J Ornithol DOI 10.1007/s10336-016-1330-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Patterns of nest attendance by female Greater Prairie-Chickens (Tympanuchus cupido) in northcentral Kansas Virginia L. Winder1 • Mark R. Herse2 • Lyla M. Hunt2 • Andrew J. Gregory3 Lance B. McNew4 • Brett K. Sandercock2 • Received: 3 June 2015 / Revised: 21 January 2016 / Accepted: 2 February 2016 Ó Dt. Ornithologen-Gesellschaft e.V. 2016 Abstract Nest attendance behavior is a critical component of avian ecology that influences nest survival and population productivity. Birds that provide uniparental care during incubation and brood-rearing must balance the benefit of reproductive success with the costs of physiological needs and predation risk. We used miniature nest cameras to record 5904 h of video footage at 33 nests of Greater Prairie-Chickens (Tympanuchus cupido) during 2010 and 2011 in northcentral Kansas. We quantified the timing and duration of incubation bouts to address alternative hypotheses about physiological requirements and predation risk as drivers of incubation behavior. We also identified nest predators and determined timing of predation events, and tested for effects of nest attendance and monitoring technique on nest survival (video vs. telemetry). Female prairie chickens exhibited high incubation constancy per day (*95 %) and typically took two *40min recesses per day: one after sunrise and one before sunset. Mesocarnivores were responsible for 75 % (18 of 24) of nest losses, and most nest predation events occurred during crepuscular or overnight hours. Controlled Communicated by C. G. Guglielmo. & Virginia L. Winder vwinder@benedictine.edu 1 Department of Biology, Benedictine College, Atchison, KS 66002, USA 2 Division of Biology, Kansas State University, Manhattan, KS 66506, USA 3 School of Environment and Society, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH 43403, USA 4 Department of Animal and Range Sciences, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT 59717, USA comparisons provided no evidence that video surveillance attracted predators to nests. Variation in nest attendance had a minimal effect on nest survival compared to height of vegetative cover at the nest site. Timing of recesses did not indicate avoidance of predator activity in our study system. The bimodal pattern of incubation breaks observed in most grouse species is likely driven by physiological requirements of the female rather than predation pressure. Female Greater Prairie-Chickens appear to prioritize their metabolic needs and future reproductive potential over current nest survival. Keywords Grouse Incubation behavior Nest survival Physiology Predation Zusammenfassung Muster der Nestbewachung bei Präriehuhn-Weibchen Tympanuchus cupido in Nordzentral-Kansas Nestbewachungsverhalten ist ein entscheidender Bestandteil der Ökologie von Vögeln, welcher Einfluss auf Nesterfolg und die Produktivität einer Population hat. Vogelarten, bei denen sich nur ein Elternteil um Bebrütung und Jungenaufzucht kümmert, müssen den Gewinn durch reproduktiven Erfolg gegen die Kosten physiologischer Ansprüche und des Prädationsrisikos abwägen. Mithilfe von Miniatur-Nestkameras filmten wir in den Jahren 2010 und 2011 5.904 Stunden Videomaterial an 33 Nestern von Präriehühnern (Tympanuchus cupido) in NordzentralKansas. Wir bestimmten Zeitpunkt und Dauer der Bebrütungsphasen, um alternative Hypothesen zu physiologischen Bedürfnissen und Prädationsrisiko als den treibenden Faktoren für das Brutverhalten anzusprechen. Außerdem ermittelten wir die Nesträuber sowie den 123 J Ornithol Zeitpunkt der Prädationsereignisse und überprüften den Einfluss von Nestbewachung und Überwachungstechnik (Video beziehungsweise Telemetrie) auf die Erfolgsraten der Nester. Präriehuhn-Weibchen wiesen eine hohe Brutkonstanz am Tag auf (etwa 95 %), und machten typischerweise zwei etwa 40-minütige Brutpausen: eine nach Sonnenaufgang und eine vor Sonnenuntergang. Mesocarnivoren waren für 75 % (18 von 24) der Nestverluste verantwortlich, und die meisten Prädationsereignisse fanden zur Dämmerung oder während der Nachstunden statt. Kontrollvergleiche ergaben keine Hinweise darauf, dass durch die Videoüberwachung Prädatoren zu den Nestern gelockt wurden. Die Variation bei der Nestbewachung hatte, verglichen mit der Vegetationshöhe am Neststandort, nur einen minimalen Effekt auf den Nesterfolg. Der Zeitpunkt der Brutpausen deutete in unserem Untersuchungssystem nicht auf Vermeidung von Prädatorenaktivität. Das bimodale Muster der Brutpausen, wie man es bei den meisten Raufußhühnern beobachten kann, wird vermutlich eher von physiologischen Bedürfnissen des Weibchens bestimmt als durch den Prädationsdruck. PräriehuhnWeibchen scheinen somit ihrem Stoffwechselbedarf und zukünftigem Fortpflanzungspotenzial mehr Bedeutung einzuräumen als dem gegenwärtigen Nesterfolg. Introduction Evolutionary pressures of minimizing predation risk while meeting physiological needs of parents and offspring have shaped reproductive decisions for many animal species (Wiebe and Martin 2000; Gowaty and Hubbell 2009). Nest attendance behavior determines individual reproductive success and population productivity, but is a difficult component of avian reproduction to study (Conway and Martin 2000; Deeming 2002; Benson et al. 2010; EllisFelege and Carroll 2012; Thompson and Ribic 2012). Incubating parents typically take at least one foraging recess each day, but recess timing and duration are flexible behaviors that can be adjusted based on the physiological requirements of the incubating parent, eggs, or young (Cartar and Montgomerie 1985, 1987; Erikstad 1986; Wiebe and Martin 1997; Caudill et al. 2014; Hovick et al. 2014), as well as predation risk to parents or their offspring (Ghalambor and Martin 2002; Tulp and Schekkerman 2006; Coates and Delehanty 2008). Moreover, priorities of incubating parents can be shaped by interactions among weather, energetics, and predation risk (Cartar and Montgomerie 1987; Wiebe and Martin 1997; Tulp and Schekkerman 2006; Smith et al. 2012a, b; Caudill et al. 2014). Benefits of current reproductive output must be balanced against risks to parental survival and potential 123 loss of future reproductive success. It can be difficult to disentangle the intrinsic and extrinsic factors that influence nest attendance behavior, but in some long-lived bird species, adult survival and residual reproductive value are prioritized over current reproductive success (Tulp and Schekkerman 2006). Nest attendance is often the most energetically expensive phase of the breeding season for species with uniparental incubation (Walsburg 1983). Females of largebodied species of grouse make a sizeable parental