GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS SECTION 1 Introduction Integration with other units This is an optional component unit of Advanced Higher Psychology. It has a value of one credit at Advanced Higher. Unit content The unit has three outcomes: 1. 2. 3. Analyse major theories in developmental psychology. Evaluate research evidence relating to theories in developmental psychology. Analyse an issue in developmental psychology. Content of this pack This pack contains resources that will assist the teacher/lecturer with the delivery of this unit. It contains material relevant to all of the outcomes. Core skills Details on core skills may be obtained from the Scottish Qualifications Authority (SQA). DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 1 GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS Statement of standards Outcome 1 Analyse major theories in developmental psychology. Performance criteria (a) Competing theoretical explanations in developmental psychology are explained accurately and comprehensively. (b) Competing theoretical explanations in developmental psychology are compared accurately in terms of their main features. (c) Competing theoretical explanations in developmental psychology are contrasted accurately in terms of their main features. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for two key concepts chosen from the following: early socialisation, cognitive development, social behaviour, adolescence and adulthood, ageing. Written/oral responses will typically be extended responses of about 1000 words for each key concept and associated research evidence, integrating Outcomes 1 and 2. Outcome 2 Evaluate research evidence relating to theories in developmental psychology. Performance criteria (a) Research evidence relating to theories in developmental psychology is described accurately. (b) Research evidence relating to theories in developmental psychology is explained clearly and accurately in terms of its strength of support for the theories. (c) Validity of conclusions based on this research evidence is explained clearly and accurately. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for research evidence in two key concepts chosen from following: early socialisation, cognitive development, social behaviour, adolescence and adulthood, ageing. 2 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS Written/oral responses will typically be extended responses of about 1000 words for each key concept and associated research evidence, integrating Outcomes 1 and 2. Outcome 3 Analyse an issue in developmental psychology. Performance criteria (a) An issue relevant to developmental psychology is explained clearly and accurately. (b) Essential arguments of this issue are explained accurately and comprehensively in a balanced way. (c) The contribution of this issue to developmental psychology is explained accurately and comprehensively. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for one issue from the following: heredity and environment, attachment and separation, genetic research in developmental psychology, developmental psychology as science, cultural/gender bias in developmental psychology, the use of nonhuman animals in research. Written/oral response will typically be an extended response of about 1000 words. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 3 GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS Approaches to learning and teaching In delivering this unit, it is useful if teachers/lecturers achieve a balance between teacher/lecturer exposition and experiential learning. It is important to recognise that learners acquire and process information in a number of ways to help them learn. These include visually, aurally, in discussion or exchange with others, during group-based problem solving activities, and during solitary reflection. Students should be encouraged from the beginning to draw on their own experiences, perceptions, and their previous and current learning. Personal experience of interacting with a variety of people, and in a number of different situations, is an invaluable source of knowledge and is highly relevant to cognitive psychology. The sharing of experiences and insights will promote general awareness that developmental psychology assists self-understanding and an understanding of humans in a variety of contexts. Students should also be encouraged to gather and use information about different people’s actions, thoughts and feelings and to consider how these affect themselves and others. Relevant quality newspapers and/or magazine articles and video/film productions are useful resources which bring cognitive psychology to life so that it can be shared by comparatively large groups of people at any one time. This remains appropriate even when the material is fictional, provided it presents us with a true picture of the human condition and is not deliberately sensationalised. In delivering this unit, it is appropriate that a multicultural approach is taken since the learning needs of individuals vary according to their cultural background. Case studies, role-play and simulations should incorporate characters and elements from different social and cultural backgrounds wherever possible. Unit induction At the beginning of the unit ‘Developmental Psychology’, teachers and lecturers should ensure that students are clear about its nature and purpose. Induction for this unit should last about two hours and should include an introduction to the content of the unit, provide a programme of work and explain the arrangements for assessment and reassessment. At this point students can be given the Candidate’s Guide from the Unit Assessment pack. This helps explain what the unit is about and how it is assessed. 4 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS In order to allow students to make a confident start, reference should be made to links with previous or other current learning with which they are familiar. It is also important to discuss and explore the nature of the Course or Group Award being undertaken by the group if appropriate. It may be necessary to include induction exercises, particularly if the group is a new one. The type and number of exercises used will however depend on the nature of the particular group, their familiarity with each other and with the teacher/lecturer concerned. Learning environment The expertise of the teacher/lecturer is invaluable in developing skills in, approaches to, and insights about the subject of developmental psychology. Teachers/lecturers should aim to create a relaxed and enjoyable learning environment, which is both motivating and supportive. In order that a people perspective is always present the following conditions should be met: • the provision of a learning climate in which students feel supported and able to express their thoughts and ideas • a teaching style that promotes a supportive learning climate • teaching and learning methods that draw on students’ past and present learning experience and which enable them to integrate new ideas and skills during their interactions with others. Further guidance can be found in the Psychology Subject Guide. How to use this pack Purpose of the pack This pack is designed to provide guidance and support materials to help teachers/lecturers in the delivery of the unit. Sections 2 and 3 contain student information and activities that may be used by teachers/lecturers in whichever way suits their preferred style of delivery and the needs of their particular student group. This pack has not been designed for open learning purposes. Additional reading, exercises, assignments, etc., and answers to enclosed exercises DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 5 GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS and worksheets will be provided and facilitated by the teacher/lecturer. The student activities in the pack will have to be followed up and brought together by the teacher/lecturer in whatever way is most appropriate. The student activities in this pack cover the three outcomes and their performance criteria at Advanced Higher level. The unit in the teaching/ learning situation calls for two key concepts, their features and explanations, and one issue to be covered. This Teacher’s Resource Pack will endeavour to cover three key concepts, their features and explanations, and two issues. The three concepts covered in Section 2 (early socialisation, cognitive development and ageing) each deal first with Outcome 1 (analyse major theories in developmental psychology), followed by Outcome 2 (evaluate research evidence relating to theories in developmental psychology), while Outcome 3 (analyse an issue in developmental psychology) is dealt with last. Section 3 deals with two issues in relation to developmental psychology. These are ‘The use of non-human animals in research’ and ‘Heredity and environment’. This sequence of delivery is by no means compulsory and may be rearranged at the discretion of the teacher/lecturer responsible for delivering the unit. Using the materials Sections 2 and 3 of this pack contain student information materials and activities. The essential knowledge required for the unit has been covered on the information pages. These are particularly useful as handout material. They could also be used as the focus of input by the teacher/lecturer and to develop ideas further as part of question and answer sessions and group discussions. These information sheets can be photocopied as a separate pack should the teacher/lecturer prefer to use them either as teaching notes or as separate handout materials. Alternatively, the materials could be assembled into smaller topic packs. 6 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS Guidance on the content and context for this unit By introducing students to a range of concepts and associated theories, research evidence and issues in developmental psychology, it is intended to develop knowledge and understanding of developmental psychology generally and to emphasise the significance of this area to the whole of Psychology. A choice of two concepts and one issue is a feature of this unit. This provides flexibility for centres to accommodate different needs and interests in studying developmental psychology at this level. More information on the content of this unit is provided in the course details. Guidance on learning and teaching approaches for this unit General proposals regarding approaches to learning and teaching are contained in the course details. Learning and teaching approaches should be carefully selected to support the development of knowledge and understanding, investigation and application. The learning experience at this level should be interesting, to encourage enthusiasm for the subject and to stimulate and prepare candidates for independent study. The unit should be approached using a wide range of stimulus materials and teaching approaches. Candidates should be encouraged to draw upon their own experiences and should have access to resources. The material should be up-to-date and relevant to the unit, the level of study and the interests of the candidates. The emphasis throughout should be on active learning, whether as part of a whole class, in small groups or as individuals. The outcomes are interconnected and should be approached as such. At Advanced Higher it is especially recommended that, wherever possible, outcomes should be covered in an integrated way. An outcome-by-outcome internal assessment approach, which could lead to a compartmentalised view of psychology, should be avoided. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 7 GUIDANCE FOR TEACHERS Guidance on approaches to assessment for this unit The National Assessment Bank will provide assessment instruments and guidance on implementation. This does not preclude teachers/lecturers from devising their own assessment tasks. Evidence of attainment of the outcomes for this unit may be provided through a variety of methods. However, restricted-response questions are considered to be most appropriate. Where an integrated approach is used for assessment, it will be necessary to identify in the candidate’s response where each outcome has been met. Where assessment evidence is gathered by means of a single assessment towards the end of the unit, care should be taken to ensure that sufficient time is allowed for remediation and reassessment if required. Where a candidate has failed to achieve one or more of the outcomes, it is only necessary to reassess those outcomes that the candidate has failed to achieve. Where assessments are set which allow candidates to demonstrate performance beyond the minimum standard required, evidence gathered for internal unit assessment might also be used for grade prediction and for appeals for external course assessment. For details of the grade descriptions for external assessment, please refer to the Advanced Higher Psychology course specification. 8 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS SECTION 2 What is developmental psychology? – Setting the scene Developmental psychology is the branch within psychology that concerns itself with the different changes that happen to us as individuals from pre-birth to death and what the causes and effects of these changes might mean for us. These changes, which occur at different ages throughout our lifetime, are continuous and progressive and fall into the following four categories: 1. 2. 3. 4. physical cognitive or intellectual social emotional. Physical development concerns the development of our body’s anatomy (bodily structure) and physiology (bodily processes) during the course of our life and how these developments help or hinder how we think and how we behave, socially and emotionally, as we age. We all physically develop at approximately the same age the world over, though the onset of physical change may vary slightly from one person to another. Cognitive development concerns perception, attention, language, memory and thinking (or problem solving). Collectively these are called our cognitive processes. An investigation into cognitive development in developmental psychology almost exclusively concerns the growth and development of thinking ability. Social development is enquiry into how we develop as social beings and here developmental psychology looks at socialisation, sociability, child-rearing practices, moral development, groups and peer group influences, etc. Emotional development, which is closely related to social development, concerns concepts like the nature of the attachments we form with others, our temperament, our personality, what motivates us to aggression, identity and our search for meaning in life. The study of these continuing and progressive changes that occur to us during our lifetime is known as lifespan development theory or the life- DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 9 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS span approach. Developmental psychology’s lifespan approach is different from other ‘approaches’ in psychology – like behaviourism or psychoanalysis – in that it does not favour any particular psychological theory, or theorist, but does make a number of assumptions concerning our development. These assumptions are that: • biological maturation, which is determined by our genetics, must occur before physical, and ultimately cognitive, social and emotional growth and development happens for us as individuals • human beings the world over experience the same sequence of physical, cognitive, social and emotional development throughout their lifespan • as we develop physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally these age-related changes are shaped, for better or for worse, by the environment within which we find ourselves. Developmental psychology studies, in the main, the various skills and abilities human beings have, including how we acquire them in the first place. Human beings are very complex animals, thus the first important influence on our development is our biology. As mankind, in the generic or non-specific sense, has evolved, biology has affected behaviour in a number of ways. Great discoveries were made in biology in Europe during the nineteenth century, and as a result some psychologists, such as Galton, began to argue that many of our skills, such as intelligence, were genetically determined in much the same way as our sex, our skin pigmentation or colour of our eyes. Psychologists who take this view are influenced by the nature or heredity perspective (our biology or genetics control and shape what we become physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally). Others, such as the behaviourists Watson and Skinner, and later the eminent psychosocial psychoanalyst Erikson, disagreed, arguing that we each (physically, and to a greater extent, cognitively, socially and emotionally) are the product of all the experiences we encounter as we grow up. Our environment makes us what we are. This is the nurture or environment perspective within the heredity–environment debate. This we will consider in greater detail below and in Section 3 which deals with the issues in developmental psychology. Over and above this some, like Maslow and Rogers, within humanistic psychology would argue that because we have free will we can, if we want, change aspects of what we have become. We can, for example, change how we think about and view the world, and this can have an 10 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS influence on our social and emotional behaviour as individuals and with others. Psychologists who believe environment is more influential in shaping us psychologically would be said to be taking a nurture perspective (our environment controls and shapes what we become physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally). An overview of the nature–nurture debate and developmental psychology The teacher, lecturer and/or learner may wish to return to this in later consideration of a possible issue for Outcome 3, i.e. heredity and environment. The ‘normal’ human baby inherits 23 pairs of chromosomes from their male and female biological parents. It is estimated that each chromosome probably contains between 10 000 to 20 000 genes. A gene carries information in the form of deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) to the developing organism, known as a foetus. A baby’s, and ultimately an adult’s, genetic inheritance is a mixture of the parents’ genes and is called our genotype. A genotype is a sort of blueprint or plan of what we will be like as we develop and grow. Many early psychologists thought that most of our personality, intellect, sex role behaviour, etc., was the result of our genes and could not be interfered with, or changed, in any constructive way. This is quite a frightening thought. It means that we have no free will and need not feel responsible, or be held accountable, for our actions. Nowadays we know this early view was quite wrong. We do not always achieve the prediction of our genotype anyway. We cannot predict with any certainty on the basis of genetic inheritance alone what we might become as human beings. Environment, and its influence on physical, cognitive, social and emotional development affect us from conception. If, for example, our mother had a certain disease, smoked, drank heavily, took drugs or had an accident while pregnant, or if we were born into a culture where people were subject to drought and starvation, we may remain undersize, underweight or have some mental problem. What we actually become then as human beings is called a phenotype – the consequence of the interaction between our genetic inheritance and the environment we find ourselves in. We are born with many abilities. For example, certain aspects of our visual system are available to us from birth. Much of what we become however, is a result of the experiences we have and the people we mix with. Whether our parents are strict or easy going, what our friends are DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 11 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS like, the culture we live in, how wealthy our family is, all combine to make each of us what we are. As the science of genetics advances, we are becoming a bit more certain as to the role genetics has, or does not have, in shaping us physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally. The degree to which genetics and environment influence us developmentally is the subject of the nature–nurture debate. Maturation Another biological influence on us, the developing organism, is the rate at which we mature. Maturation is the process of physical growth, and starts at conception. All normal human foetuses the world over develop at the same rate. We all learn to manipulate our hands and fingers, to move (or orientate) our heads, to crawl and to walk at approximately the same time. This progressive rate of development is the same for all children and is genetically determined. We can only learn to adapt our behaviour when we are maturationally, or biologically, ready to do so. This is evident in infants about one year old, who can crawl like champions and stand up with support, but because of their body shape cannot yet walk. Quite simply, their legs have not yet straightened enough to support upper body weight. Walking can only occur when the infant is physically – or maturationally – ready. Human beings do not mature very quickly. For the first few years of life we are the most helpless of all species. Almost 25 per cent of our lives are spent as an immature child. No other animal spends as long preparing for adulthood as we do. Is development continuous or does it happen in stages? As well as the nature–nurture debate, developmental psychology also has an interest in discovering what (and whether) physical, cognitive, social and emotional changes are related to identified age-related stages in our life; or if these developmental changes are continuous, or nonstage related. The history of developmental psychology has seen some controversy as to whether language, personality, moral values, intelligence, etc., are developed at, and through, fixed and identifiable stages in our life or whether they develop continuously throughout the human lifespan. If certain skills and abilities (or behaviours) do develop in stages (and there is now a lot of evidence to suggest this is the case), developmental psychologists then look for certain boundaries, or parameters, to 12 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS differentiate between stages of development associated with particular stage-related behaviours. Take what has been found out about the development of language for instance. • Each stage exhibits a particular kind of behaviour not found in a previous stage. The first stage of language development in infants is called ‘babbling’. Babbling is where a baby meaninglessly repeats consonant sounds like mama or dada. Babbling happens around 4 months of age and ceases when the next identifiable stage of language development, the learning of first words, is mastered. • The kind of behaviour (and associated thinking) that goes with each stage must be different. Extending the above example, the meaningless babbling of consonant sounds sees the developing infant, because of its parents’ excited behaviour, begin to associate particular sounds with particular objects in its environment. The infant begins to develop vocabulary as a consequence, e.g. daddy, mummy, teddy, etc. It realises that particular sounds have a particular meaning. Particular sounds represent things in its environment. As this develops, the infant acquires more words that allow even more meaningful communication with those around. Children can now ask questions and can state preferences. They could not do this earlier. • All children must go through identified stage-related behaviours in the same order, at approximately the same age and generally at the same rate. The development of language is again an obvious example, in that we can identify particular ‘language’ behaviours occurring within particular age-stages, namely: The development of language – an age-stage related process Identified behaviour Babbling Using words Using simple sentences Identified common age 4 months to 1 year 1 year to 18 months 18 months onwards This sequence of development regarding language is the same for all children in the main. It is this combination of mental activity and actual behaviour that is the subject matter of developmental psychology. From a lifespan point of view what we try to do is discover what type or kind of physical, cognitive, social and/or emotional development is occurring at a particular time for a person, what these developments are and whether they are age-stage related or develop gradually across the human lifecycle. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 13 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Lifespan development: a whole-life approach to developmental psychology The human lifecycle is often ‘compartmentalised’ in developmental psychology. An understanding of lifespan development therefore sees developmental psychologists investigate, within and across identified age-stages, physical, cognitive, social and emotional change during our lifetime. Important age-stages across the human lifespan are: • Conception to birth – often referred to as the prenatal stage of development • Infancy – often referred to as the neonatal stage of development • Childhood • Adolescence and adulthood • Old age. ‘Lifespan development’ identifies infancy, childhood, adolescence and adulthood and old age (ageing) as especially important age-stages in the human lifespan. Lifespan development is a relatively new approach in developmental psychology. As late as the mid-1970s those who would be termed ‘developmental psychologists’ today only studied physical, cognitive, social and emotional development in babies and young children (pre-birth to around age 7). They were child psychologists as such. Economic boom in Britain during the affluent 1960s, the raising of the school leaving age from 14 to 15 and then to 16 by the mid-1970s and the impact on the welfare state all came together to help expand the age-ranges developmental psychology was interested in. Adolescence, or the ‘teenager’, was invented in the 1960s. Previously, in our culture you were identified as either a school child or a worker. There was no ‘in-between’ stage. Teenagers became an identifiable group in our society, separate from children and adults. Indeed today the European Union officially recognises a young person as someone between 16 and 25. From not existing 40 years ago, they are now institutionalised! Less-well-off cultures have little need to make such a distinction. Better health care from the cradle to the grave in the United Kingdom also gave rise to a greater proportion of the population living longer. Certain psychological processes such as memory and attention, as they developed and changed over our life, became of interest to developmental psychology. Alzheimer’s disease and senile dementia began to become known. Greater understanding of memory in old age has been of benefit to an understanding of memory across the whole of the human lifecycle. Lifespan development from the point of view of 14 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS physical, cognitive, social and emotional development and change was born. In a general sense each age-stage, or particular age span, sees us investigating physical, cognitive, social and emotional development which helps developmental psychology put forward ideas as to how we develop and change throughout our life. A holistic physical, cognitive, social and emotional overview of all these four parts of developmental psychology is why the topic is often said to take a whole-life approach to lifespan development. Particular age-stages sometimes emphasise one aspect of development and change over another. For example, investigation at the prenatal stage is almost exclusively physical or biological in nature. Investigation of infants during the neonatal stage places more emphases on physical and cognitive development, rather than social or emotional. The period of childhood allows developmental psychologists to begin to investigate our social, moral and intellectual development. Adolescence sees us study aspects of emotion and social behaviours in relation to great physical (and cognitive) change for the developing teenager. Finally, adulthood and old age is particularly concerned with physical and cognitive development and change, personality and social development. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 15 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Age-stages and related physical, cognitive, social and emotional developments within and across the developmental lifespan 1. The prenatal stage Physical development The period from human conception to birth, called the prenatal stage, lasts around 280 days (9 months) and is divided into three stages of foetal development: • the germinal stage • the embryonic stage • the foetal stage. The germinal stage Conception gives rise to the first stage of prenatal development, the germinal stage. Conception involves successful fertilisation of the female ovum (egg) by a male sperm in the female fallopian tube. During the next 10–14 days, the zygote, the fertilised egg, repeatedly copies and divides itself (mitotic division) as it travels to the uterus. Masses of cells are formed (the human embryo) and implant on the lining of the uterus. These cells are undifferentiated (not different) from each other, in that each is an exact copy of its neighbour. The embryonic stage When the cell mass implants, the embryonic stage begins. For the next six weeks, the cells differentiate both in structure and function. What this means is that some cells develop into protective structures, i.e. the placenta, the umbilical cord and the amniotic sac, while others go to form basic body parts. The placenta is the protective organ that surrounds and allows nourishment to, and waste elimination from, the developing embryo. The umbilical cord carries blood containing essential nutrients from the mother to the embryo via the placenta. It discharges waste from the embryo to the mother by the same route. The amniotic sac surrounds the developing embryo in a thick protective suspension fluid called amnion. Other differentiating cells develop other body structures. By 8 weeks after conception it is possible to identify some basic body structures taking shape. At this stage many of these do not work, or more accurately are non-functional. 16 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS The foetal stage The foetal stage begins when a basic form of each structure is present. For the remaining 32 weeks of pregnancy cells continue to divide and differentiate to build functional, or working, body structures and to increase the size and body weight of the developing foetus. Physical development proceeds in a cephalocaudal fashion. This literally means ‘head first’ – hence the reason why a baby’s head is so much larger than the rest of its body when born. Indeed the next two years of life see the baby’s body catching up, size-wise, with its head. The foetus is still entirely dependent on its mother for nutrients and waste disposal. Quick Questions • Given that a developing foetus is entirely dependant on its mother’s own body environment for nutrients and waste disposal during the pre-natal stage, what implications does this have for the developing baby? • What measures can a mother take to better ensure the birth of a healthy baby? 2. Infancy The neonatal period Infancy spans the first two years of life, beginning with what is called the neonatal period of the first month of life. The neonate, as the newborn is often referred to in developmental psychology, is very competent. Although their cerebral cortex (brain and spinal cord) is not yet mature, neonates display a variety of inborn or innate behaviours called reflexes. An innate ‘rooting’ reflex (turning in the direction of an object when it brushes against your cheek) allows it to locate the nipple despite poor vision. The automatic sucking reflex of a newborn allows it to operate on its mother’s nipple, or bottle, to allow for essential feeding which helps ensures its survival. Newborns also display a well-developed sense of hearing and smell that they use to identify people. Visual acuity, the sensation of being able to see things clearly, is still developing. Quick Questions • Why do you think these reflex behaviours are available to the newborn infant? • Does a baby learn these behaviours? If not, how do you think they come about? DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 17 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Physical development From birth to two years of age it is very apparent that a baby physically develops at an astonishing rate. Observation of growing infants indicates that their body part proportions change as the arms and legs catch up with the head and upper body. Development of muscles and the motor centres of the cerebral cortex together allow the infant to progressively reach, grasp, roll, sit, crawl, walk and vocalise (talk). The order in which these psychomotor developments occur is the same for infants the world over. Known as epigenesis, this is seen as evidence of the role of maturation, which is the common process of physical growth due to biological/genetic ageing rather than learning. Cognitive development While this is dealt with later in these support notes as an optional key concept, it is useful to note that it is in infancy the symbolic function emerges. This is the term developmental psychologists use to describe how we develop our ability to think to ourselves and to speak to others, i.e. we think and speak using commonly agreed language symbols which represent objects, images, people, events, etc. Infants gradually learn that certain combinations of the babbling sounds they make represent, or symbolise, people and objects. This is aided by the maturation of brain areas associated with the development of memory and language such as Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area. When conducting a post-mortem on a patient, who could understand but not produce speech, Broca (1861) discovered very specific brain damage in the brain’s left-frontal lobe. He deduced this area to have something to do with our ability to produce the sounds of the words we make. Later, in 1874, Wernicke identified an area of the brain at the top of the temporal lobe that is responsible for our ability to understand speech. This followed a case study and post-mortem on a man who could produce speech but could not understand what was being said to him. Infancy also sees the maturation of memory. By age two our memory structure is the same as that of an adult. This gives us the ability to remember, among other things, what combination of sounds represent what objects in our complex world. We put them together first simply, and then in a more complex fashion to enquire about things and to put forward our own point of view. Toddlers have an amazing capacity for enquiry, quickly realising that the people, objects, images and sounds that make up their world have ‘names’ which represent them. Even better, everyone who can speak agrees on what these names for things 18 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS represent! The discovery of the importance and power of language by infants at this early age is an indication of just how vital the maximisation of its development is for their future survival. By age two most infants can communicate in two- or three-word sentences, can think through simple problems said to them and have a basic understanding of the power of ‘language’, as seen in the way they often deliberately use crying to get what they want. All this is related to the work of the famous psycholinguist Noam Chomsky whom we will discuss in greater detail later on. A psycholinguist is someone who studies the origins, development and importance of language for human beings. Chomsky (1959) argues human beings the world over are born with a biological predisposition towards acquiring language. Our brains are essentially wired and programmed to allow us to acquire, understand and use language. Chomsky says we have to trigger this innate language acquisition device (LAD) by our teenage years; otherwise we can lose the ability to vocalise and suffer the effects of language privation as a result. The development of language is age-stage related and depends to a great extent on physical/biological developments in our brains, or cerebral cortex. The interaction of biological maturation, our environment and individual cognitive development is most important. Quick Questions • Why is it important to learn to talk in infancy? • What does language allow us to do as individuals? • What does language allow us to do with others? Social and emotional development During the first two years of an infant’s life increasingly more complex social and emotional behaviours begin to emerge. As early as two months, infants begin to react in a consistent way towards certain objects and events, e.g. showing excitement when ‘Dadda’ comes into the room. These consistent patterns of behaviour are known as infant temperaments. They might be said to be genetic predispositions towards a particular personality type, suggesting that the beginnings of individual personality may be innate. Temperaments are the building blocks to personality. All infants display them but in differing degrees. Interestingly, the first attempt to classify people into personality types using temperaments was made over 2500 years ago by Galen in Ancient Greece. He believed we each behave in a particular way because of an excess of one of four bodily fluids in our body. These fluids are blood, DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 19 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS phlegm, black bile and yellow bile. An excess of one of these fluids or humours was the cause of a particular temperament or general personality. Thus, if you had excess blood you would have a sanguine personality; excess phlegm – a phlegmatic personality; excess black bile – a melancholic personality; or excess yellow bile – a choleric personality or disposition. We still use some of these phrases to commonly describe people’s personality today, even if the cause and effect relationship is wrong! Social behaviour also occurs during this time. By six to eight months infants form attachments to important people in their world, e.g. mum, dad, brothers and sisters (called siblings) or indeed anyone who would be thought of as the baby’s caregiver. By age 2 most infants begin to look forward to opportunities to socialise with other children. This helps them begin to realise that we are by nature gregarious creatures, in that we naturally seek company, but that living successfully with others means you have to be less selfish, more co-operative and agree to stick to certain rules. The realisation of this does not happen easily for the developing infant as he or she enters the next age-stage in developmental psychology. 3. Childhood Physical development Childhood is the period from about age 2 to 12. In general, physical growth slows markedly in comparison to the prenatal and neonatal agestages. Behavioural changes, on the other hand, appear almost daily. Children begin to show individual differences in comparison to others, i.e. observable differences in skills and behaviours among children of the same age, background, culture, etc. These individual differences arise as a consequence of genetic endowment, affecting physical and cognitive development and the child’s individual experience in the world. Individual differences are often seen in children who early on show a proficiency for music, sport, art, etc. Just how far these apparent differences will develop is heavily influenced by our experiences in our environment. By age 5–6 overall physical growth declines to a slow but steady rate. Permanent teeth begin to appear. By now we can quite accurately predict who will be short or tall in adulthood. Individually children show improvements in gross and fine motor skills and hand– eye co-ordination. They don’t fall over as much playing football and at least try to tie their own shoelaces! They can catch a ball thrown at them, cut with scissors and symbolise and draw ever more meaningful representations of things in their world. 20 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Cognitive development Cognitively, children develop progressively more sophisticated memory and reasoning skills during childhood. The reason for this is the continuing biological maturation of our cerebral cortex or brain. We usually cannot remember anything about our lives before age 3. The reason for this is that the part of our brain responsible for long-term memory is still developing during this time. Although children between 2 and 5 can clearly pay attention and learn, they often fail to recognise what they need to know to solve a problem and how to use this knowledge more efficiently as a result. They thus approach learning tasks in an unsystematic fashion. Biological maturation, increased experience of similar learning situations and increased exposure to new and novel ones, all interact during childhood to help identify particular stages in cognitive growth; especially that of thinking. It is interesting to note that the child’s developing personality also affects their thinking. Children between 2 and 5 consistently think, understand and interpret their world entirely from their own point of view. Their thinking is said to be egocentric. They have difficulty in appreciating anyone else’s point of view. Mum saying ‘No’ to a demand for sweets just does not compute! Thinking also tends to be rigid and categorical and very difficult to change once learnt. Once a young child thinks she has grasped the rules of a game, for example, it is very difficult to get her to appreciate that the ‘rule’ concerning everyone else having to let her win is one her Nana made up and does not apply in the real world! From age 5 to 12 children are better able to focus on the relevant features of a learning task, and can understand the effects their, and others’, behaviours may have. Thinking becomes more logical, flexible and creative. It becomes evident that the cognitive process of thinking and problem solving develops in stages. This is in contrast to another cognitive process, language. Language is mastered by children between ages 2 to 5. The un-grammatical two-andthree word sentences of the two-year-old are rapidly replaced by longer, more grammatically correct sentences, containing nouns, pronouns, verbs, adjectives, etc. We have an insatiable desire for the acquisition of language in early childhood. Children of this age are constantly seeking meaning, and wanting to give meaning to, the objects, people, images and situations they experience around them. They also have an awareness of what is ‘good’ and ‘bad’ vocabulary and sentence construction. Language development can be enhanced greatly by caregivers, teachers, siblings (brothers and sisters), etc., gently correcting the language mistakes of children. They don’t seem to mind and indeed seem to want to know what is the more correct word or DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 21 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS phrase in the circumstance. This development of vocabulary and grammar greatly benefits thinking. By age 5, young children are also aware of the rules or conventions used in spoken language, such as taking turns in conversations, sticking to the topic of conversation and adding information to conversations to increase its content and interest for others. The development of language is age-stage related and depends to a great extent on physical/biological developments in our brains, or cerebral cortex. The interaction of biological maturation, our environment and individual cognitive development is once again emphasised. Intelligence It is not the purpose of Advanced Higher Developmental Psychology to debate the question ‘what is intelligence?’