PERSPECTIVES killing was within or between groups is tricky to ascertain (especially from archaeological data) and varies among sites, but on average, 14% of adult deaths appear to have been due to warfare. The extent of prehistoric genetic differentiation is also difficult to estimate, but based on genetic studies of extant huntergatherers, the model shows that a realistic level of inbreeding within groups allows 10.1126/science.1175874 group benefits to offset fitness costs of roughly 3% associated with being an “altruistic warrior” relative to nonaltruists. Ironically, ANTHROPOLOGY lethal hostility toward other groups could thus underpin cooperation and support within human communities. The model does not account for sex differences, which might matter, Ruth Mace because females did not fight in wars, Human social evolution is determined by demography. were likely to join and reproduce with the victorious group after conflict nlike other animals, humans cooperate evolutionary outcomes not [they may well have been the objective with nonrelatives in coordinated act- seen in other animals (4). of the conflict in the first place (6)], ions, decorate their bodies, build comPerhaps the central differand were more likely, even in peaceplex artefacts (useful or otherwise), talk, and ence between genetic and cultime, to migrate out of their natal divide themselves into linguistic groups. To tural transmission is that we group to breed. The inheritance of the understand the evolutionary basis of such can change our cultural phealtruistic traits is assumed to be vertibehaviors, anthropologists must consider not notype during our lives—for cal and asexual, so the model could only issues connected to social evolution in ani- example, to conform to group work for a cultural trait (possibly even mals, but also the implications of the possible norms. Cultural differences better than for a genetic one). It is also coevolution of genes and culture. Two articles between groups might be possible that the measures of inbreedin this issue examine aspects of human social easier to maintain than are geing observed are maintained by culevolution: On page 1293, Bowles (1) investi- netic ones, due to processes tural processes, such as language difgates the origins of altruism toward one’s own such as conformist social learnferences between groups. Nonethesocial group, while on page 1298, Powell et al. ing and punishment; several less, Bowles suggests that the model (2) study the emergence of cultural complexity. models show that if these procould theoretically apply to other socBased on empirical evidence and modeling, cesses occur, cultural group ial animals. Zoologists, whose limited both studies suggest that the demographic selection could explain the tolerance of group selection rarely structure of our ancestral populations deter- evolution of prosocial or altru- Technological and cul- extends beyond the human species, tural complexity. So- will find this statement controversial; mined how social evolution proceeded. istic behavior (5). phisticated tools such as If, like me, you were brought up on The Bowles now makes a more but it is certainly not without the these harpoons began Selfish Gene (3), you learned that selection acts radical claim: that the demo- to emerge in the upper bounds of scientific possibility to test. on individuals or genes, and you are trained to graphic structure of hunter- Paleolithic (between ca Powell et al. address the evolube wary of group selectionist explanations for gatherer populations allowed 40,000 and 10,000 years tion of technological and cultural behavior. Group selection is generally rejected group-selected genetic traits ago). complexity—human behaviors that as unimportant because even a tiny amount of to evolve in humans. He arhave left clear traces in the archaeomigration between groups quickly destroys the gues that lethal warfare was endemic and logical record. Traits such as the creation of genetic differences needed for group selection that altruistic, group-beneficial behaviors abstract art, improvements in stone and to act. But recent literature on social evolution that hurt the survival chances of individuals other tools, long-distance “trading,” and the has reopened the debate, arguing that in some but improved the likelihood for groups to manufacture of musical instruments mark circumstances group selection might be impor- win conflicts could emerge by group selec- the emergence of modern humans who tant, especially in a cultural species like hu- tion. This argument was originally espoused behaved much as we do (see the figure). mans. Genetic and cultural traits are both heri- by Darwin, but few formal tests