6 An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception Dennis R. Proffitt People have conjectured about spatial perception for millennia, and have studied it in earnest for well over a hundred years. As can be seen in current perception textbooks, spatial perception is typically viewed as a general-purpose representation of the environment’s layout. The perception of surface layout is generally thought to be unaffected by people’s bodies, what they might be doing, or their internal physiological states. Textbooks divide spatial perception into topics defined by distal environmental properties such as the perception of distance, size, and shape. In this view, spatial perception is specific to the environmental properties that are perceived. This chapter provides a different approach. Extending Gibson’s (1979) theoretical approach, spatial perception is here viewed as a biological adaptation that supports our species’ ways of life. From this perspective, spatial perception is action-specific and the subdivision of the field is made relative to the actions that spatial perception supports, such as reaching, grasping, walking, and throwing. Perception relates spatial layout to one’s abilities to perform intended actions and also to the inherent costs associated with their performance. In essence, it is proposed that people see the world as “reachers,” “graspers,” “walkers,” and so forth. By this account, perception relates and is influenced by three factors: the visually specified environment, the body, and purpose. 177 RT20371_C006.indd 177 12/11/2007 11:51:19 AM 178 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action The Visual Specification of the Environment For a moving observer in a natural setting, the environment’s spatial layout is well specified by optical and ocular-motor variables (Proffitt & Caudek, 2002; Sedgwick, 1986). Viewed in isolation, a great deal is known about the human sensitivity to each of the myriad of visual variables that specify environmental properties. On the other hand, understanding how these variables are combined when information is redundant—the problem of cue integration—has proven to be a tough problem to solve. There are almost no studies that attempt to model the combination of more than two variables. This is because, as the number of specifying variables increases, the number of possible combinations of these variables that would need to be investigated becomes prohibitively large (Cutting & Vishton, 1995). The problem of cue integration has relevance for the current argument because of the possibility that the combination of visual information is action-specific. Most current models of cue integration rely upon some variant of weighted averaging in which each specifying variable is used to derive an estimate of the relevant environmental property, each estimate is then weighted by its prior reliability, and then a weighted average is taken (cf. Landy, Maloney, Johnston, & Young, 1995). An especially intriguing alternative has been proposed by Domini, Caudek, and Tassinari (2006). In their model, information is combined directly without first deriving the environmental property to which it relates. In contrast to weighted averaging models, in Domini et al.’s model, environmental properties are derived only after the information has been combined. The intractability of cue integration in natural environments bears upon a fundamental issue. Currently, it is not known whether cue integration is influenced by what the perceiver is doing. It is possible that the processes that weight or combine specifying variables do so differently, depending upon what the perceiver is trying to do. This is one of many possible mechanisms by which purpose and action may influence perception. Another possible mechanism derives from the fact that specifying variables are sampled differently depending upon the perceiver’s goals. It has long been know that eye movements are strongly influenced by purpose (Yarbus, 1967). In his review of eye movements and the control of actions, Land (2006) wrote, RT20371_C006.indd 178 12/11/2007 11:51:23 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 179 One of the main conclusions from this review is that eye movement strategies are very task-specific. They principally involve the acquisition of information needed for the execution of motor actions in the second or so before they are performed and in the checking of the execution of each action. (p. 322) Land reminds us that where people look depends upon what they are attempting to do, and thus, the sampling of visual information is action-specific. Visual attention is also action-specific. Employing a visual search paradigm, Bekkering and Neggers (2002) presented participants with physical arrays of blocks that varied in both orientation and color. A block’s orientation influenced the hand posture that would be required to grasp it. On each trial, participants attended to a fi xation dot and were instructed to make a saccade to a block having a specified orientation and color. Following the saccade they were instructed to either point or grasp the block. It was found that there were fewer erroneous saccades to blocks having the wrong orientation when participants were intending to grasp the block as opposed to when they were intending to point to it. The number of saccades to blocks of the wrong color was unaffected by the intended-action manipulation. This study showed that intentions to perform an action such as grasping, which must accommodate to an object’s orientation, can influence the visual processing of object orientation. The Body The body has an exterior and an interior. The exterior consists of the body’s form, which enables a behavioral potential as determined primarily by the skeleton and skeletal muscles. The exterior body performs actions in the external environment. The body’s interior consists of the plethora of organs, glands, and physiological systems that sustain life. A principal function of the brain is to control the body so as to achieve desired states in both the external environment and the body’s internal environment. Studies in behavioral ecology show that the behavior of organisms is primarily governed by energetic and reproductive imperatives (Krebs & Davies, 1993). With respect to energy, organisms have been shaped by evolution to follow behavioral strategies that optimize obtaining energy (food), conserving energy, delivering energy to RT20371_C006.indd 179 12/11/2007 11:51:24 AM 180 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action their young, and avoiding becoming energy for predators. To meet these ends, species have evolved behavioral strategies for achieving desired outcomes in the external physical environment while concurrently maintaining desired states in the internal environment of the body. The current account suggests that spatial perception promotes effective and efficient behavior by directly relating the visually specified environment to the possibilities and costs of intended actions. To achieve this, perception must be action-specific. For example, when people intend to walk, they see the world as “walkers.” Their perception will reveal where walking is possible in relation to their walking-relative physiological potential as well as the energetic costs associated