most commercial slaughter is preceded by electrical or captive-bolt stunning, most non-stunned sheep apparently experience profound brain dysfunction within 5 to 7 seconds (maximum 22 seconds) through a catastrophic decline in blood flow to the brain during exsanguination (Newhook and Blackmore 1982; Gregory and Wotton 1984). Fifty per cent of the blood that is voided following the neck cut is lost during the first 10 seconds, and this corresponds to about one third of total blood volume (Gregory and Wilkins 1984). Systemic blood pressure at this stage is substantially reduced. Indeed it is too low to allow the development of a bruise. This implies that peripheral resistance exceeds systemic pressure from about 10 seconds after the neck cut, and it is anticipated that blood flow in the uterus would cease near to this time. In pregnant ewes placental gas exchange would also stop and this would rapidly lead to very severe fetal hypoxaemia and hypercapnia and to a sudden marked reduction in O 2 delivery to the fetal brain (Jensen et al 1987). This is likely to cause a rapid flattening of the fetal ECoG and quickly eliminate any potential for behavioural arousal or awareness, because ECoG activity of fetal lambs is substantially depressed 11 within 1 minute of complete umbilical cord occlusion in utero (Mallard et al 1992) and becomes isoelectric when pregnant ewes breathe a gas mixture containing no O2 for about 5 minutes (Mann et al 1970). However, the rapid worsening of fetal hypoxaemia/hypercapnia after the throat cut of the ewe has the potential, initially, to stimulate behavioural arousal, especially in fetuses close to birth (Boddy et al 1974; Jansen et al 1982; Rigatto et al 1988), because the increased hypoxaemia/hypercapnia seen during normal vaginal birth and subsequent severance of the umbilical cord apparently does so (Berger et al 1990). Indeed, some near-birth fetal lambs delivered by hysterectomy, using epidural anaesthesia of the ewe, have been seen to lift their heads within 10 to 30 seconds while still inside the excised uterus (DJ Mellor, unpublished observations relating to Hart et al 1971). Although these fetal lambs were delivered immediately and successfully revived (the purpose was to produce live lambs), had they been left in the uterus their PaO2 would have continued to fall rapidly to levels incompatible with either behavioural arousal or awareness, because in fetal lambs in utero the PaO 2 falls to about 30 per cent of fetal normoxaemic levels within two minutes of complete umbilical cord occlusion (Mallard et al 1992). Thus, although some near-birth fetal lambs may initially exhibit behavioural arousal soon after the throat cut in slaughtered pregnant ewes, this is likely to be short-lived (1 to 2 minutes at most) and probably would not be accompanied by awareness, because the PaO2 would remain well below the suggested threshold for awareness (Brierly et al 1980; West et al 1984; Hattingh et al 1986). As the key features of the physiology of fetal lambs, calves and other ruminants are likely to be similar in these respects, slaughter of pregnant ruminants, whether near to birth or not, is not likely to cause their fetuses any suffering prior to death in utero. Implications for collection of fetal calf blood (serum) and other tissues at slaughter Collection of fetal calf blood (serum) occurs in some processing plants after the pregnant uterus has been removed from the cow at the evisceration stage, which usually occurs no sooner than 20 to 30 minutes after the neck cut in Australia and New Zealand. This, plus veterinary inspection, means that blood (serum) collection usually does not begin earlier than 25-40 minutes after slaughter of the cow. Two main collection methods, or variations of them, are used. After partial or complete removal of the calf from the uterus, either (i) a 12 to 16 gauge needle attached to a tube is inserted between the 4th and 5th ribs into the fetal heart and blood is collected into bottles under vacuum until no more flows, or (ii) the fetus, suspended from an A-frame, has a device which simulates a pumping action placed over the thorax and blood is collected from the unclamped umbilical cord until flow stops. The usual practice of collecting blood (serum) at least 20 minutes after slaughter of the cow ensures that the fetuses experience prolonged cerebral anoxia, would be unaware and therefore could not suffer during the process. Manipulating calf or lamb fetuses from 5 to 6 minutes after slaughter of the dam is usually not undertaken in commercial processing plants in Australia and New Zealand, but may occur in some plants elsewhere (Jochems et al 2002). However, this could be done humanely provided that particular precautions are taken. The observations on sheep slaughter noted above, and the observation on a small