of it have These material expressions of the modern table and subject to evolutionary processes, been done. condition emerged much later than did but cultural traits are not transmitted in a In his model, Bowles identifies two key anatomically modern humans. Some asMendelian way; they can be inherited from determinants of whether group selection can pects of behavioral modernity first apalmost anyone, including people who may not favor altruistic behavior: the individual and peared in southern Africa, possibly as early share your genetic interests. This could lead to group costs and benefits of altruism in warfare, as 90,000 years ago, only to disappear again and the extent of genetic differentiation among and reappear in Eurasia ~45,000 years ago. groups. An array of ethnographic and archaeo- The timing of these events makes a biologiDepartment of Anthropology, University College London, logical evidence shows that hunter-gatherers cal change in cognitive capacity a someTaviton Street, London WC1H 0BW, UK. E-mail: r.mace@ certainly did kill each other. How much of this what unlikely explanation. ucl.ac.uk References and Notes 1. C. Cecconi, E. A. Shank, C. Bustamante, S. Marqusee, Science 309, 2057 (2005). 2. E. M. Puchner et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 13385 (2008). 3. J. P. Junker, F. Ziegler, M. Rief, Science 323, 633 (2009). 4. A. del Rio et al., Science 323, 638 (2009). 5. X. Zhang et al., Science 324, 1330 (2009). 6. J. E. Sadler, Annu. Rev. Biochem. 67, 395 (1998). 7. S. W. Schneider et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 7899 (2007). 8. G. G. Levy et al., Nature 413, 488 (2001). 9. H. M. Tsai, Blood 87, 4235 (1996). 10. C. P. Johnson, H. Y. Tang, C. Carag, D. W. Speicher, D. E. Discher, Science 317, 663 (2007). 11. A. J. Engler, S. Sen, H. L. Sweeney, D. E. Discher, Cell 126, 677 (2006). 12. We thank A. Bausch and R. Netz for helpful comments on the manuscript. We acknowledge funding through Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft grant SFB 486 B9. Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 5, 2009 On Becoming Modern 1280 5 JUNE 2009 VOL 324 SCIENCE Published by AAAS www.sciencemag.org CREDIT: THE LONDON ART ARCHIVE/ALAMY U PERSPECTIVES ical evidence (8) that depopulation may have occurred at the relevant time (around 70,000 to 60,000 years ago). The model of Powell et al. only includes selection on cultural traits, not on individuals or populations. In the model, skills are assumed to be transmitted because they are beneficial. It is therefore possible that the advantages of cultural skill acquisition caused population density to increase, rather than emerged as a response to it. This is an area for further modeling. And, of course, Powell et al. argue that the cognitive ability to learn these skills was already present in all. Researchers seeking genes involved in cognitive ability would thus be unwise to base their evidence on correlating geographic patterns of candidate genes with geographic patterns of the emergence of culturally acquired skills—at least not without paying careful attention to demographic differences. The two models (1, 2) paint rather different pictures of Pleistocene life. Were early modern humans in frequent contact with neighboring groups to exchange cultural innovations, or were they inward looking, unwilling to travel, and constantly engaging their neighbors in lethal conflict? Probably both, at different times and in different places (although it may be possible to steal someone’s cultural innovations and kill them too). Neither study claims that its model provides the unequivocal explanation. Indeed, there are many alternative explanations both for altruistic behavior in human groups and for the emergence of cultural modernity. Some alternative explanations may be more due to semantics than real differences in evolutionary processes (9), and some may work in addition to the processes proposed here. But by combining models with data, both studies put their hypotheses firmly up the list of possibilities to be taken seriously. References 1. S. Bowles, Science 324, 1293 (2009). 2. A. Powell, S. Shennan, M. G. Thomas, Science 324, 1298 (2009). 3. R. Dawkins, The Selfish Gene (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1976). 4. P. J. Richerson, R. Boyd, Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago/London, 2005). 5. R. Andrés Guzmán, C. Rodriguez-Sickert, R. Rowthorn, Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 112 (2007). 6. N. Chagnon, Yanomamo: The Fierce People (Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, 1983). 7. J. Henrich, Am. Antiq. 69, 197 (2004). 8. S. H. Ambrose, J. Hum. Evol. 34, 623 (1998). 