with walking. If instead, people view the same scene as “throwers,” then perceiving the possibilities and cost of locomotion become irrelevant. People can throw a ball over a gorge that does not afford walking. A concrete example from behavioral ecology is here provided to illustrate a potential advantage of perceiving the environment relative to one’s action potential. An animal’s assessment of the risk of an approaching predator can be determined by measuring how close the predator can come before the animal initiates flight (Stankowich & Blumstein, 2005; Ydenberg & Dill, 1986). An iguana with a cool body temperature will flee from a predator at a greater distance than will one with a warmer body (Rocha & Bergalo, 1990). Because the reptile is more metabolically efficient at warmer body temperatures, its maximum escape speed and body temperature are positively correlated. Thus, the risk of being caught by a predator is a function of both the distance of the predator and the iguana’s body temperature. It is, of course, not known what iguanas perceive, but two possibilities come to mind. It could be that the iguana sees the distance to the approaching predator as being the same regardless of body temperature. The iguana might be supposed to have a generalpurpose distance perception system that is unaffected by intended action or physiological state. In this case, when deciding whether to flee, the reptile would have to relate the predator’s apparent distance to its body temperature and the implications of this metabolic state to its running speed. Another possibility, in line with the current approach, is that iguanas see predators as being closer when their bodies are cool as compared to when they are warm. In this case, the iguana flees whenever it sees the predator’s proximity as falling RT20371_C006.indd 180 12/11/2007 11:51:24 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 181 within an invariant flight-specifying distance. The iguana does not have to relate perception to its potential for action because this has already been achieved in perception. The mechanisms by which physiological state might influence visual processing are many. The brain resides in a chemical milieu in which many aspects of the body’s interior state are directly manifested in hormones, neurotransmitters, and various dimensions of blood’s composition. Given that the neural correlates of visual awareness are associated with very late processing in the temporal lobe (Koch, 2004), there are also numerous opportunities for neural influences from both visual and nonvisual areas. Purpose The argument that perception is action-specific demands a fundamental role for purpose; perception is specific to the action that is intended. People in the same situation will see the world differently depending upon what they are intending to do. People see the world as “walkers” only if they intend to walk or “throwers” only if they intend to “throw.” As will be discussed later, a manipulation that influences the effort required to walk but not to throw, will influence people’s perception of distance if they intend to walk but not if they intend to throw (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2004). The remainder of this chapter will describe spatial perception from an action-specific perspective. It will be shown, for example, that objects within reach appear closer than those that are out of reach, and that since reachability is extended by holding a tool, apparent distances are influenced, accordingly: Objects that are within reach when holding a tool, but out of reach when the tool is not held, appear closer when the tool is held and the “reacher” intends to use it (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2005). Other studies show that egocentric extents are expanded when walking is made more effortful due to the wearing of a heavy backpack (Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, & Epstein, 2003). Such effects cannot be accommodated by approaches that conceptualize distance perception as a general-purpose representation of the environment. Hand tools influence “reaching distance,” whereas backpacks influence “walking distance.” Perceptions are here viewed as being action-specific, as opposed to being specific to distal environmental properties such as distance. RT20371_C006.indd 181 12/11/2007 11:51:24 AM 182 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action Reaching Near space is defined by the extent of a person’s reach or slightly beyond, and thus, it is an instance of a dimension of spatial layout that has an action-specific definition. Others have referred to this region as personal space (Cutting & Vishton, 1995) or peripersonal space (Lavadas, 2002). Near space can be expanded by providing people with a hand tool that extends their reach. When this is done, previously out of reach objects will fall within near space, and as a consequence, these objects will appear closer than they did before the tool was held (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2005). Being reachable has consequences for an object’s visually perceived distance. In the Witt et al. studies, participants sat at a table upon which targets were projected by a digital projector in the ceiling. On each trial, a target was projected and participants judged its egocentric distance using a visual-matching task. After making this distance judgment, participants reached out and touched the target if it was within reach and pointed to its location if it was not. The experimental manipulation was defined by whether or not the participants held a conductor’s baton that extended their reach. It was found that targets that were out of reach without the baton, but within reach when it was held, were perceived to be closer when the baton was held, as if judgments of proximity incorporated reachability. In another study, Witt et al. showed that the influence of holding the baton is entirely dependent upon whether participants intended to reach with it. The previously described experimental design was repeated except that, after making the distance judgments, participants never reached out to touch the targets. In this study, holding the baton had no effect on the apparent distance to the targets. Two conclusions can be drawn from the Witt et al. studies. First, the apparent distance to objects is influenced by whether or not they can be touched. Extending one’s reach with a tool diminishes the apparent distance to objects that become touchable only through its use. Second, reachable space is not rescaled if a tool is held with no intent to use it. Perception is influenced by the behavioral potential to perform intended actions. Interesting parallels to the Witt et al. findings can be found in the electrophysiology and cognitive neuroscience literatures. Iriki, Tanaka, and Iwamura (1996) found that the macaque monkey possesses visual neurons in the intraparietal sulcus that fire when a rai- RT20371_C006.indd 182 12/11/2007 11:51:24 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 183 sin is in its near space. These cells fire when a visible raisin could be grasped and eaten but not when it was seen to be out of arm’s reach. Iriki et al. then trained monkeys to use a rake to acquire raisins that were beyond their grasp without it. Neurons that had previously