number of cows that 6 to 8 minutes of umbilical cord occlusion in utero is sufficient to cause subsequent death in fetal calves (Dufty and Sloss 1977), indicate that provided at least 5 to 6 minutes elapse after the neck cut of the dam, the fetus would be severely hypoxaemic/hypercapnic, would have a flat ECoG and would be unaware when removed from the uterus. However, 12 its heart may still be beating at this early stage and intermittent gasping may occur, so that near-birth fetuses which are still alive when removed from the uterus soon after slaughter of the dam have the potential to start breathing and become aware. There are two simple ways to prevent this. The fetus’s head can be retained inside part of the amniotic sac or the trachea can be clamped so that gasping cannot inflate the lungs, thereby ensuring that the fetus remains unaware and incapable of suffering. Stunning the fetus with a captive bolt has also been suggested (Jochems et al 2002). Collection of other fetal tissues, including the hide, should be done only after the fetus is dead. Processing can be delayed until then. Alternatively, if the heart is still beating when the fetus is exposed, cutting its throat immediately would guarantee that it remained unaware before death. Implications for undertaking fetotomy It is often difficult to tell whether an undelivered fetus requiring fetotomy is still alive. A straightforward method, especially for posterior and breach presentations where the umbilical cord is usually accessible, is to feel for a pulse in the cord. Note however that the presence of a pulse provides little information about fetal O 2 status. Depending on whether or not the required fetal parts are accessible, withdrawal responses can sometimes be elicited in living fetuses by strongly pinching the tongue, a lip or the anus or by applying strong pressure to the supraorbital ridge of an eye socket. Responses to these stimuli are interpreted by some veterinarians as indicating fetal awareness, but if great pressure is required to achieve a response it is more likely that deep reflexes are being elicited in unaroused fetuses which have become severely hypoxaemic during the protracted labour that usually precedes the decision to undertake a fetotomy. Fetuses that respond to moderate tongue, lip, anal or supraorbital stimulation, or that withdraw a leg in response to it being pulled or to pedal reflex stimulation, or “chew” or suck on a finger placed in the mouth, are likely to be less hypoxaemic and are therefore potentially more arousable by noxious stimulation. Where an undelivered fetus requiring fetotomy is still alive and the umbilical cord can be reached via the vagina, the cord should be severed manually. Such a fetus would not be arousable to an aware state while still in utero from 2 to 3 minutes after cord severance (Mallard et al 1992), so, allowing a safety margin and also for the attainment of an isoelectric EEG (Mann et al 1970), fetotomy could be conducted humanely from 5 to 6 minutes after cord severance. Cord severance may be more easily accomplished with fetal lambs than fetal calves because of the longer reach required in the latter. If the cord cannot be severed, other strategies should be attempted to ensure that the fetus is not aware, or to kill it, before fetotomy is conducted. When the head and neck are delivered but the shoulders are jammed, the fetus can be killed by a throat cut and exsanguination before fetotomy. Where the head is not delivered - for instance, when the fetus is in a “head-back” position with its neck and/or a shoulder presented to the cervix decapitation using an embryotomy wire, which is achievable within about 30 seconds once the wire is correctly placed, would kill the fetus. Depending on its O2 status, the initial cutting of the neck required to sever the carotid and vertebral arteries may elicit fetal behavioural arousal, but, if this occurred, it would be expected to be very brief. When the presentation of the fetus at the cervix will permit injection of agents like pentobarbitone, ketamine or xylazine, sufficient time should be given for them to act before fetotomy. Making such an injection can be very difficult when the uterus is tightly contracted around a fetus which is awkwardly positioned in relation to the cervix, and can lead to practitioners injecting themselves or the cow. Provided they can be done safely, such injections 13 should be attempted, especially when a throat cut or decapitation is not possible before cutting away another body part, and best practice would dictate that they should precede a throat cut and decapitation as well. Fetotomy is sometimes required when an almost fully delivered, breathing and aware calf has its hind quarters wedged in the dam’s pelvis. Such young must be killed before fetotomy, preferably