investment in a large clutch of energy-rich eggs and provide sole parental care during incubation and rearing of precocial young. Thus, females must balance physiological requirements and predation risk when making decisions about incubation behavior (Coates and Delehanty 2008; EllisFelege and Carroll 2012). For many species of grouse, nest survival is a key component of population viability, and the greatest impacts to population growth come from loss of eggs to predators during incubation (Wiebe and Martin 1997; Wisdom and Mills 1997; Sandercock et al. 2005; Coates and Delehanty 2008; McNew et al. 2011a, 2012). High nest predation rates can be a strong selective pressure shaping incubation behavior, and many species of birds have evolved flexible strategies for predator avoidance (Conway and Martin 2000; Ghalambor and Martin 2000; Martin 2002). The Greater Prairie-Chicken (Tympanuchus cupido, hereafter ‘prairie chicken’) is an indicator species for tallgrass prairie ecosystems in North America and is listed as Vulnerable by the International Union for Conservation of Nature and is state-listed in parts of its extant range (Poiani et al. 2001; Johnson et al. 2011; BirdLife International 2014). Female prairie chickens mate with males at leks, and then select nest sites with favorable microclimates and intermediate vegetative cover (Nooker and Sandercock 2008; Matthews et al. 2013; Hovick et al. 2014; McNew et al. 2014, 2015). Prairie chickens are a ground-nesting species, and habitats selected by females reflect a balance between concealment of the nest, maintaining lines of sight to detect predators, and shelter with a favorable microclimate. Females lay 10–15 eggs at a rate of 1 egg per day before incubating the clutch for *25 days (Hamerstrom and Hamerstrom 1973; Johnson et al. 2011; McNew et al. 2011b). Thus, prairie chickens have high reproductive potential but remain vulnerable to predators during the *38-day nesting cycle. Past studies of nest attendance in grouse have relied on direct observations or data loggers, and our study is the first to use video camera technology to investigate nest attendance behaviors of prairie chickens. Our study addressed four objectives. First, we quantified the timing and duration of incubation bouts and recesses away from the nest. If incubation patterns were related to predation risk, we J Ornithol predicted females would leave nests once per day to forage around midday and avoid crepuscular periods with the highest activity of diurnal nest predators (Cartar and Montgomerie 1987; Coates and Delehanty 2008; Burnam et al. 2012). Nest attendance behavior could also be related to the physiological needs of either the incubating parent or offspring. If nest attendance behaviors were driven by the digestive physiology and metabolic requirements of the incubating parent, we predicted females would take recesses at dawn and dusk to feed and eliminate fecal waste (Keppie and Herzog 1978; Wiebe and Martin 1997). Alternatively, if nest attendance behaviors were driven by the thermoregulatory needs of developing offspring, we predicted females would take recesses at midday to minimize egg cooling and avoid the potential costs of reheating eggs (Cartar and Montgomerie 1985, 1987; Reed et al. 1995; MacCluskie and Sedinger 1999; Schmidt et al. 2005; Tulp and Schekkerman 2006). In hot environments, females might take recesses around dawn during the coolest period of the day to minimize egg exposure to high temperatures (Wiebe and Martin 1997; Hovick et al. 2014). Second, we identified nest predators and monitored timing of predation events. Based on egg remains and signs of predator activity at nest sites, we predicted that nest predators would be mesocarnivores with greater activity during crepuscular periods (Ashby 1972; Wiebe and Martin 1997; Pietz et al. 2012). Third, we tested whether nest survival was influenced by nest attendance behaviors of incubating females (Wiebe and Martin 1997; Smith et al. 2012a). We hypothesized that nests by females that took fewer recesses and maintained higher incubation constancy would have higher daily survival rates (Ghalambor and Martin 2002; Smith et al. 2012a). Last, deploying nest cameras raises potential concerns about negative effects of video-monitoring equipment on nest survival (Richardson et al. 2009; Pietz et al. 2012; Powell et al. 2012). We compared survival rates of nests monitored with video equipment versus nests monitored remotely by telemetry. If the presence of video equipment attracted predators or increased the rate of nest abandonment, we predicted video-monitored nests would experience lower nest survival rates than unmanipulated nests. Methods Study area During the breeding seasons of 2010 and 2011, we monitored the incubation behavior of female Greater PrairieChickens at a *1300 km2 field site south of Concordia in the Smoky Hills region of northcentral Kansas (Fig. 1). The landscape in our study area was dominated by native grasses managed for cattle production (58 %) and row crop agriculture (35 %). Road density was relatively high throughout the study area at 1.4 km of road per km2. Weather conditions during our 2-year study were similar between years (Winder et al. 2014). Field methods We captured prairie chickens at leks using drop-nets and walk-in traps during a 3-month period from March to May of 2010 and 2011. We identified second-year (SY) birds by pointed tips and spotting on the outer two primaries vs. after-second-year (ASY) birds with rounded tips and less spotting on the outer two primaries (Henderson et al. 1967). Females were equipped with a necklace-style VHF radio-transmitter (10–11 g; Model A3950, ATS, Islanti, MN), a uniquely numbered metal leg band, and three colored leg bands. Radio-marked females were located 4–7 times a week by triangulation with VHF receivers (Model R2000, ATS, Islanti, MN) and handheld yagi antennas. When females were triangulated in the same location for [2 consecutive days, we used portable receivers to approach on foot and locate nests. We recorded UTM coordinates at the nest using a handheld GPS unit (±5 m). If nests were discovered during egg laying, we assumed an egg-laying rate of one egg per day and recorded the exact date for initiation of incubation. For nests that were located after incubation had been initiated, we estimated nest initiation dates by floating eggs in lukewarm water and by comparing egg buoyancy to a standardized flotation curve (±1 to 2 days; McNew et al. 2009). We monitored a subset of nests with color, low-resolution security cameras with infrared capabilities (950 nm, Model ENC-100, EZ Spy Cam, Los Angeles, CA), powered by two 12 V 26AH car batteries. We mounted a camera at