. This is more the province of units like ‘Individual Differences’. Further, the study of ‘intelligence’, as found within developmental psychology, is examined in greater detail in the later optional key concept ‘Cognitive development’. By way of introduction however, it may be useful to try to get a general idea of what intelligence might be, and more importantly, what influences there can be on the maximisation of whatever intellectual potential we have. This is us back to the nature–nurture debate, or the influence our genetics and environment play in the unfolding of intellectual ability. What intelligence is has intrigued mankind since the beginning of time. In a general sense, ‘Intelligent activity consists of grasping the essentials in a situation and responding appropriately to them’ (Heim, 1970). The ability to be able to do this is as a result of our genetic inheritance from our biological parents and our own learning experiences in the environment we are brought up in. Debate has raged as to how much genetics and environment each individually contribute to our ‘intelligence’. Psychologists like Eysenck and Jensen would argue genetics contributes over 75 per cent to our ‘intelligence’; while Kamin would argue the opposite – that environment’s contribution towards intellectual ability is over 75 per cent. Who is more correct? The answer is neither. The science of genetics nowadays allows us to say that biological/genetic inheritance does play a part in overall intellectual growth and that, as measured by intelligence tests (or IQ – Intelligence Quotient – tests), genetics contributes towards 60 to 70 per cent of our IQ. On the other hand, environment contributes between 30 and 40 per cent. As most of the population share the same IQ of around 100, the genetics–environment debate is a bit pointless. Developmental psychology is much more interested in the interaction of genetics and environment and how we can maximise all people’s ‘intelligence’ by being aware of the importance of environment as the factor which affects 22 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS the difference between one person developing their intellect to the maximum and another not being able to do so. The social setting, or environment, in which you find yourself is of immense importance to the potentialisation (maximisation) of whatever abilities you have in the first place. Social development From age 5 or 6 most children have greater opportunities to interact, or mix, with children of their own age. Parents become less important as the influence of the peer group grows. A peer group can be described as those you identify as being most like yourself, i.e. your classmates, friends, work colleagues, etc. Its influence is great, especially in the teenage years. This is particularly emphasised by sociologists and social psychologists when they talk about socialisation. They say we all go through a socialisation process where we learn the common rules, norms (standards of behaviour), beliefs and values of the society we are brought up in. Major influences on socialisation are parents, teachers and our peer groups. Experiences obtained from mixing with others will influence the child’s developing personal social identity or self. During childhood, individual children begin to show a variety of social patterns of behaviour when they are around others. This may be because of a genetically influenced temperament, and certainly be because of previous past experience. We can often identify those children who are shy or insecure and those who are confident and display leadership qualities. Popular children are typically ‘good-looking’, self-confident and competent, and find that others seek out and enjoy being in their company. Children who are perceived by other children as being behaviourally different in some way, i.e. if they are over-aggressive, often find difficulty in joining and being accepted by the peer group and ultimately can be left to themselves. Most interestingly, a child’s skin colour, class, cultural background or physical difference in comparison to others does not seem to be an issue in the formation of the peer group at this age. This is sadly not the case for long. As the child gets older, the influence of a previously important peer group will diminish. Parents have less and less influence on the child as he or she gets older. The power of whomever a child perceives as its peer group is enormous. A pre-school child is heavily influenced by its parents. This begins to wear off when the child starts school. Initially the teacher is looked upon as some kind of god, and the same child will often contradict her parents’ wishes with the phrase ‘The teacher said so’. By age 7 or 8 the same child no longer meekly accepts her mum’s idea of what is style! By the teenage years, DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 23 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS being seen to want to be like your mum, dad or teachers is definitely uncool. The socialisation process and the influence of different peer groups on each individual’s social development should not be underestimated. Sociability Buss and Plomin (1984) identify sociability as a particular temperament we all have at birth. Sociability is genetically inherited and innate. Sociability is in our nature. It is our gregarious or social side to being human – our inbuilt need to want to be with others. We need others from a biological, cognitive, social and emotional point of view. Sociability is very evident from birth. Babies react with excitement to encourage our social behaviours with them. They appear to know that smiling and being pleasant to others is a necessary prerequisite to their survival. Our biological genes will determine the amount and types of sociability we each show towards others as we grow and develop. Sociability temperaments or behaviours can be identified as: • our individual, but common, need to look for and get pleasure from being with others • how much or how little we each need to be on our own or with others • how much we need to give of ourselves to shared activities • showing emotion towards and getting emotions from other people. These predispositions of temperaments begin to unfold in infancy. Our social experiences, and what we learn as a result, are also influential in determining our sociability towards other people. If we were more shy than other babies in early infancy and this ‘shyness’ has been commented upon and viewed negatively by others as we grow, it is not at all surprising to find the same person showing introverted adult personality traits later on in life. Put another way, we all share a need for others but we do not all share the same degree of need for others. Some people will be most at home being very outgoing and constantly in other people’s company. Some others will be equally happy being just part of the group, or not part of any particular group at all. Some people will be perfectly happy being on their own, thinking or reading to themselves. Others would find this a reflection of just how popular they think they are, and would worry about it! Some of us have an immediate need to tell a new-found friend 24 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS our whole life story, warts and all; while other people on meeting someone they don’t know too well would be a lot more cautious about what they reveal of themselves. This is a sign of the degree of need each of us has towards being sociable with others. Self-image or personal identity From as young as 2, children begin to develop an idea of personal identity or self-image. They have a self-concept, in that they know who they are and who they are in relation to other people, i.e. they understand their name; that they are part of a family and know who is mum, dad, aunt, uncle, brother, sister, neighbour, nursery nurse, primary 1 teacher, etc. At age 5 most children’s descriptions of who they think they are will be very categorical and rigid. This is especially true in terms of age, gender and associated behaviour patterns. A child of 6 will be very precise as to whether he or she is 6, 6½ or nearly 7 and a girl, for example, will behaviourally display her gender through genderrelated play, preferences and conversation. ‘Star Wars’ is more appealing to little boys than little girls, while ‘Snow-white’ has more appeal for girls. Developing self-image and related sex-role identification in young children is greatly influenced by the messages they receive from their environment. Sex-role identification (comprehending that you are a little boy or a little girl) and associated gender behaviour (behaving in a masculine or feminine way) is very evident from age 2–5. Children’s sexrole behaviour is also categorical and rigid. They have a very stereotyped idea about what is masculine and feminine behaviour. Many little girls in Scotland of around age 5 will not wear trousers – despite the weather. They associate trousers with little boys of the same age, and they are definitely not one of them! This can be much to the dismay of parents and other primary caregivers who try to bring up their children in a non-stereotypical way, i.e. by not encouraging toy guns and weapons for little boys, or dolls, toy kitchenware, etc., for little girls. In this instance caregivers face an uphill struggle. What young children see other boys and girls doing, what they see brothers and sisters doing, what they see mum and dad doing, what they see on television, etc. all contribute to the development of their self-concept, their identification with male or female sex-roles and associated gender behaviour during this time. Between ages 5 and 12, a child’s self-concept and sex-role begins to widen out. The primary school years see children begin to describe themselves using terms like ‘nice’, ‘smart’, ‘tall’, ‘small’, ‘pretty’, ‘handsome’, etc. This is evidence that they are beginning to compare, contrast and most importantly evaluate, or value, themselves in DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 25 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS comparison to other children in their peer group. This social comparison is very influential across the whole of childhood and the teenage years in the development of our self-confidence (how good we feel about our self) and self-esteem (how much we value, respect and appreciate whom we have become). Emotional development Attachment: critical and sensitive periods Attachment, in psychology, is the special term used to describe the mother–infant bond formed at birth or soon after by a wide variety of species including human beings. The special name given to the unique human mother–child attachment, or bond, is called monotropy. It is an intense emotional bond that is of immense importance in the physical, cognitive, social and emotional development of any newborn. In humans its absence can seriously affect the developing person’s ability to form loving, sexual and non-sexual relationships with others. Its absence can also affect their moral development, essentially the ability to discriminate between right and wrong and also their ability develop intellectually. The social, emotional and cognitive effects of the absence of maternal love, or attachment, in early life was first brought to our attention by Dr John Bowlby in his paper ‘Maternal Care and Mental Health’ in 1951. He argued that a monotropic bond has to be established between caregiver and child either at birth or during early infancy otherwise the child may develop cognitive, social and emotional difficulties in later life. We shall return to Bowlby and other related concerns when we look at our first AH key concept: ‘Early socialisation’. Social and emotional development Personality The social experiences we go through influence the development of our personality. What is clear is that the emerging personality is shaped from very early on in childhood and appears to progress through a series of stages. The idea that personality development occurs in stages throughout early childhood was, as we know from the first unit ‘Introduction to Psychology’, first studied by Sigmund Freud in the early 1900s when he put forward his psychosexual theory of personality development. It suggests that children go through a series of psychosexual stages in childhood where they obtain pleasure from particular parts of their body known as erogenous zones. It further says that we are biologically and instinctually driven to maximise the pleasure obtained from stimulation of these zones. In doing so, our associated behaviour is often viewed as taboo. How our parents deal with this, and 26 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS our experience as a result, Freud believed, could damage the emerging personality. This ‘damage’ he identified as particular, and sometimes peculiar, behaviours or fixations in adulthood. Setting the scene: developmental psychology’s whole-life approach to lifespan development – summary Lifespan development involves developmental psychology taking a whole-life approach to the study of physical, cognitive, social and emotional growth and change from birth to death. Our lifespan is divided up into: the pre-birth or prenatal stage; infancy or neonatal stage; childhood; adolescence; adulthood and old age. Each sees developmental psychologists investigating particular aspects of physical or cognitive or social or emotional change. Very often a change in one area of human development will have an impact on others. Physical, cognitive, social and emotional developments and change at particular points in our life are all inter-related to each other. Developmental psychology is interested in whether certain physical, cognitive, social and emotional events and changes happen to us all at roughly the same time and whether we develop physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally progressively in stages or in a continuous way. Physically we all develop at approximately the same rate and in the same fashion the world over. Physical development and change is thus linked to our biological maturation. Physical change is genetically determined and age-stage related. In order to develop cognitively, we have to be biologically ready to do so. Some cognitive developments, identified as having a critical or sensitive period, happen within a particular agestage, e.g. the acquisition of language in infancy/early childhood, while others, e.g. the ability to do mathematics, must wait until we cognitively develop and grow from birth until adolescence. Biologically influenced physical change allows us to socially and emotionally develop and (hopefully) grow across all age-stages in life. We cannot walk until we are biologically ready to do so, but when we are this allows for greater, more independent experiences in our world. Puberty allows for the development of an important side of what it means to be human – the long-term establishment of a close, loving and stable relationship with another. How we handle developing social relationships with others is very much influenced by our personalities as shaped by previous past experience. How we change and develop as thinking, social and emotional individuals very much depends upon the environment we are brought up in and the experiences we have within it at certain ages and stages in our lives. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 27 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Student activity Questions 1. Identify four continuous and progressive changes across the human lifecycle. 2. What name does developmental psychology give to the approach which studies these four continuous and progressive changes over the human lifecycle? 3. What three assumptions does this approach make concerning our development? 4. What perspective in psychology does the following sentence describe? ‘Our biology or genetics control and shape what we become physically, cognitively, emotionally and socially.’ 5. What is its rival perspective called? 6. What is the process of physical growth called? 7. Name one age-stage related ability we mostly all develop from age 2 to 5. 8. What do developmental psychologists call the period of growth from conception to birth? 9. With what cognitive process are Broca, Chomsky and Wernicke associated? 10. What needs to happen for individuals before they can cognitively develop? 11. Of what might infant temperament be a sign? 12. What do you think the term ‘egocentric’ means to a developmental psychologist? 13. If you think it is 30:70 and your classmate thinks it is 70:30 what are you discussing? To what do the figures refer? 14. Name the process where we learn common norms, beliefs and values. 28 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 15. What is sociability? 16. If you know who you are, and who you are in relation to others, you could be said to have a good what? 17. With whom is the term ‘monotropy’ most associated? What did he study? 18. Name and describe the first three age-stages in Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development. 19. What aspect of being human does Erikson think our social experiences can influence? 20. What is meant by the whole person or whole-life approach in developmental psychology? DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 29 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Glossary: Lifespan development affectionless psychopath: identified by Dr John Bowlby, it is an extreme personality due, he thinks, to maternal deprivation (absence of mother-love in infancy) gravely affecting our social and emotional development. Related to imprinting in animals, it reveals itself in humans in people who are unable to form close loving, sexual and non-sexual, relationships with others; who have little or no moral values and have a very casual approach to crime. Affectionless psychopaths lack conscience for their actions. attachment: the close emotional bond best formed in very early infancy between human babies and their primary caregivers – usually their mothers. It is driven by our need for physical comfort, and the amount and quality we receive is thought to help shape the developing adult personality and drives how we deal socially and emotionally with others and ourselves. biology: the scientific study of living organisms; human, animal and plant. cerebral cortex: our brain and spinal cord. chromosomes: rod-shaped structures that appear in the nucleus of a cell during cell division; a chromosome consists of nucleoproteins arranged into genes, which are responsible for the transmission of physical, cognitive and possibly social and emotional characteristics from biological parents to child. cognitive development: concerns perception, attention, memory, language and thinking. Developmental psychology is very interested in the development of thinking in particular. critical period: a set period during our development where it is thought certain things must happen in order that the organism learns essential skills for its future survival. First identified by the ethologist Konrad Lorenz, he suggested a duckling has a critical period of 24 hours within which it must trigger its innate following behaviour by imprinting on the first large moving object it sees – usually its maternal parent. If imprinting does not happen during this critical period, later development is affected. deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA): our genetic inheritance passed onto us by our biological parents which influences physical, cognitive, social and emotional development – though not all in equal measure. DNA, 30 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS or deoxyribonucleic acid, is what the ‘nature’ side of the nature– nurture debate is all about. What influences us more – our genetic inheritance in the form of DNA or our environment? developmental pathways: Michael Rutter believes that the experiences we get during infancy and childhood, especially in the form of a stable, loving family environment, determine in many respects the relationships we form with others as we get older. If our parent(s) or primary caregivers were caring, loving, nourishing, supportive, etc., to us during our childhood, we are more likely to show the same behaviour in our own relationships with others. developmental psychology: the study of physical, cognitive, social and emotional changes and developments, including their causes, effects and inter-relationships over the course of the human lifespan. dysfunction: abnormal or disturbed behaviour as a consequence of physical, cognitive, social or emotional difficulties. egocentric: seeing and interpreting the world solely from your own point of view. Egocentricity is very evident in children from about age 2 onwards. It should disappear as children gain more experience of their worlds, especially in social and emotional relationships. In adults egocentrism would be recognised as continual and consistent selfishness. emotional development: an aspect of developmental psychology that involves personality, aggression, motivation, the development of a self-concept, etc. Emotional development is closely related to social experience, cognition and biologically determined physical changes during our lifetime. environment: the world we live in and the experiences we have in it. experimental group: a term used in scientific research that indicates that a selected group of people are receiving a ‘treatment’ of some kind or another, which another selected group, a control group, are not. This is to allow for comparison between the two groups to see if the treatment has caused any effect. epigenesis (or epigenic principles): the idea that we cannot do something until we are biologically mature enough to do so, i.e. walk, talk, remember complex events, think in an adult fashion, etc. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 31 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS external validity: how a theory holds up in relation to alternative theories on the same issue. For example, Jean Piaget has a most useful theory concerning the way children learn. However, learning can also be explained in psychology from the point of view of behaviourism, social learning theory and cognitive social learning theory. extrapolate: to generalise about your findings to a larger, and perhaps different, population. erogenous zones: areas of the body associated with pleasurable sensation and activity. Identified by Sigmund Freud, erogenous zones include the mouth, anus and genitals. ethology: the biological study of animal behaviour. fixation(s): an aspect of an adult’s personality usually identified as a peculiar behaviour which has been caused by under- or overstimulation of an erogenous zone in childhood, or as a consequence of a parent’s attitude to a child getting pleasure from an erogenous zone. foetus: the developing embryo. gender: masculine or feminine specific behaviours. Our sex, male or female, is genetically determined. Gender on the other hand is learnt. genetics: the study of inherited characteristics. genotype: an individual’s collection of genetically inherited characteristics from their biological parents that may influence their physical, cognitive, social and emotional development. holistic: a whole-person approach in psychology. Understanding a person from all relevant points of view, i.e. understanding a person’s development, taking into account the causes, effects and relationships between physical, cognitive, social and emotional changes and developments across the human lifespan. imprinting: an innate ability in many animal species that sees a newborn attaching to the first large moving object it sees by following it. Imprinting is crucial to survival. 32 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS independent group design: a research strategy used in psychology that sees two dissimilar groups of people being compared to each other in an experiment. individual differences: physical, cognitive, social and emotional differences between individuals. The study of individual differences in psychology looks at, for example, personality and intelligence. These are aspects of being human which we all share and recognise in each other, but in which we notice differences between us. infant temperament: a personality disposition displayed in infancy, e.g. a quiet baby, a demanding baby, etc. Temperaments are thought to be influenced by our genetic inheritance and are the basis to the eventual adult personality. internal validity: how scientifically correct is the method used to collect and collate appropriate data that contributes towards a theory. IQ (Intelligence Quotient): an IQ test score for a ‘normal’ person will be 100. LAD (Language acquisition device): the biological ‘hardware’ in the brain that sees us innately want to acquire and develop language. longitudinal study: a psychological study into an individual or group of individuals over at least one year following physical, cognitive, social or emotional changes and developments. maternal privation: the complete absence of mother-love in infancy which is felt can contribute towards social and emotional difficulties in later life. maturation: associated with our genetics, maturation is the biological ageing of our bodily processes and structures – often necessary before physical, cognitive, social and emotional developments can occur. monotropy: the unique mother–infant bond formed at birth. motor skills: a particular physical dexterity or ability related to biological maturation, e.g. body manipulation, crawling, walking, etc. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 33 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS nature perspective: those who believe that what we are, and what we can become, is determined largely by our genetic inheritance. Our genetics, according to this position, will determine our physical make-up, cognitive abilities, social relationships and emotional behaviours. neo-Freudian: someone who follows in the psychoanalytic tradition established by Sigmund Freud in the early 1900s. neonatal stage: the first month of human life. nurture perspective: those who believe that what we are, and what we can become, is determined largely by our environment. Our environment can affect physical make-up, cognitive abilities, social relationships and emotional behaviours. peer group: a group of people we most closely associate with. Peer groups can include friends, workmates, your community, etc. The influence of the peer group in the formation of social behaviours, our identity and our emotional responses to life is most apparent in adolescence. phenotype: what our genetic inheritance actually becomes, physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally. Our phenotype is strongly influenced by the environment we are brought up in. pre-natal stage: pre-birth – the 9 months of human foetal development. primary caregivers: includes those who look after, love and nurture us in infancy, childhood and adolescence. Primary caregivers are mothers, fathers, stepmothers, stepfathers, aunts, uncles, older brothers or sisters, etc. privation: never having something in the first place. This is opposed to deprivation which means having something and then having that ‘something’ taken away from you. psychomotor development: the development of centres of the brain which allow for physical motor skills to develop. psychosexual theory of personality development: a theory on personality development being determined by psychosexual experiences in infancy and childhood put forward by Sigmund Freud. He says this experience will form aspects of the adult personality. He divides childhood into five psychosexual stages – oral, anal, phallic, 34 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS latent and genital, and says frustration of pleasurable activities associated with related erogenous zones of the body can lead to peculiarities of personality and behaviour in later adult life. These are called fixations. psychosocial theory of personality development: an alternative theory on the development of personality put forward by Erik Erikson. He says personality is shaped not just by childhood experience but throughout our whole life. Unlike Freud he says that personality is not a result of erogenous pleasure, but as a consequence of having to make decisions based upon past experiences at certain points of our life. He calls these decision points ‘psychosocial crises’ and says we confront them at eight stages throughout our life. The influence of our social world in the ongoing development of our personality is all important. physical development: genetically determined biological body changes and developments over the course of the human lifespan. These will have a knock-on effect on cognitive, social and emotional aspects of the ageing individual. rooting: a newborn’s innate (inherited) reflex action to turn towards objects brushing against its cheek. self-concept/self-image: an idea of who you are as an individual. The self-concept begins to be formed in infancy with the gradual realisation of your sex. You begin to form your identity by behaving in a masculine or feminine way. This is helped along by observation and imitation of what other ‘boys’ and ‘girls’ are seen doing in your environment. self-esteem: how much you respect and value yourself. Our self-esteem comes from how others react to us. We think and behave accordingly. sensitive period: a period in our life where we are most susceptible, and better able, to learn how to do something. Language has a sensitive period from about 2 to 14 years of age. sequence of development: physical, cognitive, social and emotional changes across the human lifecycle, some of which happen in identifiable and progressive stages (age-stage cognitive problemsolving and language abilities, etc.) while others happen in a similarly identifiable but continuous manner (the ageing process, non-age stage development of personality, etc.) DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 35 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS sibling(s): brother(s) and sister(s) sociability: our ability to interact, or mix, with others. Sociability is related to our developing realisation that life is so much more enriched when we are with others in a family, class, work or community situation. However, being with others has a personal cost for individual freedom. We have to give up a part of our individuality to successfully work together as a group. Sociability involves balancing the rules of social interaction against our individual personality. social development: the causes and effects of an individual’s social behaviour across the human lifespan. A study of social development sees developmental psychologists looking at how we develop and change as a consequence of learning experiences in our environment. Issues of social interest include the development of play, moral development, the influence of the peer group in adolescence, the reasons why you view your self as you do, etc. socialisation: the process we go through as individuals in learning our culture’s rules, norms (acceptable behaviours) and values (beliefs). Important to the process of socialisation are parents (especially our mothers), teachers, the adolescent peer group and society’s structures and institutions like work, the judicial system, the press, etc. symbolic function: being able to think using commonly agreed symbols, e.g. words for things found around us. An infant begins to show it can think using symbolic representations when it shows continuous excited behaviour when ‘Dada’ comes into the room. whole-life approach: the causes, effects and inter-relationships between physical, cognitive and social development across our whole lifetime. An understanding of memory problems in old age gives greater understanding to the development of memory across the whole of the human lifecycle. The centres of our brains associated with memory have to be biologically ready (by about age 2) before we can begin to develop language and simple problem-solving strategies. Our memory will affect our cognitive, social and emotional behaviours thereafter. It makes sense in this, and other areas in developmental psychology, to take a whole-life approach to lifespan development. 36 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Concept 1: Early socialisation What is ‘early socialisation’ in developmental psychology? ‘In a developmental sense childhood lasts a lifetime.’ Keegan (2002) Early socialisation in developmental psychology examines how we come to be as we are. It specifically looks at sociability and attachment as early childhood experiences and how these mould our developing personality and consequentially impinge upon emotional and cognitive development. More generally, early socialisation concerns the processes in infancy and childhood of acquiring the norms and values of our society including language, gender role behaviour, moral development and the control of aggression. In human beings this involves a symbiosis between our innate abilities (nature) and the culture we find ourselves in (nurture). Early socialisation is thus both biological and cultural. The learning of these norms and values, or ways of behaving, is called enculturation. In a psychological sense early socialisation at Advanced Higher level sees interest focus upon: • • • • sociability attachment, deprivation (separation) and privation theories of attachment (Bowlby, Ainsworth) social and cultural variations in child-rearing. Sociability As proposed earlier, early socialisation includes the study of sociability. Buss and Plomin (1984) identify sociability as a particular temperament we all have at birth. They also identify emotionality and activity as the other dimensions to our individual temperaments which also influence the emerging personality, self-image or emerging personal identity. Further, sociability is genetically inherited and innate. Sociability is initially in our nature and may be influenced by the inter-uterine environment. It is our gregarious or social side to being human – our inbuilt need to want and to be with others. We need others from a biological, cognitive, social and emotional point of view. If one takes attachment alone, a baby who is sociable is more likely to form a secure attachment (and subsequent benefits) than a baby who is not. Put another way, sociability which is genetically determined not being reciprocated in kind (due to a frustration of the attachment process) DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 37 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS teaches a baby very negative lessons which may influence his/her personality from a very young age indeed. Put bluntly, if you cannot attach to someone despite giving it your genetic all in the early months of life, who can you attach to in a broader more adult sense later on? Sociability is a prerequisite to attachment – successful or otherwise. Sociability is very evident from birth. Babies react with excitement to encourage our social behaviours with them. They appear to know that smiling and being pleasant to others is a necessary prerequisite to their survival. Our biological genes will determine the amount and types of sociability we each show towards others as we grow and develop. Sociability temperaments or behaviours can be identified as: • our individual, but common, need to look for and get pleasure from being with others • how much or how little we each need to be on our own or with others • how much we need to give of ourselves to shared activities • showing emotion towards and getting emotions from other people. These biological predispositions of temperaments begin to unfold in infancy as we interact in our environment. Sociability (and personality) is therefore a dance between nature and nurture. Our social experiences, and what we learn as a result, are influential in determining our sociability towards other people. If we were more shy than other babies in early infancy and this ‘shyness’ has been commented upon and viewed negatively by others as we grow, is it not surprising to find the same person showing introverted adult personality traits later on in life? Put another way we all share a need for others but we do not all share the same degree of need for others. Some people will be most at home being very outgoing and constantly in other people’s company. Some others will be equally happy being just part of the group, or not part of any particular group at all. Some people will be perfectly happy being on their own, thinking or reading to themselves. Others would find this a reflection of just how popular they think they are, and would worry about it! Some of us have an immediate need to tell a new found friend our whole life story, warts and all; while other people on meeting someone they don’t know too well would be a lot more cautious about what they reveal of themselves. This is a sign of the degree of need each of us has towards being sociable with others. 38 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Heredity: innate social abilities Babies exhibit innate social abilities as a consequence of evolution. These social abilities that come to us through nature are adaptations which have survival value for us. The study of evolutionary algorithms in evolutionary psychology suggests that human survivors in the past were most likely to be the babies who produced innate infant-primary caregiver social signals to which the caregiver responded – probably as a result of caregiver genes they had inherited themselves. Examples of innate social abilities include: • The appearance of babies, which makes them attractive to older people. Lorenz (1943) observed that their ‘kewpie-doll’ appearance of large forehead, large round eyes and soft-rounded features was common to the infant of many species. Manufacturers of children’s dolls have been exploiting the kewpie-doll effect for decades. • Athough when a baby smiles is debatable, smiling is a very powerful means of non-verbal communication. It begins to become evident from about 8 weeks but nobody knows for sure whether this first smile is a result of personal pleasure the baby is experiencing (asocial smiling) or whether it is pleasure in interaction with others. Cooing and crying see adults respond with interest or concern. • Imitation is also an evident ability in infants. Meltzoff and Moore (1977, 1983, 1989, 1992) regularly report on this. They have found babies less than 7 days old able to imitate another’s face movement such as opening and shutting their mouth, forehead furrowing, etc. This is strong evidence for participative social interaction. • This all, of course, points to the evolutionary aspect to all our emotions. We share similar emotions the world over. A smile in John Finnie Street, Kilmarnock means the same thing as a smile in Albert Road, Hout Bay, South Africa – it is to be hoped! There has been a reawakening of interest in the psychology of emotions first brought to our attention in the nineteenth century by functionalist William James. This is mainly due to its Darwinian influences and current interest in where we come from and why we are as we are as we enter a new millennium. Genetically shared emotions include interest, joy, surprise, fear and anger, as identified in mothers of one-month old infants during interview (Johnston et al. 1982). This was confirmed by Izard (1982) with his research into these five primary emotions where he took photographs of very young infants expressing interest, joy, DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 39 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS surprise, fear and anger all as a result of non-harmful stimuli. He then got adults to successfully determine the infants’ emotional states in the photographs. Infants as young as two months old communicated all five primary emotions. Interest, joy, surprise, fear and anger at least appear to be innate. Environment: learned social abilities Social interactions appear also to be influenced by what we learn in/from our environment. A baby is an immense learning machine and it is said a baby learns more in the first two years of life than in the rest of its life put together (Keegan, 1999). One way a baby learns is through reinforcement. Reinforcement was first brought to our attention by behaviourists such as Watson, Pavlov and Skinner. They say we learn by forming stimulus-response units of (learnt) behaviours as we interact with our environment. If we show a behaviour that is encouraged by positive reinforcement (a reward of some kind) operant conditioning theory (Skinner) says we are likely as individuals to repeat the desired behaviour in the future. An adult making funny faces and getting a chuckle or two from a baby in return, the baby then making funny faces and getting an enthusiastic response from the adult in return (and so on) is an example of positive reinforcement of the innate ability to smile/laugh. It is also sad to observe a number of teenage mothers using physical punishment, or negative reinforcement, in the form of slaps administered to young babies, infants and toddlers to dissuade them when they cry in public; often as a result of some discomfort or distress they are suffering. As said, social abilities or sociability are both influenced by nature– nurture! The humanistic influence on early socialisation: self-image or personal identity From as young as 2, children begin to develop an idea of personal identity or self-image. They have a self-concept, in that they know who they are and who they are in relation to other people, i.e. they understand their name, that they are part of a family and know who is mum, dad, aunt, uncle, brother, sister, neighbour, nursery nurse, primary 1 teacher, etc. At age 5 most children’s descriptions of who they think they are will be very categorical and rigid. This is especially true in terms of age, gender and associated behaviour patterns. A child of 6 will be very precise as to whether he or she is 6, 6½ or nearly 7 and a girl, for example, will behaviourally display her gender through gender- 40 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS related play, preferences and conversation. ‘Star Wars’ is more appealing to little boys than little girls, while ‘Snow-white’ has more appeal for girls. Developing self-image and related sex-role identification in young children is greatly influenced by the messages they receive from their environment. Sex-role identification (comprehending that you are a little boy or a little girl) and associated gender behaviour (behaving in a masculine or feminine way) is very evident from age 2–5. They have a very stereotyped idea about what is masculine and feminine behaviour. Many little girls in Scotland around age 5 will not wear trousers – despite the weather. They associate trousers with little boys of the same age, and they are definitely not one of them! This can be much to the dismay of parents and other primary caregivers who try to bring up their children in a non-stereotypical way, i.e. by not encouraging toy guns and weapons for little boys, or dolls, toy kitchenware, etc. for little girls. In this instance caregivers face an uphill struggle. What young children see other boys and girls doing, what they see brothers and sisters doing, what they see mum and dad doing, what they see on television, etc., all contribute to the development of their self-concept, their identification with male or female sex-roles and associated gender behaviour during this time. Between ages 5 and 12 a child’s self-concept and sex-role begins to widen out. The primary school years see children begin to describe themselves using terms like ‘nice’, ‘smart’, ‘tall’, ‘small’, ‘pretty’, ‘handsome’, etc. This is evidence that they are beginning to compare, contrast and most importantly evaluate, or value, themselves in comparison to other children in their peer group. This social comparison is very influential across the whole of childhood and the teenage years in the development of our self-confidence (how good we feel about ourself) and self-esteem (how much we value, respect and appreciate whom we have become). An influence on sociability, it is dealt with more specifically as the key concept ‘Social behaviour’ in Advanced Higher Developmental Psychology. Student activity: Essay Describe, discuss and evaluate the concept of sociability in developmental psychology. You should use about 1000 words for your answer. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 41 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Attachment, deprivation (separation) and privation An attachment according to Ainsworth (1989) is an affectional bond involving: ‘a relatively long-enduring tie in which the partner is important as a unique individual and is interchangeable with none other.’ Deprivation in a developmental sense means having ‘something’ and then it being taken away from you, while privation means never having had that ‘something’ in the first place. In its original sense ’something’ is the mother-figure which nowadays would be broadened to include all who fall into the genus of ‘primary caregiver’. Schaffer’s phases in the development of attachments Schaffer (1996a) says the attachment process can be divided up into various phases: 1. The pre-attachment phase: birth–3 months. From 6 weeks babies show preference for human beings over and above the physical aspects of their environment (see Fantz, 1961: ‘The perception of human faces’). This is shown in behaviours such as nestling, gurgling and (social) smiling directed at most in their world. 