not fired to raisins beyond arm’s reach now fired to raisins within rake’s reach. This study indicates that macaque monkeys—and most likely people—possess visual neurons that code for the reachability of objects and that these cells rescale the spatial range of reachability when a tool is held and used. Research with neglect patients has also shown that near space can become rescaled through tool use. Neglect patients ignore much of what is present in the left side of their visual field. A common diagnostic assessment for neglect is to ask patients to bisect a line presented in the frontal plane before them. People with neglect will indicate a position on the line that falls far to the right of actual center, thereby indicating that they have neglected all or most of the left side of the line. With respect to the symptoms of neglect, a double dissociation between near and far space has been found. Some patients show neglect only for lines in near space (Halligan & Marshall, 1991), whereas others show neglect only for far lines (Cowey, Small, & Ellis, 1994). Patients who show neglect only in near space will respond accurately on the bisection task if they use a laser pointer to indicate the center of a line that is beyond reach. However, if a stick is used that allows them to indicate the line’s center by touching it, then neglect will again be exhibited (Berti & Frassinetti, 2000; Pegna et al., 2001). These latter findings show that far space can become remapped into near space through tool use, and that this remapping has an influence on the perceptual processing of these patients. Specifically, physical contact, even if indirect, seems to invoke the mechanisms underlying neglect, whereas distal localization alone does not. Together, the studies reviewed in this section indicate that reachability has visual consequences. Behavioral studies show that objects in near space appear closer than those that are not. With tool use, more distant objects become reachable, and consequently, they are perceived to be closer. The electrophysiological studies with macaque monkeys show that visual neurons exist, which code for reachable objects, and that these cells will rescale reachable space as a result of learning to use a tool. Finally, studies of patients, who experience neglect only in near space, indicate that the neural mechanisms RT20371_C006.indd 183 12/11/2007 11:51:24 AM 184 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action responsible for their neglect are specific to reachability and not to absolute distance. Grasping Grasping objects requires that people reach to an object’s location and achieve an appropriate arm and hand posture to grasp and manipulate the object. If the to-be-grasped object is a hand tool with a handle, then the orientation of the handle relative to the grasper can make the tool more or less easy to pick up. Consider a hammer. If its handle is pointed to the grasper’s right, then the hammer can be easily grasped with the right hand, but not with the left. It would be worthwhile, at this point, for the reader to place on a table an elongated object—a pen will do—and pretending that it is a hammer, notice how easy it is to pick up with the right hand when the handle points to the right as opposed to pointing to the left. When doing this demonstration, be sure to pick up the pretend hammer in a way that is appropriate for its use; that is, the grasping posture must be one that affords hammering—the hammer’s head must be above the hand, not below. The ease with which a hand tool can be grasped affects its apparent distance, but surprisingly, only for right-handed people (Linkenauger, Witt, Stefanucci, & Proffitt, 2006). In these studies, participants sat at a table. Directly in front of the participants was a small dot on the edge of the table, which served as the near endpoint when making distance judgments. On each trial, an experimenter placed a hand tool at varying distances in front of the participant, with the handle pointing either to the left or to the right. A dot was affi xed to the center of gravity of each tool. Participants were told to imagine picking up the tool with their right hand in a manner appropriate for its use, after which they indicated its apparent distance—the distance between the dot before them on the table and the dot on the tool—using a visual matching task. Finally, they picked up the tool and gave it to the experimenter. It was found that, for right-handed participants, tools appeared nearer when the handle was pointed to the right as opposed to the left, indicating that the tools appeared closer when they were easier to grasp and pick up. Another experiment replicated the above design except that the right-handed participants were instructed to use their nondominant RT20371_C006.indd 184 12/11/2007 11:51:25 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 185 left hand. The results for handle orientation reversed. The tools were now seen to be nearer when their handles pointed to the left rather than to the right, a finding which is again consistent with the notion that apparent grasping-distance is influenced by ease of grasp. A totally unanticipated finding in the Linkenauger et al. studies was that none of the results with right-handers generalized to left-handed participants. Left-handers saw the tools as being equally far away regardless of the pointing direction of the tool’s handle or which hand was used to pick it up. Left-handers are known to be more ambidextrous (Gonzalez, Ganel, & Goodale, 2006), and this may be a reason for why they were unaffected by the orientation of a tool’s handle. In everyday circumstances, if left-handers see a tool with its handle pointed away from their dominant hand, then they would be more likely than a right-hander to pick it up with their nondominant hand. In addition, left-handers have had a lifetime of experience coping with such tools as scissors, can openers, and writing desks, which have been designed for right-handed people. In summary, right-handers see tools as appearing closer when their handles are oriented in a direction that makes the tool easy to pick up with the intended dominant or nondominant hand. Left handers see the world differently; the orientation of the tool’s handle does not influence their grasping-distance perception, perhaps because they are more ambidextrous than right-handers. There exists an extensive literature on the neurophysiology of grasping (see Castiello, 2005, for a review.) The literature indicates that the visual guidance of grasping engages the dorsal stream of visual processing including the anterior intraparietal sulcus and networks of other nearby parietal areas. A human fMRI study by Valyear, Culham, Sharif, Westwood, and Goodale (2006) showed that a region in the posterior portions of the intraparietal sulcus showed strong activations associated with changes in the orientation of hand tools, but not to changes in the tool’s identity. Identity changes of tools evoked strong activations in the temporal lobe’s fusiform gyrus but not in parietal regions. These results are consistent with Milner and Goodale’s (1995) proposal that the ventral visual processing stream is responsible for shape perception and object recognition, whereas the dorsal stream controls visually guided actions. Grasping a tool must