the edge of the nest bowl using a camouflaged metal stake. We housed all recording equipment inside a camouflaged plastic container *25–30 m away from the nest, and buried the power cord in a shallow trench covered with vegetation. The camera installation process usually took less than 1 h (‘‘Appendix 1’’). We visited videomonitored nests (hereafter, ‘video nests’) every 3 days to replace batteries and memory cards. Females were not flushed during visits to check recording equipment, but were observed via a live portable viewfinder. If a female was absent and the nest status was uncertain, we watched recent footage on the viewfinder to check for signs of predation, nesting activity, or hatching. If nest fate was not captured on camera, we used eggshell fragments, predator sign, and other evidence at the nest bowl to assign nest fate. We considered nests to be successful if C1 chick hatched and left the nest, or failed if none of the eggs produced young. Once the nest attempt was complete, we recorded 123 J Ornithol Fig. 1 Map of the study area for monitoring patterns of nest attendance of female Greater Prairie-Chickens in northcentral Kansas, 2010 and 2011. Females were trapped at leks (black triangles) and marked with VHF radios. We analyzed nest survival for nests monitored with video cameras (white circles) compared to a subset of nearby nests monitored via radio telemetry (gray squares). Light gray shading is native grasslands managed as rangelands for cattle grazing; dark gray shading is row crop agriculture, and each square is a section of land (1 mi2) visual obstruction readings (VOR) at the nest site as an index of vegetative biomass and nest concealment (Robel et al. 1970). Video annotation We reviewed nest footage with a VLC media player (ver. 2.0.0, VideoLan, Paris, France). For all nest events, we determined the nest age as days since incubation was initiated, ordinal date, and time of day in relation to sunrise and sunset (US Naval Observatory 2016; data for Concordia, Kansas). Our 24-h periods began at midnight and ended at 23:59 on the same calendar day, and we considered the periods ±90 min of sunrise or sunset as crepuscular. We calculated incubation constancy as the percentage of a 24-h period a female spent incubating eggs per day, and total recess duration as the total time away from a nest in a 24-h period. We defined incubation as a female present at an active nest for C1 min, and a recess as a female being absent from an active nest for C1 min. We identified nest predators to species from video footage, and defined total loss as a predator consuming an entire clutch or killing the incubating female. We defined partial loss as a predator consuming a subset of eggs from a nest that remained active following the predation event. We defined unsuccessful predation as a snake or other predator unsuccessfully attempting to consume eggs. One observer 123 (LMH) annotated video footage for ten nests, and a second observer (MRH) annotated footage for the remaining 23 nests. Both observers independently annotated video footage for eight nests (24 %). We used the overlapping subset to check for consistency between observers and found complete agreement in video annotation between observers. Data analysis Nest attendance We limited our analyses of nest attendance behavior (incubation constancy, total daily recess duration, and number of recesses per day) to nests that had C1 day of continuous video footage that was uninterrupted by camera malfunction, loss of power, or disturbance from changing batteries and memory cards. Most nests were monitored for [1 day, and we further limited our analyses to days with continuous, uninterrupted footage. We monitored two nests within the same year for one female and two nests in separate years for a different female but accepted limited pseudoreplication to use our complete dataset. As a first step, we compared age-class specific generalized additive models (GAMs) in the package mgcv of Program R to compare incubation behavior between two female age-classes (results not shown). We found no evidence for differences J Ornithol between SY and ASY females and pooled age-classes for subsequent analyses. We used GAMs to test for effects of timing of recess initiation on recess duration (ver. 2.15.1, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; Wood 2011). We also used GAMs to test for effects of ordinal date, nest age, and VOR on incubation constancy. We explored models of nest attendance with fixed effects and with an individual nest as a random effect, and found no difference in the significance outcome when random effects were added. Our main interest was in understanding the overall patterns in prairie chicken behavior. Therefore, we present results from fixed effects models, which allowed us to generate the predictive relationship for nest attendance behavior. Nest survival Nest monitoring in our 2-year study spanned a 78-day nesting period from 18 April to 4 July. We used the package RMark in Program R as an interface to the nest survival procedure of Program MARK to model effects of female nest attendance and monitoring method on daily nest survival (White and Burnham 1999; Dinsmore et al. 2002; Laake and Rexstad 2008). All models were constructed using the logit-link function, and model selection was based on differences in AICc (DAICc) and evidence ratios from Akaike weights (wi; Burnham and Anderson 2002). We discarded models with DAICc B2 that differed from the top model by a single parameter if predictors were uninformative parameters with confidence intervals for slope coefficients (b) that included zero (Burnham and Anderson 2002; Arnold 2010). We used two separate model sets to explore daily survival rates of prairie chicken nests. The goal of our first set of nest survival models was to assess the potential effect of nest attendance behavior on nest survival. We limited our analysis to nests for which we had continuous, uninterrupted video footage for C1 day (25 nests of 23 females). We included VOR as a factor because vegetative cover at the nest has been found to explain variation in nest survival (McNew et al. 2014, 2015). Rainfall and storm events can result in direct loss of nests due to flooding or hail, or indirect losses of nests if scent-based predators detect females with damp plumage (Flanders-Wanner et al. 2004; Fields et al. 2006; Webb et al. 2012; McNew et al. 2014). We modeled the effect of precipitation on nest survival using data from a nearby (*35 km) NOAA weather station at the Concordia Blosser Municipal Airport (USW00013984, http://www.ncdc.noaa.gov). During our 2-year study, many nest losses occurred between midnight and sunrise. For overnight