2. The indiscriminate attachment phase: from 3–10 months babies start to discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar people, becoming more animated and excited in the presence of those they have a previous knowledge about. The social smile directed at anybody has disappeared. They will allow strangers to pick up and cuddle them as long as this is done in a caring considerate manner. 3. The discriminate attachment phase: evident from 7/8 months, this is the beginning of specific attachments identified by proximity behaviours, and separation anxiety. It parallels the onset of object permanence in babies (the notion that objects, events, people, etc., do exist outwith and away from themselves). 4. The multiple-attachment phase: 9 months plus. This is the phase where strong additional attachments are made with other important caregivers, e.g. dad, grandparents, siblings (brothers and sisters) and the developing peer group (other children). Proximity behaviours and separation/stranger anxiety diminishes with the strongest attachment continuing to be with the mother. 42 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Schaffer and Emerson (1964), in ‘The beginnings of the end of Cupboard Love theory’, describe how 60 infants were investigated every month up to 12 months, and then at 18 months. Mothers told of their observations of their babies in seven everyday anxiety situations such as being left alone, being babysat and put to bed. Infant protest was quantitatively and qualitatively reported. Schaffer and Emerson discovered the babies were clearly attached to multiple-caregivers who did not engage in care-type activities – most notably the father! Also in 39 per cent of cases, the caregiver who bathed, fed and changed the baby was not the child’s main attachment figure. Important predictors of attachment are responsiveness to the infant and stimulation given them by significant caregivers. Theories of attachment: cupboard love; psychoanalysis and behaviourism For the first half of the twentieth century, developmental psychology understood attachment from the point of view of ‘cupboard love’ theories as proposed by the psychoanalytic and behaviourist approaches. Freud believed that infants become attached to their mothers as a result of instinctual needs and drives within them. ‘The reason why the infant in arms wants to perceive the presence of its mother is only because it already knows that she satisfies all its needs without delay.’ Freud (1926) Freud surmised healthy attachments come about due to feeding which satisfies the baby’s need for food, security and oral (sexual) gratification – see Freud’s Oral Stage in his theory of psychosexual development of personality. Attachment is frustrated when a baby does not get its oral stage needs satisfied, or if they are satisfied too much (an oral retentive personality personified by the adolescent/adult nail biter, finger chewer, lip chewer, smoker, etc.). For psychoanalysis, breast-feeding and the presence of a maternal figure is important to healthy attachment and all that follows. Behaviourists also have a place for food in their understanding of the attachment process. They say babies become attached to those who respond to their demands for physiological comforts such as food, warmth, security, etc. Food is a primary reinforcer. This means a reinforcement which is not learnt (unconditioned). Caregivers in the DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 43 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS baby’s mind become conditioned (learnt) or secondary reinforcers. The baby associates the pleasurable satisfaction of physiological needs with the caregiver through positive reinforcement. The attachment bond is formed. The end of cupboard love theory For the background to modern attachment theory see Section 3 Issues: ‘The use of non-human animals in research’). Attachment sees the individual want to be around the attachment figure. It gives those forming an attachment security, comfort and confidence. The strength and worth of this influences our self-concept (who we think we are as a person, and as a result why we feel and behave as we do). The attachment figure (e.g. a primary caregiver) represents a secure base that should allow for safe exploration of the unknown, unfamiliar or threatening by the ‘attached’ infant or child. Attachments in humans Borrowing from ethology (see Section 3 Issues: ‘The use of non-human animals in research’) the bonding process in humans is the beginning of the attachment process. The Scots Schaffer and Emerson (1964) define attachment as: ‘the tendency of the young to seek the proximity of certain other members of the species.’ Macoby (1980) identified four attachment characteristics in infants and young children such as: • • • • wanting to be near the primary caregiver distress when separated from the primary caregiver joy on being re-united behaviour orientated towards the primary caregiver. As stated earlier, Dr John Bowlby was interested in the psychological effect imprinting has for human beings. His maternal deprivation hypothesis accepted Harlow’s idea that the first two years of an infant’s life is the critical period in its ability to form and develop an attachment to its mother. This unique human mother–child bond he called monotropy. ‘It is because of this marked tendency to monotropy that we are capable of deep feelings’ (Bowlby, 1988). It is unlike any other relationship the infant may have in its life and its frustration, or absence, 44 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS can cause the infant social and emotional difficulties in later life. The strength and worth of the monotropic bond influences our self-concept. Bowlby was able to argue this on the basis of a study he conducted in 1946 in his clinic in London where he worked with disturbed (or dysfunctional) teenagers. He was able to compare 44 teenagers identified as thieves with a similar group of teenagers who showed dysfunctional behaviour but were not criminal. The results of the study, contained within his report Forty-four Juvenile Thieves: Their Characters and Home Life, indicates that among the 44 thieves, 17 had been separated from their mothers in early infancy. The control group by comparison only contained 2 teenagers who had been separated from their mothers in early infancy. He further found that among the 17 maternally deprived thieves, there were 14 who were even more socially and emotionally dysfunctional than their colleagues in that: • they were unable to form close relationships with anyone • they had few or no moral values • they had a very casual approach to crime and the effects their crimes had on their victims. He concluded that maternal deprivation was the cause of his 44 thieves’ social and emotional problems and that ‘mother love in infancy and childhood is as important for mental health as are vitamins and proteins for physical health’ (Bowlby, 1951). So much so that according to Bowlby, lack of maternal love, or monotropy, can result in: • juvenile delinquency • low IQ • affectionless psychopath (a sociopath). Situations where these extreme effects of maternal deprivation can occur include, for Bowlby: war; famine; mother in prison; full-time working mother, etc. This was explored further by Bowlby in 1949 when he was commissioned by the World Health Organisation (WHO), a part of the United Nations (UN), to investigate the psychological effects of the Second World War (1939–45) on the thousands of displaced children who were wandering Europe at the time. He confirmed much of his earlier findings (that lack of mother-love in infancy can jeopardise intelligence and create social and emotional difficulties) in his paper to the UN Maternal Care and Mental Health, published in 1951. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 45 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Is Bowlby correct? When a theory is proposed in psychology, it is normal for it to be scrutinised at two levels. We ask: 1. how valid is the theory in the light of the research which backs it up? Has the research been conducted in a scientific manner? Are any contributory theories used in the research inappropriate? This is called looking at a theory’s internal validity. 2. how valid is the theory when compared and contrasted with alternative theories concerning the same topic, in this case the consequences of mother-love in infancy? This is called a proposed theory’s external validity. Internal validity In putting his theory together, Bowlby used two sources to give it some impetus. First, Bowlby cites Goldfarb (1943) who himself looked at one group of children who were in care until the age of 3 (his experimental group) and another who were in care very briefly after birth and were then adopted (control group). This allowed Goldfarb to conclude that the experimental group, in comparison to the control group: 1. 2. 3. scored lower on IQ tests had problems with moral rule keeping lacked social skills in their dealings with others. Goldfarb (1943), using a matched pairs design, paired 15 children from 6 months to age 3½ brought up in an institution with 15 children who had been fostered immediately on separation from their biological mother. The institutionalised children had been effectively socially isolated in their first year. By age 3 the institutionalised group were intellectually inferior to their fostered peers. They lacked social maturity for their age and had difficulty with moral rule-keeping. By age 10 to 14, the institutionalised group still showed cognitive, developmental social and moral deficits. Their IQ for example was in the order 72 and 95. Goldfarb concluded this was all down to whether the children had been fostered or not. No account was taken by him of individual differences such as race, gender, sociability, physical appearance, intellectual ability, etc., apparent at the point of fostering and indeed probably the basis upon which one child was fostered and another was not. 46 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Bowlby, secondly, used research generated by Spitz (1945, 1946) and Spitz and Wolf (1946). Spitz and Wolf proposed that children who had lost a parent in their early childhood often suffered deep depression well into their teenage years, and this slowed down cognitive, social and emotional development, in comparison to children who had two parents during their upbringing. Spitz (1945, 1946) and Spitz and Wolf (1946) investigated anaclitic depression found in orphans ‘brought up’ in South American orphanages. Here they were deprived of even the most basis sensitivity responses by their untrained and overworked caregivers. This gave rise to psychopathalogical symptoms such as apprehension, sadness, sobbing, withdrawal, appetite loss, weight loss, insomnia and cognitive deficit. If this deprivation continued more than 3 months Spitz (1945, 1946) and Spitz and Wolf (1946) said recovery is impossible. Using Goldfarb and Spitz and Wolf as the backdrop to his research, Bowlby then put forward evidence coming from his own clinic for dysfunctional teenagers in London. His research seemed to indicate that there was indeed some connection between absence of mother-love in infancy and a dysfunctional personality in later adolescence and adult life. However, critics argue: 1. Goldfarb in 1943 did not discover why it was that some children in his study were deemed suitable for adoption, and others, who remained in care, were not. Individual differences could have played a part in deciding who was to be adopted and who was to stay in care. Individual differences might be the reason behind lower IQ scores, moral rule-keeping problems and social relationships. 2. Children in institutions, it is argued, are not just deprived maternally. They do not experience the diversity of family life. Their learning experiences which shape social and emotional behaviours in the institution would have been different in comparison to those children who found themselves adopted. Maybe this was an alternative reason for these children’s shortcomings in comparison to the adopted group. 3. Spitz and Wolf were talking about paternal deprivation – the loss, after experience of having, a father or father-figure in the family home. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 47 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 4. Bowlby’s research methodology was flawed. He used a retrospective case study approach with his 44 thieves. Retrospective case studies see participants reflecting back on their lives. As a result they are prone to problems concerning a participant’s selective memory, shadow memory or incomplete records. As these 44 teenagers were themselves convicted of criminal activities, a retrospective case study approach in this instance is even more prone to these shortcomings. 5. Retrospective case studies allow the sample to pre-select itself. This is not a good research technique. It would have been better if Bowlby had been able to conduct a longitudinal case study – taking a whole group of children born on the same day and researching them over at least the next year. A good example of a televised longitudinal case study is the BBC’s 7-Up series which every 7 years looks at the development of a group of UK children all born in the same week in 1955. Another is the year 2000 longitudinal study, written and presented by Lord Robert Winston for the BBC, which will follow the physical, cognitive, social and emotional development of a group of babies born in the UK on 1 January 2000 over their human lifespan. 6. Of Bowlby’s 44 thieves, details in individual case studies suggest that 27 were not separated from their mother at birth or in infancy. Questions must arise therefore as to why they became criminal. 7. Bowlby was a psychoanalyst. As a result he over-concentrated on what happens to us in the first five years of our life, and generally ignored the later years. Developmental psychology would recommend we find out about a person’s whole life before trying to conclude what and where in their lifetime things happened which made them what they are. 8. Finally, as regards the internal validity of Bowlby’s theory on mother-love in infancy, he ignores the reality of life in Britain. He advocated a ‘constant care’ mother–child relationship as the only way to avoid social and emotional difficulties in later life. However, he did not take account of the millions of children who are brought up in a situation where mum is absent for one reason or another – and who do not suffer low IQ, juvenile delinquency or affectionless psychopathy. He also ignored the different childrearing arrangements we have in Britain, such as the use of relatives in helping to look after children if parents are absent; the 48 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS employment of nannies and nursery nurses; situations where both parents work; single-parent families; cultural child-rearing differences and practices and alternative child-rearing strategies. Look up: ‘Controversial aspects of Bowlby’s Attachment Theory’ by Juan Carlos Garelli http://www.geocities.com/Athens/Acropolis/3041/controversy.html Bowlby’s theory of maternal deprivation: Its external validity Juvenile delinquency Examining the causes of juvenile delinquency in 1972, 1979 and 1981 and while generally supporting Bowlby, Michael Rutter concluded that in his opinion the main factor leading to delinquent behaviour such as stealing was whether or not a teenager’s home-life was stressful and unhappy. He was also critical of Bowlby’s error of confusing different types of deprivation. Powers, Ash, Schoenberg and Sorey (1974) lend some support to Rutter’s view. They looked at why you get one-off juvenile offenders and recidivists (continual or habitual offenders). They believe that the factor here is stress in the home. An unstable family environment seems to be important in the development of juvenile delinquent behaviours. Affectionless psychopathy This is not, Rutter (1972, 1979) argues, the result of maternal deprivation, but maternal privation. Privation is never having had something in the first place. In looking at Bowlby’s research Rutter suggests that those who had been institutionalised and had developed dysfunctional behaviours had never had the experience of maternal love in the first place. The effects of maternal privation were examined by Pringle and Bossio (1960). They looked at a sample of children who were institutionalised. The sample broke down into two groups. One group, whom they labelled as ‘maladjusted’, had come into care soon after birth; while the other ‘stable’, more developmentally normal group of children had spent at least one year in their family home. Their maladjusted group showed traits similar to those associated with affectionless psychopathy. They showed complete indifference towards others. Privation – not having the opportunity to form close relationships with others in early DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 49 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS infancy – seems to have an effect and gives some clues as to the formation of a personality which exhibits ‘affectionless psychopathy’ or ‘sociopathy’ in particular and non-biological ‘severe personality disorders’ in general. Freud and Dann (1951) investigated children who had survived Nazi concentration camps. They found six children who had lived in a camp from an early age and had miraculously survived the Holocaust. They had not formed any close relationships with adults, were maternally deprived and if Bowlby was correct should therefore have shown symptoms of affectionless psychopathy. Freud and Dan discovered that they were, very understandably, suspicious of others but were not affectionless or maladjusted. This was probably due to the fact that while in the camps the children had stuck together and had formed close attachments with each other (see the work of ethologists such as Lorenz and Harlow). Hodges and Tizard (1978) looked at the permanency of any effect maternal deprivation had. They investigated children who had spent most of their lives in care and had not been adopted until late in childhood. Tizard and Hodges found that over time their participants could develop deep and loving relationships with the right adoptive parents. The early days were difficult for children and parents alike. Patience and support in a stable family environment were crucial in helping the children overcome the effects of maternal deprivation/ privation. Low intelligence We now know that Bowlby relied on a number of other people’s research to frame his own theory on maternal deprivation. One of these was Goldfarb. Goldfarb argued that children who suffered from maternal deprivation could end up less intelligent than peers who had a more normal family life. The development and maximisation of any skills and abilities we may have are now known to be a lot more complex than the presence or absence of mother-love in infancy alone. For example, Denis (1969) investigated children in an Iranian orphanage which seemed to produce children who were less intelligent than others. He found that, although well cared for physically, they lacked much in the way of intellectual stimulation. This he felt to be more important in the overall development of intelligence than maternal love or privation. He also believes that there is a critical period up to age 2 where intellectual stimulation (as found in the ‘normal’ family home ) has to occur otherwise children suffer an intellectual deficit in comparison to others – which they are never able to close. While there 50 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS is some merit in what Denis says there is debate over his idea of a critical period of intellectual development. Skeels (1939) thinks you can reverse the effects of privation. In the course of his work he came across two children in an orphanage who were both very intellectually challenged. They were subsequently transferred to a home for the mentally subnormal (as they were referred to in the 1960s). Within 3 months the change in the two children was dramatic. They were more alert, intelligent and sociable. Skeels discovered that because the children were so young they had been ‘adopted’ by the others in the home. They had been cuddled, talked to and encouraged to play much more than before. Skeels concluded that this was the reason for their overall improvement in intellectual, social and emotional abilities. Linking his 1939 study to his later study in 1966 is worth considering. The original study had 25 participants, all of whom were brought up in an American children’s home until age 2. The home offered little in the way of intellectual, social or emotional development. When they were 2, 13 of the children were put into a home for the mentally retarded. Their average IQ was 64.3; the average normal child of the same age would have an IQ of 100. Care was given by older, subnormal girls. The children got structured and supervised play opportunities. There was a high staff–child ratio, etc. By age 3½, the 13 children in the experimental group had either been adopted or transferred back to their original orphanage. The IQ of the experimental group had risen to 92.8, in comparison to their 12 peers in the control group who had remained in the original orphanage throughout this time. Their IQ was found indeed to have fallen from 86.7 to 60.5. By age 7, the average improvement for members of the experimental group was 36 points, while the average loss for the control group was 21 points. In a longitudinal study, Skeels followed the participants into adulthood. He found that of the experimental group: • • • • • all had had more education than the control group participants one third of them had gone on to college one third had got married one third had given birth to ‘normal intelligence’ children all were, in the main, self-supporting. The control group, who had been brought up in institutions all their childhood and teenage years, were unable to earn enough to be selfsupporting and were still regarded as mentally retarded. Skeels’ work suggests that it is the content and amount of intellectual stimulation we receive in early childhood which is important to eventual social and emotional development and not, as Bowlby thought, the DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 51 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS amount of maternal love we receive. An intellectually poor environment will ultimately stifle intellectual development which will affect our developing personality, socially and emotionally. Michael Rutter (1981, 1989), in attempting to pull together the arguments concerning Bowlby’s theory of maternal deprivation, and the intellectual, social and emotional difficulties which might emerge as a result, believes that maternal privation (but not deprivation as Bowlby thought) does have consequences. These consequences can result in intellectual, social and emotional difficulties. As a result of the early experience of having, or not having, been brought up in a stable, caring, loving and stimulating environment, children’s developmental pathways into adulthood are affected. The amount of sensitivity, support and warmth from others in our early years is vital to nourish our ability to develop genuine sensitivity, support and warmth to others in our later adult years. For later social and emotional stability for us as individuals early attachments are crucial and should be encouraged and developed. Our home-life should be relatively stress-free. The effects of an inability to form attachments with caregivers in our early years are not permanent nor irreversible. Low IQ is not as a result of maternal deprivation, or privation, but because we have grown up in an intellectually unstimulating environment. This can be enhanced by putting someone into a more stimulating environment which is itself loving, caring, supportive and encouraging for that person. In 1998, Rutter et al. were able to revisit the influence of severe privation on physical, cognitive social and emotional development. They studied 111 Romanian orphans adopted in the UK before the age of 2. When they arrived in Britain they were deemed severely developmentally impaired. With physical attributes only being shared by 3 per cent of similar British children they showed low weight and small head circumference. A control group of UK children showed none of these physical delays. By age 4 Rutter et al. reported the Romanian children had caught up with their host country cohort. Age of adoption, however, correlated negatively with achievement of key developmental milestones. The later adopted, the slower and more difficult the progress. Another group of Romanian orphans were researched by Morison et al. (1995). They compared two groups of age- and sex-matched children. One group spent at least 8 months in a Romanian orphanage before being adopted while those in the other group were adopted within 4 months. They discovered the later adopted children made up on previous delays in development on a progressively successful month-bymonth basis. 52 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Social and cultural variations in child-rearing The reason why developmental psychology is interested in social and cultural variations in child-rearing is because cross-cultural study allows us to consider how much of the attachment process is as a result of heredity and/or the environment or culture we find ourselves in. A culture, according to Cara Flanagan (1999), ‘refers to a set of rules, morals and methods of interaction that bind a group of people. These rules, morals and so on are the products of socialisation, that is we learn them through our social interactions with other members of our culture.’ We come from diverse cultures and within each are sub-cultures separated by class, ethnicity, religion, economic opportunity, education, etc. These account for cultural variations in the socialisation process, a major contributor to which is the way children are ‘brought up’. This is referred to as ‘child-rearing’ in psychology and has resulted in a large amount of study into attachment and child-rearing practices which influences the process and other wider aspects to personality. By far the most important contributor in this area is Mary Ainsworth (1967, 1971, 1978). In 1967 she studied 28 breast-fed babies near Kampala, Uganda. When her study commenced, the babies were aged from 15 weeks to 2 years. They were observed at home every fortnight, for 2 hours during a 9-month period. Using naturalistic observations and interviews with the mothers she identified species of specific attachment behaviours. These fell into three types. She made strong correlations between the mother’s own sensitivity rating (derived from interview) and amount of cuddling given by the mother (naturalistic observation). One group she labelled securely attached, the second insecurely attached and the third not-yet-attached. She inferred the independent variable resulting in these individual differences in attachment behaviours to be maternal sensitivity to signals made by the infants. In 1971, Ainsworth replicated her study in Baltimore, USA. Van Ijzendoorn and Schuengal (1999) feel so moved with this that they call it ‘the most important study in the history of attachment research’. As in Uganda, Ainsworth relied on interview and naturalistic observation in her longitudinal study of 26 mother–infant pairs. They were visited at home every 3–4 weeks for 3–4 hours at a time. Each mother–infant pairing generated 72 hours worth of data. In order to make sense of this DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 53 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Ainsworth used a previously set standard against which to compare her observations of attachment behaviours. This was her own ‘Strange Situation’ devised by herself and a colleague earlier (Ainsworth and Wittig, 1969). In the Baltimore study their Strange Situation was altered to allow for mother–infant behavioural patterns to be identified. Eight Strange Situations in Baltimore (Ainsworth et al. 1978) Episode Present? Length of time Description 1 Mum, baby, observer 30 secs Observer brings mum and baby into room then leaves. 2 Mum, baby 3 mins Mum sits passively while baby explores. If needed, play stimulated/encouraged after 2 minutes. 3 Stranger, mum, baby 3 mins Stranger comes in. 1st minute quiet. 2nd minute stranger talks with mum. 3rd minute stranger approaches baby. After 3 minutes mother leaves without being seen by baby. 4 Stranger, baby –3 mins* First separation episode. Stranger involved in active play with baby. 5 Mum, baby +3 mins** Stranger leaves. Mum greets/ comforts baby. Attempts active play then leaves. ‘Bye, bye.’ 6 Baby –3 mins Second separation episode. 7 Stranger, baby –3 mins Continued separation. Stranger tries active play with baby. 8 Mum, baby 3 mins Mum returns, greets and picks up baby. Stranger leaves without being seen. *episode stopped if baby very distressed ** episode continued if baby became distracted away from it 54 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS The Strange Situation gives four measures of the security of the baby’s attachment based on four behaviours. These are separation anxiety (the distress the baby shows when mum leaves), the infant’s willingness to explore (attached infants are less proximal) and stranger anxiety (attached infants show more distress when left in presence of stranger). Furthermore, Ainsworth et al. were particularly interested in the types of reunion behaviours shown by the babies on the return of their mothers. This she felt was an indication of the quality of attachment that existed between mother and baby. As a result of the quality of attachment, came associated infant behaviours and emergent personalities, functional and dysfunctional. The variable influencing quality of attachment, as identified earlier in Uganda, she found to be maternal sensitivity to the baby. Sensitive mothers have securely attached babies and insensitive mothers have insecurely attached babies who fall along two category axes: anxious avoidant/detached or anxious resistant/ambivalent. The Type A insecurely attached anxious avoidant infant cares not one jot when mum returns; shows little stranger anxiety; reunion behaviours are absent on mum’s return. The Type B securely attached infant plays happily when mum is present; ignores mum’s presence while playing; plays happily when stranger is around/absent; gets distressed when mum leaves; play reduces; seeks comfort and proximity immediately she returns; calms down quickly; resumes play. Distress is caused by mum’s absence, not being alone. Qualitative and quantitative behaviours are evident in infant–mother and infant–stranger interactions. The Type C insecurely attached anxious resistant infant shows distress when mum leaves and is not easily comforted on her return. The child seeks and rejects cuddling contiguously. Mum is inconsistent in her behaviours, sometimes insensitive (angry and rejecting) and subsequently sensitive to the point of over-responsiveness. Limited exploratory play by the child as it tends to be proximal to the mother. A fourth and recognised Type D insecurely attached disorganised infant has been identified by Main and Solomon (1986). This infant shows no set pattern of behaviour either when separated or reunited with its mother. Flanagan (1999) notes that ‘this kind of behaviour is associated with abused children or those whose mothers are chronically depressed’. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 55 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Evaluating the Strange Situation 1. Methodological comment: While the Ugandan and Baltimore studies confirm that sensitivity is the key variable to secure/ insecure attachment, in both relatively small samples were used. 2. However, Ainsworth’s contribution has led to greater work being done in this area. An important supporting study comes from Ijzendoorm and Kroonenberg (1988) who reviewed 32 worldwide studies across eight countries and 2000 children. They concluded on the basis of this meta-analysis of cross-cultural data that different cultures (and thus factors involved in socialisation such as childrearing practices) give rise to different weightings of Type A, B and C infants. In one study from Japan there is an absence of Type A insecurely attached anxious avoidant, but a high proportion of Type C insecurely attached anxious resistant infants. In the other, the results are more consistent with Ainsworth’s types and spread. There appears to be a worldwide pattern of Type A, B and C securely/insecurely attached infants as indicated by Sainsworth, but the USA shows greater variation when samples are contrasted. This is probably due to the many cultural backgrounds adhered to in America. It is itself a microcosm of the world’s different cultures. Ijzendoorm and Kroonenberg (1988) also found cross-cultural differences in that while Type B securely attached infants are found worldwide, Type A securely attached are more common in Western European societies while Type C insecurely attached anxious resistant infants are more common in the likes of Japan and Israel. 3. Vaughn et al. (1980) discovered that attachment type can alter as home circumstance changes. They investigated children at 12 and 18 months of single parents living in poverty. Attachment types differed significantly dependent on changing family circumstance. Better housing and less stress from the point of view of the mother seemed important. This suggests attachment types are not permanent as theory might imply. 4. Another implied observation about attachment behaviours is that they reflect temperament. If this were so, in the case of simple multiple attachments between infant and mother and infant and father, the attachment behaviours (secure/insecure) should be the same (van Ijzendroon and De Wolff, 1997). This was not the case as found by Main and Weston (1981) who say there is a quality of distinct relationship found between infant–mother and infant– father. The baby can be securely attached to one but not the other. 56 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 5. Lamb et al. (1985) and Melhuish (1993) criticise the Strange Situation in general as being too artificial, limited in terms of the information it obtains and failing to take account of the mother’s behaviour. There is also the ethical issue of deliberately putting infants under stress. Marrone (1998) counters this from the point of view of ecological validity. The Strange Situation does reflect reality. Let us leave the last words on attachment to Scroufe et al. (1983) who said ‘securely attached infants are likely to be more confident, enthusiastic, and persistent in problem solving later on as young children’. Student activity: Essay In around 1500 words describe, discuss and evaluate Dr John Bowlby’s theory of monotropy. Further reading: early socialisation Bowlby, J, A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory, London: Tavistock/Routledge, 1988 Durkin, K, Developmental Social Psychology: From Infancy to Old Age, Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, 1995 Flanagan, C, Early Socialisation: Sociability and Attachment, Routledge Modular Psychology Series, 1999 Rutter, M, Maternal Deprivation Reassessed (2nd ed.), Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1981 Schaffer, R, Mothering, Glasgow: Fontana/Open Books, 1977 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 57 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Glossary: Early socialisation affectionless psychopath (a sociopath): someone who lacks conscience as regards their often criminal activities. attachment: active, emotional and mutual relationship between two people (usually infant and parent/primary caregiver) with interaction reinforcing and strengthening the link or monotropic bond. critical period: a set time where something must occur otherwise a deficit is experienced. developmental pathways: the physical, cognitive, social and emotional paths we journey along in life influenced by experience. external validity: ability of a theory/piece of research to sustain itself when confronted with alternative explanations for the phenomenon under investigation. enculturation: the learning of society’s norms, values or ways of behaving. gender: roles and behaviours regarding the significance of being male or female. internal validity: the ability/inability of contributory pieces of research to sustain a proposed theory. longitudinal study: a (case/observational) study that lasts more then one year. matched pairs design: an experimental design where groups of participants are matched for extraneous physical, cognitive, social and emotional confounding variables. monotropy: the attachment/affectional bond formed (usually) between mother and infant. nature: aspects of an individual’s physical, cognitive, social and emotional make-up influenced by genetics. nurture: aspects of an individual’s physical, cognitive, social and emotional make-up influenced by environment. 58 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS self-concept: knowledge of personal identity – who you are and who you are in relation to other people. self-image: a person’s sense of self or personal identity. primary caregivers: person who qualitatively and quantitatively nurtures a baby, infant, child, etc., in the absence of the biological mother. reinforcement: something which increases/decreases the likelihood of a behaviour being repeated, i.e. a reward or punishment. self-esteem: how much we value, respect and appreciate whom we have become sex-role identification: sex-role and associated gender behaviour; behaving in a masculine or feminine way evident from age 2–5. socialisation: process of developing the habits, skills, values and motives shared by members of a particular society or culture. sociability: the particular temperament we have at birth; influenced by genetics and foetal environment and is the building block to personality. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 59 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Concept 2: Cognitive development The study of cognitive development is, as is ‘early socialisation’, an optional key concept at Advanced Higher level. Students are asked to consider two key concepts and one issue. This is the second key concept considered in this AH pack. Cognitive development is the study of how our brains are able to take in information from the outside world and what we do with this information. It concerns how we create perceptions concerning our world, how we form and are able to retrieve memories and ultimately how our cognitive development affects our actual behaviour. At Advanced Higher level SQA ask that we at least consider here: • Theories and research of cognitive development – Piaget, Vygotsky Bruner, information processing • Practical applications of theory and research to education • Factors which affect language development – Chomsky. In developmental psychology, cognitive development is particularly interested in what changes in cognitive functioning occur as we age and what factors are responsible for these changes. It is an area of study in psychology in general, and developmental psychology in particular, which has benefited from the work of Jean Piaget. Jean Piaget was born in Neuchatel, Switzerland in 1896 and died in 1980. He was educated at, and received his PhD (doctorate) from, the University of Neuchatel in 1917. His important works include: The Origins of Intelligence in Children (1952), The Language and Thought of the Child (1952), The Psychology of the Child (1969). Mainly interested in the study of the development of thought and knowledge in human beings (genetic epistemology) he proved that children think differently from adults. This was contrary to what was previously thought. He believed the reason children think (feel and behave) differently from adults was because their brains continue to biologically mature throughout childhood. Quite simply, they cannot think, feel and do certain things in the same manner as adults principally because they are not yet biologically ready to do so. Cognitive development is thus affected by biological maturation. Accordingly all children follow the same sequence of (cognitive) development. 60 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS It should be noted that Piaget took ‘cognitive development’ to be the same thing as ‘intelligence’. For Piaget, cognition (and therefore intelligence) is a biological process. He was consequently interested in the ‘biology of thinking’ and how it develops and unfolds as we biologically age and mature. For him, children’s intellectual development falls into certain identifiable stages. These stages are common to all children and all children go through them in the same order. Some children manage to go through a stage or stages quicker than others. This is evidence of individual differences in cognitive abilities. Individual differences are also evident in other areas of developmental psychology, e.g. personality. Cognitive development is seen as a building block process, where children have to be biologically mature before they can enter a particular stage. They master specific intellectual abilities during a particular stage, and having done so, if biologically ready, they enter and go through the next stage – which intellectually is different from the one which came previously. Most definitely, Piaget’s theory of cognitive development in children is an age-stage related theory which emphasises the relationship between biological maturation, physical maturation and developing cognitive abilities. As shall be seen, this developing of a child’s cognitive ability during infancy and childhood has social and emotional consequences. Quick Question • Name two behaviours most children have by age 2, which were not evident at birth. Why did they take time to develop? Piaget’s theory of cognitive development in children Essential vocabulary In order to appreciate the great contribution Piaget made to developmental psychology (and beyond) it is necessary to be aware of two important Piagetian principles. These are organisation and adaptation. Organisation It is thought that an infant learns more in the first two years of its life than in the rest of its life put together. As infants physically and cognitively develop they must come across countless new learning experiences every day. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 61 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS How do they deal with these experiences? Organisation is the term given to a baby’s ability to order and classify these new experiences in the mind. Organisation is an innate process common to all infants. We are born with this ability to organise all experiences we come across. Piaget said we innately organise experiences into a schema. A baby, when exposed to a new situation or stimuli, e.g. daddy, cows in a field, buses, etc., uses its organisational ability to form schema representing these new stimuli. It will have in its head a mental representation or schema of ‘daddy’, cows, buses, etc. Our innate ability to organise our world into schema is our ability to classify and categorise objects, events, situations and so on. Organisation is the beginning of intellectual functioning for all newborns. It is not learned, but biologically inherited or innate. Adaptation Adaptation is the infant’s growing ability to understand its surrounding world. Adaptation cannot occur unless there is a schema already established regarding the concept or aspect of its environment it is trying to understand and act upon. The two times table is completely meaningless to a child unless it has established a number schema! Adaptation depends upon two mental processes which Piaget labels assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation is said to happen when a child behaves in a way that suggests it has formed a schema about a particular set of stimuli. Assimilation is more primitive than accommodation. Assimilation sees a young child generalise about its world on the basis of an existing schema. For example, a toddler, Des Browne, is being pushed through Kilmarnock and sees a bus. Des points towards it, obviously wanting to know what this big object is! Mrs Browne says it’s a bus. Thereafter, anytime Des sees a bus he points at it and says ‘bus’. Des has formed a bus schema. Unfortunately he also points at caravans, lorries and tractors classifying them as ‘bus’ as well! This is assimilation – a young child’s general interpretation of its world based upon an existing schema. Des Browne has not yet developed the mental ability to discriminate between stimuli, or things that look a bit like each other – but are different. Accommodation is a more advanced form of assimilation. It is where the child restructures existing schemata in order to accommodate similar but different stimuli in its world. It essentially changes its existing mental structure (formed by existing schemata) so that new experiences can be added, understood and acted upon. Using our above example, accommodation is said to have happened when Des consistently and 62 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS successfully points to buses, lorries and tractors and identifies them correctly. This is accommodation – a young child’s specific interpretation of its world based upon restructuring existing schemata. Des has developed the mental ability to discriminate between stimuli that seem the same but are different. The development of thinking depends upon our ability to change our mental structures to face new challenges in life. We are able to do this from a young age. What pushes us towards having to change old schemata in order to accommodate our complex world is the tussle between equilibrium and disequilibrium. For a while at the assimilation stage, Des can happily interpret buses, lorries and tractors all as ‘buses’. It is not a problem. His existing bus schema does quite well. As time goes on and Des is exposed to more and more buses, caravans, lorries and tractors, and on being corrected by his mummy, he realises that a bus is different from a caravan/lorry/tractor. This puts the existing ‘bus’ schema in his mind in a state of disequilibrium (imbalance) in that caravans, lorries and tractors are not buses. As a consequence accommodation, or the restructuring of an existing schema, happens in order for Des to return to a state of equilibrium (or balance) in his interpretation of his world. On seeing a bus, caravan, lorry or tractor thereafter, he specifically identifies it correctly. Look up: The Construction of Reality in the Child by Jean Piaget (1955) ‘The Elaboration of the Universe’ http://www.marxists.org/reference/subject/philosophy/works/fr/ piaget2.htm Genetic Epistemology by Jean Piaget (1968) http://www.marxists.org/reference/subject/philosophy/works/fr/ piaget.htm DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 63 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Piaget’s stages of cognitive development in children Identifying a sequence of cognitive development common to all children, Piaget identified four age-stages each of which show the development of distinct thinking and problem-solving behaviours and abilities not available to the child in a previous stage. This is because the child is not biologically ready and therefore unable able to deal with the more complex problems each developmental stage contains. Epigenesis (in this instance, the biological maturation of our cerebral cortex) is therefore directly linked to cognitive development. This can be illustrated below. Epigenesis and cognitive development 1. Between the ages of 0–2 biological maturation of our sense organs and sensory and motor areas of our brain occurs. This must happen before cognitive functioning takes place. 2. From age 2–7 it has been found that the areas of our brain concerning hearing and speech must mature before basic thought processes dealing with vocabulary, mental imagery (thinking in pictures) and understanding can happen. The development of language is an obvious example here. 3. From 7–11 our brains continue to mature and develop in order to bring our cognitive ability to a more adult level. We develop an ability to see other people’s points of view and improve our social behaviours towards others. This was not apparent in the earlier years. 4. Finally at around age 11–13, our brains are thought to have finally matured to an adult level. We are able from this time to think abstractly about our world, reason logically and think in a scientific (or hypothetical) way. This also eluded us in the earlier years of life. 64 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Cognitive stages of development The sensori-motor stage (0–2) The baby initially can only deal with its world using innate reflex actions such as rooting and sucking. There is a gradual increase in sensory and motor awareness, i.e. vision, hearing and bodily movements. The baby has no realisation of the existence of anything outwith its own immediate experience, i.e. if something is hidden from a baby’s view then it no longer exists in its reality. This is what Piaget calls object permanence. He said that babies under 8 months had no object permanence. The concept of object permanence can be demonstrated quite simply. A ball is placed directly in the line of sight of a baby. It is moved to left and right. The experimenter knows that the baby is attending to the ball as its eyes are following its movements. When the ball is covered by a cloth, the baby no longer attends to it with its eyes and no longer reaches out for it. It has not yet developed object permanence (the knowledge that things do exist outwith and away from itself) and is egocentric, in that it cannot understand that the world can be seen from a variety of perspectives (or points of view) other than its own. Based on observations of his own children Piaget (1952) divided the sensori-motor stage into six sub-stages (see next page). By the end of the sensori-motor stage other cognitive structures such as selfrecognition (in a mirror) and symbolic thought (language) have also begun to emerge. Piaget terms these the ‘general symbolic function’ and also include deferred imitation and representational play. Deferred imitation means the ability to reproduce or imitate something now absent while representational play is play with objects which represent something else in the child’s mind. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 65 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Sensori-motor sub-stages Sub-stage 1: 0–1 month. Exercising reflexes. Reflexes practised. No ‘intentionality’, i.e. they mean to do something. No understanding of objects. Sub-stage 2: 1–4 months. Primary circular reactions. Reflexes extended to new objects. Infants coordinate simple schemata (grasping, looking). Realise and repeat ‘behaviour causing specific events’. Babies briefly look to the last place they saw an object. Sub-stage 3: 4–10 months. Secondary circular reactions. Coordination of all the senses. Anticipatory thoughts, feelings and behaviours evident. Can anticipate results of own actions. Partially hidden objects can be found. Sub-stage 4: 10–12 months. The co-ordination of secondary circular reactions. Infants represent objects in their minds and show beginnings of symbolic behaviours and memory. Goal driven behaviours and actions. Sub-stage 5: 12–18 months. Tertiary circular reactions. Infants search for environmental novelty and can use several interchangeable schemata to achieve goals. Beginnings of experimentation and curiosity as to ‘What might happen if….?’ Well-hidden objects can be found. Sub-stage 6: 18–24 months. Invention of new means through mental combinations. Evidence of thinking and problem-solving before actions. Mental manipulation of objects to achieve desired goal. Objects contained within others, i.e. hidden in a container, can be found. Based on Tomlinson-Keasey (1985) 66 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS The pre-operational stage (2–7) As toddlers enter this stage in their cognitive development, egocentrism gradually disappears. This can be seen in their less self-centred behaviours after about age 2 or 3. Object permanence becomes established and is correlated with anxiety fears in the presence of strangers as discussed earlier. Children at the younger end of this age range (2–4) do not yet have an ability to decentre, which is being able to see the world as others do. This Piaget calls the pre-conceptual substage. This is evident in Piaget’s mountain study. Here Piaget presented young children with an apparatus which had papier-mâché models of three mountains. One of the mountains had a house on top of it, the second had a snow-capped peak and the third had a wooden cross at its summit. A doll was placed and moved (orientated) around the base of the mountains. Children were asked what they thought the dolly could see. Piaget found that children during the pre-operational stage could not decentre very well. They reported what their eyes ‘told’ them as opposed to what the dolly’s point of view was. The most interesting concept of animism begins to emerge at this time as well. Animism is where children give toys and playthings a life-like quality. Dolls for example are perceived as ‘real’. Animism is very obvious in observations of children’s play. Children also have problems of conservation at the pre-operational stage, which is caused by only being able to take into account one aspect of an object or situation at any one time. Children between 2 and 4 have, for example, difficulty understanding volume and number. The cause of this is the perceptual appearance of the stimulus. The children have not yet cognitively developed reversibility (an understanding of what can be done without gain or loss). This is demonstrated below. Piaget’s conservation of number and volume (a) (b) (c) (d) d) In the diagram, (a) and (b) represent rows of chocolate buttons in a conservation of number task. If a child is presented with a choice between row (a) or row (b), it will choose row (b). This is because row (b) looks longer! The child can see one row of buttons is longer than DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 67 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS the other, but cannot yet understand the significance of the spaces between each button. It fails to conserve number. In the diagram, (c) and (d) represent a conservation of volume task. Both the tall and the fat glass are filled with equal amounts of lemonade. A child between 2–4, Piaget believes, if given a choice between (c) and (d) would invariably choose (d). This is because the tall glass looks as if it contains more to the child than the smaller but fatter glass. The child is looking at the heights of the liquid, but not the shape of the glass the lemonade is contained in. Children can only deal with one aspect of the stimulus at any one time. Students should note that Piaget thinks children at the pre-operational stage have difficulty not only conserving number and volume but also length and mass. By age 5 or so Piaget says they begin to throw off aspects of pre-conceptual thought and gravitate towards the second pre-operational sub-stage which he calls the intuitive sub-stage, evident in their ability to discriminate gradually between relative statements and actions such as ‘bigger’, ‘smaller’, etc. Their ability to do so in the pre-operational intuitive sub-stage he calls seriation. The ability to classify objects (centration) also begins to emerge, as does a reduction in egocentrism. Tests for the conservation of number, length, substance or quantity Conservation of number Two rows are presented to the pre-operational child. In the pretransformation question he/she agrees each row contains the same amount. One row is then lengthened or shortened. In the post-transformational question the child is asked whether each row has the same number of counters. 68 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Conservation of length Two sticks are presented as below. The child agrees they are of the same length. One is moved, left or right. The child is asked whether they are the same length. Conservation of substance or quantity Two similar Plasticine balls are shown as below. Child agrees they are of the same amount. One is altered. The child is asked whether they are still of the same amount. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 69 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS The concrete-operational stage (7–11) By age 7, children should be able to understand the law of conservation – certainly as regards number and volume. They can now deal with reversibility of thinking, i.e. in the above example being able to mentally move the lower row together, or to mentally move one amount of liquid from one glass to the other. This allows them to see that indeed the rows contain the same number of buttons and the glasses the same amount of lemonade. They can classify and order objects in a series, i.e. arrange things in size, and can understand relational problems, i.e. A is larger than B which is larger than C. They begin to show an ability to think in the abstract, but only if the problem is put to them in a real and meaningful way based on their own knowledge of the world. A child of around 8 would be completely unable to make sense of the equation: x > y; t > x. What is the smallest, x, y or t? but would be able to work out the following: ‘Nana is older than Mummy; Granny is older than Nana. Who is the youngest?’ This is the concrete operation transitivity – which could not be done at an earlier stage. Concrete operational thinking is the young child thinking in the abstract, but using real-life examples to help it work out complex issues. Another example is where a child between 7 and 11 may not understand the complexities of fractions when put in ‘number’ form, e.g. ¼, ½, ¾, but may very well be able to understand if the fraction is put in ‘real’ terms like ‘Half an apple and half an apple make...?’ Concrete operational thinking will develop and refine up until the next stage where the child develops more adult-like thinking strategies and associated behaviours. The formal operational stage (11–13) This is the last stage in cognitive development. It sees children beginning to develop and use abstract thought and scientific reasoning. Problems are approached in a more adult logical way. Teenagers can follow arguments and reason in a hypothetical way, i.e. tackle problems which ask ‘What if...?’ Much of a secondary curriculum is geared towards the development of hypothetical and abstract thinking. There is some evidence to suggest that many who enter secondary school are not yet in a biological position to cope with abstract mathematical and scientific concepts. They still have to endure the curriculum nonetheless. As a 70 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS consequence much of what they are exposed to in abstract subjects is lost to them. They are not yet biologically mature enough in a cognitive sense to deal with this type of thinking. This is one of the reasons why so many of the adult population feel uncomfortable when asked to think in number terms. They have not – even by their adult years – come through and mastered all aspects of formal operational thinking. Inhelder and Piaget (1958) gave adolescents five containers filled with a clear liquid. Four, they were told, were ‘test chemicals’ and one an ‘indicator’. When a particular combination of the test chemicals were added to the indicator it turned yellow. Children at the pre-operational stage simply mixed the chemicals randomly to see what transpired. Concrete operational children were a bit more systematic but also failed to hypothesise and think through the problem with much success. Only those in the formal operational stage were able to successfully think through the problem, testing all combinations of the test chemicals in a methodical, systematic manner, varying one factor at a time. They also wrote down results and tried to make general conclusions about each test chemical. Evaluation of Piaget While of immense importance to psychology and much respected for his contribution, Piaget can be and has been criticised. As Flavell (1982) says: ‘Like all theories of great reach and significance … it has problems that gradually come to light as years and years of thinking and research get done on it. Thus, some of us now think that the theory may in varying degrees be unclear, incorrect and incomplete.’ 1. Piaget thought that a young child was egocentric and unaware of a world outwith its own immediate one. Margaret Donaldson (1978) says that an understanding of egocentrism and object permanence is not quite as simple as Piaget proposed. If egocentrism exists then how an object disappears should still see children engage in egocentric behaviour. She reran his ball-cloth experiment using a ball and a light switch. Instead of covering the ball with the cloth she turned the lights off in the room instead. In this instance babies reached for the ball even although they could not see it. This was confirmed by Bower and Wishart (1972). Bower suggests that the reason for this difference in egocentric/non-egocentric behaviour is not that the baby has no object permanence early in life, but because it is confused by a cloth covering a ball. The baby has not learnt that balls and cloths can be manipulated this way. It is, by comparison, well used to lights going on and off in its world. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 71 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS It knows that if a light goes off, the things in its world still exist. The issue of object permanence, or lack of it, at the sensori-motor stage is open to question. Experience seems to make a difference. 2. As regards decentering at the pre-operational stage, Hughes (1975) replaced his mountain and doll apparatus with a policeman and doll one. Hughes found children were able to decentre (see the world through other’s eyes, i.e. the policeman or doll) at an earlier age than Piaget imagined. One reason for this was that the apparatus used by Hughes was known to the children. The task made more sense to them. They knew what the objects were and could understand better what was being asked of them. 3. Similar criticisms can be made about Piaget’s underestimation of children’s conservation ability concerning weight, number, volume and mass. Having experience of the things you are being asked to deal with, and being able to understand what is being asked of you, appears to lower the age at which children can/cannot conserve. This would be apparent in nursery schools for example, where the pre-fives are daily encouraged to play with water, sand, Plasticine, etc. Rose and Blank (1974) found that when the pre-transformation question was not asked, 6-year-olds often succeeded on the conservation of number task. They also made fewer errors when tested in the standard way a week later. These findings have been replicated by Samuel and Bryant (1984)1 using conservations of number, liquid quantity and substance, and McGarrigle and Donaldson (1974)2 in their Naughty Teddy transformation experiments and observations. 4. Piaget only used a small number of children, including his own, in his research. He generalised his findings to the whole population of children. This research strategy would be unacceptable today. 5. His sequence approach to children’s growing cognitive development from birth to age 11/13 is firmly based in biological maturation of the cerebral cortex. His theory has been criticised as being anti-educational in that it implies that education can play 1 2 72 Samuel and Bryant, ‘Asking only one question in the conservation experiment’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 315–318. McGarrigle, J and Donaldson, M (1974), ‘Conservation accidents’, Cognition 3: 341– 350 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS little part in cognitive development until a child is biologically ready to move through or up a stage. Alternative psychological theories to cognitive development would suggest that education does have an important part to play in unfolding cognitive ability. 6. According to Meadows (1995) Piaget discounted the influence of others in the child’s environment to the development of cognitive abilities. Piaget saw children as isolated ‘scientists’ trying to independently understand their highly complex worlds. As shall be seen when we look at the contribution of Lev Vygotsky, there does appear to be a substantial social influence regarding the development of knowledge and thought. Look up: The Jean Piaget Society http://www.piaget.org/ Student activity: Essay Describe, discuss, and evaluate Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development. Your essay should be around 1000 words in length. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 73 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Further challenges to Piaget Some psychologists feel that much of what Piaget proposes is difficult to test scientifically. This is because Piagetian concepts like organisation, assimilation, accommodation, equilibrium, etc. do not exist in reality. They are merely names, or hypothetical constructs, given to what are thought to be the human mental functions involved in cognitive development. The main thrust of criticism comes from the behaviourists. Their bottom line is that if you cannot measure something objectively it does not exist in fact. If something does not exist in fact, the study of it is more the realm of philosophy as opposed to science. This came from the work of the early American behaviourist John Broadus Watson who in 1913 said that if psychology wants to be a science it must use experimental research methods much like physics, chemistry and biology. Psychological enquiry should only, in his opinion, be concerned with human behaviour – not hypothetical constructs thought to apply to the workings of the human mind. His emphasis on the study of behaviour, instead of the mind, is because behaviour is real – our behaviour can be seen to happen and therefore can be accurately measured. Accurate, objective measurement, or empirical data is the mark of a true science. Behaviourists think much of what we learn is a result of our interaction with our environment. It is through these everyday experiences that children learn various types of behaviour, like how to get along with others, pass exams and cope with new and novel situations. Children’s behaviour as a consequence of learning in, and from, their environment is known as conditioning. The influence of conditioning (learnt behaviours) became apparent to Watson after he read the work of Ivan Petrovitch Pavlov (1849–1936), a Soviet Nobel Prize winning physiologist, who advocated that organisms learn particular behaviours over the course of time by associating one event with another. Using dogs as his subjects, Pavlov found he could stimulate their salivary glands by putting food powder in their mouths. The amount of saliva produced was then measured. The saliva produced was a reflex action, or unconditioned response (unlearnt), to food (the unconditioned stimulus) being placed in their mouths. He then began to notice that his dogs also salivated whenever their keepers appeared with their meals. Could this salivation be a clue that they had learnt that the appearance of their keepers meant that they were just about to get fed? Had the dogs learnt to predict that one event will follow another? Put another way, had the dogs become conditioned to expect that a particular stimulus (the keepers) would lead to a particular response (being fed)? 74 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS In an attempt to try and understand this better he set up a laboratory experiment, the results of which would eventually give psychology a great insight into how we learn as individuals. Pavlov’s work gave rise to his theory of classical conditioning. He knew from his earlier work that an unconditioned stimulus (food powder) led to an unconditioned response (salivation) for his dogs. He wanted to know if he could condition, or teach, his dogs to salivate to another completely unconnected stimulus – called a conditioned stimulus. He put his dogs into a cage-like apparatus and followed the procedure below. Pavlov’s theory of classical conditioning Before conditioning ! Food put in dog’s mouth (the unconditional stimulus: UCS) dog salivates (unconditioned response: UCR) then ! Pavlov rang a bell (called the neutral stimulus: NS) no response (the dog did not salivate) During conditioning ! Bell rung/food given at the same time (NS + UCS) dog salivates (unconditioned response: UCR) DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 75 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS After conditioning ! Bell rung (the conditional stimulus: CS) dog salivates (conditioned response: CR) Conditioning, or learning, is said to happen when the dog responds (conditioned response) to a previously neutral, or unconnected, stimulus (NS), referred to after conditioning as the conditional stimulus – the stimulus to which the unassociated behaviour has become conditioned. Some interesting points about classical conditioning 1. Pavlov discovered that dogs aren’t daft! What is meant by this is that he found out that after conditioning his dogs only responded (salivated) to the sound of the bell temporarily. After a while on hearing the bell they did not salivate. It was as if they realised that they were being conned. When an organism does not conditionally respond to a conditioned stimulus, this learnt stimulus-response connection has been broken or extinguished. Pavlov emphasised that reinforcement is needed to keep the learnt response happening when the organism comes up against the conditional stimulus. Put simply, after conditioning every so often the dogs had to be presented with the bell and the food, otherwise they would not salivate to the bell alone. 2. If the learnt, or conditioned, stimulus-response connection does become extinct, i.e. that bell → salivation no longer happens (because the reinforcer of food is removed), Pavlov found that after a period of rest, when the conditional stimulus (bell) is represented, the conditioned response (salivation) may recur. This is called spontaneous recovery. 3. Pavlov found that his dogs would, initially, conditionally respond (salivate) to bells which had slightly different tones to that of the original bell. He also found the more like the original bell-tone other bells had, the greater the amount of salivation produced. This he calls stimulus generalisation. You may like to consider the similarity here with Piaget’s idea of assimilation. 4. Pavlov further discovered that over time his dogs went from stimulus generalisation to stimulus discrimination. Experience allowed them to discriminate between bells of different tones. They would only respond to the tone of the original conditional 76 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS stimulus bell. You may like to consider the similarity here with Piaget’s idea of accommodation. Watson, on reading Pavlov’s work, believed that the conditioning process was the explanation to all human behaviours. For Watson, and other ‘hard’ behaviourists, human behaviour is explained in terms of learnt responses to stimuli in our environment. All behaviour is the product of environmental learning. Classical conditioning – learning that one event follows another – is central to the behaviourists’ understanding of all aspects of our behaviour. To go some way towards proving that behaviour – both ‘normal’ and ‘abnormal’ – is solely the product of learning Watson conducted his famous ‘Little Albert’ experiment. Watson and Raynor’s (1920) ‘Little Albert’ experiment Watson and Raynor wanted to find out if a phobia could be learnt or induced. The unwilling participant in their questionable study was a 9month boy called Little Albert. Albert had a pet white rat. Every time Albert played with the rat, Watson and Raynor whacked a hammer off a steel bar. This startled Albert! So much so that very quickly he became ‘conditioned’ to responding in a distressed state, by crying and trying to crawl away, when the stimulus of his rat was brought to him. He had gone from a previously happy response to the stimulus of his rat to a learnt (conditioned) distressed response to it. Watson and Raynor had induced a fear response to the rat stimulus. Watson and Raynor had ‘proved’ that phobias could be learnt. It was not until the 1950s, however, that behaviourists began to look at very useful therapies to help people ‘unlearn’ behaviours which caused them, and others, a problem. B F Skinner (1904–90) No discussion on behaviourism, however brief, would be complete without mentioning another American psychologist, called Burrhus Frederic Skinner. Also a behaviourist, he extended Pavlov’s work in his theory of operant conditioning. Using cats, DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 77 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS rats and a piece of apparatus which has become known as a Skinner Box, he was able to prove that an organism can be encouraged to perform a particular behaviour if it has been rewarded for it in the past; and similarly an organism can be discouraged from a particular behaviour through the use of punishment. Simply put, reward, or the use of positive reinforcement, encourages positive behaviours; punishment, or the use of negative reinforcement, discourages negative behaviours. Quick Questions • What classically conditioned behaviours can we observe in an infant? • From your own experience what rewards have been/are used with you to encourage positive behaviour? What punishments have been/are used with you to discourage negative behaviours? • Do they work? If so why? If not, why not? 78 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Alfred Bandura’s contribution to ‘How do children learn?’ Born in 1925 in Canada, Bandura is best known for his social learning theory. He was critical of behaviourism in that he thought the behaviourist understanding of how we learn – through classical and operant conditioning – was too far removed from real-life situations. Behaviourists generated their learning theory in a laboratory using animals. They then ‘jumped’ the theory to apply to human beings. For Bandura, this was too much. He went about observing children’s behaviour in real-life situations to try and discover if learnt human behaviour was more complex than classical and operant conditioning alone. His social learning theory is based upon a belief that children learn by observing the behaviours of others. For Bandura, children learn more by modelling their behaviour on others. They learn via observation and imitation. This is especially the case as regards gender role and the development of moral thoughts and behaviours. In a series of studies, Bandura et al. showed how, and in what circumstances, children learn behaviour through observational learning. For example, they would bring 5-year-old children separately into a room (the experimental group) where each would see an adult model playing with various toys. Each saw the adult suddenly turn and assault an inflatable ‘Bobo doll’. They saw the adult model knock it over, sit on it and punch it on the nose. The adult then proceeded to smash the doll over the head with a large wooden mallet. Finally he would throw and kick the doll up in the air, while shouting, ‘Sock him on the nose, hit him down, throw him in the air, kick him pow!’ Shortly after this, each child was aggravated and deliberately made angry by having an attractive toy teasingly withheld from them! At the same time, another group of 5-year-olds who had not seen the adult model’s behaviour (the control group) were similarly made angry. The adult model left and all the children were put into the room with the Bobo doll, wooden mallet, etc. Observing via a two-way mirror Bandura rated the behaviour of the children. He found that his experimental group (those who had seen the adult model) copied the adult’s behaviour almost identically. He was in a position to now say that not only do children learn by observing, imitating and modelling the behaviour of others, but more importantly and contrary to what the behaviourists thought, learnt behaviour does not need any kind of reinforcement. Having established that learning can occur in the absence of reinforcement, Bandura and Walters (1963) set about to try and determine the role, if any, reward and punishment has in the learning process. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 79 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Three groups of children saw three different films of a model yelling and punching the Bobo doll. One film ended with the model being praised for his behaviour, one ended with the model being punished and the third concluded with the model being neither praised nor punished (no treatment). Each group of children were left alone in the room and observed via the two-way mirror. They discovered that the Praise Group children behaved more aggressively than either the punishment or no treatment groups. The Punishment Group behaved less aggressively than either the praise or no treatment groups. In a later phase of the experiment the children were told they would be rewarded if they did everything they saw the adult model do in the film. Differences in behaviour between the praise and punishment groups disappeared almost immediately! What this suggests is that reinforcement does not need to be present to acquire a behaviour – observation, imitation and modelling of another is enough – but if reward or punishment is used, the behaviour of the child can be influenced in part. Nowadays social learning theory has been extended into the area of cognitive social learning theory, which emphasises children’s developing ability to actively process information coming to them from their environment. We act upon our environment as it in turn acts upon us. The experience of this is thought about by the child, and is used to deal with the same and similar situations later on. Quick Questions • Throughout 1999, a secondary school in Wales gave points to pupils for attending school on a regular basis. Points made prizes in that pupils who had a good attendance record won a week-long adventure holiday. Truancy rates fell dramatically. What aspect of behaviourist theory did this school successfully apply? • What might teachers have to do from time to time throughout the school year to keep truancy rates down to 1999 levels? • When at secondary school many people take unofficial time-off. This is not to be recommended, if only because we know that if we get caught some sort of punishment will be coming our way. Why then do we do it? • If you saw your best friend jump fully clothed into a river, would you do likewise. If not, why not? What major criticism might be made about social learning theory? Why might a knowledge of cognitive social learning theory provide a better explanation of this scenario? 80 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Cognitive social learning theory Vygotsky and Bruner: Learning and our social world In consideration of challenges to Piaget’s theory of cognitive development in children, Advanced Higher Developmental Psychology sees the work of Vygotsky and Bruner, with their emphases on learning and the child’s social world, as important. The influences coming from Vygotsky and Bruner have allowed for the development of what is called cognitive social learning theory in psychology. Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934) An alternative to the ‘child as scientist’ as proposed by Piaget, Vygotsky views the child (and any learner) as an apprentice. Unlike Piaget (who thought cognitive development was based on biological maturation), Lev Vygotsky, a Russian psychologist, believed that cognitive development was more influenced by the social world we live in. Underlying the theory is Vygotsky’s observation that our mastery of the essentials of life is often initially beyond us. We need help. This help comes from others, i.e. mum, dad, older siblings, teachers, etc. Vygotsky calls the assistance they give us scaffolding. Scaffolders nourish the seeds of cognitive ability and potential. The ‘fertiliser’ they use – in a psychological sense – is the cognition language. Scaffolders use language to describe and explain our complex world to us. The better use they make of language in the description and explanation of phenomena helps our understanding. Your teachers and lecturers, as scaffolders in the development of your knowledge of psychology, it is hoped will use the richness of their language skills to explain and describe matters psychological to you. Their further use of anecdotes, stories and games will also help you to understand better the science of mind and behaviour. We use (internalise) other people’s interpretations of life to help us problem-solve now and in the future. You, for example, will probably use the language and explanation of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development from these notes in an assessment – and will also apply it in an everyday sense to help your understanding of your own and children’s cognitive development. We use other people’s explanations as our own inner speech and verbal thoughts. The point here is fairly obvious. If something is poorly explained to us this affects our present understanding of it – plus any future understanding of related, more complex, issues. This is illustrated on the next page in consideration of a question often asked by young children. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 81 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS In an investigation into scaffolding, Wood et al. (1976) set children of 4 and 5 (in the presence of their mothers) a building task. The mothers were encouraged to assist their children verbally in the construction. They noted that mothers all used instructions which went from the general to specific. Some mums were very general; some were very specific and some were both. They found that while no single strategy guaranteed learning, those mothers who mixed general instruction with the more specific at appropriate points saw a more successful and efficient outcome in the building of the construction. This was confirmed by Bruner (1983) who says the most useful learning environment is one where general instruction is used by the scaffolder to encourage autonomy of thought and problem-solving and where difficulties emerge, at that juncture, to use more specific helpful interventions. Children are not stupid creatures. They are active learners, or apprentices, full of enquiry about the world they see around them. ‘Where do babies come from?’ is a question often asked by young children around age 5 or 6. This question often causes concern for a parent. If a parent explains ‘babies’ from the point of view of a baby being like a flower in that it grows from a seed in mummy’s tummy, this very often satisfies the child’s understanding at that time about an important aspect of its life. Evading the question does not aid the child’s need for understanding (maybe as a consequence it will not seek advice on sex from this parent again). The flower/seed story helps the child cognitively mature at age 5/6 and later on to understand the biology of foetal conception and development. The gap between an individual’s actual ability level and potential ability as regards task/skill/concept mastery is called their ZPD or zone of proximal development. The richness of help received from scaffolders helps the child maximise its potential within any particular ZPD. ZPDs differ from person to person. They are affected by a person’s genetics, environment and interaction of both. If, for example, a 7-year-old Scottish boy has a better-than-average football ability and this is recognised and encouraged by a primary school teacher who herself has a coaching background, with support and encouragement from family and friends, later secondary school teachers and boys’ club officials, the boy may one day end up day playing professional football for a premiere league club. If, on the other hand, potential ability is not encouraged, the boy may never even kick a ball for a Sunday League pub team let alone club and country. 82 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS For Vygotsky, it is therefore a child and young person’s social world which influences cognitive development more than biological maturation. Vygotsky believes the quality and ability to think and reason using inner speech or verbal thoughts is a consequence of a social process with others. It develops as a product of our social interactions with others (especially adults) during infancy, childhood, adolescence and beyond. Vygotsky views language and cognitive development as inter-related. For him there are three major elements in the process towards fully developed cognitive ability. These are: • Initially the infant and toddler deal with their world through action. This does not need language per se. • As children age they then are able to reflect on their own thought processes through language, using strategies such as talking through the problem to themselves. • Understanding comes about in mutual interaction with others in our environment or social settings. Social interactions with teachers, peers, parents and other significant people help cognitive development flourish. As a result children acquire their culture’s knowledge and skills ‘through graded collaboration with those who already possess them’ (Rogoff, 1990), in that, according to Vygotsky: ‘Any function in the child’s cultural development appears twice, or on two planes. First it appears on the social plane, and then on the psychological plane.’ Meadows (1995) gives a good example of cultural development which takes the infant from the social to the psychological. Very often a young child will use pointing as an unsuccessful attempt at getting at something beyond its reach. Mum often picks up on this, makes the gesture herself, follows it through and picks up the object and hands it to the child. Thereafter a more deliberate gesture is made often accompanied by cries, orientating towards the mother and eventually words and telescopic sentences. The gesture is made towards the mother, not the object. The child has now internalised this social experience at a more psychological level. Unlike Jean Piaget, Vygotsky did not believe children had to be ready before they could tackle something new or beyond their apparent ken. He advocated scaffolders provide children with challenges above their cognitive level but within their respective ZPDs. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 83 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Look up: Lev Vygotsky The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology: A Methodological Investigation http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/crisis/index.htm Lev Vygotsky Thinking and Speaking http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/words/index.htm Vygotsky Centennial Project 1996 (an excellent gateway.) http://www.massey.ac.nz/~ALock/virtual/project2.htm Social Development Theory: L. Vygotsky http://www.gwu.edu/~tip/vygotsky.html 84 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Bruner ‘If agency and esteem are central to the construction of a concept of Self, then the ordinary practices of school need to be examined with a view to what contribution they make to these two crucial ingredients of personhood.’ Jerome Bruner, The Culture of Education Jerome Bruner (1964) was much impressed by Vygotsky’s work, especially the influence of scaffolders as regards children and young people’s maximisation of cognitive potential and cognitive development. Like Vygotsky he articulated that thought is dependent on, or caused by, language. Critical of Piaget’s non-consideration of language per se, he however shared similar beliefs to Piaget in that he accepted we are born with a biological predisposition to organise our world and that biological maturation of the cerebral cortex enhances complex thinking and problem-solving abilities. He also agreed with Piaget concerning the natural curiosity and inquisitiveness of children. Abstract knowledge grows out of action with competence developing from experience and concrete mental operations. His contribution to cognitive development concerns the ways children think about their world. He says children have to develop progressively more sophisticated forms of knowledge in their minds to help them think and deal with life. The more complex they discover their world to be as they get older, the greater the need to use more complex ways of thinking (representations) about it. As a consequence Bruner says children use three different ways, forms or modes of representation to think about their environment. These are: • the enactive mode occurs at the same time as Piaget’s sensori-motor stage (0–2). The infant thinks in and uses the enactive mode, or form of representation, to think about its world, which is represented (thought about/dealt with) in terms of its sensori-motor actions. This sounds complicated but all it need mean for us is that infants use a lot of gestures, facial expressions and obvious emotions (crying, laughing) which mean something for them in their simple environment, and which others are able to interpret and act upon. • the iconic mode is the growing child’s use of mental imagery to think about, deal with and represent its world. Showing itself just before the onset of language, the infant begins to form mental pictures to represent and remember people, objects, etc., in its environment. This is necessary because the child’s world is becoming more complex. Gestures and so on just will not do anymore. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 85 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Children begin to store visual images or mental pictures of their world in their cerebral cortex. • the semantic mode is when the child begins to represent or think about its world using language. Language is important in that it allows the young child to enquire, think about, deal with and act upon its environment. For Bruner, being able to use the semantic mode, or form of representing knowledge, to deal with the world is the key to intellectual growth and development. Iconic thinking – or its limitations – were considered in a classic experiment run by Bruner and Kenny (1966). Bruner and Kenny (1966) presented 5 to 7-year-olds with an arrangement of differing shaped glasses (height and diameter) as above. When the glasses were removed, all the children were competent at putting them back in their original positions. This is a reproduction task using the iconic mode. The glasses were again removed. The children were then asked to replace all the glasses in a mirror image of their original position. This is the transposition task. Those older children capable of symbolic thought could do this, while the younger iconic thought children could not. Bruner and Kenney surmised that the younger children were unable to restructure their original representation of the stimulus to allow them to deal with the new situation, that is manipulating the glasses in their heads into the pattern required. Results: Reproduction task Transposition task 86 60% 5-year-olds 0% 5-year-olds DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 80% 7-year-olds successfully 79% 7-year-olds successfully THEORIES AND CONCEPTS The major difference between Piaget and Bruner concerns language. Bruner believes the jump from iconic to symbolic thought is because of language. He argues that by activating children’s language, encouraging symbolic mode thought, their cognitive development is improved. Piaget, on the other hand, believes cognitive development to be all about the successful acquisition of operations (sensori-motor, preoperational, etc.). Language is not the cause of development but a tool used in the course of the development of operational thinking. Look up: Constructivist Theory: J. Bruner http://tip.psychology.org/bruner.html The information-processing (IP) approach to cognitive development ‘Information-processing theories of cognitive development focus on the processes of cognition and the kinds of information children are capable of acquiring from the environment and what they are capable of doing with it.’ Krebs and Blackman (1988) While in part influenced by Piaget, IP is not hidebound by one theory. It tries to understand learning from the point of view of information processes or how we store, interpret, retrieve and evaluate information. It concerns the greater study of perception, attention, language, memory and thinking or problem solving. Of developmental interest is sequential development and working memory (Case, 1978, 1985). The IP approach agrees here with Piaget in that it advocates the sequential nature of cognitive development during which a child’s cognitive processes become more developed, efficient and effective. Central to IP in developmental psychology is the (biologically limited) working memory the developing child has at its disposal. Called M space (like short term memory), a useful analogy to its understanding might be ‘you can’t put a gallon into a pint pot’. Its limited capacity frustrates information processing in general and thinking and problem solving in particular. Central to the IP approach (within developmental psychology) is an understanding of children’s minds based on ‘the computer analogy’. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 87 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Cognitive development is as a result of children’s ageing process and experience – they develop better strategies for remembering and organising knowledge and for encoding more aspects of the problem on hand. 2. Child stores, retrieves and manipulates input 1. Input received from the environment 3. Child responds behaviourally In order to study children’s information processes, IP psychologists have developed a technique called task analysis. Their logic is that if a child cannot do the things adults do one must understand the problemsolving steps and sequences involved in their problem-solving strategies. Task analysis Take the following problem. If O’Neill does not tell as many fibs as Jamieson, and Jamieson does not tell as many fibs as Browne, who is the most truthful? Oakhill (1984) identifies five elements needed to solve this. 1. The child must perceive and encode the necessary propositions in the question – which means they must attend to it. 2. The propositions have to be stored in working memory. Groome et al. (1999) see working memory (a hybrid of STM) as like a computer screen, a kind of mental workspace, where various operations are performed on current data. 3. The propositions must be combined in memory to form an integrated representation in the mind. 4. The question must be encoded. 88 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 5. The representation of the propositions must be ‘scanned’ to answer the question or to conclude on what is being asked. Failure to deal with the question is, according to Trabasso (1977), due to a breakdown in information processing (encoding, storage or retrieval) associated with perception, attention, language, memory and problemsolving and their inter-relationships. Keeney et al. (1967) presented children of differing ages with a number of pictures to remember. Before the recall test, they observed most of the 8–10 year olds ‘mouthing’ the picture series (rehearsal) while younger ones of 5 did not. Although the 5-year-olds remembered less of the picture series than their older counterparts, when taught the rehearsal strategy they thereafter did as well as the older children concerning memory recall (remember Vygotsky). At age 5 memory appears just as good as an older child’s but is not used as efficiently or effectively. Bee (2000) and Pine (1999) each suggest that as children grow and mature: • • • • they acquire more and more powerful strategies to remember they use these strategies more efficiently and flexibily they apply them to an increasing array of problems those of school-age are capable of using a broader range of different strategies for the same problem, so that if strategy number 1 fails, a back-up or alternative is available for application. One new strategy is the child’s growing awareness of its own mental processes. This is called metacognition. Part of the larger category executive process (planning what to do and considering alternatives), metacognition and executive processes may gradually emerge as a result of ageing. The IP approach does not emphasise Piaget’s qualitative stages to cognitive development. Siegler (1976) says that it is not biological maturation and resultant cognitive change in children that accounts for differing operational abilities but it is their acquisition of increasingly complex rules for problem-solving. Seigler agrees with Piaget to a sequence in a child’s grasp of new strategies, but for him these are not the same as distinct developmental stages. According to Bee (2000), some of the changes Piaget attributed to new mental structures are easier explained in the light of increased experiences with the same (or similar) task or problem. As a result the child becomes more efficient DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 89 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS and effective at encoding, storing and retrieving information necessary for thinking and problem-solving. These, for the IP approach, are quantitative changes. However, at the same time Bee comments: ‘There also seems to be a real qualitative change in the complexity, generalisability, and flexibility of strategies used by the child.’ Student activity: Essay Describe, discuss, and evaluate the theories of Vygotsky and Bruner in relation to cognitive development. Your essay should be about 1000 words in length. 90 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Practical applications of theory and research to education Whether cognitive development is biological or social in origin (or both) much can be found in the world of education regarding applications of theories as advocated by Piaget, Vygotsky and Bruner. Piaget and education Piaget did not see himself as an educationalist; however his theory has had an impact especially in the twenty-first century child-centered nursery and primary classroom. Some implications of Piagetian theory, most especially readiness, the curriculum and teaching methods, are considered below. 1. ‘Readiness’: parents, teachers and society at large, due to Piaget’s teachings, understand children to qualitatively differ in what they can think, feel and do in comparison to adults. Teachers therefore are more aware of age-related stages of development and work accordingly. Nurseries, for example, place great emphasis on developing physical and motor skills, conservation work with sand, water, Plasticine, counters, etc. Scottish nurseries now have a statutory duty to have a set curriculum. This very much reflects what Piaget identified children should be able to do at the sensorimotor and pre-operational stages. Few are ‘hard line’ and do not fully concur with Piaget’s idea that if children are biologically immature to do certain things they will get confused and upset when they try but do not succeed on tasks thought by Piaget beyond them. 2. ‘Stimulating active learning’: Piaget believed in active participation and interaction with the environment to encourage cognitive development and operational thought. Active learning is evident in nurseries and primaries in Scotland today – and was indeed 40 years ago with active teaching/learning in some workingclass primary schools used to encourage and develop cognitive abilities to allow any chance of success in the 11+ ‘qualifying’ examination needed to enter a senior secondary. One school, St. Paul’s Primary at Tollcross in Glasgow had a 99 per cent success rate in the Qualifying Examination as a consequence of active learning – at a level difficult to recognise today. To enhance discovery learning, the classroom environment should be awash with materials and activities to stimulate curiosity and allow transition to greater cognitive levels (stages). DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 91 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 3. ‘Questioning’: Questioning of children also helps active learning. When wrong answers are forthcoming the teacher should look to clues in the child’s response to identify their thinking process, or where they may be developmentally at present. 4. ‘Use of concrete operational materials’: Again fairly obvious teaching practice for those working with children under the age of 11 (formal operational stage). This enhances concrete operational cognitive operations and makes easier the transition to formal, more abstract, operational thought. 5. ‘Assimilation and accommodation’: Awareness of these by the teacher is important in the learning process. New concepts/ experiences should be linked to what the child already knows (Hughes, 1978 – the policeman/doll). This aids assimilation and ultimately quicker and more efficient accommodation. Vygotsky and education Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, in an applicable sense, takes its cue from the child. Progress is dependent upon the child. It is a child-centred approach which views the teacher as a facilitator or enabler. Vygotsky takes a different tack. For him, intelligence is the capacity to learn by instruction. He sees teachers as guides, assisting their apprentice to pay attention, concentrate and thus learn more effectively. This is done through listening, watching, doing, discussing, conversing, etc. with teachers and other significant scaffolding others. This is what is called a didactic teaching methodology. It is the flavour of education in Britain in the twenty-first century. We have moved away (and back?) to a more teacher-centred or traditional approach. This would worry Vygotsky in that he did not: ‘…advocate mechanical form teaching where children go through the motions of sitting at desks and passing exams that are meaningless to them… On the contrary Vygotsky stressed intellectual development rather than procedural learning.’ Sutherland (1992) Vygotsky did not see teachers as some kind of dictator in the classroom but like Piaget advocated their control of the classroom. They are there to develop, extend and challenge children to go beyond where they are at present. This, as was said earlier, is done via language, culture and the child’s social world. The key to the maximisation of cognitive potential is determined by each individual’s zone of proximal development (ZPD). 92 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Meaning given to stimuli is a social construct obtained from linguistic interactions between the scaffolder and apprentice. Vygotsky did not see children in the same light as Piaget. They do not develop competence via their own actions. They need some help to develop knowledge, concepts and skills. This is forthcoming from the more expert in interactions as described above. However, as Keegan (2000) indicates, ‘A culture of teachers as scaffolders in Scottish classrooms is maybe not as obvious as it once was.’ Interestingly, Vygotsky saw a place for collaborative learning with group learning and peer tutoring from older more able children. This was because these environments especially encourage language, explanations and co-operative working. The latter was much appreciated in Vygotsky’s Soviet culture at the time. His theory has a political message which has much to be said for it. According to Sutherland (1992): ‘The socialist rationale was one of all children working for the general good rather than the capitalist one of each child trying to get out of school as much benefit as s/he can without putting anything back into it. The brighter child is helping the less able one since the latter … will be more of an asset to society as a literate rather than an illiterate adult.’ Scaffolding in an educational context Bruner et al. conducted a number of research studies to examine the role of scaffolding in learning with 3-, 4- and 5-year-old children. Wood, Bruner and Ross (1976), discovered that there are a number of factors that typify effective scaffolding in children’s cognitive development: • Recruitment: The teacher captures interest and motivates students to attempt the task. • Reduction of degrees of freedom: The teacher simplifies the task and breaks it down into manageable steps in order for students to achieve success. • Direction maintenance: In the early stages of the task, the teacher encourages and motivates to succeed. Later on, the student should find the task motivating in its own right. • Marking critical features: The teacher talks up relevant aspects of the task, in order that the student can gauge how far off the mark they are/are not. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 93 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS • Demonstration: The teacher demonstrates a correct solution or, where a part solution exists, should offer explanations of any error. This should lead to the student imitating on this and thus improving on effort, success and self-esteem. They carried out an observational study where a tutor taught each individual child to build a 3-D structure, a superordinate task the child could not do on its own. It was agreed previously that the tutor would allow the children to complete as much of the task as possible themselves and would offer verbal help before demonstration of the task. The tutor’s actions and reactions at each stage of the task were dependent upon the child’s success or failure. If they were doing well they said nothing. This study shows the process of scaffolding. The tutor guided the children through a task, at each stage allowing the child’s level of achievement to determine the next level of tutoring. The tutor worked within the child’s ZPD (zone of proximal development) – the area between what the child could do on her own and what she could achieve with help. Is this possible in a classroom of 30 children? Look up: ‘Change law with a mind to the future’, Gerard Keegan, TES Scotland, 14 April 2000. Bruner and education Sutherland (1992) asks: ‘How can teachers reach the targets set by the government without some acceleration of the slow learner? Are the most able pupils being fully stretched in comprehensive schools particularly in the mixedability classes? Should they be accelerated to the next stage?’ To answer the questions posed above it is useful to consider Bruner’s theory of cognitive development in an educational context. Piaget lays emphasis in his theory on ‘readiness’. Essentially he is saying a child has to be biologically and thus cognitively ready to deal with sensori-motor, pre-operational, concrete operational or formal operational thought. Bruner (1953) disagrees. He believes much more in active interventions in the form of a stimulating learning environments as ‘any subject can be taught effectively in some intellectually honest form to any child at any stage of development’. 94 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Stimulating differing levels of thinking for Bruner is to be found in his spiral curriculum. This is where a child doing a particular subject comes to understand its principles and concepts at a deeper and deeper level. This is evident in the nursery where aspects of Piagetian conservation are first introduced. It is revisited in a more structured form in the early years of primary and this continues throughout with children exposed during planned active learning activities to the concept in ever increasing and diverse contexts. By the concrete operational stage conservation should, among other things, be synonymous with volume. This is helped along by teachers making links within and between the subjects being investigated. Very often a glue in the form of a term long project is used to help do this. During secondary school the topic is again looked at, this time using and applying it in abstract terms, i.e. using formulae – now in the absence of primary school concrete operational props. Adey et al. (1989) investigated the effectiveness of CASE (Cognitive Acceleration Science Education) materials in England (also piloted in Scotland in 1999). A number of schools formed both a control and experimental group. In all cases the control/experimental group were taught by the same teachers. The experimental group were exposed to CASE materials and teaching methods very much based on the ‘stimulation of active learning’ model proposed by Bruner. CASE consists of practical problems for pupils to tackle that require formal operational thought (hypothesis testing and abstract thought). Elements of behaviourism and cognitive learning theory are present with pupils having to successfully complete one task before moving on to the next more complex one. The more complex task demands knowledge gained from the previous ones. In comparison to the control group who were not exposed to CASE, and using Piagetian formal operational reasoning tasks as their measure, Adey et al. reported that after three years the boys in the experimental group did significantly better than both boys and girls in the control group. The research indicates that accelerated learning programmes in at least one subject may benefit some children, if biologically and thus cognitively ready to be accelerated. Once again Piaget in relation to Bruner seems at least part correct. Or should that be the other way around?! Such is the nature of psychology. Like Vygotsky, Bruner views teachers as ‘interventionists’ who are ‘obliged to make demands on their pupils’ (Sutherland, 1992). Both see the use of culture as important vehicles to learning. Bruner is more child-centred than Vygotsky who leans more towards teacher-centred learning and direct instruction. All of them – Piaget, Bruner, Vygotsky, and indeed the IP approach – have valuable things to say for the teacher and learner alike. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 95 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Student activity: Essay In around 1000 words describe, discuss and evaluate where appropriate the practical contributions made to education by Piaget, Vygotsky and Bruner. The information-processing approach and education Originating in the USA, IP makes a contribution from the point of view of memory and the limited ability of young children to process information. These temporary biological ‘deficits’ are the cause of poor reading, and memory retention and recall difficulties. IP aids the teacher/learner in suggesting strategies to dilute memory load in the young learner and still achieve the learning goal. This sounds complicated but all it might mean is that in working out ‘sums’ children are encouraged to write them down! IP implies a teacher-centred learning environment with the teacher as a kind of cognitive repair man or woman. It also places an emphasis on metacognition in making learners aware of their own learning: the ‘why can I/can’t I do this?’ It is seen when children say ‘I can’t do this because I haven’t learnt my 12 times table yet.’ This helps both cognitive development and insight into how we process information (Nippold, 1984). Davis (1984) suggests IP should not take a passive view of the learner. IP should actively encourage, from an information-processing perspective, hypothesis testing by learners and their use of visual imagery to enhance memory encoding, retention and recall when applying what they have discovered to real-life situations. Factors which affect language development – Chomsky Mike Cardwell in his excellent Complete A–Z Psychology Handbook defines language as ‘an agreed set of symbols that enable us to convey meaning and converse with other members of the same culture who share the same language’. The Collins Dictionary and Thesaurus see language as: ‘A system for the expression of thoughts, feelings, etc., by the use of spoken sounds or conventional symbols... The faculty for the use of such systems, which is a distinguishing characteristic of man... The language of a particular nation or group. The specialised vocabulary used by a particular group.’ 96 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Agreed definition is difficult. Language has been studied from the beginnings of time. Is there such a thing as one agreed language? We learn spoken language at home. We are then sent to school to learn to read, write and converse better in language! How we speak in the home will perhaps not be the language we use with our peers. Similarly the language some of us use with our peers will not be the same when we are talking to small children. We learn foreign languages. We learn the language of academic subjects which allows us to converse with others who use the same language, i.e. mathematics, music, chemistry, physics, psychology, etc. While we all share the same spoken language, why is it that the language coming from a particular person is perceived as more important than another? Language is power. A knowledge of language should equip you to be more effective in your interpersonal relationships. The psychological study of language is heavily influenced by British philosophy from the establishment of the British empirical tradition by John Locke in the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries and continued by George Berkeley, David Hume, John Stuart Mill, G E Moore and Bertrand Russell in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. These English (and Scottish) philosophers gave birth to what is nowadays called analytic or linguistic philosophy. How is language acquired? Conditioning, imitation and rules have all been found to play a part but before these environmental influences begin, our biology must come into play. We have a biologically determined critical period for language acquisition. Noam Chomsky first brought this to psychology’s attention. Chomsky, an American linguist, writer, teacher and political activist, founded what is both brilliantly and absurdly called transformationalgenerative linguistic analysis! What Chomsky means by this is that as well as the rules of grammar specific to individual languages, there are also universal rules or linguistic universals common to all languages. These include the use of nouns, adjectives and verbs at the deep structure level of language. All languages have the same deep structure. This shows that the ability to form and understand language is innate to all human beings. Chomsky believes that children the world over are born with an innate language acquisition device to help them cope with the complexities of language. There appears to be a critical period for language acquisition. The theory lies within the biological approach in psychology and is DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 97 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS beyond the scope of this course, but suffice to say it is all down to the epigenic maturation of those parts of the brain that deal with language. Once this critical period is reached (11/12 according to Lennenberg, 1964 and 1967) little can be done to kick-start language and its attendant potentialities for the individual. Our ability to switch on to the acquisition of language in early childhood is uncanny. Bilingualism need only prove this point. One criticism of Chomsky’s work is Bruner’s observation that you cannot divorce language from the context within which it is used. Bruner (1983) views language as ‘a by-product (and a vehicle) of culture transmission’. Not only does it have rules and structure (however acquired) but much more importantly it has a social function. Furthermore, Bloom (1970) in researching into toddler utterances suggests Chomsky ignores meaning at the expense of rules and structure. Bloom observed a toddler say ‘Mummy sock’ in two situations. In one mummy was putting on the infant’s sock, in another she was picking it up. Intention and meaning in both utterances are different. His theory also takes little account of the way adults simplify their language to help infants’ understanding. We use shorter sentences, speak high-pitched (like a Tellytubby!) and emphasise important words and phrases. Called ‘motherese’ (a term now apparently deemed politically incorrect), Gelman and Shatz (1977) feel it is important to the acquisition of language. Young children, when talking to even younger children, like adults adjust their speech to the perceived level of linguistic skill they think the other child possesses. The author has observed his own 9-year-old daughter talk to 3- and 4-year-olds as if they were from the planet Zog! The author’s own preference, while observing pitch and tone conventions, is to talk to learners of all ages using elaborated code with concrete operational examples to aid cognitive development. This is in line with Chomsky in that he thinks talking to children in a simplistic way is inadequate to the development of their linguistic competencies. The author here would go further and suggest it is not just inadequate to linguistic development, but damaging to intellectual potential. ‘Talk up’ to our learners: don’t ‘talk down’. Research: privation and language acquisition Two pieces of research deserve mention here. These are the Koluchová twins (Koluchová, 1972, 1976, 1991) and the case of Genie as reported by Rymer (1993) and Curtiss (1977). The Koluchová (1972, 1976, 1991) longitudinal study of twin Czechoslovak boys is a classic. Their mother died when the twins were 98 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS born and they were fostered for the first 18 months of their lives. They were then returned to the father and stepmother who subjected them to a cruel régime. They were isolated from everyone and were kept malnourished, given no exercise or intellectual stimulation. When found at age 7 they were physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally retarded. They were then placed in the care of two sisters who nurtured, loved and cherished them. They both gradually made up physical, cognitive social and emotional deficits. Now both are reported as developmentally normal and are themselves married and bringing up children. They did catch up cognitively and intellectually with their cohort age but remain in the lower reaches of intellectual ability in relation to their cohort normal distribution. Rymer (1993) and Curtiss (1977) report the famous case of Genie. Born to dysfunctional parents, her father locked Genie away from birth thinking she was vulnerable because she was retarded. She was physically, cognitively, socially and emotionally deprived throughout her entire childhood, only being brought to the attention of the authorities at the age of 13 when she was removed from these dire straits and placed in the care of the local authority. It was at this point her father committed suicide. She had immense difficulty in acquiring spoken language. This may have been because she was retarded and/or some evidence for Chomsky’s LAD. She did acquire and used Ameslan (American Sign Language). Her overall recovery was disrupted by unsettled and unstable foster homes and while eventually physically recovering she was cognitively, socially and emotionally dysfunctional showing autistic symptoms such as disinterest in the people around her. Look up: The Noam Chomsky Archive http://www.lbbs.org/chomsky/index.cfm Theory http://theory.lcs.mit.edu/~iandc/Authors/chomskynoam.html Student activity: Essay In around 1000 words and using relevant research evidence where appropriate describe, discuss and evaluate Chomsky’s view on the acquisition of language. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 99 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Further reading: cognitive development Bee, H, The Developing Child (7th ed.), NY: Harper Collins, Chapters 7– 8, 1995 De Villiers, P A and De Villiers, J, Early Language, London: Fontana, 1979 Donaldson M, Children’s Minds, Glasgow: Fontana/Collins, 1978 Meadows S, ‘Cognitive Development’ in P E Bryant and A M Coleman (eds), Developmental Psychology, Harlow: Longman 100 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Glossary: Cognitive development accommodation: more advanced form of organising our world. Piaget says we create schemas or schemata (mental structures) about our world, first simply (assimilation), but realise as we develop that all we come across cannot quite ‘fit in’ or be dealt with by these existing simple schemata. We therefore adapt or change existing schemata to accommodate new information from our increasingly more complex world. animism: evident in childhood play – especially during Piaget’s preoperational stage. Animism is where children give a living personality to teddies, dolls, toy soldiers, etc. Animism gives us a clue as to how young children perceive their world. apprentices: term used by Vygotsky to describe children as active learners. It implies that children need help to maximise the development of their cognitive skills, abilities and competencies. Help comes from parents, caregivers, older siblings, teachers, the peer group, etc. Piaget sees children as ‘scientists’ who cognitively develop by testing hypotheses in/about their environment. Vygotsky takes a more cautious approach, by suggesting they are apprentices who need assistance from others. assimilation: a simple form of organising information about our world (see accommodation). Piaget says that assimilation occurs when we create a mental schema about something in our world. Assimilation sees a child deal with its world in terms of existing schemata. After a while the child comes to realise that its existing assimilated schemata is inappropriate to deal with its growing knowledge of the world. It changes or creates new schemata as a result (accommodation). Bandura, Alfred: Canadian psychologist who advocated social learning theory as another way of looking at how children learn from their environment. They learn certain behaviours by observation, imitation and modelling of others. behaviourists: sometimes called the first force in psychology. Behaviourism emerged as a reaction to psychoanalysis (second force) because it felt psychology should be about actual observable behaviour, as opposed to the unobservable and inaccessible contents of the human mind, which behaviourists felt were private and personal to an individual. Behaviourism had become a powerful force by the middle of the twentieth century in the USA and has had a DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 101 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS tremendous influence on psychology worldwide. Behaviourists such as Pavlov, Watson and Skinner gave psychology an understanding of learning from the point of view of conditioning. They say we learn as a consequence of classical and operant conditioning, and that individuals can be seen as the sum total of learnt stimulus-response (S-R) units of behaviour. Bruner, Jerome: a psychologist who says children’s intellectual development depends on how their minds use the information they receive. He identified enactive, iconic and semantic types of thinking used by the growing child to represent its world. classical conditioning: discovered by Ivan Pavlov while working for the Soviet secret service, classical conditioning, or classical learning, is where we as organisms learn that one event (a stimulus) will predict another stimulus or event. Pavlov first noticed classical conditioning when he observed that dogs in his laboratory salivated at the sight of their keepers. They had learnt that their appearance meant that they were to be fed. conditional stimulus: a stimulus you teach a participant to respond to in some way or other. Pavlov conditioned his dogs to salivate when they heard the conditional stimulus of the bell. conditioned/conditioning: behaviourist term for learning. cognitive development: includes perception, attention, language, memory and thinking (or problem solving). Developmental psychology has been particularly interested in what influences thinking and intellectual development. First thought to be dependent on biological development and genetic endowment, developmental psychology now realises that our environment plus the active learning experiences we get from it all contribute to cognitive development. concrete-operational stage: Piaget’s third stage of cognitive, or intellectual, development between the ages of 7–11. The concreteoperational stage is identified in the developing child’s ability to think more abstractly about its world. The child solves problems using abstract reasoning such as reversibility and simple hypothesising. conservation: a child’s ability to understand physical aspects of its world such as number, volume, length, weight and mass. The child’s ability to conserve begins to develop at the end of the pre-operational stage (2–7). 102 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS control group: in order to allow us to narrow down the possibility of error in psychological experiments we very often have two groups of people taking part. This helps us make comparisons between them. One group we call the experimental group, and the other we call the control group. The experimental group are given a ‘treatment’ of some kind while the control group go without. Both groups are then tested for performance. If the experimental group significantly do better/worse on the performance measurement than the control group, we are more confident that there is a connection (causeeffect) between the treatment given and performance. decentre/decentring: a young child during the pre-operational stage will begin to decentre, or show it can see the world from other people’s point of view. Up until then the child is egocentric, only seeing the world from its own viewpoint. disequilibrium: a Piagetian term referring to a child during accommodation where it is changing/forming new schemata in order to deal better with its world. Old schemata just won’t do. egocentric: a child’s inability to see the world from another person’s point of view. When a child does, it is said to now be able to decentre. See Jean Piaget’s ‘mountain study’. empirical data: results obtained by careful experimental research, as opposed to just thinking (philosophically) about an issue. enactive mode: Jerome Bruner’s notion as to how children first ‘represent’ or think about their world. The enactive mode sees a baby, for example, represent and deal with its environment using sensori-motor behaviours. epigenesis: epigenic principles. The unfolding of our genetic blueprint. The biological trigger necessary for the onset of physical, cognitive, social and emotional development and change. The fact that biological development is necessary before other physical, cognitive and emotional developments can take place, e.g. walking, where a baby has to biologically distribute body mass, build muscle, etc., before it can toddle. The onset of puberty is another example of epigenic principles in action. equilibrium: a Piagetian term where a child is said to be assimilating with its world. It is dealing with its environment in terms of existing knowledge structures or schemata. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 103 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS et al.: abbreviation for et alia, a Latin term which means ‘and others’. experimental group: see control group. The experimental group in psychology is a group of participants who are given some kind of ‘treatment’ that allows comparison with a control group who do not receive any treatment. formal operational stage (11–13): the last stage in Piaget’s theory of cognitive development. By this stage children can deal formally with their world; they can think and reason logically, hypothesise, etc. Interestingly, there is some evidence that a few people do not successfully acquire formal operational thought, especially that of working with abstract numbers. genetic epistemology: the study of the growth of thought and knowledge in relation to evolutionary biology. ‘hard’ behaviourists: Watson, Skinner, Pavlov, etc. These early behaviourists saw all human behaviours as a consequence of learnt S-R (stimulus-response) units of behaviour. We are what we are, and become what we become, as a result of learning. hypothetical constructs: a phrase used in psychology that gives names to things that do not exist in reality (and consequentially are very hard to measure/observe), e.g. perception. iconic mode: Jerome Bruner’s idea that children as they get older (2+) begin to think and deal with their world using mental images. imitation: The Canadian psychologist Alfred Bandura proposed in his social learning theory that children model their behaviour by copying others that they see. laboratory experiment: a major research method used in psychology, especially by behaviourists and their modern day equivalent. A laboratory experiment is characterised by the scientists’ control of extraneous variables in order to work out any cause and effect relationship between an independent variable (the one that is manipulated or changed) and a dependent variable (the one that is measured or observed). language: a cognitive process identified as particular to human beings. 104 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS mastery play: play, as considered by Piaget, was seen as important as it helps a child practise and develop skills and abilities. He thought children go through a sequence of development regarding play behaviours. Mastery play is the first where the child repeats behaviours, in play, in order to master them. mental representation: the images we use in our minds to represent or think about our world. model: term used by Bandura to identify the person whose behaviour a child is imitating. modelling: the process of imitation. modes of representation: ways of thinking about our world. See Jerome Bruner. object permanence: Piaget’s observation that a very young child appears to deal with its world in the ‘here and now’. If the child cannot directly see or experience something, it does not exist. See Piaget’s ‘ball and cloth’ experiment. observation: a popular research strategy used in psychology. Observation is of three kinds. Natural observation sees a psychologist directly observe a situation in nature (as it happens). They do not/ cannot exercise any control in a situation of natural observation. Natural observations might be looking at play as it happens within a school playground. Participant observation is where a psychologist sets up and takes part in an observation with his/her participants, e.g. Piaget’s mountain study. Non-participant observation is where a psychologist will set up an observation but observe what happens from afar, e.g. via a two-way mirror, or as a bystander taking notes, e.g. Bandura’s Bobo doll. In observations there is no manipulation of an independent variable. observational learning: Bandura’s term for the process of learning certain behaviours by a child as it observes and imitates the behaviours of others. operant conditioning: attributed to a major player in behaviourism, B F Skinner. Skinner believed that we are encouraged towards certain behaviours by the use of positive reinforcement (reward) and discouraged from certain behaviours by the use of negative reinforcement (punishment). DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 105 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS organisation: an innate or inborn ability to organise information about our world identified by Jean Piaget. participant: more modern term used to signify people who take part in psychological research. Previously referred to as subjects. Pavlov, Ivan Petrovitch: Soviet Nobel Prize winning physiologist (1849–1936) whose work with dogs saw him putting forward a learning theory regarding classical conditioning. Classical conditioning is where we learn that one event will follow another; or that one event predicts another. Piaget, Jean: Swiss psychologist (1896–1980) whose main contribution to psychology is that cognitive development (notably intelligence) occurs in stages for children. Children cannot do certain cognitive tasks until they are biologically ready. pre-operational stage (2–7): Piaget’s second stage of cognitive development in children. Important aspects of the pre-operational stage are conservation of number, volume, length, etc; animism, egocentrism and decentring. positive reinforcement: behaviourist term attributed to B F Skinner which means the use of reward to encourage particular behaviours. reflex action: or unconditioned response is a behaviourist term concerning any response that is produced by an organism whenever an unconditioned stimulus is present. In Pavlov’s famous experiment into classical conditioning, the unconditioned response was salivation, when food powder (the unconditioned stimulus) was put into each dog’s mouth. reversibility: at the concrete operational stage of cognitive development, Piaget believes we develop reversibility. Reversibility is our mental ability to move concepts around in our minds in order to solve problems. It might be seen in a child of between 7 and 11 when they are presented with two (undisclosed) half-pint jugs of water and are asked to work out without touching anything whether or not the two half-pint measures will ‘fit into’ a third jug of undisclosed size. rule-bound play: develops according to Piaget during the concreteoperational stage of cognitive development. It is where the developing child begins to appreciate that in order to work, play and live together rules are necessary to govern people’s behaviour. 