9. S. A. West, A. S. Griffin, A. Gardner, J. Evol. Biol. 21, 374 (2008). 10.1126/science.1175383 CELL BIOLOGY The range of protective effects that a sirtuin deacetylase affords to cells and organisms under stressful conditions continues to grow. Hypoxic Hookup Leonard Guarente ypoxia-inducible factors HIF-1α and HIF-2α are homologous transcription factors that activate an adaptive response in mammalian cells to low concentrations of environmental oxygen (1, 2). HIFactivated genes protect against damaging reactive oxygen molecules generated by mitochondria in response to hypoxia, and also stimulate erythrocyte proliferation and blood vessel formation to enhance organism survival. Under normal oxygen conditions, HIF1α and HIF-2α are hydroxylated on key prolines, which promotes their degradation. However, under hypoxic conditions, the HIF proteins are stabilized and accumulate in cells. On page 1289 of this issue, Dioum et al. show that during hypoxia, HIF-2α is deacetylated and thereby activated by SIRT1, a nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide (NAD)–depend- H Paul F. Glenn Lab and Department of Biology, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA. E-mail: leng@mit.edu ent deacetylase (3). This finding extends the reach of SIRT1 as a cell and tissue maintenance and anti-aging factor to include resistance to hypoxic stress. SIRT1 has already been shown to protect against metabolic, genotoxic, and heat stress by deacetylating other key transcription factors that respond to those stressors (4–8) (see the figure). Dioum et al. show that in cultured mammalian cells, SIRT1 bound to and deacetylated HIF-2α and activated the HIF-2α target genes Sod2 (superoxide dismutase 2), VegfA (vascular endothelial growth factor A), and Epo (erythropoietin). A catalytically inactive form of SIRT1 was unable to alter HIF-2α activity, indicating the importance of HIF-2α deacetylation in the activation mechanism. Mutating three lysines of HIF-2α, which are acetylated, did not completely abolish activation by SIRT1. This indicates that other, unidentified lysines are important for HIF-2α regulation. It is not yet clear whether additional deacetylases (histone deacetylases or SIRTs 2 to 7) also play important www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 324 Published by AAAS roles in HIF-2α activation. However, depletion of SIRT1 alone increased the amount of acetylated HIF-2α during hypoxia, suggesting that SIRT1 is the primary HIF-2α deacetylase. The relationship between SIRT1 and HIF2α was also shown in biological settings. The liver is the chief source of erythropoietin production during mid-gestation in mice. Embryos lacking either Sirt1 or HIF-2α showed a defect in erythropoietin expression in the liver during this gestational period. Tail vein injection of adenoviral expression vectors containing Sirt1-encoding DNA along with vectors encoding either HIF protein demonstrated the potential of Sirt1 to activate HIF-2α, but not HIF-1α, in the adult liver. And a viral vector that triggered RNA interference of endogenous Sirt1 reduced expression of the liver Epo gene. Because HIF-2α is only stable under hypoxic conditions in the studies described, Dioum et al. do not address whether SIRT1 itself is activated by hypoxia. This issue is extremely relevant to possible roles for sirtuin 5 JUNE 2009 Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 5, 2009 Powell et al. now argue that changes in population size and structure can explain the patterns of acquisition (and loss) of culturally inherited skills. The authors build on a model by Henrich (7), who showed that small populations were more likely to lose complex skills. Powell et al. examine a more realistically structured population, in which individuals live in groups (subpopulations) and inherit (learn) skills from others in the group or by contact due to migration between groups. The results show that the time since first occupation of a region is a far less reliable predictor of the accumulation of cultural skills than is the density of subpopulations and the degree of migration between them. The authors then use coalescent models (a statistical approach assuming neutral traits which are subject to drift) of genetic variation in mitochondrial DNA to estimate prehistoric population densities at various sites. The resulting population density estimates are compatible with those necessary to prompt the onset of behavioral modernity at the appropriate time in sub-Saharan Africa, and then later in the Middle East. The genetic evidence cannot really help to explain the subsequent disappearance of these traits in subSaharan Africa, but there is some archaeolog- 1281