predation events, precipitation during the previous calendar day could play a role in the probability of a predator detecting a nest. Thus, we combined daily precipitation from the current and previous day to create a time-varying covariate for each day of our nest monitoring period. We modeled main effects and interactions of mean incubation constancy, mean number of recesses per day, VOR, and precipitation on daily survival rates of prairie chicken nests. In a second set of models, we compared video vs. unmanipulated nests to test for potential negative effects of video-monitoring on nest survival. Video nests had a disturbance from camera installation plus visits every *3 days when females occasionally flushed. For unmanipulated nests, we flushed the female from the nest once in early incubation and subsequently monitored nests via telemetry at distances of C100 m. We used Hawth’s Tools for ArcMap (ver. 9.3) to match each video nest with the nearest unmanipulated nest as a control (Fig. 1; spatialecology.com/htools; ESRI, Redlands, California; Beyer 2004). Renesting females remained close to their previous nest attempt, and we excluded replacement clutches by the same individual in our matched pairs design. Our matched set of unmanipulated nests included nest attempts by 33 unique females, and none of these females were included in our dataset of video nests. We modeled the main effects of the nest monitoring method (video vs. telemetry) and interactions with VOR and precipitation on daily nest survival of prairie chickens. We compared overall nest survival between video vs. telemetry nests by extrapolating the product of daily survival probabilities for each monitoring group to a 38-day exposure period, and by calculating variances of the extrapolated estimates with the delta method (Powell 2007). Results Nest attendance We included 5904 h of video footage (2010: n = 1848; 2011: n = 4056) in our analyses of nest attendance of Greater Prairie-Chickens. Our sample included 464 incubation recesses (2010: n = 151; 2011 n = 313) during 246 complete monitoring days at 25 prairie chicken nests (2010: n = 7; 2011: n = 18) attended by 23 females. Mean incubation constancy per day was 94.9 % ± 0.3 SE (range 61.0–99.8 %), and typically exceeded 90 % regardless of vegetative cover at the nest (F1,244 = 4.28, P \ 0.0001, adjusted r2 = 0.11; Fig. 2a). Females took an average of 1.87 ± 0.04 recesses per day (range 1–4), and each recess duration averaged 39 ± 2 SE min (range 1–479 min). Female prairie chickens exhibited a bimodal distribution of recess timing with most recesses taking place within the 123 J Ornithol Fig. 2 Generalized additive models for the effects of visual obstruction reading (VOR, a), ordinal date (b), and nest age (c) on incubation constancy (percent of 24-h period spent incubating eggs) of female Greater Prairie-Chickens in northcentral Kansas, 2010 and 2011 (n = 25 nests attended by 23 individual females). Circles observed incubation constancy; solid line predicted line of best fit from a generalized additive main effects model for VOR, ordinal date, or nest age; dotted lines upper and lower 95 % confidence intervals Fig. 3 Timing (a) and duration (b) of incubation recesses for female Greater Prairie-Chickens nesting in northcentral Kansas, 2010 and 2011 (n = 464 recesses from 25 nests attended by 23 individual females). Light gray boxes indicate crepuscular periods within 90 min of sunrise and sunset (vertical dashed lines), and dark gray box indicates nighttime hours. Key for panel a: black star predation event resulting in total clutch loss; white star predation event resulting in partial clutch loss; open circle unsuccessful predation attempt. Placement of symbols is approximate for nighttime events because the duration of nighttime hours varied over the breeding season. Key for panel b circles observed recess duration (gray recesses before sunset, P = 0.003; white recesses near sunrise, P \ 0.0001); solid lines predicted lines of best fit from generalized additive main effects models for timing of recess initiation; dotted lines upper and lower 95 % confidence intervals. Two recesses with durations [300 min were omitted from the figure for clarity. Data for timing of sunrise and sunset at Concordia, Kansas were obtained from the US Naval Observatory (2016; http://aa.usno.navy.mil/) 3-h period after sunrise and the 3-h period before sunset (Fig. 3a). About one-third of all recesses (32 %, 147 of 464) occurred within the 90-min period after sunrise or before sunset (Fig. 3a). Only three incubation recesses occurred before sunrise, and no recesses occurred after sunset (Fig. 3a).Total recess duration per day was unaffected by timing of recess initiation (GAMs: morning recess duration *minutes after sunrise, F1,200 = 35.35, P \ 0.0001, adjusted r2 = 0.45; evening recess duration *minutes before sunset, F1,262 = 3.67, P = 0.003, adjusted r2 = 0.06; Fig. 3b). Number of recesses per day was not related to nest age (F1,244 = 2.59, P = 0.11, adjusted r2 = 0.006) or ordinal date of the season (F1,244 = 0.14, P = 0.71, adjusted r2 = 0.004). We observed slightly lower rates of incubation constancy at the start and end of the breeding season (F1,244 = 28.3, P \ 0.0001, adjusted r2 = 0.72; Fig. 2b). Similarly, we found lower rates of incubation constancy at 123 J Ornithol Fig. 4 Still photographs captured from video footage at nests of Greater Prairie-Chickens in northcentral Kansas, 2010 and 2011. Female Greater Prairie-Chicken returning to nest after incubation break (a), female leaving nest with chicks (b), coyote (Canis latrans; c), bullsnake (Pituophis catenifer; d), American badger (Taxidea taxus; e), striped skunk (Mephitis mephitis; f) the start and end of the 38-day in the nesting cycle (F1,244 = 2.10, P = 0.04, adjusted r2 = 0.05; Fig. 2c). sunrise and sunset (9 of 24, 38 %; Fig. 3a). Four of six predation events during daytime hours were by snakes (Fig. 3a; ‘‘Appendix 2’’). Predation resulted in total clutch loss in 20 of 24 events (83 %). We observed two cases of partial predation, and both occurred at night. One nest was partially consumed by a Virginia opossum (Didelphis virginiana), and then completely consumed 14 days later by a striped skunk (Mephitis mephitis; Fig. 4f). A second nest was partially consumed by a bullsnake and then completely consumed 1 day later by a striped skunk. We also observed two unsuccessful predation events, during daytime hours. A thirteen-lined ground squirrel (Ictidomys tridecemlineatus) was unsuccessful at removing eggs during a female recess, Predation events We recorded 24 predation attempts at 33 nests of prairie chickens (Fig. 4). Coyotes (Canis latrans; n = 9) and bullsnakes (Pituophis