106 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS sequence of development: physical, cognitive, social and emotional changes across the human lifecycle, some of which happen in identifiable and progressive stages (age-stage problem solving and language abilities, etc.) while others happen in a similarly identifiable but continuous manner (the ageing process, non-age stage development of personality, etc.) scaffolding: attributed to the Soviet psychologist Lev Vygotsky. He believed that cognitive ability depended very much on what we learn from our social world. Scaffolding is the support we get as children, teenagers and adults to help us develop whatever potentials we have. Scaffolding comes from parents, brothers, sisters, teachers or indeed anyone who has a significant positive influence on the nurturing of cognitive development in individuals. schema: an organised structure or bundle of knowledge we have about aspects of our world that can change with age and experience. Piaget thinks our ability to organise our world on the basis of schemata is innate. semantic mode: a term used by Jerome Bruner to identify how infants and children develop ways to think about their world. Thinking about the world using the semantic mode occurs when children begin to think, or represent, their world using language. Language is important in that it allows the young child to enquire, think about, deal with and act upon their environment. For Bruner, being able to use the semantic mode, or form of representing knowledge in a meaningful way to deal with the world, is the key to intellectual growth and development. Semantic thinking turns on language. sensori-motor stage (0–2): Piaget’s first stage of cognitive development from birth to age 2. Initially the baby can only deal with its world using innate reflex actions such as rooting. A baby has no knowledge of a world outwith and away from itself. If it can’t see an object, that object no longer exists in the baby’s mind in relation to its world. This egocentrism gradually diminishes during this stage as the infant develops object permanence sequences of development concerning play behaviours. Sylva (1980) and others propose that play can be categorised broadly into challenging play, and simple/passive play. She identifies four categories of play behaviours: 1 Simple play; 2 Complex play; 3 Practice play; 4 Symbolic play. Another psychologist, Parten (1933), confirms much of Sylva’s work. For Parten, play develops in five stages: 1 Solitary play; 2 Onlooker play; 3 Parallel play; 4 Associative play; 5 Co-operative play. The development of play DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 107 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS behaviours and strategies influences cognitive, social and moral development. Personality is also affected. Skinner, B F: Burrhus Frederic Skinner (1904–90) was an early American behaviourist whose theory of operant conditioning has been hugely influential in understanding how we learn behaviours from early infancy onwards. For Skinner, the use of reward (positive reinforcement) will encourage the repetition of desired behaviours and the use of punishment (negative reinforcement) should discourage unwanted behaviours. Operant conditioning is very obvious in all aspects of life. It is used by parents in the home, teachers in school and society in general. Skinner Box: apparatus used by the behaviourist B F Skinner to develop his theory of operant conditioning. It is a cage-like structure that contains a food delivery chute and a lever. A rat would be put in the box. First the rat explores its environment and will investigate the lever. It finds that food is delivered when the lever is pressed. When the rat is later observed to successively press the lever to obtain food, this is taken to be proof that learning has occurred. The rat has come to associate the pressing of the lever with the delivery of food. Food is the positive reinforcer that will see the rat press the lever thereafter. social learning theory: attributed to the Canadian psychologist Alfred Bandura, social learning theory believes that children learn social behaviours by modelling themselves on others using imitation and observation. What they see adults do is especially influential on children. stages: developmental psychology has a special interest in finding out what physical, cognitive, social and emotional changes happen is stages during our life and what changes happen continuously. Changes that occur in identifiable and distinct stages in our life would include the cognitive development of language. A change that occurs in a continuous fashion would be ageing. stimuli: an object, event, situation, etc., which we come across in our environment and to which we normally respond in some way, i.e. if it rains (the environmental stimulus) we put an umbrella up (the organism’s response). symbolic play: Sylva et al. (1980) report that play behaviours develop in a sequence of stages. Symbolic play is imaginary, make-believe or 108 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS pretend play that becomes more and more evident in children from 4 onwards. It is play which involves children using objects to represent other things, e.g. a brush handle being used to ride around on because it represents a horse; a collection of books end-up on the floor which become a cave; dolls, teddies, etc., which become real people. Sylva believes symbolic play to itself represent the child’s realisation that there is a difference between the real and imagined world. Make-believe play encourages imagination and related language and creative thinking ability. Symbolic play helps shape social skills and the development of personality. spontaneous recovery: behaviourist term associated with Pavlov. In the absence of reinforcement, the desired response to a conditional stimulus will disappear. This is known as extinction. Occasionally however, after a period of rest, the organism may once again respond to a conditional stimulus when it is presented. This is called spontaneous recovery. treatment: when used in the context of psychological research, ‘treatment’ is the giving of something to an experimental group to see whether it has any effect on behaviour. If you were trying to measure the effect alcohol had on reaction time you would give measures of alcohol to people and test their reactions as a consequence. The measure of alcohol is the ‘treatment’. Vygotsky, Lev: Soviet psychologist (1896–1934) who emphasises the importance of social interaction with others as the key to cognitive development and fulfilment of whatever zone of proximal development an individual might have. Watson, John Broadus: American founding father of the behaviourist school in psychology (1878–1958) who, in the early part of the twentieth century, did much to move psychology towards being as like a real science as it can. He was influenced by the work of Pavlov in the former USSR. ZPD or zone of proximal development: each individual’s range of physical/cognitive potential as illustrated by Vygotsky. We all have similar potential for different things, but not all in equal measure. ZPD is influenced by genetics, but if genetic potential is not encouraged and nurtured by scaffolders, an individual’s potential in a particular area may never be realised. The richness of the child’s social world is important for Vygotsky to the development and fulfilment of every child’s zones of proximal development. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 109 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Concept 3: Ageing The study of ageing is, as is ‘early socialisation’ and ‘cognitive development’, an optional key concept at Advanced Higher level. Students are asked to consider two key concepts and one issue. This is the third key concept considered in this AH pack. What is ‘ageing’ in developmental psychology? Ageing in developmental psychology generally examines the ‘meaning’ of old; physical and physiological change and social adjustment theories as a consequence of ageing – notably social disengagement theory; activity theory and the psychosocial theory of personality development as it applies in the later years of life. In a psychological sense ageing sees interest at Advanced Higher focus upon: • Changes in physical and cognitive functioning • Psychological ageing • Theories and research of ageing – disengagement theory, activity theory • Social ageing. Ageing Average life expectancy in industrialised countries now stands at 75, with women living seven or eight years longer than men. For most of their history, humans have lived, on average, a mere 40 or 50 years – a lifespan heavily influenced by deaths in infancy. It is only in the last 50 years, when improvements in health care have enabled more people to survive into adult life, that we have seen an acceleration in longevity and an increasing proportion of old people in the population. But these startling advances have a downside: a rise in disability and chronic diseases, the most prevalent of which is Alzheimer’s disease. The World Health Organisation’s statement – ‘Health expectancy is more important than life expectancy’ – has never been more apt. 110 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Things you might want to know about ageing and demographic change in the UK The make-up and distribution of our population will be very different in 30 years time. In 1995 there were fewer than 9 million people over 65 in the UK. By 2030 there will be 50 per cent more – almost 14 million. In 1991, 21 per cent of the workforce were aged 20–34. By 2001 this had dropped to 14 per cent. People are having fewer children; family life and the world of work have changed for almost everyone and life expectancy is increasing. Taken together, these developments will have significant economic implications for all of us – whatever our age. In just one generation, the number of first time marriages has halved and divorces have trebled. Lone parents now head almost a quarter of families with children, nearly three times the proportion in 1971. For the first time in 1997 more women had babies between the ages of 30 and 35 than those aged 20 to 25. When the National Health Service was designed, life expectancy was around 50 years. Today it is around 80. It was set up when 60 per cent of the population were under 20. Soon 50 per cent of the population will be over 50. This is a tremendous tribute to medicine and to improved social conditions. It creates great opportunities for a healthier old age. But it also pays testament to the substantial changes which are taking place in our society now. By 2021 41 per cent of the population will be over 50. By 2031 the proportion of those aged over 60–65 compared with those of working age will have doubled. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 111 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS UK demographic facts of life 1. In 1901 life expectancy for men was 49 and 45 for women. In 2001 life expectancy for women was 80 and 75 for men. 2. In 1951 there were 300 centenarians; in 2031 there will be 36,000. 3. In 1961 there were almost four people of working age to support each pensioner; by 2040 there will only be 2. 4. In 1996 there were 5.8 million single-person households. By 2011 this figure will rise to 7.9 million representing a third of all households. In Great Britain in 1994/95, 21 per cent of dependent children lived in single-parent families – three times the proportion in 1972. 5. In 1971 women’s share of employment was 35 per cent. Today it is just under 50 per cent and it is forecast to exceed male employment by early in the twenty-first century. 6. For the first time ever in 1997, more women aged 30–34 had babies than women aged 20–24. 7. When the National Health Service was designed, 60 per cent of the population were under 20 – by 2020 that proportion will be less than a quarter. 8. When the Welfare State was designed, actuarial projections estimated only 3 years of life after retirement. 9. In just one generation, the number of first time marriages has halved and divorces have trebled. 10. In 1995 there were fewer than 9 million people over 65 in the UK – by 2030 there will be almost 50 per cent more. From 1997 to 2040, the number of people aged over 65 will increase at 10 times the overall rate of population growth. 112 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS So what do we mean by ‘ageing’? As is often the case in psychology, definition of our topic is at first straightforward but can soon turn problematic. ‘Ageing can be defined as growing old. It is the process of progressive change, which occurs with the passage of time.’ Reber, A, Dictionary of Psychology (1995) However, in consideration of ‘what is age’ Kastenbaum (1975) distinguishes between five different kinds of age: • • • • • chronological (the exact number of years a person has lived) biological (the condition of the body) subjective (how old a person feels in themselves) functional (what kind of life they lead) social (how we mix/interact/are accepted with and by others). These ages can be, and often are, quite different – not often correlating with one another at all. Few elderly people would describe themselves consistently in terms of Kastenbaum’s ‘ages of me’. For example, it is quite common for someone’s subjective age to be very different from his or her chronological age, maybe as a consequence of a young/old social age, in that many chronologically old people feel young inside. Some people, through paying attention to exercise and fitness routines, may have a biological age that is quite different from their chronological age. Others, who maybe engage in a lot of activities, feel functionally younger than their chronological years. Whatever ‘ages of me’ (Kastenbaum, 1985) one feels one reflects, the demographic imperative (Swenson, 1983) (of far more people living longer nowadays than ever before) has seen a growth in interest in the psychology of ageing from within developmental psychology. Developmental psychology is nowadays particularly interested in physical, cognitive, social and emotional change and developments (and their inter-relationships) as much at the end of life as at the beginnings. Life expectancy of people today is much longer than in previous years. Life expectancy refers to the age that people are expected to live, if the conditions are right. This is due to the better living conditions of our more modern times. A better standard of living, good medical services and advances, better social environments and proper nutrition have all helped contribute to a much longer life span in the UK than previously experienced. Saying this, there is a class issue inherent in life expectancy. Research consistently shows that in Scotland those who are DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 113 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS deemed working class have a life expectancy on average 7 years less than their middle- and upper-middle class counterparts. Lifespan is the actual amount of time a person lives for. Vaupel’s (1997) nature/nurture estimate indicates that 25 per cent of the variation in human lifespans is caused by genetic factors; another 25 per cent by factors such as class, education, diet and behaviour, which are fixed by the age of 30; and 50 per cent by short-term factors like standing in front of a truck! The ‘elderly’ or ‘aged’ is a man or woman over retirement age. It is clear however that this is too general. This alone could mean 60, 65, or any age in-between or even lower if you have taken ‘early retirement’. Burnside et al. (1979) view the last years of our lives in terms of a decade-by-decade model, probably better reflecting a more specific reality concerning older people. Burnside’s decade-by-decade model identifies: • The young-old (60–69): a major transition in the Levinson sense (see his 1978 ‘Seasons of Man’s Life’ proposition in the HSDU Intermediate 1 and 2 Developmental Psychology pack 6544, 2000). Most in their young-old decade see themselves taking up a new role structure to cope with changes found with their new ‘status’ – such as income and expenditure adjustments, friends dying, social networks disappearing, etc. While physical strength may not be as it once was, many aged 60–69 report new energies and enthusiasms for things they value or have come to value in life. • The middle-aged old (70–79) have to confront the now regular reminder of their own mortality by coping with increasing numbers of their contemporaries dying around them. They are no longer as active in organisations as they were pre- and early post-retirement which can lead to psychological frustration. Personal health becomes a major issue. They have the task of keeping their young-old reintegrated personality together which is often made difficult with physical decline if nothing else. • The old old (80–89) show difficulty in adapting to environmental changes around them and resulting interactions. Their social, cognitive and emotional interactions often have to be (physically) assisted by others. • The very old old (90–99) experience ever-increasing health difficulties which are balanced against a new-found freedom from 114 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS responsibility. If crises in previous elderly decades have been resolved the very old old may here enjoy a decade of serenity, joy and fulfilment. The smile on the face of the redoubtable and psychologically interesting HRH The Queen Mother when she reached her 100th birthday in 2000 neatly illustrates this point. Craig (1992) reminds us that the elderly are not one homogenous group but a number of sub-groups, each one of which has particular issues with which to deal. He also views getting old in a more positive way than most in that ‘having a problem is not the same as being a problem, and the all-to-popular view of those over 65 as needy, non-productive and unhappy needs revision.’ Changes in physical functioning Biological ageing Multi-cellular animals have two types of cells: germ cells, which go on generation after generation, and somatic or body cells. There are two basic strategies for survival. Either the animal puts energy into regeneration and repair of its body, so it can go on indefinitely, or it puts energy into early reproduction in which case its body will age and die. Most animals, including humans, have evolved the latter, which produces more offspring to survive to the next generation. Although there is good evidence that body maintenance is more efficient in longer-lived animals like humans than in those that live for shorter times, we age because our maintenance mechanisms eventually fail and we cannot replace essential parts of ourselves when they go wrong (for example brain cells, heart muscle, major nerves and blood vessels). In the context of comparative and evolutionary biology, although humans show a steadily increasing death rate with advancing age, there is nothing inevitable about this pattern. Most animals do age for two reasons. The first is that because animals in the wild do not generally survive predation to live into old age, natural selection has only limited opportunity to affect events which happen late in life. This means that any mutations or changes harmful in later life can accumulate in the genome practically unchecked by natural selection, and their effects finally age the whole organism. The second is that to reproduce many offspring early on in a lifespan is a better survival strategy than to reproduce at a constant rate throughout life. Early reproduction, however, uses up so much energy that animals which follow this strategy do not have as much to expend on maintaining their bodies, so they sacrifice their chances of longer life span. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 115 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Humans have evolved the longest life span of any mammalian species because our brains have enabled us to reduce the risk posed by our environment. Menopause seems to be a paradox: it shuts down reproductive function when a woman is still capable of bearing children. However, it enables the older woman to contribute to the survival of her grandchildren – and thus her genes. Genetics can help us understand ageing also. Although genes are currently thought to be responsible for only 30 per cent of longevity, and the environment for 70 per cent, genes lead us to fundamental mechanisms. People who have aged particularly successfully may owe this to genetic factors which warrant research. The ageing process in the brain sees healthy brains lose volume and weight and up to 50 per cent of their nerve cells (neurons). The long extensions of the nerve cells, and the regions in which a message is passed from one to another, proliferate in an attempt at compensation. These genetically triggered events and changes are the same as, but less severe than, those seen in Alzheimer’s brains, which also show characteristic senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. The senile plaques are outside cells, and the protein amyloid is in the plaque cores and around them. The relationship between plaques and tangles is not clear. The tangles, made of tau protein, are inside the nerve cells and develop in a regular pattern throughout the brain. They disrupt the transport of messages from one nerve cell to another. The cells atrophy and die, and dementia results. A much studied aspect of ageing such as early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is genetically determined: it is caused by mutations in three known genes, and a gene which makes a protein called APOE e4 is a risk factor. APOE e4 is 2.5 times more common in Alzheimer’s patients than in the general population. There is probably also a genetic component to late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, but that is not related to the three known genes. There are also environmental risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease, such as head trauma. This aside, senescence, or primary biological ageing, is when obvious signs of ageing become apparent. These signs of ageing are physical, occur gradually and can be affected by individual differences. Senescence involves physiological and biochemical changes which have behavioural implications. From the time of late adulthood the physiological ageing process can be clearly seen, for instance, the hair may become grey and even begin to thin. The skin loses some of its elasticity and wrinkles appear, muscle tone also diminishes. Some people even lose some of their height; this due to the cartilage between the vertebrae wearing. The body organs are likely to function less efficiently, for instance in the case of the heart and lungs; this causes a decrease in physical stamina. Some of the senses dull, such as hearing. 116 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Many old people may find it difficult to understand speech, particularly if spoken in a high pitched voice. Sight also may become impaired as the lenses in the eyes become hard and discoloured with age. Bee and Mitchell (1980) summarise the physical aspects of ageing thus: • Smaller: tissue holding long bones in arms, legs, spine, etc., becomes compressed and flat. Our calcium metabolism changes in old age leading to lesser body weight in comparison to younger people. Muscle mass reduces with some organs getting smaller (e.g. bladder). • Slower: neural signals take longer to travel to the brain and reaction time slows. Immune system functions less efficiently to illness, disease and accident. Liver and skin cell reproduction slows. • Weaker: bones (due to calcium metabolism) become more brittle and prone to fracture. Muscle strength weakens, senses dull. • Lesser: elasticity of skin diminishes causing wrinkles and sags! Lack of elasticity of tissue in the ear drum and eyes gives rise to hearing/sight difficulties. Blood vessels harden which leads to circulatory problems. • Fewer: body hair, teeth and taste receptors on tongue. Adapted from Bee and Mitchell (1980) There are numerous theories that attempt to explain biological ageing. One such theory is the Error Theory. This theory assumes that the deficits of ageing are due to the accumulation of random genetic damage, or from the flow of genetic information being disrupted by small errors. This would culminate in the reduction of proper cell function, or may even prevent it. Regeneration of cells is crucial to keeping us looking young. As we age the regeneration of cells is frustrated. American microbiologist Leonard Hayflick found that human tissue cells are capable of only a limited number of cell divisions before they die. Hayflick argues that cells are already pre-programmed with each person’s blueprint of ageing. This would explain why different animal species have different life spans. Physiological theories of ageing focus on the functioning, and inter-relationships, of our organic system. Particular emphasis on physiological ageing is placed on the immune system which appears to lose its capacity to fight off infection as we age. Antibodies are produced but tend to have difficulty in differentiating between good and bad cells in the body. Psychological ageing The process of human ageing need not only be considered in the context of physiology. There are other important aspects of ageing to take into account. These are psychological and social. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 117 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Psychological aspects of ageing include cognition, especially intelligence and memory. Social aspects of ageing concern the outcomes of interaction of the ageing person’s social world and their personality. It is at this juncture that students may be interested to learn that those interested in the psychology of ageing are of two theoretical orientations. On the one hand there are those theorists who favour a decrement model – where ageing, viewed in a negative sense, is talked about in terms of decay in physical, cognitive, social and emotional functioning. On the other hand are those who advocate a more positive personal growth model which emphasises the opportunities old age can bring in terms of leisure time, reduced responsibility and focused priorities which can enhance physical, cognitive, social and emotional functioning (Kalish, 1982). At the level of the person, which model or category one falls into is very much determined by previous past experience and physical, cognitive, social and emotional development. Some older people have a decremental experience while others tend towards personal growth. The influence of previous past experiences, a psychologically healthy/ unhealthy self, society’s view of the elderly, your ability to actualise on opportunities for personal growth, your culture, your personality, etc., all come together to give the end to life a negative or positive outcome. Psychological ageing and cognitive functioning One worry the older person has about ageing is the fear that they lose cognitive abilities such as thinking productively, memory, decision making, etc. Initially thought to be the case as a consequence of crosssectional studies, where different age groups were tested on the same things at the same time, there is no conclusive evidence to support wholesale cognitive dysfunction as a result of biological ageing. It is estimated that only about 5 per cent of the population will suffer from senility to the extent of major loss of cognitive functioning (Zimbardo et al. 1995). Cross-sectional studies attract the cohort effect (all taking part in different age groups have had different experiences) and as a result cognitive differences cannot conclusively be said to be down to ageing alone. Unless we have evidence of intellectual ability for each participant at an earlier age we cannot conclude intelligence declines with age on the basis of cross-sectional study. Better evidence concerning cognitive functioning has come from longitudinal studies which test and re-test the same people on particular variables at different points during their 118 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS lives. Longitudinal studies indicate that while cognitive functioning can be retained well into old age (Holahan and Sears, 1995) evidence shows that there are some age-related changes regarding different kinds of intelligence and aspects of memory. Changes in intelligence Cognitive changes, although apparent in some people to some degree, are not evident in all. As said earlier, cognitive change in a negative sense is not a pre-requisite of old age. However, if cognitive functioning is impaired, it is usually limited to certain cognitions such as thinking and problem solving. Herein lies ‘intelligence’ and our interest in how/if it is influenced as a result of biological ageing. It is not the purpose of AH Developmental Psychology to engage in the ‘What is intelligence and can it be measured?’ debate. This is more the province of the AH Psychology of Individual Differences. While it can be criticised, the psychometric approach to intelligence here is relevant. The psychometric approach is a way of studying intelligence that emphasises the products of intelligence (including IQ scores) using a statistical technique called factor analysis. It tries to look for evidence to support one general intelligence trait or specific abilities. The psychometric approach has led to Spearman’s g (general intelligence), Thurstone’s factor analysis and seven primary mental abilities, and Cattell’s fluid intelligence and crystallised intelligence. What all agree about intelligence, whatever it may be, is that it is multi-dimensional in that it is composed of a number of different capacities and abilities. One way of looking at intelligence is, as indicated above, to separate it out into two general ‘types’: fluid and crystallised intelligence (Cattell, 1963). Crystallised intelligence (gc) refers to knowledge that a person has already acquired such as vocabulary, arithmetic and general knowledge. It is accumulated knowledge, linked to education, experience and culture and is measured by tests of general information. Fluid intelligence (gf) refers to the ability to solve novel and unusual problems creatively and learn and retain new information quickly. Fluid intelligence is about memory span and mental dexterity. Practically, it is identified in behaviours such as finding a pattern to numbers presented, visualising and problem solving on an object in your mind, doing jigsaws, etc. Fluid intelligence aids problem solving and is measured by tests measuring new and abstract problems not previously encountered and not based on previous knowledge or learning. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 119 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Gf and gc intelligences are found within the Thurstone (1938) Primary Mental Ability (PMA) test and the later 1958 WAIS (Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale). Gf are measures of abilities such as response time to set problems, memory span and non-verbal reasoning. Gc are measures of abilities such as reading comprehension and vocabulary. Crystallised and fluid intelligence measures are substantially correlated. Horn (1984) reported a study in which gf and gc measures were got from an analysis of the WAIS (Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale). The correlation between crystallised and fluid intelligence factors was 0.61. Such findings have led those who believe in (just) one intelligence to argue that gf and gc are simply different aspects of a more general intelligence (IQ) factor. This cannot be answered conclusively by looking at correlations between tests alone. By stepping outside of factor analysis we can look at how gf and gc measures respond to manipulations that might change mental competence. It turns out that they respond differently. They therefore may not be the two aspects of the same thing, i.e. IQ. The most striking example is ageing. Measures of gf generally decrease from early adulthood onward, whereas gc measures remain constant or even increase throughout most of our working life (Horn 1985; Horn and Noll 1994). This is not surprising. Experience counts; most of the key leadership positions in our society are held by people over 40. On the other hand, middle-aged and older people do take longer than younger people to understand new problem-solving methods and to deal with unfamiliar tasks. Age is not the only variable that can be shown to have different influences on fluid and crystallised intelligence. Alcoholism shows similar effects. 120 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Crystallised Intelligence Fluid Intelligence Infancy Childhood Adolescence AGE Adulthood Old age Research indicates that crystallised intelligence increases with age (Horn, 1982) while fluid intelligence declines. In a cross-longitudinal study into crystallised intelligence Schaie and Hertzog (1983) say that fluid intelligence peaks at between 20 and 30 and declines for all age groups thereafter. On the other hand crystallised intelligence improves. The reasons are unknown. It may be that the decline in fluid intelligence is related to the biological ageing process in the brain which influences this particular intellectual aspect of neurological functioning. Alternatively, according to Denney and Palmer (1981), it may be because crystallised abilities are called for throughout our life span, while demands on fluid intelligence are less likely in old age (Cavanaugh, 1995). Research in this area has been confirmed by Botwinick (1978) who found that gf begins to decline in middle age and drops sharply for most in late adulthood while Nesselroade et al. (1972) discovered that gc remains the same or actually improves until the early 80s. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 121 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Studying g using a cross-longitudinal methodology In 1956, Schaie gave Thurstone’s PMA to a group of 161 subjects whose ages ranged from 20–70. Initially arranged into discrete seven yearly agegroups cohorts, i.e. 25, 32, 39, 46, 53, 60 and 67, they were similarly tested in 1963 and 1970. Number, word fluency, comprehension and spatial abilities were tested. On comprehension or word meaning, scores for the 25 and 32 age group cohorts got better over the 14-year period. For the 50 to 60-year-olds comprehension showed minimal decline while for the 60 to 67-year-olds comprehension became more noticeably difficult for them. This pattern was repeated in the main for overall scores, e.g. fluid intelligence, as measured by for example word fluency, showed a decline for all age groups (Schaie and Hertzog, 1983). Some controversy exists as to whether decline in fluid intelligence is due to inevitable biological ageing factors or simply lack of practice. Baltes and Willis (1982) in establishing Project ADEPT trained adults in those PMAs that showed a decline with age. Fluid intelligence improved significantly with initial training and booster sessions in 1981 and 1986 with improvement being maintained among even the oldest in their 70s and 80s (Willis and Nesselroade, 1990, in Cavanaugh, 1995). A discussion of a landmark longitudinal study into cognitive ageing In 1996 Ian Deary, Professor of Differential Psychology at the University of Edinburgh and President of the International Society for the Study of Individual Differences, was contacted by Professor Lawrence Whalley, Head of Mental Health at the University of Aberdeen. Professor Whalley was researching into cardiovascular disease among the Aberdeen Birth Cohort of 1932, then 64. Professor Whalley wanted to know if there would be any benefit in testing the cohort on cognitive abilities such as intelligence as a consequence of ageing. Deary thought this, while laudable, problematic. There was no data available on any of the samples’ intelligence from an earlier point in their life. Measurement of any relative change in intelligence as a result of the ageing process could therefore not be done. Almost at the same time however, Deary made an astonishing discovery. He found out that SCRE (the Scottish Council for Research in Education) had inherited the results of the Scottish Mental Survey of Monday 1 June 1932 of 87,498 Scottish 11-yearolds born in 1921. The test given them was a version of one of Sir Godfrey Thomson’s Moray House Tests as used in the English 11-plus examinations. Teachers administered and scored the tests. The purpose of the exercise was to discover the distribution of the ability of the age group and to use the data as an aid in formulating educational provision for the 1940s and 1950s. The same exercise was repeated in 122 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 1947 involving 70,805 people born in 1936. This was the 1947 Scottish Mental Survey. At the time, and for some years thereafter, the Scottish Mental Surveys were internationally renowned. The data on mental ability and demographic, educational and physical factors had academic and practical worth. Research agendas however move on. As the 11year-olds of 1932 celebrated their half-centuries and passed through retirement into their three score years and ten, surveys became forgotten history. As luck would have it, SCRE had far-sightedly retained the data. The ledgers and brown-paper-tied-with-string parcels of data recorded in copperplate writing were safely stacked and locked away in Edinburgh maturing to a research resource of unparalleled richness. Whalley’s interests combined with Deary’s. Both then set about realising the potential of the Scottish Mental Survey data. Now the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 (funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council) and parallel studies in Aberdeen are under way. People in their late 70s (from the 1932 Survey) and in their early 60s (from the 1947 Survey) are being re-tested. The purpose is to discover the social, educational, medical, psychological and genetic factors that assist people, over a lifetime, to preserve their mental abilities. In other words, to find the secrets of a sagacious and successful old age. Mental ability and growing old Populations in the West are shifting towards a greater proportion of older people. In tandem with the better-known physical changes of age, the brain grows old too. Some, but not all, mental skills decline, especially from the seventh decades. The quality of life in old age is affected by how well mental ability is maintained. A healthy mind begets, to a degree, a healthy and happy, not to say serene, old age. But some people’s thinking skills stay relatively sharp while others’ are blunted. These differences in the ageing of the brain’s functions have become a priority in recent research. Irritatingly, one essential datum is almost always missing from studies trying to discover why people differ in cognitive ageing: the way they were. It is almost impossible to tell how much a person’s mental abilities have changed if one does not know what they used to be. Here’s the problem. Examine the mental abilities of a group of older people. Some score better than others on tests of memory, reasoning, perception, and so forth. That does not provide information about what the slings and arrows of a half-century’s miscellaneous fortunes have wreaked on the brain. The high scorers might always have scored highly. The low scorers might always have been practical rather than cerebral; common-sensical rather than brainy. The interesting people DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 123 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS are those who held steady or even improved their thinking skills with age. And those who have slid down the slope into cognitive disability are interesting too, though more poignantly. To find out the extent to which people have altered in their thinking skills one must know their former level. The problem is that studies of old people’s intellectual functions rarely have access to mental ability test scores from earlier in life. Such data are of a value that is hard to overestimate. That is why the rediscovery of the Scottish Mental Survey data will be so important for this area of research. Still stable after all these years… With current research underway on a study that began in SCRE’s offices in 1932 there is still much to be done. Thus far hundreds of people in their late 70s have been tested at the University’s Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility at the Western General Hospital with many more yet to be contacted and seen in the laboratories in Edinburgh and Aberdeen. Nevertheless, the studies progress in the knowledge that because of the Scottish Mental Surveys, there exist valid mental test data on an entire population, now old, and that Scotland will be the source of some first-rate research on the determinants of differences in cognitive ageing which can be utilised in a global sense. Some dividend has come through already. On 1 June 1998 the Aberdeen participants in the 1932 survey came back for a mass-retesting on the Moray House Test at the Music Hall in Aberdeen’s Union Street. At this memorable meeting, and a smaller one a few weeks later, a total of 101 people sat looking at the same test questions 66 years on to the very day. The same instructions were read out as those given by their teachers in 1932 and time limits maintained. The results indicate that the seventysomethings scored quite a bit better than they previously did at age 11 and that mental ability differences appear quite stable from age 11 to age 77. With some interesting exceptions the original high scorers did as well while the modest remained … modest. This is by far the longest follow-up study of mental ability differences to date found in psychological research and epoch-making in its importance. The author gratefully acknowledges the assistance of Professor Ian Deary of Edinburgh University in the above. 124 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Changes in memory Memory, one of our five cognitive processes (perception, attention, language, memory and thinking or problem solving) has long had negative connotations with ageing. Studies show that short-term memory (STM), which is our ability to recall immediate information that is still being attended to, suffers only minimal decline. There is little difference between the younger and older adult as regards STM capacity (7±2 ‘chunks’). The older person is less likely to be more susceptible to distraction and less able to recall memorised information in a different form. As far as ageing and long-term memory (LTM) goes, older people have difficulty remembering information that they have actively ceased to need or use, but have no difficulty recalling knowledge and experiences that they recall often. On recall tests, it has been found that older adults generally do less well than younger participants. The reasons for any memory deficit may be due to biological decline which frustrates some aspects of memory and information processing (Stuart-Hamilton, 1994). On recognition tasks, differences due to ageing are not as obvious. This biological rationale to memory decline has been challenged by the likes of Diamond (1978) whose study shows 90+ per cent of those over 65 exhibit little cortical deterioration (loss of cortical neurons) and indeed under enriched conditions in old age new neural connections can be identified being formed as a result of cognitive ‘hot-housing’. Further support for this comes from Rogers et al. (1990) who says, as with general intellectual functioning, the more well-educated and mentally active you are in older age, the better able you are in holding off (long-term) memory decline. Biological decline of memory and inevitable deficit as a consequence of old-age is not clear-cut. Those advocating the alternative explanation of negative cultural stereotypes put forward an interesting position. In 1994 Levy and Langer investigated the influence of stereotypes on memory decline by looking at the memory capabilities of three groups of people: • Hearing Americans • Members of the American deaf community • People from mainland China. They assumed members of the US deaf community had been less exposed to negative stereotypes concerning assumptions about memory decline (among other things) in old age. China was chosen due to the deference Chinese society and culture gives its old people. Levy and Langer discovered that the older American deaf and Chinese subjects DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 125 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS did significantly better on memory tasks than their hearing American counterparts. Qualitatively it was further found that hearing younger Americans had less positive perceptions about ageing than any of the other groups. Among the older groups, a positive correlation was found between attitude to ageing and memory performance. This is reminiscent of the self-fulfilling prophecy where low expectations, as a kind of learned helplessness, make it less likely older people will engage in activities to enhance memory ability in old age. Levy (1996) concludes that a negative self-stereotype influences memory ability in old age in a negative fashion, while positive self-stereotypes improve it. Levy found no such self-stereotype with younger generations because for them selfstereotypes of ageing are not important … yet! Stereotypes of the elderly contain negative elements whatever the age group investigated. Some of the strongest negative stereotypes of the elderly are held by children and teenagers. Goldman and Goldman (1981) interviewed more than 800 children aged 5–15 in Australia, England, Sweden and the USA about their perceptions of old age. Two generalities were found: 1. All, no matter their age, were more likely to hold negative attitudes towards the elderly than positive. The older the child, the greater the negative image in that over 90 per cent of 15-year-olds perceived the elderly in negative ways. 2. Goldman and Goldman emphasise the disgust shown by participants on being asked about old age. Physical deterioration was particularly commented upon in negative terms, i.e. wrinkly, feeble, infirm, sick, etc., as were psychological perceptions, i.e. grumpy, bad-tempered, irritable, mentally ‘slow’, etc. This may be due to a number of reasons, i.e. person perception and the nature of physical attractiveness (Jackson, 1992) and the further perception that with old age comes a downward change in status (Durkin, 1995). Student activity: Essay Identify, describe and discuss physical and cognitive issues associated with the ageing process. Your essay should be about 1000 words in length. 