using the best practice approach just noted. Discussion From the evidence outlined in this review, one can piece together the factors that bring about behavioural arousal and awareness when a lamb is born. During the lead-up to parturition, the fetus is exposed to progressively lower circulating levels of progesterone and its metabolites and to an acute rise in oestrogen concentrations. These changes act to enhance behavioural arousal, which, at the time of delivery, is provoked by five factors. These factors are physical stimulation of the face and ears as the fetus passes along the birth canal, severance of the umbilical cord, hypoxaemia/hypercapnia arising from cord severance, sudden loss of an arousal-inhibiting factor normally released by the placenta, and eventually, increased oxygenation brought about by the onset of breathing following delivery. It is likely that a crucial prerequisite of awareness is the increased oxygenation that occurs once the newborn starts to breathe air. The normal levels of O2 in the circulations of the fetal lamb and of the newborn before it starts to breathe air, are apparently below those required to support awareness in the neonate and adult. This means that, under normal circumstances, it is improbable that the fetus could experience perceptual awareness before it is delivered. If so, it is only after the onset of breathing that the brain would receive blood that contains sufficient O 2 to support awareness. However, there is no direct evidence which proves that fetuses remain unaware and unable to experience pain throughout pregnancy. It can be argued that the greater capacity of fetal as opposed to adult haemoglobin to deliver O2 to tissues at low O2 tensions (Meschia et al 1961), the higher haematocrit (Meschia et al 1965) and the higher rate of blood perfusion through fetal tissues (Rudolph et al 1981; Rudolph 1984) may be sufficient to support fetal awareness, or it may confer on the fetus the capacity to be aroused to an aware state during noxious (e.g. surgical) stimulation. It would be difficult to test this. Nevertheless, it would be helpful in the future to test for responsiveness in cortical regions of the brain associated with perceptual awareness, using neural imaging techniques. This could add to our understanding of the likelihood of fetal perceptual awareness. Physical activity in a fetus can be disconcerting when it occurs in response to manipulation of the pregnant animal, for example during surgery or slaughter. However, provided the mother has not been super-oxygenated, and provided the fetus is prevented from breathing air, the analysis in this review suggests that physical activity is not a major cause for concern about the welfare of fetuses. This is especially so when the manipulation is associated with marked fetal hypoxaemia. At slaughter, the rapid onset of cerebral hypoxia/anoxia in the fetus is terminal and ensures that fetuses do not suffer, whether or not their blood (serum) is collected. On the other hand, where the aim is to deliver live newborns, it is important that normal breathing be established as soon as possible after the umbilical cord has been severed. Some slaughtermen occasionally try to salvage fetuses on the slaughterboard, which they 14 hope to hand-rear. This practice carries risks, as the period of hypoxaemia experienced by the fetus before it is taken from the uterus of the dead mother may be too protracted to permit it to survive for long. In situations where a fetotomy has to be conducted, the primary concern should be the survival and welfare of the dam. Some practitioners favour performing a Caesarean section over a fetotomy when the fetus is still alive. Bearing in mind that the reasoned conclusion from this review is that the fetus is not likely to be in a position to suffer until it breathes air, especially if it is severely hypoxaemic, it is suggested that the decision to do a Caesarean or a fetotomy should be based primarily on welfare considerations for the dam rather than the fetus. In particular, the focus should be on the post-operative pain and discomfort the dam is likely to experience following either procedure. An additional factor is the practitioner’s skill with both procedures, the welfare consequences of a well executed fetotomy generally being preferable to those of a badly completed Caesarean, and a Caesarean done well being preferable to a poorly executed fetotomy. In general terms, sheep are precocial animals at birth. The newborn lamb is behaviourally and neurologically welladvanced, and this provides it with survival advantages. The concepts about the physiology of the development of behavioural arousal and awareness described in this review are also likely to apply to other livestock species, and in particular to those which are at a comparable stage