catenifer, n = 5) were the most common predators and accounted for 58 % of total losses (Fig. 4c, d, ‘‘Appendix 2’’). All predation events occurred while the female was present at the nest, and the incubating female was killed in 2 of 9 attacks by coyotes. Threequarters (18 of 24) of all nest predations occurred at night (9 of 24, 38 %) or during crepuscular periods around 123 J Ornithol Table 1 Model selection to evaluate the effects of mean incubation constancy, mean number of recesses per day, visual obstruction reading (VOR), and precipitation on survival of Greater PrairieChicken nests in Kansas during 2010 and 2011 (n = 5904 h and 25 nests) Model factors ka Deviance DAICbc wi VOR 2 132.19 0.00 0.26 VOR ? incubation constancyc 3 131.55 1.38 0.13 VOR ? number of recesses per dayc 3 131.98 1.81 0.10 Constant 1 136.03 1.82 0.10 VOR ? precipitationc,d 3 132.17 2.00 0.10 VOR 9 incubation constancyc 4 130.18 2.04 0.09 VOR 9 number of recesses per dayc 4 130.99 2.86 0.06 Number of recesses per day 2 135.42 3.23 0.04 Incubation constancy 2 135.77 3.57 0.04 Precipitationd 2 136.02 3.82 0.04 Incubation constancy ? number of recesses per dayc 3 135.11 4.94 0.02 a k = number of parameters, deviance = -2lnL b Minimum AICc = 136.22 c Models with uninformative parameters that were not competitive with the top model (Arnold 2010) d Daily precipitation data were from a weather station at Concordia Blosser Municipal Airport (USW00013984, http://www.ncdc.noaa.gov) our candidate set included the effect of VOR on nest survival (wi = 0.26, b = 0.75, 95 % CL -0.05, 1.56; Table 1). We found a weak negative relationship between daily nest survival and mean number of recesses per day (wi = 0.05; b = -0.30, 95 % CL -1.03, 0.43; Fig. 5), but this feature of nest attendance behavior did not improve model fit compared to a constant model (wi = 0.10; wi/ wj = 2.0; Table 1). Nest survival was not affected by variation in incubation constancy (wi = 0.04; b = -0.003, 95 % CL -0.02, 0.01) or precipitation (wi = 0.04; b = 0.01, 95 % CL -0.25, 0.28; Table 1). Fig. 5 Relationship between daily nest survival and mean number of incubation recesses per day. We modeled nest survival for 25 Greater Prairie-Chicken nests monitored in northcentral Kansas during 2010 and 2011. Solid line daily nest survival from a main effects model with mean number of recesses per day; dotted lines upper and lower 95 % confidence limits and a massasauga rattlesnake (Sistrurus catenatus) attempted but was unable to ingest eggs from a nest after flushing the attending female. Nest survival Effects of nest attendance We included 25 nests in our first nest survival analysis (2010 = 7 and 2011 = 18). Nine of 25 (36 %) nests successfully produced C1 chick. The best supported model in 123 Effects of monitoring technique We matched 33 video nests with 33 unmanipulated nests to test for effects of monitoring technique on nest survival. Median distance from a video nest to the nearest unmanipulated nest was 1.2 km (range 0.1–8.1 km). Nine of 33 (27 %) video nests successfully produced C1 chick vs. 8 of 33 (24 %) unmanipulated nests (Fisher’s exact test, P = 1.0). Nest monitoring method did not improve model fit, and 95 % confidence limits for our estimate of the effect of monitoring method overlapped zero (wi = 0.05; b = 0.22, 95 % CL -0.36, 0.79; Table 2). Contrary to our predictions, video nests tended to have higher 38-day survival rates (0.17 ± 0.06) than unmanipulated nests (0.11 ± 0.05). Vegetative cover (VOR) was the only predictor in the best supported model (wi = 0.28) and was the best overall predictor of daily nest survival in our model set J Ornithol Table 2 Model selection to evaluate the effects of nest monitoring technique (videomonitored vs. unmanipulated), visual obstruction reading (VOR), and precipitation on daily survival of Greater PrairieChicken nests in Kansas during 2010 and 2011 (n = 66 nests) Model factors ka Deviance DAICbc wi VOR 2 360.71 0.00 0.28 VOR ? monitoring techniquec 3 359.85 1.15 0.16 VOR 9 monitoring techniquec 4 357.90 1.21 0.15 VOR ? precipitationc,d 3 360.29 1.59 0.13 Constant 1 364.63 1.91 0.11 Monitoring technique 2 364.07 3.36 0.05 Precipitationd 2 364.22 3.51 0.05 VOR 9 Precipitationc,d 4 360.28 3.60 0.05 Monitoring technique ? precipitationd 3 363.62 4.92 0.02 Monitoring technique 9 precipitationd 4 362.77 6.09 0.01 a b c k = number of parameters, deviance = -2lnL Minimum AICc = 364.73 Models with uninformative parameters that were not competitive with the top model (Arnold 2010) d Daily precipitation data were from a weather station at Concordia Blosser Municipal Airport (USW00013984, http://www.ncdc.noaa.gov) (Rwi = 0.75). Daily survival rates of prairie chicken nests increased with increasing VOR (b = 0.27, 95 % CL -0.05, 0.59; Table 2). Precipitation was not supported as having an important effect on daily nest survival rate (wi = 0.05; b = 0.05, 95 % CL -0.11, 0.21; Table 2). Discussion Female Greater Prairie-Chickens maintained a high incubation constancy of *95 % per day and typically took two *40-min recesses per day, usually within 2–3 h after sunrise or before sunset. Our results are consistent with past studies of grouse with uniparental incubation by females where nest attendance consists of high incubation constancy C90 %, and few recesses per day (2–7), with most breaks taken at sunrise and sunset. The same pattern of incubation rhythm has been observed among multiple species in different habitats; including prairie grouse (Coates and Delehanty 2008; this study), forest grouse (Pulliainen 1971; McCourt et al. 1973; Maxson 1977; Summers et al. 2009), and arctic and alpine ptarmigan (Giesen and Braun 1979; Erikstad 1986; Wiebe and Martin 1997). We discuss our results in the context of potential selection pressures acting on incubation behavior related to predation risk versus the physiological needs of the incubating female and the eggs. Female grouse sustain long stretches of incubation punctuated by relatively short recess bouts, despite being uniparental incubators. In contrast, shorebirds and waterfowl with uniparental incubation typically exhibit lower incubation constancy (*70–85 %), and take longer or more frequent recesses (1–12 per day; Cartar and Montgomerie 1985; Afton and Paulus 1992; Smith et al. 2012a, b). High incubation constancy speeds up embryonic development and reduces the incubation period, minimizing physiological costs and the duration of predation risk (Thompson and Raveling 1987; Tombre and Erikstad 1996; Wiebe and Martin 1997; Samelius and Alisauskas 2001; Hepp et al. 2006; Carter et al. 2014). Other advantages of high incubation constancy include fewer trips to and from the nest, which may attract predators to the