conform to the orientation of its handle, and this orientation sensitivity was seen in parietal but not temporal activations. Identifying a tool does not require viewpoint-specific encoding, and RT20371_C006.indd 185 12/11/2007 11:51:25 AM 186 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action thus, temporal regions showed sensitivity to changes in an object’s identity but not to changes in its orientation. To anticipate the last section of this chapter, “Putting What, Where, and How Together,” for conscious spatial perception to be action-specific, aspects of both dorsal and ventral processing must be combined. Ventral processing is required to identify a hammer as being a hammer, and dorsal processing is required to take its orientation into account when picking it up. Interestingly, patients with ventral stream damage may not be able to identify an object as being a hammer and, although they can pick it up, they may do so in a manner that is inappropriate for its use; they may grasp the handle with the hammer’s head below the hand (Carey, Harvey, & Milner, 1996). Creem and Proffitt (2001b) showed that people have a strong tendency to pick up tools by their handles in a manner that is appropriate for their use, even if the handles are pointing away from them and appropriate grasping is difficult. However, if participants are required to do another task that puts a heavy load on semantic processing, which interferes with concurrent object recognition processing, then they behave like the patients with ventral damage. They pick up tools with the easiest grasp, even if this results in a posture that is inappropriate for the tools’ use (Creem & Proffitt, 2001b). These studies indicate that grasping a tool appropriately requires both ventral and dorsal processing. The initial processing of what, where, and how may be functionally and anatomically distinct (Creem & Proffitt, 2001a), but the action-specific nature of spatial perception manifests contributions from all three functions. Walking Perceiving the surface layout of the ground is of primary importance for walking. Visual perception provides information about where walking is possible as well as the difficulty associated with any chosen path. With respect to the geometry of its spatial layout, the ground plane has two walker-relative parameters, egocentric distance and slant. Both of these parameters are influenced by the energetic costs associated with walking. A recent review provides an in-depth summary of studies showing energetic influences on perceiving the ground’s layout (Proffitt, 2006). The basic findings are that hills appear steeper and egocentric distances farther, following RT20371_C006.indd 186 12/11/2007 11:51:25 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 187 manipulations of the anticipated metabolic energy costs associated with walking an extent. With respect to geographical slant perception, people grossly overestimate the slant of hills in all circumstances. Five-degree hills are typically judged to be about 20° and 10° hills appear to be 30° (Proffitt, Bhalla, Gossweiler, & Midgett, 1995). A large increase in this overestimation occurs following manipulations of the metabolic energy required to ascend hills (Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999). In these studies, the energetic costs associated with walking were experimentally manipulated by having people wear a heavy backpack or become physically tired by taking an hour-long exhausting run. Other studies selected people based upon their physical fitness, age or health. Creem-Regehr, Gooch, Sahm, and Thompson (2004) used a harness to manipulate walking effort in a virtual environment and found that increased effort was associated with an increase in perceived geographical slant. Overall, it was found that hills appear steeper when people are encumbered by a backpack or harness, tired, of low fitness, elderly, and in declining health. The overestimation of slant—both normative and experimentally induced—occurs in conscious awareness. These overestimations are obvious to anyone looking at a hill with knowledge of its actual slant. Participants are surprised and incredulous when, following an experiment they are told that the hill that was judged by them to be 20° is, in fact, only 5°. The conscious perception of slant was assessed with both verbal reports and a visual match task. Another assessment used a visually guided action measure that is dissociated from conscious awareness. Participants placed their hand flat on a rotating palmboard and, while looking at the hill but not their hand, attempted to make the board parallel with the slant of the hills. These adjustments are quite accurate and unaffected by any of the energetic manipulations (Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999). The dissociation between the measures of explicit awareness and the visually guided action measure may reflect the two streams of visual processing, which Milner and Goodale (1995) proposed. The explicit measures may entail ventral processing, whereas the visually guided action may be controlled by the dorsal stream. At present, no direct evidence exists for this account of slant perception. The overestimation of slant in conscious awareness is thought to promote effective long-term planning of locomotion, whereas accuracy in visually guided actions promotes effective behaviors in the RT20371_C006.indd 187 12/11/2007 11:51:25 AM 188 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action immediate proximal environment. It is obvious why actions directed at the immediate environment should be accurate. People would be clumsy creatures indeed if, whenever they encountered a 5° hill, they lifted their foot to accommodate a 20° incline. The functional utility of overestimation in conscious awareness takes a bit of explaining. The normative bias to overestimate geographical slant is an instance of psychophysical response compression that is found in many magnitude estimation tasks—see Proffitt (2006) for an expanded discussion of why response compression leads to overestimation in slant perception. Another example of response compression occurs in the human sensitivity to light intensity. When asked to indicate when a change in brightness has occurred, dark-adapted people in a completely dark environment can detect the presence of a few photons of light. On the other hand, in a well-illuminated environment, it takes an increase of orders-of-magnitude more light before people can notice the change. This is an instance of psychophysical response compression. Its virtue in the case of luminance detection is that people have a higher sensitivity to changes in light intensity when ambient light is low compared to when it is high. Similarly for geographical slant perception, normative overestimation allows people to be more sensitive to changes in small slants, for example, noticing the difference between 5° and 6° compared to detecting a difference between 75° and 76°. Seeing differences in the former has real consequences for planning locomotion, whereas there are no behavioral consequences that depend upon detecting the difference between the latter two inclines. There are two advantages associated with the increase in slant overestimation that occurs when the effort required to ascend hills is