of neural development by the end of pregnancy. Acknowledgements We are particularly grateful to the following people for helpful discussion during the long gestation period of this review: Alistair Gunn and Jane Harding (Auckland University), Carina Mallard and Sandra Rees (Melbourne University), Philip Berger, Richard Harding, Adrian Walker, David Walker and Ross Young (Monash University), Howard Tyler (Iowa State University), Cheryl McMeekan, Kevin Stafford and Jos Vermunt (Massey University), and David Bayvel (Ministry of Agriculture and Forestry). We also thank MAF for financial support. References Adamson SL, Richardson BS, Homan J. Initiation of pulmonary gas exchange by fetal sheep in utero. Journal of Applied Physiology 62, 989-98, 1987 Adamson SL, Kuipers IM, Olson DM. Umbilical cord occlusion stimulates breathing independent of blood gases and pH. Journal of Applied Physiology 70, 1796-1809, 1991 Akerstedt T, Hume K, Minors D, Waterhouse J. Experimental separation of time of day and homeostatic influences on sleep. American Journal of Physiology: Regulation, integrative and Comparative Physiology 274, R1162-8, 1998 Alexander G. Husbandry practices in relation to maternal and offspring behaviour. In: Wodzicka-Tomaszewska M, Edey TN, Lynch JJ (eds). Behaviour – Reviews in Rural Science, Behaviour in Relation to Reproduction, Management and Welfare of Farm Animals 4, 99-107. University of New England Press, Armidale, 1980 15 Alvaro R, de Almeida V, Al-Alaiyan S, Robertson M, Nowacsyk B, Cates D, Rigatto H. A placental extract inhibits breathing induced by umbilical cord occlusion in fetal sheep. Journal of Developmental Physiology 19, 23-8, 1993 Baars BJ. There are no known differences in brain mechanisms of consciousness between humans and other mammals. Animal Welfare 10, S31-40, 2001 Baier RJ, Hasan SU, Cates DB, Hooper B, Nowaczyk B, Rigatto H. Effects of various concentrations of O2 and umbilical cord occlusion on fetal breathing and behavior. Journal of Applied Physiology 68, 1597-1604, 1990 Barcroft J, Barron DH. Movements in midfoetal life in the sheep embryo. Journal of Physiology 91, 329-51, 1937 Barcroft J, Barron DH. The development of behaviour in foetal sheep. Journal of Comparative Neurology 70, 477-502, 1939 Barcroft J, Kennedy JA, Mason MF. Oxygen in the blood of the umbilical vessels of sheep. Journal of Physiology 97, 347-56,1940 Barlow RM. The fetal sheep: morphogenesis of the nervous system and histochemical aspects of myelination. Journal of Comparative Pathology 135, 249-62, 1969 Bassett JM, Oxborrow TJ, Smith ID, Thorburn GD. The concentration of progesterone in the peripheral plasma of the pregnant ewe. Journal of Endocrinology 45, 449-57, 1969 Barron DH, Meschia G. The carbon dioxide gradient between the foetal and maternal bloods of sheep and goats. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine 29, 480-95, 1957 Berger PJ, Walker AM, Horne R, Brodecky V, Wilkinson MH, Wilson F, Maloney JE. Phasic respiratory activity in the fetal lamb during late gestation and labour. Respiratory Physiology 65, 55-68, 1986 Berger PJ, Horne RSC, Soust M, Walker AM, Maloney JE. Breathing at birth and the associated blood gas and pH changes in the lamb. Respiratory Physiology 82, 251-66, 1990 Berger PJ, Kyriakides MA, Cooke IRC. Supraspinal influence on the development of motor behavior in the fetal lamb. Journal of Neurobiology 33, 276-88, 1997 Bernhard, C.G. and Meyerson, B.A. Morphological and physiological aspects of the development of recipient functions in the cerebral cortex. In: R.S. Comline, K.W. Cross, G.S. Dawes and P.W. Nathanielsz (eds). Fetal and Neonatal Physiology. Pp 1-19. Cambridge University Press, 1973 Bocking AD, Harding R. Effects of reduced uterine blood flow on electrocortical activity, breathing, and skeletal muscle activity in fetal sheep. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 154, 655-62, 1986 Bocking AD, Gagnon R, Milne KM, White SE. Behavioural activity during prolonged hypoxemia in fetal sheep. Journal of Applied Physiology 65, 2420-6, 1988 Boddy K, Dawes GS, Fisher R, Pinter S, Robinson JS. Foetal respiratory movements, electrocortical and cardiovascular responses to hypoxaemia and hypercapnia in sheep. Journal of Physiology 243, 599-618, 1974 Brierly JB, Prior DF, Calverly J, Jackson SJ, Brown AW. The pathogenesis of ischaemic neuronal damage along the cerebral arterial boundary zone in Papio anubis. Brain 103, 929-45, 1980 Challis JRG, Patrick JE. Fetal and maternal oestrogen concentrations throughout pregnancy in sheep. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology 59,970-8, 1981 Clapp JF, Peress NS, Wesley M, Mann LI. Brain damage after intermittent partial cord occlusion in the chronically instrumented fetal lamb. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 159, 504-9, 1988 Clewlow F, Dawes GS, Johnston BM, Walker DW. Changes in breathing, electrocortical and muscle activity in the unanaesthetized fetal lamb with age. Journal of Physiology 341, 463-76, 1983 16 Comline RS, Silver M. Daily changes in foetal and maternal blood of conscious pregnant ewes with catheters in umbilical and uterine vessels. Journal of Physiology 209, 567-86, 1970 Comline RS, Silver M. The composition of foetal and maternal blood during parturition in the ewe. Journal of Physiology 222, 233-56, 1972 Cook CJ, Gluckman PD, Johnston BM, Williams C. The development of the somatosensory evoked potentials in the unanaesthetised fetal sheep. Journal of Developmental Physiology 307, 335-53, 1987 Crenshaw MC, Meschia G, Barron, DH. Role of progresterone in inhibition of muscle tone and respiratory rhythm in foetal lambs. Nature 212, 842, 1966 Crossley KJ, Nicol MB, Hurst JJ, Walker DW, Thorburn GD. Suppression of arousal by progesterone in fetal sheep. Reproduction Fertility and Development 9, 767-73, 1997 Dawes GS (1988). The 1987 James A.F. Stevenson Memorial Lecture. The development of fetal behavioural patterns. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology 66, 541-8, 1988 Dawes GS, Fox HE, Richards RT. Variations in asphyxial gasping with age in lambs and guinea-pigs. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology 57, 131-8, 1972a Dawes GS, Fox HE, Leduc BM, Liggins GC, Richards RT. Respiratory movements and rapid eye movement sleep in the foetal lamb. Journal of Physiology 220, 119-43, 1972b de Haan HH, Gunn AJ, Gluckman PD. Experiments in perinatal medicine: what have we learnt? Prenatal and Neonatal Medicine 1, 16-25, 1996 Dolling M, Seamark RF. Progesterone metabolites in fetal sheep plasma: the effect of nephrectomy. Journal of Developmental Physiology 1, 399-413, 1979 Dufty JH, Sloss V. Anoxia in the bovine foetus. Australian Veterinary Journal 53, 262-7, 1977 Eales FA, Small J. Summit metabolism in newborn lambs. Research in Veterinary Science 29, 211-8, 1980 Eales FA, Small J. Effects of acute hypoxia on heat production capacity and summit metabolism in newborn lambs. Research in Veterinary Science 39, 212-15, 1985 Eales FA, Small J. Practical lambing and lamb care - a veterinary guide (2nd edition). Longman Group Ltd, 1995 Eisele JH, Eger EI, Muallem M. Narcotic properties of carbon dioxide in the dog. Anesthesiology 28, 856-65, 1967 Empson J. Sleep and Dreaming (2nd Ed.). Pp 22-39. Harvester Wheatsheaf, New York, 1993 Endo T, Roth C, Landolt H-P, Werth E, Aeschbach D, Achermann P, Borbely AA. Selective REM sleep deprivation in humans: effects on sleep and sleep EEG. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 274, R1186-94, 1998 Fewell JE, Konduri GG. Repeated exposure to rapidly developing hypoxemia influences the interaction between oxygen and carbon dioxide in initiating arousal from sleep in lambs. Pediatric Research 24, 28-33, 1988 Fewell JE, Konduri GG. Influence of repeated exposure to rapidly developing hypoxaemia on the arousal and cardiopulmonary response to developing hypoxaemia in lambs. Journal of Developmental Physiology 11, 77-82, 1989 Fitzgerald M. Development of somatosensory function. In: Eds. Gluckman PD, Heyman MA. Pediatrics and Perinatology (2nd edition). Pp 379-82. Arnold (Hodder Headline Group), Auckland, 1996 Fitzgerald M. Development and neurobiology of pain. In: Wall PD, Melzack R. (eds). Textbook of Pain. (4th edition). Pp 235-51. Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, 1999 Fraser AF. The phenomenon of pandiculation in the kinetic behaviour of the sheep fetus. Behaviour Science 24, 169-82, 1989 17 Applied Animal Fraser AF, Broom DM. Farm animal behaviour and welfare. Pp 198-207, 227-38, 247-55. Beilliere Tindall, London, 1990 Gluckman PD, Gunn TR, Johnston BM, Quinn JP. Manipulation of the temperature of the fetal lamb in utero. In: Nathanielsz PW (ed). Animal Models in Fetal Medicine (IV). Pp 37-56. Perinatology Press, Ithaca, New York, 1984 Gluckman PD, Johnston BM, Nathanielsz PW. (eds). Advances in fetal physiology: reviews in honour of G.C. Liggins. Advances in Perinatal Medicine (VII). Pp 1-420. Perinatology Press, Ithaca, New York, 1989 Gluckman PD, Gunn TR, Johnston BM. The effect of cooling on breathing and shivering in unanaesthetised fetal lambs in utero. Journal of Physiology 343, 495-506, 1993 Gregory NG, Wilkins LJ. Effect of cardiac arrest on susceptibility to carcass bruising in sheep. Journal of Science of Food and Agriculture 35, 671-6, 1984 Gregory NG, Wotton SB. Sheep slaughtering procedures. II. Time to loss of brain responsiveness after exsanguination or cardiac arrest. British Veterinary Journal 140, 354-60, 1984 Gunn AJ, Parer JT, Mallard EC, Williams CE, Gluckman PD. Cerebral histologic and electrocorticographic changes after asphyxia in fetal sheep. Pediatric