eggs, and lower thermoregulatory costs of rewarming eggs after recesses (Drent 1975; Ghalambor and Martin 2002). Thus, high incubation constancy reduces predation risk to parents and offspring while reducing physiological costs for the parent (Erikstad 1986; Cartar and Montgomerie 1985; Ghalambor and Martin 2002; Coates and Delehanty 2008). Duration and timing of recesses can involve tradeoffs between the survival of attending parents and offspring (Erikstad 1986; Wiebe and Martin 1997; Davis and Holmes 2012; Pietz et al. 2012; Reidy and Thompson 2012). If incubating females adjusted recess timing to avoid predator activity, we predicted recesses to occur at midday when mesocarnivores were least active. Instead, we observed prairie chickens taking most recesses near sunrise and sunset. Grouse populations under high levels of predation pressure from avian and mammalian predators exhibit the same bimodal pattern of recess timing, but previous researchers have attributed the pattern to different ecological and physiological drivers. Bimodal recess patterns for Greater Sage-Grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus) in 123 J Ornithol Nevada and Willow Ptarmigan (Lagopus lagopus) in Norway were explained as responses to predation risk from corvid predators (Erikstad 1986; Coates and Delehanty 2008). Greater Prairie-Chickens in Kansas and Whitetailed Ptarmigan (Lagopus leucura) in Colorado also exhibit a bimodal distribution of recess timing, but most clutch loss occurred during overnight hours from mesocarnivore predators (Wiebe and Martin 1997; this study). A bimodal pattern of recess timing may not be driven primarily by predation pressure, but by physiological needs of the incubating female. Female grouse may maintain steady state metabolism by taking recesses at dawn after fasting overnight, and recesses at dusk to obtain energy reserves for overnight hours (Wiebe and Martin 1997). Moreover, female grouse store waste products during incubation, defecate infrequently, and produce large ‘clocker’ droppings that are usually deposited away from the nest (Watson 1972; Keppie and Herzog 1978; Steen et al. 1985). Patterns of nest attendance appear to be conserved in grouse and may indicate that females may be prioritizing their own physiological needs and residual reproductive value over current nest survival (Tulp and Schekkerman 2006). In our study, incubation behavior was not an important driver of variation in nest survival. The apparent tendency of females to prioritize their own needs and future reproductive value may represent an optimal tradeoff between competing needs of current vs. future reproductive value. We found no evidence for negative effects of video surveillance on nest survival, similar to past work (Richardson et al. 2009; Powell et al. 2012). If anything, survival rates for video nests tended to be higher than those for unmanipulated nests in our study, consistent with neophobia among mammalian predators (Richardson et al. 2009). A recent study on Greater Sage-Grouse found no evidence for negative effects of monitoring on daily nest survival when the female was not flushed, but abandonment due to observer-induced flushing resulted in artificially depressed nest survival rates (Gibson et al. 2015). In our study, nest survival was higher when females took fewer recesses per day, but variation in nest attendance behaviors had a minimal effect on nest survival compared to vegetative cover, in part because we observed little variation for both incubation constancy and number of recesses per day. High quality nesting habitat is a limiting resource for female prairie chickens in nesting tallgrass prairie, because most rangelands are privately owned and intensively managed for cattle production (McNew et al. 2014, 2015, Sandercock et al. 2015). Intensive application 123 of fire and grazing reduces vegetative cover, and the effect of vegetative cover on nest survival outweighs other potentially contributing factors (Matthews et al. 2013; Hovick et al. 2014; McNew et al. 2014, 2015). As video surveillance studies become more common, we will continue to gain insights into site- and species-specific drivers of nest attendance behaviors and their effect on demographic rates. Future research should investigate the tradeoffs between adult and offspring survival, and the possible interactions between changes in habitat quality, inclement weather, and predation pressure on incubation behavior and daily nest survival of grouse populations. The study design should include appropriate controls to ensure that camera methods are not negatively affecting incubation behavior or nest survival. Acknowledgments We thank the many field technicians who helped with data collection for our project and A. Ricketts for assistance with predator identification. We especially thank S. Richards, D. Weaver, L. Perry, and other landowners in Kansas for allowing us access to private property. All capture, marking, and tracking activities were performed under institutional animal care and use protocols approved by Kansas State University (IACUC protocol 2781) and state wildlife research permits (SC-082-2010, SC-0112011). Research funding and equipment were provided by a consortium of federal and state wildlife agencies, conservation groups, and wind energy partners under the National Wind Coordinating Collaborative (NWCC) including the Department of Energy, National Renewable Energies Laboratory, U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Kansas Department of Wildlife, Parks, and Tourism, Kansas Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit, National Fish and Wildlife Foundation, Kansas and Oklahoma chapters of The Nature Conservancy, BP Alternative Energy, FPL Energy, Horizon Wind Energy, and Iberdrola Renewables. Compliance with ethical standards Conflict of interest of interest. The authors declare that they have no conflict Ethical approval All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. All procedures performed in studies involving animals were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institution or practice at which the studies were conducted. Appendix 1 Equipment used to video-monitor nests of Greater Prairie Chickens in northcentral Kansas, 2010 and 2011. AC alternating current, BNC Bayonet Neill-Concelman, DC direct current, DVR digital video recorder, LED lightemitting diode, RCA Radio Corporation of America, TRS tip, ring, sleeve. J Ornithol Appendix 2 Predation attempts at Greater Prairie-Chicken nests monitored by video cameras in north central Kansas, 2010 and 2011. Year Time (24 h) 2011 00:05 2011 2010 2011 2010 2011 2011 Nest age (days)a Predatorb Outcomec Predatorb Outcomec 9 Bullsnake Total