increased. First, increased overestimation implies an increase in sensitivity to small geographical slants. This means that as the metabolic costs of ascending hills increases, people become more sensitive to hill slants. Second, when choosing walking speed, people need not relate their current ability to expend walking energy to the apparent slant of the hill. Instead, this relationship is immediately apparent in perception. Recall that the discussion of how an iguana’s body temperature influences its flight-distance for an approaching predator. If the iguana sees an invariant flight-distance in which its body temperature and resulting flight speed influence the apparent distance to the predator, then the iguana does not have to relate these variables when it is deciding when to flee. Similarly for people RT20371_C006.indd 188 12/11/2007 11:51:25 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 189 viewing hills, by seeing their potential to expend energy in the perceived slant of inclines, people can decide how fast to walk based upon how steep the hill appears. They do not have to relate slant to their current physiological state because this has already been done in perception. Regarding energetic influences on spatial perception, the research findings for egocentric distance perception are much the same as for slant. Distances to targets appear greater when people are encumbered by a backpack or have just gotten off a treadmill—an experience that causes an adaptation in which the visual/motor system learns that it takes forward walking effort to go nowhere, and consequently, that it takes more effort to walk a prescribed distance (Proffitt, Stefanucci et al., 2003). Across these studies, a variety of dependent measures were used including verbal reports, visual match tasks, and blind walking, in which participants view a target, don a blindfold, and attempt to walk to the target’s location without sight. It has also been found that apparent distances on steep hills are expanded (Stefanucci, Proffitt, Banton, & Epstein, 2005). Steep hills require more energy to ascend. The hills that were assessed in these studies could not be ascended without considerable difficulty. The finding of increased perceived distance on hills presents a geometrical paradox. Given that the slant of hills is overestimated, when people look up a hill, the apparent distance to a target should be underestimated. Given that the angular elevation to the target does not change, the steeper the apparent hill, the short must be the egocentric extent along the ground to its location. This is what geometry requires. Such findings of geometrical inconsistencies in perception have been found in earlier studies on perceiving spatial layout (Epstein, 1977; Epstein, Park, & Casey, 1961; Sedgwick, 1986). Manipulations that influence the effort required to walk may not affect the effort required to perform other distance-relative behaviors. For example, walking on a treadmill without experiencing optic flow causes an adaptation in which more effort is associated with walking to a target, and consequentially, its apparent distance increases. However, treadmill walking does not affect the effort required to throw a beanbag to a target location. In accord with an action-specific approach to spatial perception, when people view a target—following a period of treadmill walking—with the intention of throwing a beanbag to its location, then the treadmill adaptation has no effect on their distance judgments (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, RT20371_C006.indd 189 12/11/2007 11:51:26 AM 190 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action 2004). Perceiving distances in these cases is specific to what a person is intending to do next. Walking adaptation influences perception if a person is a “walker” but not a “thrower.” That perceived extent has been found to be action-specific is in accord with prior studies on perceptual-motor adaptation conducted by Rieser, Pick, Ashmead, and Garing (1995). In their experiments, Rieser et al. had participants walk on a treadmill that was placed on a trailer being pulled across a field by a tractor. Through this means, the rate of optic flow was decoupled from the rate that participants were walking. Following this adaptation, participants were shown targets, and after being blindfolded, they attempted to walk to the target locations. Participants whose treadmill-walking rate was greater than the tractor’s speed walked too far, and conversely, those who walked at a slower speed than that of the tractor walked too short a distance. Of particular relevance to the action-specificity argument, other participants who attempted to throw balls to the location of targets were unaffected by the treadmill-walking adaptation. As with slant perception, the advantage of perceiving distances in terms of walking energy is that long-term motor plans can be based upon perception as opposed to requiring that perception and physiological state be combined during the planning process. Recall that the body has both an exterior and an interior. Most of the behaviors performed in the environment with the body’s exterior have as a goal the maintenance of a desired state in the body’s interior, a good example being maintaining a desired rate of energy expenditure. Both aspects of the body are related in walking-specific perception: In the apparent surface layout of the ground, people see both the possibilities and the associated energetic costs for walking. Consider another example from behavioral ecology. A recent study used GPS to track the movement of elephants in northern Kenya (Wall, Douglas-Hamilton, and Vollrath, 2006). It was found that elephants almost never ascended steep hills even when there was rich vegetation to be had. Wall et al. proposed that a principal reason for this reluctance to ascend hills was that, because of their body weight, elephants incur an enormous energetic cost when climbing. It would cost the elephants more calories to climb the hills than they would obtain by consuming the vegetation that could be obtained there. Wall et al. stated, “We conclude that megafauna probably take a rather different view of their surroundings than more light weight animals. This is especially true if the heavyweights, like elephants, RT20371_C006.indd 190 12/11/2007 11:51:26 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 191 are herbivores for which energy replenishment is so much more time consuming than it is for carnivores” (pp. R528). We, of course, do not know what elephants perceive, but from the current perspective it seems likely that their apparent topography would be highly exaggerated so as to enhance their sensitivity to geographical slant and to relate the possibilities and associated energetic costs for obtaining food. Throwing When throwing balls to targets, people perceive the distance to the targets relative to the effort associated with throwing (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2004). In these studies, participants viewed sports cones in a large open field and threw either light or heavy balls to their locations. After throwing the ball, participants reported the apparent distance to the cone and then threw the ball again. Depending upon the experiment, either verbal reports or a visual-match