Cell Tissue Res (2002) 309:11–26 DOI 10.1007/s00441-002-0569-0 REVIEW Ralf Stanewsky Clock mechanisms in Drosophila Received: 12 February 2002 / Accepted: 8 April 2002 / Published online: 29 May 2002 © Springer-Verlag 2002 Abstract Mechanisms underlying circadian clock function in Drosophila melanogaster have been revealed by genetic and molecular approaches. Two interlocked transcriptional feedback loops involving at least the period, timeless, Clock, and cycle genes generate molecular oscillations that are believed to control behavioral rhythmicity and other clock outputs. These oscillations are further enhanced and fine-tuned to match the duration of the solar day by post-transcriptional and post-translational mechanisms depending on the PERIOD and TIMELESS proteins and on the protein kinases DOUBLETIME and SHAGGY. Light is the principal zeitgeber for synchronizing molecular and behavioral rhythmicity via the blue-light photoreceptor CRYPTOCHROME and the TIMELESS protein. In addition, light seems required for maintaining robust molecular oscillations at least in peripheral clock-gene-expressing tissues like the eyes, antennae, or Malpighian tubules. Relaying temporal information to cells and tissues expressing overt biological rhythms involves regulation of “output genes” at multiple levels. Although their regulation depends on the major clock genes, the majority of the clock-controlled genes are not direct targets of clock factors. Keywords Circadian rhythms · Clock genes · Cryptochrome · Drosophila · Feedback loops Introduction The study of clock mechanisms in Drosophila started in 1968 when R.J. Konopka systematically screened for Research in my group is sponsored by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) R. Stanewsky (✉) Universität Regensburg, Institut für Zoologie, Lehrstuhl für Entwicklungsbiologie, Universitätsstrasse 31, 93040 Regensburg, Germany e-mail: ralf.stanewsky@biologie.uni-regensburg.de Tel.: +49-941-9433083, Fax: +49-941-9433325 mutants showing altered temporal pupal eclosion patterns in light:dark (LD) cycles. In the wild, Drosophila melanogaster individuals eclose from their pupal case at dawn; Konopka’s mutant strains were aperiodic, or showed either advanced or delayed eclosion peaks (Konopka and Benzer 1971). When tested in constant conditions (constant darkness, DD), the phase-altering mutants had short (19 h) and long (29 h) free-running periods, respectively. Subsequent genetic mapping experiments revealed that all three mutations map to the same locus, which was then dubbed period (per); the mutant alleles per01 (aperiodic), perS (Short), and perL (Long). Not only the eclosion rhythms were affected by these variants: the daily rest:activity cycle of mutant individuals was altered in the same way, both in DD and LD cycles (Konopka and Benzer 1971; Hamblen-Coyle et al. 1992). This placed per function right in the heart of the circadian clock and prompted cloning and molecular analysis of this “clock gene,” as well as the search for novel rhythm variants. Now, more than 30 years later (and mainly by continuing Konopka’s approach) several new clock genes have been identified and characterized, so that the wheels and turns of Drosophila’s clock seem unraveled. In this article I will summarize the current view of core clock mechanisms in flies. Special care will be taken in the interpretation of results drawn from in vitro experiments and those involving clock-gene-expressing cells other than those controlling the rhythmic behaviors described above. This is because clock gene expression is rather widespread throughout the fly and is even under circadian control in most of these tissues (Hall 1995; Plautz et al. 1997). But both the pupal eclosion and adult activity rhythms mentioned above are controlled by only a small number of per-expressing neurons (Dushay et al. 1989; Zerr et al. 1990; Ewer et al. 1992; Frisch et al. 1994; Helfrich-Förster 1998; Renn et al. 1999; Kaneko et al. 2000a; Blanchardon et al. 2001). These “lateral neurons” (LNs) are located bilaterally between the optic lobes and the central brain (Fig. 1; cf. Kaneko and Hall 2000). In the adult they consist of two groups of more ventrally located cells: typically five small LNvs (s-LNv) 12 Fig. 1A, B PERIOD proteinexpressing neurons and their axonal projections in the brain of adult Drosophila. A Fluorescence image of a whole-mount brain-half immunostained with an anti-serum against PER. The fly was synchronized to a 12 h:12 h light-dark cycle and sacrificed for staining at ZT 23. Neuronal and glial staining is indicated by arrows or arrowheads, respectively. B Projections of PER-expressing neurons as revealed by Helfrich-Förster (1995) and Kaneko and Hall (2000). Note that most LNvs also express the neuropeptide PDF (filled circles). The projections from the DN3 to the s-LNv were revealed by C. Helfrich-Förster (personal communication). All neuronal groups are present symmetrically in both brain hemispheres (see A), but shown only on one