clutch loss 13 Bullsnake Total clutch loss Unknown mammal Total clutch loss Coyote Total clutch loss Year Time (24 h) Nest age (days)a 2011 18:41 2010 18:49 2010 20:02 18 2011 21:08 1 2011 21:48 14 Bullsnake Total clutch loss Coyote Total clutch loss Bullsnake Partial clutch loss Total clutch loss 17 Coyote Total clutch loss 2011 21:55 22 00:21 2 Skunk Total clutch loss 2010 21:56 9 01:02 21 coyote Total clutch loss 01:22 0 Skunk Total clutch loss 2010 22:02 19 Badger 01:41 23 Skunk Total clutch loss 2011 22:10 14 Coyote Total clutch loss 02:39 4:14 14 1 Badger Opossum Total clutch loss Partial clutch loss 2011 22:58 22 Skunk Total clutch loss 2011 04:48 14 2011 05:23 10 Coyote Total clutch loss 2011 05:33 4 Coyote Total clutch loss 2011 9:51 14 Ground squirrel Unsuccessful attempt Coyote Total clutch loss Bullsnake Rattlesnake Total clutch loss Unsuccessful attempt 2011 10:26 3 2011 2011 12:47 15:29 17 7 Coyote Total clutch loss a Number of days since female initiated incubation b Predators included: massasauga rattlesnake (Sistrurus catenatus), bullsnake (Pituophis catenifer), Virginia opossum (Didelphis virginiana), thirteen-lined ground squirrel (Ictidomys tridecemlineatus), coyote (Canis latrans), American badger (Taxidea taxus), striped skunk (Mephitis mephitis) c Total clutch loss = predator completely or partially consumed a female or eggs from a nest that subsequently became inactive; partial clutch loss = predator consumed a portion of eggs from a nest that remained active following the predation event; unsuccessful attempt = predator unsuccessfully attempted to consume eggs 123 J Ornithol References Afton AD, Paulus SL (1992) Incubation and brood care. In: Batt BDJ, Afton AD, Anderson MG, Ankney CD, Johnson DH, Kadlec JA, Krapu GL (eds) Ecology and management of breeding waterfowl. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, pp 62–108 Arnold TW (2010) Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike’s Information Criterion. J Wildl Manag 74:1175–1178 Ashby KR (1972) Patterns of daily activity in mammals. Mamm Rev 1:171–185 Benson TJ, Brown JD, Bednarz JC (2010) Identifying predators clarifies predictors of nest success in a temperate passerine. J Anim Ecol 79:225–234 Beyer HL (2004) Hawth’s Analysis Tools for ArcGIS. http://www. spatialecology.com/htools BirdLife International (2014) Species factsheet: Tympanuchus cupido. IUCN Red List for Birds. http://www.birdlife.org. Accessed 20 Sep 2014 Burnam JS, Turner G, Ellis-Felege SN, Palmer WE, Sisson DC, Carroll JP (2012) Patterns of incubation behavior in Northern Bobwhites. Stud Avian Biol 43:77–88 Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. Springer, New York, pp 1–488 Cartar RV, Montgomerie RD (1985) The influence of weather on incubation scheduling of the White-rumped Sandpiper (Calidris fuscicollis): a uniparental incubator in a cold environment. Behavior 95:261–289 Cartar RV, Montgomerie RD (1987) Day-to-day variation in nest attentiveness of White-rumped Sandpipers. Condor 89:252– 260 Carter AW, Hopkins WA, Moore IT, DuRant SE (2014) Influence of incubation recess patterns on incubation period and hatchling traits in Wood Ducks Aix sponsa. J Avian Biol 45:273–279 Caudill D, Guttery MR, Bibles B, Messmer TA, Caudill G, Leone E, Dahlgren DK, Chi R (2014) Effects of climatic variation and reproductive trade-offs vary by measure of reproductive effort in Greater Sage-Grouse. Ecosphere 5:art154 Coates PS, Delehanty DJ (2008) Effects of environmental factors on incubation patterns of Greater Sage-Grouse. Condor 110:627–638 Conway CJ, Martin TE (2000) Evolution of passerine incubation behavior: influence of food, temperature, and nest predation. Evolution 54:670–685 Davis SK, Holmes TG (2012) Sprague’s Pipit incubation behavior. Stud Avian Biol 43:67–76 Deeming DC (2002) Behaviour patterns during incubation. In: Deeming DC (ed) Avian incubation: behaviour, environment, and evolution. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 63–87 Dinsmore SJ, White GC, Knopf FL (2002) Advanced techniques for modeling avian nest survival. Ecology 83:3476–3488 Drent R (1975) Incubation. In: Farner DS, King JR (eds) Avian biology, vol 5., Academic PressNew York, New York, pp 333–420 Ellis-Felege SN, Carroll JP (2012) Gamebirds and nest cameras: present and future. Stud Avian Biol 43:35–46 Erikstad KE (1986) Relationship between weather, body condition and incubation rhythm in Willow Grouse. Cinclus 9:7–12 Fields TL, White GC, Gilbert WC, Rodgers RD (2006) Nest and brood survival of Lesser Prairie-Chickens in west central Kansas. J Wildl Manag 70:931–938 Flanders-Wanner BL, White GC, McDaniel LL (2004) Weather and prairie grouse: dealing with effects beyond our control. Wildl Soc Bull 32:22–34 123 Ghalambor CK, Martin TE (2000) Parental investment strategies in two species of nuthatch vary with stage-specific predation risk and reproductive effort. Anim Behav 60:263–267 Ghalambor CK, Martin TE (2002) Comparative manipulation of predation risk in incubating birds reveals variability in the plasticity of responses. Behav Ecol 13:101–108 Gibson D, Blomberg EJ, Atamian MT, Sedinger JD (2015) Observer effects strongly influence estimates of daily nest survival probability but do not substantially increase rates of nest failure in Greater Sage-Grouse. Auk Ornithol Adv 132:397–407 Giesen KM, Braun CE (1979) Nesting behavior of female Whitetailed Ptarmigan in Colorado. Condor 81:215–217 Gowaty PA, Hubbell SP (2009) Reproductive decisions under ecological constraints: it’s about time. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:10017–10024 Hamerstrom FN Jr., Hamerstrom F (1973) The prairie chicken in Wisconsin—highlights of a 22-year study of counts, behavior, movements, turnover, and habitat. Technical Bulletin 64. Wisconsin Department of Natural Resources, Madison, Wisconsin, USA Henderson FR, Brooks FW, Wood RE, Dahlgren RB (1967) Sexing of prairie grouse by crown feather patterning. J Wildl Manag 31:764–769 Hepp GR, Kennamer RA, Johnson MH (2006) Maternal effects in Wood Ducks: incubation temperature influences incubation period and neonate phenotype. Funct Ecol 20:308–314 Hovick TJ, Elmore RD, Allred BW, Fuhlendorf SD, Dahlgren DK (2014) Landscapes as a moderator of thermal extremes: a case study from an imperiled grouse. Ecosphere 5:art35 Johnson JA, Schroeder MA, Robb LA (2011) Greater Prairie-Chicken (Tympanuchus cupido). In: Poole A (ed) The birds of North America online. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca Keppie DM, Herzog PW (1978) Nest site characteristics and nest success of Spruce Grouse. J Wildl Manag 42:628–632 Laake J, Rexstad E (2008) RMark—an alternative approach to building linear models in MARK. In: Cooch E, White GC (eds) Program MARK: a gentle introduction, pp C1–C113 MacCluskie MC, Sedinger JS (1999) Incubation behavior of Northern Shovelers in the subarctic: a contrast to the prairies. Condor 101:417–421 Martin TE (2002) A new view of avian life-history evolution tested on an incubation paradox. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:309–316 Matthews TW, Tyre AJ, Taylor JS, Lusk JJ, Powell LA (2013) Greater Prairie-Chicken nest success and habitat selection in southeastern Nebraska. J Wildl Manag 77:1202–1212 Maxson SJ (1977) Activity patterns of female Ruffed Grouse during the breeding season. Wilson Bulletin 89:439–455 McCourt KH, Boag DA, Keppie DM (1973) Female Spruce Grouse activities during laying and incubation. Auk 90:619–623 McNew LB, Gregory AJ, Wisely SM, Sandercock BK (2009) Estimating the stage of incubation for nests of Greater PrairieChickens using egg flotation: a float curve for grousers. Grouse News 38:12–14 McNew LB, Gregory AJ, Wisely SM, Sandercock BK (2011a) Human-mediated selection of life-history traits of Greater Prairie-Chickens. Stud Avian Biol 39:255–266 McNew LB, Gregory AJ, Wisely SM, Sandercock BK (2011b) Reproductive biology of a southern population of Greater Prairie-Chickens. Stud Avian Biol 39:209–221 McNew LB, Gregory AJ, Wisely SM, Sandercock BK (2012) Demography of Greater Prairie-Chickens: regional variation in vital rates, sensitivity values, and population dynamics. J Wildl Manag 76:987–1000 McNew LB, Hunt LM, Gregory AJ, Wisely SM, Sandercock BK (2014) Effects of wind energy development on nesting ecology J Ornithol of Greater Prairie-Chickens in fragmented grasslands. Conserv Biol 28:1089–1099 McNew LB, Winder VL, Pitman JC, Sandercock BK (2015) Alternative rangeland management strategies and the nesting ecology of Greater Prairie-Chickens. Rangel Ecol Manag 68:298–304 Nooker JK, Sandercock BK (2008) Phenotypic correlates and survival consequences of male mating success in lek-mating Greater Prairie-Chickens (Tympanuchus cupido). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1377–1388 Pietz PJ, Granfors DA, Ribic CA (2012) Knowledge gained from video-monitoring grassland passerine nests. Stud Avian Biol 43:3–22 Poiani KA, Merrill MD, Chapman KA (2001) Identifying conservation-priority areas in a fragmented Minnesota landscape based on the umbrella species concept and selection of large patches of natural vegetation. Conserv Biol 15:513–522 Powell LA (2007) Approximating variance of demographic parameters using the delta method: a reference for avian biologists. Condor 109:949–954 Powell LA, Giovanni MD, Groepper S, Reineke ML, Schacht WH (2012) Attendance patterns and survival of Western Meadowlark nests. Stud Avian Biol 43:61–66 Pulliainen E (1971) Behaviour of a nesting Capercaillie (Tetrao urogallus) in northeastern Lapland. Annales Zoologica Fennica 8:456–462 Reed A, Hughes RJ, Gauthier G (1995) Incubation behavior and body mass of female Greater Snow Geese. Condor 97:993–1001 Reidy JL, Thompson FR III (2012) Predator identity can explain nest predation patterns. Stud Avian Biol 43:135–148 Richardson TW, Gardali T, Jenkins SH (2009) Review and metaanalysis of camera effects on avian nest success. J Wildl Manag 73:287–293 Robel RJ, Briggs JN, Dayton AD, Hulbert LC (1970) Relationship between visual obstruction measurements and weight of grassland vegetation. J Rangel Manag 23:295–297 Samelius G, Alisauskas RT (2001) Deterring arctic fox predation: the role of parental nest attendance by Lesser Snow Geese. Can J Zool 79:861–866 Sandercock BK, Martin K, Hannon SJ (2005) Demographic consequences of age-structure in extreme environments: population models for arctic and alpine ptarmigan. Oecologia 146:13–24 Sandercock BK, Alfaro-Barrios M, Casey AE, Johnson TN, Mong TW, Odom KJ, Strum KM, Winder VL (2015) Effects of grazing and prescribed fire on resource selection and nest survival of Upland Sandpipers in an experimental landscape. Lands Ecol 30:325–337 Schmidt JH, Taylor EJ, Rexstad EA (2005) Incubation behaviors and patterns of nest attendance in Common Goldeneyes in interior Alaska. Condor 107:167–172 Smith PA, Tulp IT, Schekkerman H, Gilchrist HG, Forbes MR (2012a) Shorebird incubation behavior and its influence on the risk of nest predation. Anim Behav 84:835–842 Smith PA, Dauncey SA, Gilchrist HG, Forbes MR (2012b) The influence of weather on shorebird incubation. Stud Avian Biol 43:89–104 Steen JB, Pedersen HC, Erikstad KE, Hansen KB, Høydal K, Størdal A (1985) The significance of cock territories in Willow Ptarmigan. Ornis Scand 16:277–282 Summers RW, Willi J, Selvidge J (2009) Capercaillie Tetrao urogallus nest loss and attendance at Abernethy Forest, Scotland. Wildl Biol 15:237–319 Thompson SD, Raveling DG (1987) Incubation behavior of Emperor Geese compared with other geese: interactions of predation, body size, and energetics. Auk 104:707–716 Thompson FR III, Ribic CA (2012) Conservation implications when the nest predators are known. Stud Avian Biol 43:23–34 Tombre IM, Erikstad KE (1996) An experimental study of incubation effort in high-arctic Barnacle Geese. J Anim Ecol 65:325–331 Tulp I, Schekkerman H (2006) Time allocation between feeding and incubation in uniparental arctic-breeding shorebirds: energy reserves provide leeway in a tight schedule. J Avian Biol 37:207–218 Walsburg GE (1983) Avian ecological energetics. In: Farer DS, King JR, Parkes KC (eds) Avian Biology, Vol VII. Academic Press, New York, pp 161–220 Watson A (1972) The behavior of the ptarmigan. Br Birds 65:6–26 Webb SL, Olson CV, Dzialak MR, Harju SM, Winstead JB, Lockman D (2012) Landscape features and weather influence nest survival of a ground-nesting bird of conservation concern, the Greater Sage-Grouse, in human-altered environments. Ecol Process 1:art4 White GC, Burnham KP (1999) Program MARK: survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46:S120–S139 Wiebe KL, Martin K (1997) Effects of predation, body condition and temperature on incubation of White-tailed Ptarmigan Lagopus leucura. Wildl Biol 3:219–227 Wiebe KL, Martin K (2000) The use of incubation behavior to adjust avian reproductive costs after egg laying. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:463–470 Winder VL, McNew LB, Gregory AJ, Hunt LM, Wisely SM, Sandercock BK (2014) Effects of wind energy development on survival of female Greater Prairie-Chickens. J Appl Ecol 51:395–405 Wisdom MJ, Mills LS (1997) Sensitivity analysis to guide population recovery: prairie-chickens as an example. J Wildl Manag 61:302–312 Wood SN (2011) Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. J R Stat Soc (B) 73:3–36 US Naval Observatory (2016) Astronomical Applications Department, Washington DC. http://aa.usno.navy.mil/ 123