Research 31, 486-91, 1992 Gunn AJ, Gluckman PD, Gunn TR. Selective head cooling in newborn infants after perinatal asphyxia: a safety study. Pediatrics 102, 885-902, 1998 Gunn TR, Johnston BM, Iwamoto HS, Fraser M, Nicholls MG, Gluckman PD. Haemodynamic and catecholamine responses to hypothermia in the fetal sheep in utero. Journal of Developmental Physiology 7, 241-9, 1985 Gunn TR, Butler J, Gluckman PD. Metabolic and hormonal responses to cooling the fetal sheep in utero. Journal of Developmental Physiology 8, 55-66, 1986 Gunn TR, Ball KT, Gluckman PD. Reversible umbilical cord occlusion: Effects on thermogenesis in utero. Pediatric Research 30, 513-7, 1991 Harding JE, Jones CT, Robinson JS. Studies on experimental growth retardation in sheep. The effects of a small placenta in restricting transport to and growth of the fetus. Journal of Developmental Physiology 7, 427-42, 1985 Harding R, Poore ER, Cohen GL. The effect of brief episodes of diminished uterine blood flow on breathing movements, sleep states and heart rate in fetal sheep. Journal of Developmental Physiology 3, 231-43, 1981 Hart R, Mackay JMK, McVittie CR, Mellor DJ. A technique for the derivation of lambs by hysterectomy. British Veterinary Journal 127, 419-24, 1971 Hasan SU, Rigaux A. The effects of lung distension, oxygenation, and gestational age on fetal behavior and breathing movements in sheep. Pediatric Research 30, 193-201, 1991 Hattingh J, Cornelius ST, Ganhao MF, Fonseca F. Arterial blood gas composition, consciousness and death in rabbits. Journal of the South African Veterinary Association 57, 13-16, 1986 Hirst JJ, Egodagamage KC, Walker DW. Effect of neuroactive steroid infused into the cerebral ventricles of fetal sheep in utero using small infusion volumes. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 97, 37-44, 2000 Horne RSC, Berger PJ, Bowes G, Walker AM. Effect of sinoaortic denervation on arousal responses to hypotension in newborn lambs. American Journal of Physiology 256, H434-40, 1989 Jansen AH, Ioffe S, Russell BJ, Chernick V. Influence of sleep state on the response to hypercapnia in fetal lambs. Respiratory Physiology 48, 125-42, 1982 Jensen A, Hohmann M, Kunzel W. Dynamic changes in organ blood flow and oxygen consumption during acute asphyxia in fetal sheep. Journal of Developmental Physiology 9, 41-55, 1987 18 Jensen A, Roman Ch, Rudolph AM. Effect of reducing uterine blood flow on fetal blood flow distribution and oxygen delivery. Journal of Developmental Physiology 15, 309-23, 1991 Jochems, CEA, ven der Valk JBF, Stafleu FR and Baumans V. The use of fetal bovine serum: ethical or scientific problem? Alternatives to Laboratory Animals 30, 219-227, 2002 Johnston RV, Grant DA, Wilkinson MH, Walker AM. Repetitive hypoxia depresses arousal from active sleep in newborn lambs. Journal of Physiology 510, 651-9, 1998 Jones CT. The development of some metabolic responses to hypoxia in the foetal sheep. Journal of Physiology 265, 743-62, 1977 Jones CT, Boddy K, Robinson JS, Ratcliffe JG. Developmental changes in the responses of the adrenal glands of foetal sheep to endogenous adrenocorticotrophin, as indicated by hormone responses to hypoxaemia. Journal of Endocrinology 72, 279-92, 1977 Kent JE, Molony V, Graham MJ. Comparison of methods for the reduction of acute pain produced by rubber ring castration or tail docking of week-old lambs. The Veterinary Journal 155, 39-51, 1998 King KJ, McCullagh P. Splenectomy of the fetal lamb early in development as a model of congenital asplenia. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Surgery 71, 41-5, 2001 Kirkwood JK, Hubrecht RC, Wickens S, O’Leary H, Oakeley S (eds.) Consciousness, cognition and animal welfare. Animal Welfare 10 Suppl, S1-251, 2001 Ladds PW, Summers PM, Humphrey JD. Pregnancy in slaughtered cows in north-eastern Australia. Australian Veterinary Journal 51, 472-7, 1975 Landolt H-P, Raimo EB, Schierow BJ, Kelsoe JR, Rapaport MH, Gillin JC. Sleep and sleep electroencephalogram in depressed patients treated with phenelzine. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 26876, 2001 Lehman H Animal awareness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 57, 315-25, 1998 Liggins GC, Faircough RT, Grieves SA, Kendall JZ, Knox BS. The mechanism of initiation of parturition in the ewe. Recent Progress in Hormone Research 29, 111- 59, 1973 Lynch JJ, Hinch GN, Adams DB. The behaviour of the lamb. In: The Behaviour of Sheep: Biological Principles and Implications for Production. Pp 153-77. CAB International and CSIRO Australia, 1992 Mallard EC, Gunn AJ, Williams CE, Johnston BM, Gluckman PD. Transient umbilical cord occlusion causes hippocampal damage in the fetal sheep. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 167, 1423-30, 1992 Mann LI, Prichard JW, Symmes D. EEG, ECG, and acid-base observations during acute fetal hypoxia. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 106, 39-51, 1970 Mattsson JL, Stinson JM, Clark CS. Electroencephalographic power-spectral changes coincident with onset of carbon dioxide narcosis in rhesus monkey. American Journal of Veterinary Research 33, 2043-9, 1972 Mellor DJ. Vascular anastomosis and fusion of foetal membranes in multiple pregnancy in sheep. Research in Veterinary Science 10, 361-367, 1969 Mellor DJ. Investigations of the fluid spaces of the sheep conceptus. In: Nathanielsz PW (ed). Animal Models in Fetal Medicine (I). Pp 59-106. Elsevier/North Holland, 1980 Mellor DJ. Nutritional and placental determinants of fetal growth rate in sheep and consequences for the newborn lamb. British Veterinary Journal 139, 307-24, 1983 Mellor DJ. Investigations of fetal growth in sheep. In: Nathanielsz PW (ed). Animal Models in Fetal Medicine (IV). Pp 149-73. Perinatology Press, Ithaca, New York, 1984 Mellor DJ. Feeding pregnant ewes and newborn lambs during experiment. In: Nathanielsz PW (ed). Animal Models in Fetal Medicine (VI). Pp 55-92. Perinatology Press, Ithaca, New York, 1987 19 Mellor DJ. Integration of perinatal events, pathophysiological changes and consequences for the newborn lamb. British Veterinary Journal 144, 552-69, 1988 Mellor DJ, Cockburn F. A comparison of energy metabolism in the newborn infant, piglet and lamb. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology 71, 361-79, 1986 Mellor DJ, Murray L. Effects of tail docking and castration on behaviour and plasma cortisol concentrations in young lambs. Research in Veterinary Science 46, 387-91, 1989 Mellor DJ, Pearson RA. Some changes in the composition of blood during the first 24 hours after birth in normal and growth retarded lambs. Annales de Recherches Veterinaires 8, 460-7, 1977 Mellor DJ, Slater JS. Daily changes in amniotic and allantoic fluid during the last three months of pregnancy in conscious, unstressed ewes with catheters in their foetal fluid sacs. Journal of Physiology 217, 573-604, 1971 Mellor DJ, Stafford KJ. Animal welfare implications of neonatal mortality and morbidity in farm animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science (submitted). Mellor DJ, Mackay JMK, Williams JT. Effects of oestrogen on activity and survival of lambs delivered by hysterectomy. Research in Veterinary Science 13, 399-401, 1972 Meschia G, Hellegers A, Blechner JN, Wolkoff AS, Barron DH. A comparison of the oxygen dissociation curves of the bloods of maternal, foetal and newborn sheep at various pHs. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology 46, 95-100, 1961 Meschia G, Cotter JR, Breathnach CS, Barron DH. The haemoglobin, oxygen, carbon dioxide and hydrogen ion concentrations in the umbilical bloods of sheep and goats as sampled via plastic indwelling catheters. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology 50, 185-95, 1965 Mohan Raj AB, Wotton SB, Gregory NG. Changes in the somatosensory evoked potentials and spontaneous electroencephalogram of hens during stunning with a carbon dioxide and argon mixture. British Veterinary Journal 148, 147-56, 1992 Mott JC. Ability of young animals to withstand total oxygen lack. British Medical Bulletin 17, 144-8, 1961 Newhook JC, Blackmore DK. Elecetroencephalographic studies of stunning and slaughter of sheep and calves: Part 1 – The onset of permanent insensibility in sheep during slaughter. Meat Science 6, 221-33, 1982 Nicol MB, Hurst JJ, Walker DW, Thorburn GD. Effect of alteration of maternal plasma progesterone concentrations on fetal behavioural state during late gestation. Journal of Endocrinology 152, 379-86, 1997 Nicol MB, Hurst JJ, Walker DW. Effect of pregnane steroids on electrocortical activity and somatosensory evoked potentials in fetal sheep. Neuroscience Letters 253, 111-4, 1998 Nitsos I, Rees S. Development of immunoreactivity for calcitonin gene-related peptide, substance P and glutamate in primary sensory neurons, and for serotonin in the spinal cord of fetal sheep. Neuroscience 54, 239-52, 1993 Nitsos I, Sexton PM, Rees S. The ontogeny of [125I]rat--CGRP binding sites in the spinal cord of sheep: a prenatal and postnatal study. Neuroscience 62, 257-64, 1994 Pallis C. Reappraising death. British Medical Journal 285: 1409-12, 1982 Paul SM, Purdy RH. Neuroactive steroids. Federation of American Society of Experimental Biology Journal 6, 2311-22, 1992 Pearson RA, Mellor DJ. Some physiological changes in pregnant sheep and goats before, during and after surgical insertion of uterine catheters. Research in Veterinary Science 19, 102-4, 1975 Persson HE. Functional development in the somatosensory cortex of foetal sheep. In: Comline RS, Cross KW, Dawes GS, Nathanielsz PW (Eds.). Fetal and Neonatal Physiology. Pp 20-7. Cambridge University Press, 