task were used as dependent measures. In both cases, distances were judged to be greater by those participants throwing heavy balls as opposed to light ones. In another experiment, Witt et al. also showed that the influence of throwing was contingent upon viewing the target cone with the intention of throwing. The experiment had two groups and both threw the heavy ball at targets. After throwing the ball, each group made a distance judgment. The groups differed in what they did next; one group attempted to throw the ball to the target while blindfolded, whereas the other group attempted to blind walk to the target’s location. Thus, when viewing the target, one group anticipated throwing and the other anticipated walking to the target location. The “throwers” viewed the target cones to be farther away than did the “walkers.” Throwing heavy balls makes targets appear farther away, but only if one is about to throw again. Recall that Witt et al. also showed the converse of these findings; treadmill-walking adaptation influenced apparent distance only when participants anticipated walking to a target and not when they anticipated throwing a beanbag to its location, instead. A final set of studies was conducted to assess whether these findings were due to changes in perception itself, or to some action-specific postperceptual process (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2006). Two RT20371_C006.indd 191 12/11/2007 11:51:26 AM 192 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action groups of participants were adapted to walking on a treadmill. Both groups then viewed a target, but with different expectations. One group anticipated that after donning a blindfold, they would attempt to walk to the target’s location. The other group expected to throw a beanbag to the target location while blindfolded. Thus, one group viewed the target as “walkers,” whereas the other group viewed it as “throwers.” Both groups put on the blindfolds and those in the walking group attempted to blind walk to the target location, as expected. After donning their blindfolds, the “throwers” were told that a mistake had been made in the instructions and that, in fact, they were to attempt to walk to the target location while blindfolded. Those participants in the walking condition were influenced by the treadmill walking adaptation and walked farther than those participants in the throwing condition. The “walkers” viewed the target relative to the energy required to walk to it, and thus, they were influenced by the treadmill adaptation. The “throwers” had experienced the same treadmill adaptation; however, because this experience influenced the effort associated with walking but not throwing, the former being an action they had not anticipated, they were unaffected by the adaptation. In other words, it seems to be anticipated effort that induces re-calibration. In a control experiment, this experiment’s design was repeated except that the treadmill-walking adaptation was eliminated and, in this case, the groups did not differ in their blind walking. Note that, in the initial experiment, both groups did exactly the same thing; both were adapted to treadmill walking and both attempted to blind walk to a target. The only difference between the groups was their behavioral intention when they viewed the target. The results indicate that each group’s spatial perceptions, as calibrated by required energy, were specific to these behavioral intentions. Falling Falling is an inherent danger associated with human locomotion. An adult could be injured by a slip and fall. Body size matters: A 2 m tall man, when tripping, will have a kinetic energy upon hitting the ground 20-100 times greater than a small child who learns to walk. This explains why it is safe for a child to learn to RT20371_C006.indd 192 12/11/2007 11:51:26 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 193 walk; whereas adults occasionally break a bone when tripping, children never do. (Went, 1968, pp. 407) The cost of injury increases with locomotion speed, and in the case of falling from a height above the ground, with altitude. As locomotion speed and attitude increase, fear of falling becomes palpable. All of these factors—speed, altitude, and fear of falling—have been found to influence spatial perception. Stefanucci, Proffitt, and Clore (2005) investigated how the risk of falling on a steep hill at a high speed might affect geographical slant perception. Participants viewed a steep sidewalk from the top, either standing on a skateboard or on a box of equivalent height. Given the steepness and extent of the sidewalk, descending on the skateboard would be very fast and risky. As in prior studies, explicit awareness of geographical slant perception was assessed with verbal reports and a visual matching task. The visually guided palmboard was also used. In addition, participants provided rating-scale judgments about how fearful they were of descending the sidewalk. It was found that the sidewalk appeared steeper—as assessed by explicit awareness measures—for those participants who were standing on the skateboard and reported feeling frightened compared to those who stood on the box and reported little or no fear. The visually guided palmboard adjustments were unaffected by both the skateboard manipulation and reported levels of fear. Although participants knew that they would not actually have to ride the skateboard down the hill, fear of an action seems to elicit the same kind of processing as anticipating its performance. Being at the edge of a high drop off, such as a cliff or balcony, makes people uneasy as the penalty for falling could entail severe injury or worse. Jackson and Cormack (in press) found that people overestimate vertical distances and, most importantly, that their overestimation is much greater when the height is viewed from above than from below. Jackson and Cormack concluded that the greater overestimation that is exhibited when heights are viewed from above is a consequence of an evolved adaptation, which through perceptual exaggeration, motivates people to avoid falling off heights. Stefanucci and Proffitt (2006) similarly found that high vertical extents are overestimated much more from the top than from the bottom. Participants used a visual matching task to judge vertical extent. They either stood atop a 26-foot balcony looking down or RT20371_C006.indd 193 12/11/2007 11:51:26 AM 194 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action they stood at the bottom looking up. The height of the balcony was overestimated by about 60% from the top and slightly less than 30% from the bottom. In addition, participants provide rating scale judgments of their fear of falling. It was found that the assessed anxiety related to falling was positively correlated with distance estimations. These studies suggest that an emotion, in this case fear, influences spatial perception. Other emotional influences on spatial perception have also been demonstrated (Riener, Stefanucci, Proffitt, & Clore, 2003). Hitting and Putting People who play sports often report that the spatial dimensions of balls, goals, hurdles, swimming pools, and so forth appear to be influenced by how well they are performing. Baseball, which has a rich journalistic tradition, provides many examples of apparent ball