126 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Theories and research on ageing: disengagement theory and activity theory Social disengagement theory: a decrement model of ageing In 1961, Cummings and Henry, during a five-year study of 275 people aged between 50 and 90 in Kansas City, USA, attempted to describe what happens to us socially as we grow old. Social disengagement refers to mutual society-older individual withdrawal recognised in compulsory retirement, children leaving home, death of a spouse, friends, etc. It also includes mutual withdrawal of the individual from society in forms such as reduced social activities and social isolation. Cummings and Henry propose that there is a biological process of social disengagement which takes place as we grow old. Consequentially the older person gradually withdraws from wider society and participation in it. Thus social disengagement is: ‘an inevitable process in which many of the relationships between a person and other members of society are severed, and those remaining are altered in quality’. Cummings and Henry, 1961, p. 210. Alternatively, according to Bromley (1988), it is: ‘a systematic reduction in certain kinds of social interaction. In its simplest and crudest form, the theory of disengagement states that diminishing psychological and biological capacities of people in later life necessitate a severance of the relationships they have with younger people in the central activities of society, and the replacement of these older individuals by younger people. In this way, society renews itself and the elderly are free to die’. Cummings (1975) suggests social disengagement to be: • Shrinkage of life space • Increased individuality • Acceptance, often wholeheartedly, of these age-related changes. Growing Old – The Process of Disengagement (1975) Shrinkage of life space is an aspect of entering old age that sees us having fewer roles and therefore shrinking social interactions. This is due to retirement, physical aspects of ageing affecting mobility, death of spouse and friends, etc. Increased individuality arises as a result of the DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 127 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS above and consequentially older people are not as hidebound by rules and conventions in their roles and interactions that remain. You are effectively in a better position to ‘Please yourself’! Acceptance sees the healthy older person consciously withdraw from roles and relationships, becoming more introspective, as if themselves preparing for death. Withdrawal is viewed as the most appropriate way to age. Cummings and Henry suggest that disengagement might be both natural and inevitable and by implication have a biological basis to it in order to help prepare the older person for death. Disengagement became popular in the 1960s and early 1970s because it seemed to explain how there could be so many old people in society and yet how they could seem so ‘invisible’. It was also popular because it presented a genetic explanation for the phenomenon, and genetic explanations were popular with the media, at least in the USA at that time. While most would hold with the first two propositions that with ageing comes a shrinkage of life space and increased individuality it is with the third claim of inevitable acceptance that criticisms are most directed (Bee, 1994). Bromley (1988) offers three criticisms that challenge social disengagement theory. 1. Practical: social disengagement theory offers a negative view supporting societal indifference, exclusion, isolation and segregation of the elderly. 2. Theoretical: social disengagement theory offers little in the way of evidence that this is the actualité of the aged population. It is a proto-theory in that it is a ‘collection of loosely related assumptions and arguments’ (McIlveen and Gross, 1997). 3. Empirical: social disengagement theory is questionable in the absence of empirical data to support its claim that all the elderly psychologically disengage as it suggests. Prompted by the absence of empirical data supporting disengagement theory, Havighurst et al. (1968) followed up about 50 per cent of the original Cummings and Henry sample. While they found evidence to support our orientation towards social disengagement as we age they also found older people who were still ‘active and engaged’. These participants were found to be happy and contented, and the more active an individual was, the happier they appeared to be. They had good morale and indeed lived longer than their more disengaged peers. This 128 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS goes against the view that disengagement is natural and part and parcel of the ageing process (Bee, 1994). Disengagement is not therefore a prerequisite for good mental health in old age. It is also useful to take on board the further criticism that social disengagement is cohort specific, i.e. refers to a cohort of the elderly in the USA at a particular point in time in the early 1960s where an ageist attitude was endemic. Since then we have seen improved health-care, early retirement, higher educational levels, etc., influencing the thoughts, feelings and behaviours of our elderly population which has made wholesale social disengagement by them questionable. This is given some impetus when one considers the fact that Ronald Reagan became President of the USA in his 70s; the astonishing success of the likes of companies like SAGA – who nowadays see their travel market as anyone 55+; the thousands of ‘elderly’ who attend learning in later life programmes at colleges and universities, etc. Not to mention of course the likes of the Grey Wolves: a collective of elderly political activists associated with the Scottish Socialist Party who are extremely active and central to the campaign in Scotland for a return to an index-linked inflation-proof state pension. The issue of individual differences in type and degree of disengagement is thus apparent. Not all psychologically disengage from ‘everything’ in whole or in part immediately upon retiral. Bromley (1988) posits a disposition towards a personality dimension to disengagement as a characteristic of getting old. This had been earlier identified by Havighurst et al. (1968) who found two types of personalities among Cummings and Henry’s original sample: reorganisers and the disengaged. Reorganisers are personalities who on retirement reorganise their lives around new and old activities to fill the gap left by withdrawal from the workforce. The disengaged personality voluntarily withdraws from social roles, commitments and activities. Reorganisers reported being very happy as did the voluntary disengaged who, despite their inactivity, said they were highly satisfied with life. Thus it is useful to note that: ‘Disengagement represents only one of many possible paths of ageing. It has no blanket application to all people.’ Kermiss (1984) Look up: ‘Successful Ageing: What does the “good life” look like?’ Concepts in Gerontology by Lucille B. Bearon, Ph.D. http://www.ces.ncsu.edu/depts/fcs/pub/aging.html DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 129 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Activity (or re-engagement) theory: a personal growth model of ageing The major alternative to social disengagement theory (as alluded to earlier) is activity theory as proposed by Havighurst et al. (1964) and Maddox (1964). It accepts the biological ageing process as fact but disputes that psychological and social needs change in old age as a consequence. Havighurst et al. say that the older person has the same psychological and social needs as those of middle-age. Further, social disengagement in the form of social interaction is one-way, in that it is society withdrawing from the individual, not the individual voluntarily withdrawing from society. There is no mutual relationship as Cummings and Henry believe. They suggest that optimal ageing is enhanced by keeping active and compensating for any shrinkage of life space that may come with industrial (Bromley) disengagement. Compensation for lost activities allows for the upkeep of the ageing person’s role count and related selfesteem, self-respect and self-worth. While an optimistic theory it does not account for findings which suggest that the disengaged are content with their lot. Personality may be the key here. Neugarten and Neugarten (1987) believe people will choose an ageing ‘style’ that reflects their personality and previous past experiences. There is thus no single activity style for psychologically healthy and successful ageing. ‘Activity’ appears to be what best suits you. Some will vigorously take up pursuits they have always wanted to pursue but never had the time. Some will concentrate on their immediate family, happy in their role as grandparent. Others will take up educational opportunities or increase their voluntary commitments. In contrast to disengagement theory, activity theory sees social interaction to be as natural in old age as it was in earlier years. An overall evaluation of social disengagement and activity theory Hayslip and Panek (1989) see both theories as ecologically valid. For them disengagement or re-engagement are options, which may either be voluntarily or enforced on us. Voluntarism is influenced by physical health and social circumstance. As a result an older person could have an option chosen for them that does not reflect their personality and is thus maladaptive. Both theories ignore the individual’s phenomenology and personal agency as regards the degree of control they truly have over what ageing model they choose. 130 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS It is apparent that disengagement is not an inevitable consequence of growing older, even though it is equally clear that some people disengage from society. A more likely explanation for disengagement seems to lie in the combination of lack of opportunities for involvement and the stereotyping of ageing, which can mean that many people do not try to get involved. Both activity and disengagement, it would seem, are appropriate strategies for some people but not for others. Although many people clearly do benefit from the increased opportunities and additional roles offered by Havighurst’s approach, there are other people who look forward to retirement as a time when they can deliberately take a rest, becoming less involved and disengaging themselves from society, at least to some extent. Dyson (1975) proposed that these differences might be explained by seeing retirement as a social exchange or contract between the ageing individual and society. According to Dowd (1975), who also looked at retirement as social exchange, the two sides to this social contract are: the old person implicitly agrees to participate less in society’s activities, thus freeing places in the work force for younger people, while society agrees to grant that person an ‘honourable discharge’ from their role as worker. In retirement the older person gains social approval for their years of leisure in retirement and the right to enjoy them as they feel inclined. For some old people, this social exchange results in disengagement, as they feel themselves to be no longer fully part of society, while for others social exchange means that they seek alternative activities in other fields. Atchley (1976) showed that there may be social class differences regarding which choice is made. In a comparison between working-class men and male teachers reaching retirement age, the working-class men tended to disengage while male teachers would reengage looking for new activities in which to become involved. Dyson suggested that the social exchange of retirement actually placed the old person in a dilemma where they are trapped by their situation. As members of society, old people acknowledge the fairness of the unspoken agreement. As individuals, however, often with many healthy and active years ahead, the contract is often seen as inappropriate for them personally. This makes it difficult for old people to challenge the way that society marginalises them, because any attempt to do so is seen as demanding special and unfair treatment. This has the effect of reducing the possibility for social cohesion among older people. Since they implicitly accept the social contract they conclude that if they do not fit the stereotypical view of the older person there must be something wrong with themselves; that they are not typical. So where victims of other social prejudices have been able to challenge these by DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 131 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS banding together and becoming more vocal, it is much harder for old people to do so. Student activity: Essay Describe, discuss and evaluate alternative theories of ageing. You should write about 1000 words in your essay. Social ageing Reflecting developmental psychology’s emphasis on physical, cognitive, social and emotional development and inter-relationships across the human life span, another alternative to disengagement and activity theory is Erik Erikson’s psychosocial theory of personality development, in particular the psychosocial stage of integrity v despair. Erik Erikson (1902–94), a neo-Freudian, proposed that it is our social world at particular age-stages of our life, and not our instinctual sexual urges, which affects personality development. Personality does not just depend on childhood alone but is shaped, changed and developed throughout the whole of our lives. He put forward a psychosocial theory of personality development which stresses the importance of our individual need for social approval and belonging, and that if this is frustrated, hindered or impeded we can end up with a jaundiced personality as a consequence of our negative experiences. Inherently a positive growth theory, he divides the human lifecycle into eight stages. At each stage we decide and make choices about our life based on what we experience in and from our environment. We make positive or negative psychosocial decisions which affect our personality. Erikson terms these eight stages psychosocial crises. How we deal with each psychosocial stage, or crisis, in life shapes our personality. It should be noted that Erikson did not see these outcomes in an ‘either–or’ fashion. Most personalities exhibit aspects of positive and negative outcomes connected with a particular stage or stages. The healthy personality would see more positive rather than negative outcomes (adaptive rather than maladaptive psychosocial outcomes) being apparent. Look up: Erik Erikson compiled by Wendy Sharkey http://elvers.stjoe.udayton.edu/history/history.asp?RURL=http:// elvers.stjoe.udayton.edu/history/people/Erikson.html 132 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Erik Erikson’s Theory of Psychosocial Crisis: an age-stage approach to personality development Psychosocial crises 1. Trust v Mistrust: Age 0–1 On the basis of a baby’s experiences in early life, an infant will begin to form an impression of its world. If it has enjoyed a stable, nourishing, loving and caring early life it should come to view the world in a secure and trusting way. If, on the other hand, its early life hasn’t been so stable, nourishing, loving, etc., it will see itself as insecure in an uncaring, untrusting and harsh world. 2. Autonomy v Shame or Doubt: Age 2–3 Showing a Freudian influence, Erikson says our ‘toilet training’ years lead us to begin to believe ourselves either to be in control and physically competent or doubtful about our developing independence, abilities and skills. How others react to our efforts to become more autonomous affects how we view ourself. Doubting our ability, especially on the basis of what those we love tell us, gives rise to a lack of confidence in what we do and in our dealings with others. 3. Initiative v Guilt: Age 3–5 A time of increasing motor dependence, age 3–5 is also a period of intense curiosity, questioning and enquiry for the young child. How caregivers develop or discourage this can, Erikson believes, affect the developing personality and related social skills. Having our curiosity encouraged, satisfied and extended develops initiative. Being made to feel a burden or a nuisance leads alternatively to feelings of insecurity and guilt about how to deal with, and solve, all the challenges life throws at us. 4. Industry v Inferiority: Age 6–12 In their primary school years children are exposed to an environment where cognitive, social and emotional growth is demanded and encouraged. Their experiences here – from teachers, parents, siblings (brothers and sisters) and increasingly, their peer group – will result in individuals sensing that they are competent, productive and valued or incompetent, underproductive and undervalued. Where we get our feelings of inferiority is most interesting here. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 133 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS 5. Identity v Role Confusion/Diffusion: Age 12–19 Puberty triggers our psychological journey for identity. We are pulled between beginning to value and recognise our own unique identity, and previous experience of knowing that to be seen as different from everyone else can pose problems. These years see teenagers begin to deliberately associate with others they see as similar to themselves. As if having to confirm who they think they are individually, a group of teenagers will very often dress the same way, like the same type of music and enjoy the same activities and social scene. Movement between different groups is normal as we try and find friendship with those who better share our own individual opinions, preferences, outlook on life, etc. Successful resolution of Identity v Role Confusion/Diffusion would see someone in their early 20s who is happy with their own unique self and their role in life. If they thought about it long enough, they would also realise that their unique personality and life also mean something to others. They are valued as such. If you have been unable to discover your true identity during the teenage years, on the other hand, you can come through this crisis with a confused or diffused (all over the place) identity. You would undervalue your own unique personality and probably have difficulty sustaining long-lasting friendships with others. 6. Intimacy v Isolation: 20s–30s This is the age-stage where we search for a partner with whom we think we want to spend the rest of our life. Biologically, socially and emotionally there is a great drive for intimacy with another during this time. Everyone else around us, it seems, is doing the same thing. Everyone else seems to have found love. How successful, or otherwise, we are during this psychosocial stage sees us either form an intimate, loving relationship with another or doubt our ability to do so and facing a lifetime of isolation and loneliness. The downside of this stage can be extremely destructive for individuals. 7. Generativity v Stagnation: 30s–50s Between these years childcare responsibilities for adults diminish. We begin to examine how productive and creative our work and relationships are. Very often we use this time to widen our education and also give something of ourselves back to the work place, trade union, club, community group, religious organisation, etc., to which we belong. We benefit others and simultaneously feel we have ‘made a contribution’ in our own unique way. If, however, you find that your life has become unproductive, routine 134 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS and stagnant, you feel psychosocially and psychologically unfulfilled. There is something missing from our lifelong quest to discover what it means to be human. We lose direction and purpose in life. 8. Integrity v Despair: 60s+ This sees us approach the inevitable end of our life. During this stage we reflect on what our past life has been like and what sort of difference we have made as individuals. We each weigh up our life ‘balance-sheet’. Was it good, bad or indifferent? If, on balance, we feel we have had a good and worthwhile life we go to our deaths feeling good and at peace showing ego-integrity; if it has been not so good we may feel despair, fear death and have a sense of never having been here in the first place. Points to note Erikson, on the basis of cross-cultural comparison and working as a practitioner in the USA, believes that we develop our personalities as a result of particular social experiences at eight stages in our lives. His theory is said to be universal in that it can be applied to all cultures. If we had a negative experience during a particular psychosocial stage this can affect the adult personality, e.g. if we conclude at the trust v mistrust stage that our world and the people in it should not be trusted, this can affect the establishment of relationships with others then and in the future. He also says that we can ‘revisit’ past stages in our life, and if we think we have made a negative decision which has affected our personality and gives us discomfort we can realise and rectify this. This is one reason why older people often try to mend bridges and make amends with others they have had a serious fall-out with at some earlier point in their life. Faced with integrity v despair they have a desire to die more at peace and less in despair with themselves. His psychosocial theory of personality has great worth at all points in the human lifecycle to try and understand why we are as we are and what we might become. It is an age-stage related theory of personality which allows us an insight into the outcome of our physical, cognitive and social experiences coming together to affect an important aspect of human emotion – personality. What is clear from the point of view of ageing is that previous outcomes, positive and negative, will affect who you are and how you perceive yourself at 60+ before this outcome at the end of our life can be addressed. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 135 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Look up: Erik Erikson 1902–94 Dr C George Boeree http://www.ship.edu/~cgboeree/erikson.html Student activity: Essay Describe, discuss, and evaluate Erik Erikson’s contribution to developmental psychology in general and the psychology of ageing in particular. You should write about 1000 words. Further reading: ageing Bromley, D B, Human Ageing (3rd ed.), Harmondsworth: Penguin Sugarman, L, Lifespan Development, London and New York: Routledge Durkin, K, Developmental Social Psychology: From Infancy to Old Age, Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, 1995 Turner, J S and Helms, D R, Lifespan Development (5th ed.), Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace College Publishers, 1995 136 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Glossary: Ageing activity (or re-engagement) theory: a personal growth model of ageing advocated by Havighurst et al. (1964) and Maddox (1964). It accepts the biological ageing process as fact but disputes that psychological and social need change in old age as a consequence. ageing: growing old. The process that occurs with the passage of time of progressive chronological ageing (the exact number of years a person has lived); biological ageing (the condition of the body); subjective ageing (how old a person feels in themselves); functional ageing (what kind of life they lead); and social ageing (how we mix/interact/ are accepted with and by others). cohort specific: a group of individuals investigated at a particular point in historical time. cross-sectional study: where different age groups are tested on the same things at the same time, crystallised intelligence (gc): the knowledge that you have already acquired, e.g. vocabulary, arithmetic and general knowledge. It is linked to education, experience and culture and is measured by tests of general information. decade-by-decade model: Burnside et al. (1979) identified in their segmentation of the ‘elderly’: the young-old (60–69); the middle-aged old (70–79); the old old (80–89); and the very old old (90–99). decrement model: a view of ageing which is negative in orientation. demographic imperative: recognition having to occur regarding the fact of far more people living longer nowadays than ever before. ego-integrity: the positive outcome of Erikson’s integrity v despair stage. A psychological sense of well-being and ability to go to one’s grave feeling good about one’s life. fluid intelligence (gf): your ability to solve novel and unusual problems creatively and learn and retain new information quickly based upon memory span and mental dexterity. life expectancy: the age that people are expected to live to given the right conditions. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 137 THEORIES AND CONCEPTS lifespan: the actual amount of time a person lives for. optimal ageing: the maximisation and potentialisation of all you can psychologically be despite (or because of) the ageing process. personal growth model: an alternative to the decrement model which sees ageing as an opportunity rather than a threat. psychological ageing: aspects of an individual’s thoughts, feelings and behaviours affected by the ageing process, most especially intelligence, memory and personality. reorganisers: those people who on retirement reorganise their lives around new and old activities to fill the gap left by withdrawal from the workforce. role count: the number of different responsibilities we acquire that become part of our personality. role structure: the roles you adopt in life that are part of your identity which invariably change as a consequence of ageing, i.e. worker, nonworker, etc. self-fulfilling prophecy: based on others’ low expectations you become the low expectation in your thoughts, feelings and behaviours. senescence: primary biological ageing where obvious signs of ageing become apparent, i.e. you get smaller, slower, weaker, lesser, fewer (Bee and Mitchell, 1980). social ageing: related to how the ageing process influences personality and addressed by Erikson’s psychosocial crisis of integrity v despair for 60s+. social disengagement: where as a result of ageing you withdraw from previously enjoyed social activities (see Cummings and Henry, 1961) and is evident in shrinkage of life space, increased individuality and acceptance, often wholeheartedly, of age-related changes. WAIS (Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale): an IQ test for adults measuring gf and gc. 138 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES SECTION 3 Outcome 3 of this unit requires students to ‘analyse an issue in development psychology’. See page 3 of this publication for further details. Two issues are considered in this section: 1. 2. The use of non-human animals in research Heredity and environment. Teachers/lecturers and students should note that some detail on a third issue, ‘attachment and separation’, is also included in this pack within the key concept ‘Early socialisation’ in Section 2. Issue 1: The use of non-human animals in research Two major concerns are raised by the use of animals in psychological research. 1. First, ethical questions relating to the pain or suffering which may be inflicted on laboratory animals. 2. Second, ecological validity. There is a wide-ranging debate about what animal studies can, in theory, reveal about human behaviour. The early behaviourists believed that the basic principles of learning operated in the same way in many species. Behaviourists’ ‘laws’ of learning could be equally well demonstrated in rats as in man. This belief is no longer accepted and generalisations from animal to human behaviour have come to be regarded with a good deal of suspicion. Please read: Richard Gross, Rob McIlveen, Hugh Coolican, Alan Clamp and Julia Russell, Psychology – A New Introduction (2nd ed.), Chapter 63, pages 773–785. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 139 THE ISSUES Ethics and non-human animal research in psychology Jones, Gross and McIlveen (1999) presciently introduce the topic of ethical considerations and psychological research by stating: ‘Just as Orne (1962) regards the psychological experiment as a social situation, so every psychological investigation is an ethical situation.’ To this end the British Psychological Society (BPS) lays down codes of conduct which members in their research of human and non-human participants should adhere to. In the UK this would see practitioners using inter alia: • The Code of Conduct for Psychologists (BPS, 1983) • The Ethical Principles for Conducting Research with Human Participants (BPS, 1990, 1993) and • The Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research (BPS and Committee of the Experimental Psychological Society, 1985). Conducting research with human participants The following notes are based on the British Psychological Society’s, Ethical Guidelines and Code of Conduct, 1985. 1. General considerations: especially as much developmental psychology investigates children and young people, always ensure that the research you do is done from the standpoint of the participants taking part. Research should never be offensive to anyone. This means that you should do nothing which threatens a person’s health, well-being or dignity. You should also be aware that we live in a multi-cultural country of diverse ethnic communities. Research should be considered from a socially inclusive, non-sexist, anti-racist and non-ageist perspective. 2. Consent: wherever possible consent should always be obtained from participants. 3. Deception: deception is not allowed if participants would be unlikely to co-operate in its absence. If in doubt the researcher should seek advice from a teacher, lecturer, etc. 140 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES 4. Debriefing: any research should provide participants with an opportunity to discuss the outcomes of it. This is called debriefing, and allows discussion of the specific purpose of the research, interpretation of the participants’ particular performance, scores, answers, etc., and gives them an opportunity to ask questions. 5. Withdrawal from the investigation: all participants should give their permission to take part in your research. They should also be allowed to withdraw at any time if they so wish. 6. Confidentiality: unless subject to Scots law and UK statute, e.g. the Data Protection Act, confidentiality between participant and researcher should be observed at all times. If in doubt seek advice from your teacher, lecturer, etc. 7. Protection of participants: all participants in a piece of research should be protected from any physical or mental harm. 8. Observational research: any observation should observe the privacy and psychological well-being of those studied. If consent to be observed is not possible, observations should only occur where it would be normal that those observed would/could be by others. If in doubt consult your teacher or lecturer. 9. Giving psychological advice: sometimes during research the researcher will be asked their advice concerning a psychological matter which is of concern to a participant. The golden rule is not to give advice if not qualified to do so. If in any doubt you should seek advice from your teacher or lecturer. 10. Colleagues: all of us who study psychology share the above set of ethical principles. It is our duty to encourage others who do psychological research to observe these ethical guidelines at all times. Look up: BPS Code of Conduct http://www.bps.org.uk/about/rules5.cfm and http://www.bps/org.uk.charter/codofcon.htm DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 141 THE ISSUES Why use animals in psychological research? Cross-comparative species–species research has its origins in Darwin’s Origin of Species research (1859) in that the study of simpler life forms from our evolutionary history gives us a greater insight into why we think, feel and behave as we do. In 1992 Brehm wrote, ‘In our necessary concern with treating subjects well and protecting them from any harmful effects, we must not overlook the other side of the ethical issue: the ethical imperative to gain more understanding of important areas of human behaviour. Intimate relationships can be a source of the grandest, most glorious pleasure we human beings experience; they can also be a source of terrible suffering and appalling destructiveness. It is, I believe, an inherently ethical response to try to learn how the joy might be increased and the misery reduced.’ What she means is that ethical and practical considerations are related, in that while psychologists have a duty to protect the welfare of individual participants (and implicitly non-human participants), there is the over-riding duty to do socially relevant research as being the stuff of psychology. Put more bluntly, animal research allows for experimentation into aspects of thinking, feeling and behaving that would be ethically difficult, if not impossible, to conduct on human beings. These ethically sensitive areas include: severe sensory deprivation (Reisen, 1947; Blakemore and Cooper, 1970); complete social isolation (Harlow and Zimmerman, 1959); extreme stress (Brady, 1958; Seligman, 1974); surgical procedures (Olds and Milner, 1954). In a clinical sense such procedures and investigations with animals also allow for greater control of variables in such experimentation, e.g. the Skinner Box whose environment is completely under the control of the experimenter. Further, the use of non-human animals in research allows the claim of evolutionary continuity to be made. What this means is that differences between humans and non-humans are quantitative rather than qualitative. We share the same basic evolutionary adaptations. Mammals such as rats, cats, dogs, monkeys, apes and humans have all developed similar brain structure. It is size, number of neurons and the interconnections between these nerve cells which make the inherent difference. It is said that ‘at the level of its basic units, evolution has been highly conservative’ and as a result parallels can be made via experimentation with non-human animals concerning cognitive, conatative and affective behaviours (Green, 1994). 142 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES From a practical point of view, i.e. size, manipulation, shorter life-span, shorter gestation period, etc., the use of animals in a laboratory experiment is easier than using humans. Study of this kind also gives rise to the generation of hypotheses and later testing of humans. Examples of this from within developmental psychology would include Bowlby’s theory of attachment (see later) which was greatly influenced by the earlier work of the ethologist Lorenz into imprinting. From a scientific point of view, due to easier manipulations, animal experimentation gives rise to data about cause–effect relationships between variables, as opposed to correlations between covariants when using humans. The obvious example here, however distasteful from an animal perspective, would be in the study of variables influencing cancers. With humans we can only make correlations between smoking and lung cancer. With animals, smoking can be induced (the independent variable) and cancer observed and measured (the dependent variable). Finally, Coolican (1994) affirms that species– species comparisons across the phylogenic (evolutionary) scale can indicate what we as humans have lost or gained in our evolutionary history when compared to animals, i.e. the discovery of a redundant structure in the human nervous system when found active in animals today may suggest its previous purpose and function for human beings. Why use animals at all? The rationale behind animal experimentation is twofold: • The pursuit of scientific knowledge • The advancement of science. As a result we have made inroads into cause, diagnosis, prognosis and treatments in the form of vaccines for infectious diseases, the development of antibacterial and antibiotic drugs, heart surgery, organ transplants, kidney failure, diabetes, malignant hypertension gastric ulcers, etc. The price is any distress and suffering caused to any animal used in experimentation. Codes of practice make for such distress and suffering to be minimised as much as possible. The decision to use, or not to use, animals is taken using a cost-benefit analysis of animal pain, distress and suffering as measured against the developments of new scientific knowledge to alleviate human misery and suffering. The medical and scientific justification argument concerning animal experimentation is strong in that Gray (1991) argues that ‘not only is it not wrong to give preference to the interests of one’s own species, one has a duty to do so’ (Shackleton-Jones, Gross and McIlveen, 1999). This DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 143 THE ISSUES speciesism is countered by opponents who say that medical advances have been delayed due to misleading results from animal experimentation and that in the early stages of such investigation little in the way of scientific advancement for human beings is forthcoming in that scientific understanding of the issue is still developing. It can be many years before any cost-benefit can be seen, tempered by the cost to the animal in terms of pain and suffering, as against the scientific and medical benefit for humans as a result. Whether or not to use animals in experimentation can be decided upon using Bateson’s Decision Cube (1986, 1992). The three dimensions upon which the researcher’s decision is made are: • The quality of the research • The certainty of medical benefit • The degree of animal suffering. Bateson believes that his third dimension should only be excused if the quality of research and medical benefit is deemed high. Experimentation on animals should not be undertaken for its own sake. Look up: Guidelines for Ethical Conduct in the Care and Use of Animals Developed by the American Psychological Association’s Committee on Animal Research and Ethics (CARE) http://www.apa.org/science/anguide.html Ethology and imprinting A fairly obvious example of the use of animals in developmental psychology is in the area of ethology and imprinting which very much influenced the development of Dr John Bowlby’s theory of attachment in the 1950s and 60s. The observation and study of species-to-species behaviours and human– animal comparison is called ethology. Ethology emphasises that any characteristic which increases the chances of successful reproduction will be more likely to stay in the genetic make-up of the species. This allows ethologists to identify behaviours they think are adaptive which go towards better ensuring the survival of the species. Ethology first gave us an insight into the impact the mother–infant bond has on the successful development of a healthy adult animal. The 144 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES process of the establishment of the mother–infant bond found in the animal world is called imprinting. Imprinting is an adaptive behaviour. Imprinting was first brought to our attention by Konrad Lorenz (Nobel Laureate for Medicine in 1973) in 1952 in his book King Solomon’s Ring. In it he recorded observations he had made of geese, mallard ducks, cats, hedgehogs, horses, fish, etc. He also reported again on his classic 1935 observations of greylag geese. A teenage ethologist, Lorenz noticed that species such as ducks appeared to instinctively follow (as a consequence of genetics) the first moving object they saw (usually their mother) from birth. He deduced this when he realised that ducklings had imprinted on him, and followed him around thereafter. He said ducklings have to develop this learnt imprinted bond within 24 hours in order to develop the necessary duck survival, social and sexual behaviours essential to adult life. If this bond is not formed within 4–24 hours – which Lorenz calls a critical period – ducks are socially and sexually damaged. Imprinting is crucial to individual and general species survival. Lorenz (1935) divided a clutch of greylag goose eggs into two groups. One group stayed with the biological mother goose while the other was hatched in an incubator. On hatching the first moving stimulus the incubated group saw was Lorenz. They began following him around. He marked all the goslings and placed them beside the mother goose. They very quickly separated into two distinct groups: those naturally born in the presence of the mother followed her; the incubated group followed Lorenz. The power of this discovery from the point of view of an innate survival skill such as imprinting cannot be underemphasised. What happens if imprinting does not occur during the critical period? Sluckin (1965) reran Lorenz’s original experiment concerning imprinting in ducklings. He hatched ducklings in isolation from a mother-figure, and each other (bar one). He found that they could still imprint on a parent-figure (or any large moving object), 4–5 days after hatching. Lorenz’s critical period to imprint of less than 24 hours was not as fixed and narrow as previously thought. Sluckin argued that the reason Lorenz’s ducklings, who were hatched apart from their mother, could not imprint on their mother or any large moving object after 24 hours was that they had imprinted on each other during this time. Because Sluckin’s ducklings had been kept isolated from each other DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 145 THE ISSUES their period of imprintablity had increased fivefold. Sluckin therefore renamed the critical period as the sensitive period, although it should be said that it is thought the best time for imprinting to occur successfully, at least for ducklings, is the critical period of the first 24 hours. Are the effects of imprinting permanent? As implied above, Lorenz believed imprinting to have a permanent effect on a duck’s later social and sexual success. Imprinting appears to be of huge significance from the point of view of personal, social and sexual survival in prosocial species. It is only through this learnt imprinted mother–infant bond that correct social and sexual behaviours are learnt. Guiton (1966), in a most interesting experiment, disputed the permanency of the imprinting bond. He reared some male leghorn chicks in isolation for 47 days. During this time they were tended by an assistant who wore yellow rubber gloves. The only physical contact they had with anything/anyone else were these yellow rubber gloves coming through the aperture of their pen to feed them. On their release from isolation after 47 days, the chicks all attempted to mate with the yellow rubber gloves! After this observation, Guiton kept the experimental group of male chicks in an environment with normally reared female hens of the same age. Although well past imprinting age the change in company had its impact. The male chicks successfully mated with the female hens and no longer yearned for the yellow rubber gloves! Guiton argues that any subsequent social or sexual dysfunction (disturbed behaviour) that might have arisen as a consequence of lack of imprinting would only have come about if his male chicks had never been exposed to others of the same species. For Guiton, therefore, the effects of imprinting (or the lack of it) are neither permanent nor irreversible. Harlow’s monkeys Interested in the emotional development of rhesus monkeys and species–species comparison with humans, Harlow (1959) took eight newborn rhesus monkeys and reared them in isolation in their own individual cages. In each cage were two identical ‘models’. One model had a large round head with eyes, ears and mouth with its wire-mesh body covered in terry-towelling; the other had a rectangular head with non-monkey-like features supported on a bare wire-mesh body. 146 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES Harlow wanted to discover if the monkeys would develop a mother– infant attachment to any of the models, and whether the introduction of food had any part to play in the resulting attachment. To this end he alternately had a feeding teat protruding from the body of one or other of his terry-towelling or wire-mesh models in each of his eight cages. Four were thus fed via the terry-towelling models while the other four were fed via the wire-mesh ones. He found that who fed his monkeys had no effect on whom they attached to. While the monkeys fed from the teat coming from whatever model was in their cage, all clung onto the terry-towelling one most. To test whether they had formed an attachment to one or other of his models he first removed the wire-mesh model from the cage. This produced no reaction. When Harlow removed the terry-towelling model from the cages however, all eight monkeys showed observable signs of distress. In a variation of this, when a wind-up teddy was put into each cage and let go, the monkeys all ran and clung to the terry-towelling model burying their heads in its chest, in much the same way as a small child might when confronted with an unusual and strange situation. Interestingly, they all displayed similar ‘peeking’, or ‘keeking’, behaviour to that of human children when, after a while, they would ‘keek’ up from the terry-towelling ‘mother’s’ chest and look in the direction of the teddy – becoming a wee bit more confident each time they did so. In a field test situation (in this instance in the open air) the young monkeys explored quite happily when the terry-towelling model was present but became distressed if it was removed. Harlow’s work confirms that attachment is not related to food (as thought at the time due to the dominant influence of behaviourism) but more so to do with our need for tactile, or sensual, comfort. It is the physical, and related mental, experience of someone else’s love for us. After this longitudinal study – which used elements of laboratory, field, natural experiment and natural observation – Harlow (1962) wrote another report on his monkeys. He reported that they had grown up physically well, but it was evident that all eight had developed social and emotional difficulties no matter whether their ‘mother’ had been wiremesh or terry-towel! In comparison to other normally reared rhesus monkeys they were more timid and more frightened of new experiences, had immense difficulty in social situations and found reproduction impossible. They exhibited aspects of the autistic spectrum as found with autistic people, i.e. detachment from environment and repetitive, stereotyped movement. Harlow concluded that there is a critical period of around 6 months where a rhesus monkey has to form DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 147 THE ISSUES an attachment with other monkeys otherwise social and emotional development becomes impaired, or damaged. To try to rectify the situation, and to measure the permanency of maternal privation (not being able to experience monotropy due to not having a mother around in the first place), Harlow put his monkeys into rehabilitation with a community of more naturally reared monkeys on ‘Monkey Island’. Visiting 6 months later, Harlow observed that this ‘group therapy’ had helped social and emotional development slightly, though sexual reproduction still eluded them. Harlow concluded that for rhesus monkeys, the outcome of maternal and social privation jeopardises social, emotional and sexual development and that these effects are permanent if a rhesus monkey is deprived of maternal or other rhesus monkey contact for the first six months of its life. Making a species–species comparison, Harlow went on to say that human babies had a critical period of 2 years within which to form necessary attachments with other humans, otherwise their social and emotional development may be similarly affected. A few interesting experimental variations have been forthcoming in this area. Rosenblum and Harlow (1963) exposed monkeys to two types of cloth mother. One blew compressed air from time to time. They found that infant monkeys showed the strongest attachment to this ‘punitive’ mother – a finding relevant to the relationship found between abused children and their parents. This has been confirmed by Hogg et al. (1994) who looked at the effects of a 24-week separation on three young gorillas who had previously been living with their mothers. During separation the initial reactions were threat responses and pacing about menacingly. This was thought to mirror human anaclitic depression. When reunited with their mothers the infant gorillas did not immediately exhibit attachment behaviours with them and spent more time in contact with each other. What can ethology tell us? 1. That bonding is a consequence of body contact rather than feeding. 