1973 20 Piggins D, Phillips CJC. Awareness in domesticated animals – concepts and definitions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 57, 181-200, 1998 Pinheiro Machado FLC, Hurnik JF, Burton JH. The effect of amniotic fluid ingestion on the nociception of cows. Physiology & Behavior 62,1339-44, 1997 Power SGA, Patrick JE, Carson GD, Challis JRG. The fetal membranes as a possible source of progesterone in the amniotic and allantoic fluids of pregnant sheep. Endocrinology 110, 481-6, 1982 Rees S, Nitsos I, Rawson J. The development of cutaneous afferent pathways in fetal sheep: a structural and functional study. Brain Research 661, 207-22, 1994a Rees S, Rawson J, Nitsos I, Brumley C. The structural and functional development of muscle spindles and their connections in fetal sheep. Brain Research 642, 185-98, 1994b Richardson BS, Carmichael L, Homan J, Johnston L, Gagnon R. Fetal cerebral, circulatory, and metabolic responses during heart rate decelerations with umbilical cord compression. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 175, 929-36, 1996 Richardson BS, Carmichael L, Homan J, Patrick JE. Electrocortical activity, electroocular activity, and breathing movements in fetal sheep with prolonged and graded hypoxemia. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 167, 553-8, 1992 Rigatto H, Blanco CE, Walker DW. The response to stimulation of hindlimb nerves in fetal sheep, in utero, during the different phases of electrocortical activity. Journal of Developmental Physiology 4, 175-85, 1982 Rigatto H, Lee D, Davi M, Moore M, Rigatto E, Cates D. Effect of increased arterial CO2 on fetal breathing and behaviour in sheep. Journal of Applied Physiology 64, 982-7, 1988 Robinson JS, Kingston EJ, Jones CT, Thorburn GD. Studies on experimental growth retardation in sheep. The effect of removal of endometrial caruncles on fetal size and metabolism. Journal of Developmental Physiology 1, 379-98, 1979 Robinson JS, Jones CT, Kingston EJ. Studies on experimental growth retardation in sheep. The effects of maternal hypoxaemia. Journal of Developmental Physiology 5, 89-100, 1983 Ruckebusch Y, Gaujoux M, Eghbali B. Sleep cycles and kinesis in the fetal lamb. Electroencephalographic & Clinical Neurophysiology 42, 226-37, 1977 Rudolph AM. The fetal circulation and its response to stress. Journal of Developmental Physiology 6, 11-19, 1984 Rudolph AM, Itskovitz J, Iwamoto H. Reuss ML, Heymann M. Fetal cardiovascular responses to stress. Seminars in Perinatology 5, 109-21, 1981 Seamark RF, Nancarrow CD, Gardner J. Progesterone metabolism in ovine fetal blood: the formation of 3hydroxy-pregn-4-en-20-one and other substances. Steroids 15, 589-603, 1970 Sieker HO, Hickam JB. Carbon dioxide intoxication: the clinical syndrome, its etiology and management with particular reference to the use of mechanical respirators. Medicine 35, 389-423, 1956 Silver M, Steven BH, Comline RS. Placental exchange and morphology in ruminants and mare. In: Comline RS, Cross KW, Dawes GS, Nathanielsz PW (Eds.). Fetal and Neonatal Physiology. Pp 245-71. Cambridge University Press, 1973 Slee J, Springbett A. Early postnatal behaviour in lambs of ten breeds. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15, 22940, 1986 Sommerville BA, Broom DM. Olfactory awareness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 57, 269-86, 1998 Thorburn GD, Challis JRG. Endocrine control of parturition. Physiological Reviews 59, 877-88, 1979 Tyler H and Ramsey H. Hypoxia in neonatal calves: effect on selected metabolic parameters. Journal of Dairy Science 74, 1957-1962, 1991 21 Villiger JW, Taylor KM, Gluckman PD. Multiple benzodiazapine receptors in the ovine brain: ontogeny, properties, and distribution of 3H-diazapam binding. Paediatric Pharmacology 2, 179-87, 1982 Walker AM, Carroll J, de Preu ND, Horne RSC. Modification of arousal responses following hypoxia in newborn lambs. In: Walker AM, McMillan C, National SIDS Council (eds). Second SIDS International Conference and First SIDS Global Strategy Meeting. Pp. 238-42. Perinatology Press, New York, 1993 West JB, Hackett PH, Maret KH, Milledge JS, Peters RM, Pizzo CJ, Winslow RM. Pulmonary gas exchange on the summit of Mt. Everest. Journal of Applied Physiology 55, 678-87, 1984 22 Table 1: Summary of Suppressors and Activators of Fetal Behavioural Arousal Suppressors fetal blood oxygen and/or blood carbon dioxide levels. fetal blood levels of progesterone and progesterone metabolites. fetal body and skin temperature – warmth. fetal somatosensory stimulation. placental inhibitor of arousal and breathing. Activators fetal and newborn blood oxygen levels. fetal and newborn blood levels of oestrogens. fetal and newborn skin temperature activating cold thermoreceptors. fetal and newborn somatosensory stimulation (tactile, gravitational). 23