size being influenced by hitting performance. When describing a massive home run, Mickey Mantle said, “I never really could explain it. I just saw the ball as big as a grapefruit” (Ultimate New York Yankees, n.d.). George Scott of the Boston Red Sox said, “When you’re hitting the ball [well], it comes at you looking like a grapefruit. When you’re not, it looks like a blackeyed pea” (Baseball Almanac, n.d.). During a slump, Joe “Ducky” Medwick of the St. Louis Cardinals said he felt like he was “swinging at aspirins” (ESPNMAG.com, n.d.). Witt and Proffitt (2005) found that batter’s hitting performance does, in fact, influence their recollection of the ball’s size. Softball players were approached after completing a game and were asked to indicate the size of a softball by selecting one of many differently sized circles, which were displayed on a poster board. Afterwards, their batting average for the completed game was obtained. The recalled size of a softball was found to be positively correlated with player’s batting average. Golf is another sport in which reports abound of apparent spatial distortions. Golfers will claim that when they are putting well, the hole looks as big as a basket, and when their putting is off, the hole can look as small as a dime. Witt, Linkenauger, Bakdash, and Proffitt (2006) tested golfers after they had completed an 18-hole round. Similar to the study with softball players, the golfers were asked to choose, from among many circles, the one that was the size of a golf RT20371_C006.indd 194 12/11/2007 11:51:27 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 195 hole. Following this, other information about their round and golfing ability was obtained. It was found that apparent hole size was negatively correlated with the golfers’ scores for the 18-hole round. Because, in golf, low scores are good, this implies that the recollected hole size was positively related to performance. Of particular interest, it was found that apparent hole size was not related to how good a player was as assessed by his or her handicap. This implies that good players do not always see the hole as bigger, but rather that anyone will see the hole as being bigger on days when he or she is playing better. Finally, it was found that apparent hole-size was correlated with putting performance on the last hole but not to overall score on the last hole, suggesting that these effects are specific to the relevant task, which was putting. Both the softball and putting studies obtained size judgments from memory. Participants had completed play and were not looking at the ball or putting hole. Thus, the results of these studies could be due to performance influences on perception, memory, or both. There are, however, other results that suggest that perception could have been affected. Wesp, Cichello, Gracia, and Davis (2004) conducted a study on dart throwing and perceived target size. They found that participants who were more successful in hitting the target viewed it to be bigger than did participants who performed less well. In their study, the target was visibly present when the size judgment was made. Putting What, Where, and How Together Current views on the two cortical visual systems make claims, not only about the anatomical localization of visual functions, but also about consciousness (Goodale & Milner, 2004; Milner & Goodale, 1995). With respect to function, the ventral stream has been defined as the “what” system and is responsible for object identification. The dorsal stream is responsible for processing “where” and “how,” terms that refer to spatial localization and the visual guidance of actions (Creem & Proffitt, 2001a). The two cortical pathways have also been implicated in accounts of the neural correlates of consciousness. Milner and Goodale (1995) proposed that conscious awareness was associated with visual processing in the ventral but not the dorsal stream. They suggested that the term, perception, should be applied RT20371_C006.indd 195 12/11/2007 11:51:27 AM 196 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action to conscious visual awareness, whereas the visual guidance of action should be considered to be a distinct, unconscious visuomotor process. The evidence for Milner and Goodale’s proposal is well known (Goodale & Milner, 2004; Milner & Goodale, 1995). Patients with brain damage in the temporal lobe may have very limited shape awareness, and yet, they can accommodate their grasp when picking up objects as well as fully-sighted persons. Conversely, patients with parietal damage have unaffected shape perception abilities, but have difficulty grasping objects effectively. Some have gone so far as to describe the dorsal stream as a “zombie,” an instance of a system that is capable of guiding behavior, but without any attendant consciousness or will (Koch, 2004). It is difficult to imagine, however, how spatial perception could be action-specific without dorsal processes contributing to conscious experience. Consider the influences of reachability and graspability on distance perception. Objects appear closer when they can be touched with a tool compared to when no tool is held and the objects are out of hand’s reach. Visually guided reaching is a function of the dorsal stream. The cells in macaque monkeys that responded to reachable raisins are found in the dorsal stream (Iriki et al., 1996). With respect to grasping, tools appear closer to right-handed people when the handles are oriented toward the right as opposed to the left hand, and consequently, are easier to grasp (Linkenauger et al., 2006). The human brain areas that show sensitivity to the orientation of tools in fMRI studies are in the dorsal stream (Valyear et al., 2006). This chapter has argued that spatial perception relates to and is influenced by the visually specified environment, the body, and purpose. It has been suggested that people see spatial layout in terms of the actions that they intend to perform and the bodily opportunities and costs of these actions. Anticipated actions are formative in how spatial relationships are perceived. Recall the study in which people walked on a treadmill, and thereby, acquired a visual/motor adaptation in which the effort associated with walking an extent was increased. Following this adaptation, targets appear farther away if participants anticipate walking to its location but not if they are about to throw a beanbag to it (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2004). The adaptation acquired through treadmill walking affects “walkers” but not “throwers.” The world is seen relative to the behavior that is about to be performed. RT20371_C006.indd 196 12/11/2007 11:51:27 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 197 The what, where, and how of the two visual streams are coordinated and combined in perception in accordance with the choice of purposive behaviors. By choosing to reach, grasp, walk, or throw, a person sees the world in terms of their body’s abilities to perform these intended actions and also in relation to the inherent costs associated with their performance. REFERENCES Baseball Almanac (n.d.). Retrieved May 18, 2004, from http://www.baseball-almanac.com/players/player.php?p=scottge02 Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002) Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370–374. Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415–420. Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096. Carey, D. P., Harvey, M., & Milner, A. D. (1996). Visuomotor sensitivity for shape and orientation in a patient with visual form agnosia. Neuropsychologia, 34, 329–337. Castiello, U. (2005). The neuroscience of grasping. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 726–736. Cowey, A., Small, M., & Ellis, S. (1994). Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1059–1066. Creem, S. H. & Proffitt, D. R. (2001a). Defining the cortical visual systems: “What,” “where,” and “how.” Acta Psychologica, 107, 43–68. Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2001b). Grasping objects by their handles: A necessary interaction between cognition and action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 218–228. Cutting, J. E., & Vishton, P. M. (1995). Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.), Perception of space and motion (pp. 69–117). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Domini, F., Caudek, C., & Tassinari, H. (2006). Stereo and motion information are not independently processed by the visual system. Vision Research, 46, 1707–1723. Epstein, W. (1977). Stability and constancy in visual perception: Mechanisms and processes. New York: Wiley. RT20371_C006.indd 197 12/11/2007 11:51:27 AM 198 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action Epstein, W., Park, J., & Casey, A. (1961). The current status of the size-distance hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin, 58, 491–514. ESPNMAG.com (n.d.). Retrieved May 18, 2004, from http://espn.go.com/ magazine/vol5no11ichiro.html Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gonzalez, C. L., Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). Hemispheric specialization for the visual control of action is independent of handedness. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3496–3501. Goodale, M. A., & Milner, D. (2004). Sight unseen: An exploration of conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1991). Left neglect for near but not far space in man. Nature, 350, 498–500. Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurons. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330. Jackson, R. E. & Cormack, L. K. (in press). Evolved navigation theory and the descent illusion. Perception & Psychophysics. Koch, C. (2004). The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts. Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (1993). An introduction to behavioural ecology (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Land, M. F. (2006). Eye movements and the control of actions in everyday life. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 25, 296–324. Landy, M. S., Maloney, L. T. Johnson, E. B., & Young, M. J. (1995). Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: In defense of weak fusion. Vision Research, 35, 389–412. Lavadas, E. (2002). Functional and dynamic properties of visual peripersonal space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 17–22. Linkenauger, S. A., Witt, J., Stefanucci, J., & Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Ease to grasp an object affects perceived distance. Journal of Vision, 6(6), 724a. Milner, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pegna, A. J., Petit, L., Caldara-Schnetzer, A.-S., Khateb, A,. Annoni, J.-M., Sztajzel, R. et al. (2001). So near yet so far: Neglect in far or near space depends on tool use. Annals of Neurology, 50, 820–822. Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1,110–122. Proffitt, D. R. & Caudek, C. (2002). Depth perception and perception of events. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Vol. Eds.), I. B. Weiner (Editor-in-Chief), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 4. Experimental psychology (pp. 213–236). New York: Wiley. RT20371_C006.indd 198 12/11/2007 11:51:27 AM An Action-Specific Approach to Spatial Perception 199 Proffitt, D. R., Bhalla, M., Gossweiler, R., & Midgett, J. (1995). Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 409–428. Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, 106–112. Riener, C. R., Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Clore, G. (2003). An effect of mood on perceiving spatial layout. [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 3(9), 227a. Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 480–497. Rocha, C. F. D., & Bergalo, H. G. (1990). Thermal biology and flight distance of Tropidurus oreadicus (Sauria Iguanidae) in an area of Amazonian Brazil. Ethology, Ecology, and Evolution, 2, 263–268. Sedgwick, H. (1986). Space perception. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (Vol. 1, pp. 1–57). New York: Wiley. Stankowich, T., & Blumstein, D. T. (2005). Fear in animals: A meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 272, 2627–2634. Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Looking down from high places: The roles of altitude and fear in the perception of height. Journal of Vision, 6(6), 723a. Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2005). Distances appear different on hills. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1052–1060. Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Clore, G. (2005). Skating down a steeper slope: The effect of fear on geographical slant perception. Journal of Vision, 6(6), 723a. Ultimate New York Yankees (n.d.). Retried May 18, 2004, from http://www. ultimateyankees.com/MickeyMantle.htm Valyear, K. F., Culham, J. C., Sharif, N., Westwood, D., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). A double dissociation between sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation in the ventral and dorsal visual streams: A human fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 218–228. Wall, J., Douglas-Hamilton, I., & Vollrath, F. (2006). Elephants avoid costly mountaineering. Current Biology, 16, R527–R529. Went, F. W. (1968). The size of man. American Scientist, 56, 400–413. Wesp, R., Cichello, P., Gracia, E. B., & Davis, K. (2004). Observing and engaging in purposeful actions with objects influences estimates of their size. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1261–1267. Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., & Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Golf performance can make the hole look as big as a bucket or as small as a dime. Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia, Charlottesville. RT20371_C006.indd 199 12/11/2007 11:51:28 AM 200 Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2005). See the ball, hit the ball: Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average. Psychological Science, 16, 937–938. Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2004). Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent. Perception, 33, 577–590. Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888. Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2006). Effects of effort and intention on perception: The locus of the effect. Journal of Vision, 6(6), 721a. Yarbus, A., (1967). Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum Press. Ydenberg, R. C., & Dill, L. M. (1986). The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 16, 229–249. RT20371_C006.indd 200 12/11/2007 11:51:28 AM