2. That this body contact has to be interactive between those involved. Passive bonding to an inanimate object, for example, can result in abnormal social and emotional development. 148 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES 3. That absence of an interactive attachment figure (usually the mother) can be compensated by other alternative attachment figures (peer group). Look up: ‘The Nature of Love’ Harry F. Harlow (1958)[1] University of Wisconsin http://www.yorku.ca/dept/psych/classics/Harlow/love.htm DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 149 THE ISSUES From: Boston Globe Online http://www.boston.com/globe/search/stories/nobel/1989/1989w.html KONRAD LORENZ, 85; NOBEL LAUREATE PIONEERED STUDY OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR in their lives, become strongly attached to their biological mothers, a process known as imprinting. Author: Associated Press Date: Wednesday, March 1, 1989 Page: 37 Section: OBITUARY He showed that the process could be altered, however, by demonstrating that mallard ducklings would happily follow a human who greeted them shortly after birth and imitated a mother’s quacking. VIENNA – Konrad Lorenz, the Austrian scientist who won a Nobel Prize in 1973 for his pioneering studies of human and animal behaviour, died of kidney failure Monday in his home in Altenburg, 30 miles northeast of Vienna. He was 85. Dr Lorenz, Austria’s most famous scientist, held doctorates in medicine, zoology and psychology. His studies on the organization of individual and group behaviour patterns won him the Nobel Prize in medicine together with Karl von Frisch and Nikolaas Tinbergen. Dr Lorenz turned to research in animal behaviour shortly after obtaining his medical degree. He had become an animal lover as a child, collecting a variety of animals at his expansive boyhood home outside Vienna. The collection included fish, dogs, monkeys, insects and especially ducks and geese. His first important findings concerned the social life of birds. Those studies convinced him that many aspects of the birds’ behaviour were innate and instinctive, rather than learned. His views were controversial, and they became even more controversial when he suggested that such instinctive behaviour might be important in humans, too. One of his best known findings was that young animals will, at some time In 1939, Dr Lorenz was given a chair in psychology at Immanuel Kant University in Koenigsberg, then a German town and later the Soviet port of Kaliningrad. His tenure there and publications during that time led in later years to allegations that Lorenz was a Nazi sympathizer. When accepting the Nobel Prize, he apologized for a 1940 publication judged to reflect Nazi views of science, saying that ‘many highly decent scientists hoped, like I did, for a short time for good from National Socialism, and many quickly turned away from it with the same horror as I.’ After World War II, Dr Lorenz returned to Vienna, where he lectured at the university and published dozens of books. One of his most controversial publications was the 1966 study ‘On Aggression’, in which he asserted that aggressive impulses are to some degree innate, drawing on analogies between human and animal behavior. Later best-selling books included The Eight Deadly Sins of Civilized Humanity, a plea against overpopulation and environmental destruction, and The Decay of the Humane, a gloomy look at mankind’s future that sold 390,000 copies. © 1997 Globe Newspaper Company 150 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES Look up: Lecture Support Material: Ethological Experiments University of Plymouth Department of Psychology SALMON (Study and Learning Materials Online) http://salmon.psy.plym.ac.uk/year1/ETHEXPT.HTM#FAP http://altweb.jhsph.edu/ You may need to download Apple Quicktime Plug-in to see videos on this page. Student activity: Essay Analyse the use of non-human animals in research in developmental psychology. Your answer should be around 1000 words in length. • First you must tell the reader what it is you intend to do and how you are going to tackle it. • You then discuss in a non-emotive way the issue, why it developed and its importance to developmental psychology. • Identify and give arguments for the ‘issue’ with relevant theorists/research. • Identify and give arguments against the ‘issue’ with relevant theorists/research. • Identify and give arguments for relevant ‘solutions’ to the difficulties/problems posed by the issue. • Identify and give example(s) from at least one appropriate concept of the issue. • Identify and give relevant research indicating the importance of the issue in relation to appropriate concept(s). • Finally, state relevant conclusions related to the importance of the issue for developmental psychology. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 151 THE ISSUES Issue 2: Heredity and environment Nature or nurture, or the heredity v environment issue or debate, has raged within philosophy and psychology since their beginnings. In summary what the issue concerns is to what extent behaviour is the result of your genetic code (nature – the genes you inherit) or life experiences (nurture – your parents, your upbringing, your experiences generally). Heredity v environment is both a theoretical and practical controversy, most importantly in the areas of thinking and problem solving, language and atypical behaviour – but also perception, aggression and gender. Undoubtedly nature and nurture interact, and the best solution is to see nature as ‘potential’ (genotype) which is modified by nurture (phenotype). This has been called a reaction range. Nature or nurture? • Heredity/Nature: the view that behaviour is determined by inherited factors. • Environment/Nurture: the view that behaviour is determined by acquired or environmental factors. The nature–nurture, or ‘heredity–environment’, debate is a controversy which has raged in philosophy and psychology for centuries, and nowhere more vehemently than in the area of intelligence and thus thinking and problem solving. The question then is: to what extent is a person’s intelligence the inescapable product of his/her genetic make-up or the outcome of his/her life experiences? The nature–nurture debate has important practical consequences. For example, if intelligence is entirely inherited then why should we not test children as early as possible and place them in suitable schools and occupations in order to potentialise their intellectual abilities on the basis of identified fixed intellect? This was the position adopted in psychology by the late Sir Cyril Burt. On the other hand if intelligence is influenced by environmental factors, then it is critical that children are given enriching experiences wherever possible to enhance their abilities. Other examples of the nature–nurture debate in psychology include: • Perception. To what extent is what we ‘see’ – for example the perception of depth or visual illusions – an innate aspect of our 152 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES physical visual system? Or are some aspects of perception learned and therefore different in different cultures? • Language. To what extent are humans hardwired to acquire language, or is their ability to use language based on exposure and imitation? If the latter is true, then it should be possible to train non-human animals who don’t have the innate brain mechanisms to use language. • Aggression. To what extent is violent behaviour an aspect of a person’s nature, or is it learned? For example we might propose that men are more aggressive then women because they are innately more aggressive or it may be that, in our society, men are taught/learn to respond more aggressively than women (nurture). The question has important practical applications in the reduction of aggression. If aggression is determined by nature then drug therapies may offer the best means of prevention. • Gender development. Sex is determined by each individual’s chromosomes; but gender identity and gender role behaviour are moulded by society. • Causes of mental illness. Recent research has found evidence of a genetic basis for many atypical disorders; however the diathesis-stress model suggests that a person’s genetic makeup will predispose them to certain disorders but it is environmental factors (stressors) that actually trigger the expression of the disorder. The more susceptible an individual is, the fewer stressors are necessary. This means that neither nature nor nurture individually cause most mental dysfunction. Aytipical behaviour comes about as a result of the interaction of nature with nurture. Look up: http://objana.com/frog/natnurt.html Further reading Richard Gross, Rob McIlveen, Hugh Coolican, Alan Clamp and Julia Russell, Psychology A New Introduction (2nd ed.), Chapter 66, pages 812–824. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 153 THE ISSUES Which is it that ‘makes’ us what we are – heredity, environment or interaction? There are a number of arguments that demonstrate that the answer must be ‘an interaction’: • Hebb (1949) said that the question ‘nature or nurture’ is a meaningless one. It’s like asking whether the length or breadth of a rectangle determines the area. The answer is simply – both. • If you plant a seed in good soil and provide plenty of sunshine, warmth and food, it thrives. If you plant the identical seed in poor soil with little nourishment it will grow less well. If you plant genetically different seeds in the same soil there will be differences; in this case they are due to nature whereas in the first example the differences are due to nurture. If we compare genetically different groups of people (different racial groups) we must be certain they are sharing the same environment before we attribute any differences to nature, otherwise the differences must be at least in part due to nurture. • There is an inherited metabolic disorder called phenylketonuria (PKU), where certain proteins are not processed properly leaving a poisonous substance in the blood which causes brain damage. If the condition is detected early (and all infants are tested) then the particular proteins can be eliminated from the child’s diet and there is no brain damage. The question is whether intellectual impairment, should it occur in this instance, is due to nature or nurture? If the child’s environment doesn’t contain the proteins, no damage will occur. Therefore here is an interaction between nature and nurture. This interactionist relationship is the same as the diathesis-stress model described earlier. • The concept of heredity presumes that we can isolate an individual who has had no interaction with the environment. People often talk of abilities being present at birth but at this time the human infant has already had 9 months’ worth of environmental experience. Even before conception the state of the infant is not all ‘nature’, as illustrated by the ‘transgenerational effect’. If a woman has a poor diet during pregnancy her foetus suffers. Perhaps more importantly if the foetus is female the foetus’ eggs for her own children, which are already formed, will be adversely affected. Therefore the next generation will be underdeveloped because of its grandmother’s poor environment. What may appear to be inherited by a future generation of offspring is in fact environmentally determined. 154 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES • There is no true experimental evidence. Nature–nurture studies compare individuals with the same or different genetic makeup. Study has focused in on identical twins (monozygotic) who are genetically the same because they come from a single egg – one zygote – and non-identical twins (dizygotic) who come from two zygotes. They are genetically as similar as any siblings (other brothers and sisters) except they share a more similar environment than other siblings do right from conception. In recent years it has become clear that even though identical twins are genetically the same there are differences right from the start. • The best solution to the heredity–environment controversy may lie in Gottesman’s (1963) concept of a reaction range similar to the concept of susceptibility and predisposition in the diasthesis-stress model discussed above. Our genetic make-up limits our range of potential development in terms of all our chacteristics, e.g. height, intelligence and atypical behaviours. Actual development is related to environmental opportunities, or lack of them. This is the concept of potential. Look up: http://genetics.nature.com Where do the different perspectives stand in the nature–nurture debate? • The biological perspective by definition takes a nature position though, as in the case of phenylketonuria, the environment clearly influences behaviour. • The behaviourist perspective is entirely on the side of nurture, though the potential for learning is innate. • The cognitive perspective similarly makes no special claims for nature except insofar as the structure of the mental system is innate; its development however is a response to experience. • The psychoanalytic perspective combines both nature and nurture in the view that innate, sexual forces are modified by experience to produce adult personality. • The humanistic perspective emphasises nurture but holds certain views about the nature of humankind – that it is positive, inclined towards psychological good health, and has the potential for selfactualisation. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 155 THE ISSUES Look up: http://www.apa.org/releases/mother.html and http://www.learner.org/exhibits/personality/genes.html Yale-New Haven Teachers Institute ‘Heredity And Environment’ Narrative by Carolyn N. Kinder http://www.yale.edu/ynhti/curriculum/units/1990/6/90.06.04.x.html#a and Heredity http://school.discovery.com/homeworkhelp/worldbook/atozscience/h/ 253940.html plus ‘Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota study of twins reared apart’ by Thomas J. Bouchard Jr. et al. Science magazine, Oct 12, 1990 http://www.duke.org/library/intelligence/bouchard.html 156 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) THE ISSUES Student activity: Essay Analyse the issue of heredity and environment in relation to developmental psychology. Your answer should be about 1000 words in length. • First you must tell the reader what it is you intend to do and how you are going to tackle it. • Define heredity and environment. Identify and give example(s) from at least one appropriate concept of the issue, namely, learning theory, sociability, cognitive development and language, etc. • Then discuss the issue in a non-emotive way, why it developed and its importance to developmental psychology. • Identify and give arguments for the heredity side of the ‘issue’ with relevant theorists/research. • Identify and give arguments against the heredity ‘issue’ with relevant theorists/research (the environment side of the issue). • Identify and give arguments for relevant ‘solutions’ to the difficulties/problems posed by the issue, i.e. interactionism. • Elaborate the issue from at least one appropriate concept, namely heredity and environment as regards learning theory, sociability, cognitive development, language, etc. • Identify and give relevant research indicating the importance of heredity, environment and interactionism in relation to appropriate concept(s). • Finally, state relevant conclusions related to the importance of ‘heredity v environment’ for developmental psychology. DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 157 158 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GENERAL REFERENCES APPENDIX General references Ainsworth, M D S and Wittig, B A, ‘Attachment and exploratory behaviour of 1-year-olds in a strange situation’. In B M Foss (ed.) Determinants of Infant Behaviour, Volume 4, London: Methuen, 1969 Ainsworth, M D S; Blehar, M C; Walters, E and Wall, S, Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc, 1978 Baltes, P B and Brim, O G Jr. (eds), Life-span development and behaviour (Vol. 6), New York: Academic Press, 1984 Baltes, P B and Willis, S L, ‘Plasticity and enhancement of intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State’s Adult Development and Enrichment Project (ADEPT)’. In F I M Craik and S Trehub (eds), Aging and cognitive processes, New York: Plenum, 1982 Baltes, P B, ‘Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline’, Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626, 1987 Bandura, A; Ross, D and Ross, S A, ‘Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models’, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 575–582, 1961 Bateson, P, ‘How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for?’, Animal Behaviour 27(2), 470–486, 1979 Bateson, P, ‘When to experiment on animals’, New Scientist, 109 (1496), 30–32, 1986 Bee, H L, The growing child (7th edn), New York: Harper & Row, 1995 Blakemore, C and Cooper, G F, ‘Development of the brain depends on the visual environment’, Nature, 228, 477–8, 1970 Bloom, L, Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1970 Botwinick, J, Aging and behaviour (2nd edn), New York: Springer, 1978 Bouchard, T J; Lykken, D T; McGue, M M; Segal, N and Tellegen, A, ‘The sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart’, Science, 250, 223–228, 1990 Bower, T G R and Wishart, J G, ‘The effects of motor skill on object permanence’, Cognition 1: 165–172, 1972 Bower, T G R, ‘The visual world of infants’, Scientific American, 215: 80– 92, 1966 Bowlby, J, Maternal Care and Mental Health, Geneva: World Health Organisation, 1951 Bowlby, J, Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1, ‘Attachment’, New York: Basic Books, 1969 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 159 GENERAL REFERENCES Bowlby, J, Attachment and Loss, Vol. 2, ‘Separation, Anxiety and Anger’, New York: Basic Books, 1973 Bowlby, J, Attachment and Loss, Vol. 3, ‘Loss, Sadness and Depression’, New York: Basic Books, 1980 Breuer, J and Freud, S, ‘Studies on hysteria’. In J Strachey (ed. and trans.), Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud (Volume 2) London: Hogarth, 1895 British Psychological Society and The Committee of the Experimental Psychological Society, Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research, Leicester: BPS, 1985 British Psychological Society, ‘Ethical principles for conducting research with human participants’ (revised), The Psychologist, 6 (1), 33–35, 1993 Bromley D B, Human Ageing, 3rd ed., Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1988 Brown, R (ed.), New Directions in Psychology, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston Bruner, J, The Process of Education, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1960 Bruner, J, Toward a Theory of Instruction, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1966 Bruner, J, Going Beyond the Information Given, New York: Norton, 1973 Bruner, J, Child’s Talk: Learning to Use Language, New York: Norton, 1983 Bruner, J, Actual Minds, Possible Worlds, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1986 Bruner, J, Acts of Meaning, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1990 Bruner, J, ‘The ontogenesis of speech acts’, Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19, 1975 Bruner, J S; Goodnow, J J and Austin G A, A Study of Thinking, New York: Wiley, 1956 Buss, A H and Plomin, R, Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1984 Buss, A H and Plomin, R A, Temperament Theory of Personality Development, New York: Wiley, 1975 Butterworth, G and Harris, M, Principles of Developmental Psychology, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: UK, 1994 Cardwell, M, The Complete A–Z Psychology Handbook, Hodder and Stoughton, 1996 Case R, Intellectual Development, Birth to Adulthood, Academic Press Inc, London, 1985 Cattell, R B, ‘Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment’, Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22, 1963 Cavanaugh, J C and Blanchard-Fields, F, Adult development and aging (4th edn), Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 2001 160 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GENERAL REFERENCES Chomsky, N, Syntactic structures, The Hague: Mouton, 1957 Chomsky, N, ‘Review of Skinner’s verbal behaviour’, Language, 35, 26– 58, 1959 Chomsky, N, Language and Mind, New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1968 Christensen, L, ‘Deception in psychological research: When is its use justified?’, Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 665–675, 1988 Cohen, D, J B Watson: The Founder of Behaviourism, Boston, MA: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1979 Coolican, H, Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology (3rd edn), London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1999 Coopersmith, S, The Antecedents of Self-Esteem, San Francisco, CA: Freeman Cumming, E, ‘Engagement with an old theory’, International Journal of Ageing & Human Development, 6, 187–191, 1975 Cumming, E and Henry, W E, Growing old: The process of disengagement, New York: Basic Books, Inc, 1961 Curtiss, S, Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern Day ‘Wild Child’, London: Academic Press, 1997 Darwin, C, The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, London: John Murray, 1859 Darwin, C, The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex, London: John Murray, 1871 Darwin, C, The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals, London: John Murray, 1871 Davis, A M, ‘Non-canonical orientation without occlusion: children’s drawings of transparent objects’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Vol 37, pp 451–462, 1984 Davison, G and Neale, J, Abnormal Psychology (6th edn), New York: Wiley Dennis, W, ‘Causes of retardation among institutional children: Iran’, J Genet. Psychol., 96, 47–59, 1960 Denny, N W and A M Palmer, ‘Adult age differences on traditional and practical problem-solving measures’, Journal of Gerontology, 36: 323– 328, 1981 Diamond, A, ‘Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on A-not-B’, Child Development, 56(4), 868–883, 1985 Diehl, M; Coyle, N and Labouvie-Vief, G, ‘Age and sex differences in strategies of coping and defence across the life span’, Psychology and Aging, 11, 127–139, 1996 Dollard J; Doob L W; Miller N E; Mowrer O H and Sears R R, Frustration and Aggression, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1939 Donaldson, M, Children’s Minds, Glasgow: Collins, 1978 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 161 GENERAL REFERENCES Dowd, J J, ‘Ageing as exchange: a preface to theory’, Journal of Gerontology 30, 584–594, 1975 Durkin, K, Developmental social psychology: From infancy to old age, Oxford: Blackwell, 1995 Dyson, J, ‘Socio-political influences on retirement research’, Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 33, 128–130, 1980 Elkind, D, ‘Erik Erikson’s eight ages of man’, New York Times Magazine, 5 April, 1980 Ellis, A, Humanistic Psychotherapy, New York: Julian Press, 1973 Ellis, W D, A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology, New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1938 Erikson, E H, Childhood and Society (2nd edn), New York: Norton, 1963 Erikson, E H, Identity: Youth and Crisis, New York: Norton, 1968 Erikson, E H, Identity and the Life Cycle, New York: Norton, 1980 Eysenck, H J, Dimensions of Personality. London: RKP, 1947 Eysenck, H J, Decline and Fall of the Freudian Empire, Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1985 Fisher S and Greenberg R, The Scientific Credibility of Freud’s Theories and Therapy, Harvester, 1977 Flanagan, C, Early Socialisation: Sociability and Attachment, Routledge Modular Psychology Series, London: Routledge, 1999 Freud, A, The Ego and Mechanisms of Defence, London: Chatto and Windus, 1936 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. IV, The Interpretation of Dreams (I), 1900 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. V, The Interpretation of Dreams (II) and On Dreams, 1900–1901 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. VI, The Psychopathology of Everyday Life, 1901 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. VII, A Case of Hysteria, Three Essays on Sexuality and Other Works, 1901–1905 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. X, The Cases of ‘Little Hans’ and the Rat Man, 1909 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. XV, Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis (Parts I and II), 1915–1916 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. XVI, Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis (Part III), 1916–1917 162 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GENERAL REFERENCES Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. XVIII, Beyond the Pleasure Principle, Group Psychology and Other Works, 1920–1922 Freud, Sigmund, Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, (trans.) Strachey, J, Vol. XIX, The Ego and the Id and Other Works, 1923–1925 Freud, S, ‘The passing of the Oedipus complex’. In E Jones (ed.), Collected Papers of Sigmund Freud, Volume 5, New York: Basic Books, 1924 Freud, S, New Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis, New York: Norton, 1933 Freud, S, An Outline of Psychoanalysis, London: Hogarth Press, 1949 Freud, A and Dann, S, An Experiment in Group Upbringing. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, New York: Summit, 1951 Galton, F, Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry onto its Laws and Consequences, Macmillan: London, 1869 Glassman, W E, Approaches to Psychology (2nd edn), Buckingham: Open University, 1995 Goffman, E, The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1971 Goldfarb, W, ‘The effects of early institutional care on adolescent personality’, Journal of Experimental Education 12: 106–129, 1943 Goldfarb, W, ‘Infant rearing and Problem Behaviour’, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 13: 249–65, 1943 Goldman, A, Epistemology and Cognition, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1986 Groome, D et al, An Introduction to Cognitive Psychology, Processes and Disorders, London: Psychology Press, 1999 Gross, R, Themes, Issues, and Debates in Psychology, London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1995 Gross, R, Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behaviour (3rd edn), London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1996 Gross, R, Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behaviour (4th edn), London: Hodder and Stoughton, 2001 Gross, R, Key Studies in Psychology, London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1999 Gross, R; McIlveen, R; Coolican, H; Clamp, A and Russell, J, Psychology: A New Introduction. London: Hodder and Stoughton, 2000 Guiton, P, ‘Socialization and imprinting in brown leghorn chicks’, Journal of Animal Behaviour, 7, 26, 1959 Harlow, H F and Zimmermann, R, ‘Affectional responses in the infant monkey’, Foundations of Animal Behaviour, 843, 376–387, 1996 Harlow, H et al, ‘Social rehabilitation of separation-induced depressive disorders in monkeys’, American Journal of Psychiatry, v. 133(11), 1279–1285, 1976 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 163 GENERAL REFERENCES Harlow, H et al, (1976), ‘Effects of maternal and peer separations on young monkeys’, Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, v. 17(2), 101–112, 1976 Havighurst, R J, ‘Successful aging’, The Gerontologist, 1 (1), 8–13, 1961 Havighurst, R J, ‘Flexibility and the social roles of the retired’, American Journal of Sociology, 59, 309–311, 1964 Hayes, N, Foundations of Psychology (2nd edn), Walton-on-Thames: Nelson, 1998 Hayflick L, How And Why We Age, New York: Ballantine Books Hayslip, B and Panek, P E, Adult development and aging (2nd edn), London: Harper Collins, 1993 Heather, N, Radical Perspectives in Psychology, London: Methuen, 1976 Hebb, D O, The Organization of Behaviour: A neuropsychological theory, New York: Wiley, 1949 Heim, A, Intelligence and Personality – Their Assessment and Relationship, Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1970 Hess, E H, ‘Imprinting in a natural laboratory’, Scientific American, 227 (2), 24–31, 1972 Higher Still Development Unit, Higher Still Support Materials, HSDU: Edinburgh, Scotland, 2000 Hilgard, E R; Atkinson, R L and Atkinson, R C, Introduction to Psychology (7th edn), New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1979 Hodges, J and Tizard, B, ‘Social and family relationships of exinstitutional adolescents’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30: 77–97, 1989 Hodges, J and Tizard, B, ‘IQ and behavioural adjustment of exinstitutional adolescents’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30.1: 53–75, 1989 Horn, J L, ‘Thinking about human abilities’. In Nesselroade, J R; Cattell, R B, The Handbook of Multivariate Experimental Psychology, Volume 2, 645–685, NY: Plenum Press, 1988 Hull, C L, Principles of Behaviour, New York: Appleton, Century Crofts Izard, C, Human Emotions, New York: Plenum Press, 1977 Jacobs, M, Freud, London: Sage Publications, 1992 James, W, ‘What is an emotion?’, Mind, 9, 188–205, 1884 James, W, The Principles of Psychology, New York: Henry Holt & Company, 1890 Jung, C G, (1963) Memories, Dreams, Reflections, London: Collins/RKP, 1963 Jung, C G (ed.), Man and His Symbols, London: Aldus-Jupiter Books, 1964 Kagan, J, The Nature of the Child, New York: Basic Books, 1984 Kail, R; Hale, C A; Leonard, L B and Nippold, M A, ‘Lexical storage and retrieval in language impaired children’, Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 37–49, 1984 164 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GENERAL REFERENCES Kalish, R A, Death, Grief and Caring Relationships, 2nd edn, Brooks/ Cole Publishing Company, 1985 Kamin, L J, The Science and Politics of IQ, Potomac, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1974 Kastenbaum, R, New thoughts on old age, New York: Springer, 1975 Keegan, G, ‘Delirium on Helium’, Psychology Review, Vol 9, Phillip Allen Updates, 2002 Koluchová, J, ‘Severe deprivation in twins: A case study’, Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 13, 107–114, 1972 Koluchová, J, ‘Severely deprived twins after 22 years observation’, Studia Psychologica, 33, 23–28, 1991 Lamb, M; Thompson, R; Gardner, W; Charnov, E and Estes, D, ‘Security of infantile attachment as assessed in the “strange situation”: Its study and biological interpretation’, Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 7, 127–171, 1984 LeFrançois, G R, The Lifespan (6th edn), Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 1999 Lennenberg, E H, Biological Foundations of Language, New York: Wiley, 1967 Liebert R M and Sprafkin J, The Early Window: Effects of Television on Children and Youth, New York: Pergamon Press, 1988 Levinson, D J, The seasons of a man’s life, New York: Knopf, 1978 Levy, B and Langer, E, ‘Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory among the American Deaf and in China’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66: 935–943, 1994 Levy, B, ‘Improving memory in old age by implicit self-stereotyping’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71:1092–1107, 1996 Macoby, E E; Newcomb, T M and Hartley, E L (eds), Readings in Social Psychology, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston Maddi, S R, Personality theories: A comparative analysis (6th edn), Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole, 1996 Main, M and Solomon, J, ‘Discovery of a new, insecure-disorganized/ disoriented attachment pattern’. In T B Brazelton and M Yogman (eds), Affective development in infancy, pp. 95–124, Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1986 Main, M and Weston, D, ‘The quality of the toddler’s relationship to mother and to father: related to conflict behaviour and the readiness to establish new relationships’, Child Development, 52, 932–940, 1981 Malinowski, B, The Sexual Life of Savages, New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1929 Markle, S, Good Frames and Bad (2nd edn), New York: Wiley, 1969 Martin, P and Bateson, P, Measuring Behaviour (2nd edn), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996 Maronne, M, Attachment and Interaction, London: Jessica Kingsley, 1998 Maslow, A, Towards a Psychology of Being, 2nd edn, New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1968 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 165 GENERAL REFERENCES Maslow, A, Motivation and Personality, 2nd edn, New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1970 McDougall, W, An Introduction to Social Psychology, London: Methuen, 1908 McGarrigle, Grieve and Hughes, ‘Interpreting Inclusion’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 528–550, 1978 McGarrigle, J and Donaldson, M, ‘Conservation accidents’, Cognition, 3, 341–350, 1974 Meadows, S, The Child as Thinker, London: Routledge, 1995 Meins, E, Security of attachment and the social development of cognition, 1997 Meltzoff, A N and Moore, M K, ‘Why faces are special to infants’. In Developmental Neurocognition, Goysson-Bardies, et al, (eds), Dordrecht: Kluwer, 1993 Meltzoff, A N, ‘Imitation of televised models by infants’, Child Development, 59, 1221–29, 1988 Meltzoff, A N and Moore, M K, ‘Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates’, Science 198, 75–78, 1977 Meltzoff, A N and Moore, M K, ‘Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures’, Child Development 54, 702–9, 1983 Miller, G A, ‘The acquisition of word meaning’, Child Development, 49, 999–1004, 1978 Mowrer, O H, Learning Theory and Behaviour, New York: John Wiley, 1960 Neisser, U, Cognition and Reality, San Francisco, CA: W H Freeman, 1976 Nesselroade, J R; Baltes, P B and Schaie, K W, ‘Ontogenetic and generational components of structural and quantitative change in adult cognitive behaviour’, Journal of Gerontology, 27, 222–228, 1972 Oakhill, J and Beard, R, Reading development and teaching of reading: a psychological perspective, Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, 1999 Ornstein, R, The Psychology of Consciousness, 2nd edn, Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1986 Pavlov, I P, Conditioned Reflexes, Oxford: OUP, 1927 Penfield, W and Roberts, L, Speech and Brain Mechanisms, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1959 Pennington, D C; Gillen, K; Hill, P, Social Psychology, London: Arnold, 1999 Piaget, J, The Psychology of Intelligence, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1950 Piaget, J, The Child’s Conception of Number, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1952 Piaget, J, The Origins of Intelligence in Children, New York: Norton, 1963 Piaget, J, The Child’s Conception of the World, London: Paladin, 1973 Piaget, J and Inhelder, B, The Child’s Conception of Space, London: RKP, 1956 166 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GENERAL REFERENCES Piaget, J and Inhelder, B, The Psychology of the Child, London: RKP, 1969 Piaget, J, ‘The psychogenesis of knowledge and its epistemological significance’. In M Piatelli-Palmarini (ed.), Language and Learning (pp. 23–34). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1980 Pine, K and Messer, D, ‘What children do and what children know: looking beyond success using Karmiloff-Smith’s RR framework’, New Ideas in Psychology, 17, 17–30, 1999 Pinel, J P J, Biopsychology. 2nd edn, Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, 1993 Pinker, S, How The Mind Works, New York: Norton, 1997 Plutchik, R, The Psychology and Biology of Emotion, London: Harper Collins, 1994 Pringle, M L K and Bossio, V, ‘A study of deprived children. Part I. Intellectual, emotional and social development. Part II. Language development and reading attainments’, Vita Humana, 1: 142–170, 1958 Reber, A S and Reber, E S, Dictionary of Psychology, 3rd edn, London: Penguin Books, Ltd, 2001 Rogers, C R, On Becoming a Person, Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin, 1961 Rosenhan, D L, ‘On being sane in insane places’, Science, 179, 250–258, 1973 Rubin, Z and McNeil E B, The Psychology of Being Human (3rd edn), London: Harper and Row, 1983 Rutter et al, Cycles of Disadvantage, London: Heinemann, 1976 Rutter, M, Maternal Deprivation Reassessed, 2nd ed, Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1981 Rybash, J M; Rodin P A and Hoyer W J, Adult development and aging, Wm C Brown and Co, 1995 Ryckman, R M, Theories of Personality (7th edn), Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co, 2000 Samuel, J and Bryant, P, ‘Asking only one question in the conservation experiment’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 315– 318, 1984 Schacter, S and Singer, J E, ‘Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional states’, Psychological Review, 69, 379–399, 1962 Schaffer, H R, ‘Some Issues for Research in the Study of Attachment Behaviour’, in Foss, B (ed.), Determinants of Infant Behaviour, 2, London: Methuen, 1963 Schaffer, H R and Emerson P E, ‘The development of social attachments in infancy’, Monographs of Social Research in Child Development, 29: no. 94, 1964 Schaffer, H R, ‘Joint involvement episodes as context for development’, in H Daniels (ed.), An Introduction to Vygotsky, London: Routledge, 1996 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 167 GENERAL REFERENCES Schaie, K W, ‘The course of adult intellectual development’, American Psychologist, 49, 304–313, 1994 Scottish Qualifications Authority, Arrangements for Psychology, 5th edn, Glasgow, Scotland, 2002 Segall, M H; Dasen, P R; Berry, J W and Poortinga, Y H, Human behaviour in global perspective (2nd edn), Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, 1999 Seligman, M E P and Hager, J (eds), Biological Boundaries of Learning, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972 Selltiz, C et al, Research Methods in Social Relations, 3rd edn, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1976 Skeels, H M et al, ‘A study of environmental stimulation: An orphanage preschool project’, Iowa Studies in Child Welfare 15, 4, 1938 Skeels, H M and Dye H B, ‘A study of the effects of differential stimulation on mentally retarded children’, Proceedings and Addresses. American Association for Mental Defectiveness 44: 114–, 1939 Skinner, B F, ‘Are theories of learning necessary?’, Psychological Review, 57(4), 193–216, 1950 Skinner, B F, Science and Human Behaviour, New York: Macmillan, 1953 Skinner, B F, ‘The science of learning and the art of teaching’, Harvard Educational Review, 24(2), 86–97, 1954 Skinner, B F, Verbal Learning, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957 Skinner, B F, Beyond Freedom and Dignity, New York: Knopf, 1971 Skodak, M and Skeels, H M, ‘A final follow-up study of one hundred adopted children’, J Genet. Psychol., 75: 85–125, 1949 Sluckin, W, Imprinting and Early Learning, Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company, 1965 Spitz, R A, ‘Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood’, Psychoanalytic Studies of the Child 1: 53–74; 2: 113–117, 1945 Spitz, R A and Wolf, K, ‘Anaclitic depression’, Psychoanalytic Studies of the Child, 3–4: 85–120, 1946 Tizard, B and Hodges, J, ‘The effect of Early Institutional Rearing on the development of eight-year-old children’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19: 99–118, 1978 Tolman E C and Honzik C H, ‘Introduction and removal of reward and maze-learning in rats’, University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 257–275, 1930 Turner, J S and Helms D B, Lifespan Development (5th edn), Harcourt Brace College, 1995 Van Ijzendoorn, M; Schuengel, C and Bakermans-Kranenberg, ‘Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae’, Development and Psychopathology, 1998 168 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) GENERAL REFERENCES Vaughn, B; Egeland, B; Sroufe, L A and Waters, E, ‘Individual differences in infant–mother attachment at twelve and eighteen months: Stability and change in families under stress’, Child Development, 50, 971–975, 1979 Watson, J B, ‘Psychology as the behaviourist views it’, Psychological Review, 20, 158–177, 1913 Watson, J B, Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviourist (1st edn), Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, 1919 Watson, J B and Rayner, R, ‘Conditioned Emotional Reactions’, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3(1), 1–14, 1920 Wood, D; Bruner, J and Ross, G, ‘The role of tutoring in problem solving’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100, 1976 Wundt, W, Outlines of Psychology, Wilhelm Engelmann, 1897 Yarrow, L J, ‘Maternal deprivation: Toward an empirical and conceptual re-evaluation’, Psychological Bulletin, 58.6: 459–490, 1961 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY (AH) 169