side here for clarification. Drawing adapted from Kaneko and Hall (2000) (OL optic lobe, Me medulla, La lamina, Es esophagus, POT posterior optic tract). Scale bar 400 µm (A) and four large LNvs (l-LNv) projecting to the dorsal protocerebrum and to the second optic lobe neuropil (medulla) respectively (Fig. 1; cf. Stanewsky et al. 1997a; Kaneko and Hall 2000). In addition, the l-LNv also project to the contralateral LNvs and the medulla. Both LNv groups express the pigment dispersing factor (pdf) gene, which was initially used to reveal their projection pattern (Helfrich-Förster 1995). The neuropeptide encoded by this gene is crucial for transmitting temporal information from the LNs to downstream neurons in the dorsal brain (Renn et al. 1999; Helfrich-Förster et al. 2000; Park et al. 2000a). A third, more dorsally located group of ca. six LNds also projects to the dorsal brain but does not express pdf (Fig. 1; Kaneko and Hall 2000). The dorsal brain region receiving input from the LNs is also connected to another group of clock-gene-expressing neurons, the “dorsal neurons” (DNs; see Fig. 1). They split into three groups characterized by their position within the dorsal brain: ca. 15 DN1, 2 DN2, and ca. 40 DN3, all terminating in the same dorsal brain region as the LNs (Fig. 1). In addition the DN1 and DN3 also project towards the s-LNv cell bodies so that most likely all clockgene-expressing neurons are interconnected (Fig. 1; Kaneko and Hall 2000; Helfrich-Förster 2002 and personal communication). The role of the DNs is less well understood, but from genetic mosaic studies it seems clear that they are not crucial for maintaining behavioral rhythms (Ewer et al. 1992; Frisch et al. 1994). Neverthe- 13 less they seem to contribute to the behavioral pattern observed in wild-type flies since animals with a mutant version of the disconnected (disco) gene (leading to the loss of PER expression in all three LN groups, but not affecting the DNs; Zerr et al. 1990; Blanchardon et al. 2001) still show entrained behavior under LD conditions and stay rhythmic for the first 2 days upon transfer to DD before becoming arrhythmic (Dushay et al. 1989; Hardin et al. 1992; Helfrich-Förster 1998). Moreover, per expression remains rhythmic in the DNs of disco mutants, even for 2 days in constant conditions (Blanchardon et al. 2001), and it is therefore likely that the residual rhythmicity is mediated by at least some of the DN groups. In larvae, only the precursors of the s-LNv and a subset of the DNs are present (e.g., Helfrich-Förster 2002). Light pulses given during the early larval stages are able to synchronize pupal eclosion and adult activity rhythms (Brett 1955; Sehgal et al. 1992), and PER expression in the larval LNs has been correlated with this “time memory” (Kaneko et al. 1997, 2000b). Lack of the larval LNs or disruption of rhythmic PER results in an arrhythmic eclosion pattern, demonstrating that fluctuating PER levels in these cells are required for this clock output (Dushay et al. 1989; Blanchardon et al. 2001; cf. Helfrich-Förster 2002 for a further discussion concerning the role of individual larval, pupal, and adult clock-geneexpressing brain neurons and a detailed description of their connections). The biological function of per in the great majority of the remaining, mainly non-neuronal tissues is completely unknown. Nevertheless, most of the current clock models are based on findings obtained from experiments involving those “peripheral tissues” or from in vitro studies involving embryonic Drosophila or clockless yeast cell lines. Undoubtedly these studies were important and led to major breakthroughs unraveling molecular clock function, but one should be careful extrapolating these results to the few tissues with a known biological clock function like the LNs. That this is not “overcautious” becomes clear after considering a different gene related to the circadian clock: cryptochrome (cry) encodes a factor that is crucial for the light response of the circadian clock and a mutation in this gene (cryb) results in the loss of overt clockgene oscillations in peripheral tissues (Stanewsky et al. 1998). Surprisingly cryb flies showed normal locomotor rhythms in DD, and sure enough it turned out that clockgene cycling in the LNs was not abolished by this mutation, pointing to different functions of this gene in peripheral versus pacemaker tissues (Stanewsky et al. 1998 and see below). Moreover, even the different groups of LNs were affected differentially by cryb (Helfrich-Förster et al. 2001), so one should be even more cautious in generalizing results. A similar differential effect was also observed for two other clock-gene mutations, which will be discussed in more detail below: ClockJrk and cycle01 mutant flies inhibit expression of the pdf gene in the s-LNv but not in the l-LNv (Park et al. 2000a). Consis- tent with this, per expression levels are normal in the l-LNv but reduced in all other cells of the two mutants, including the s-LNv and LNd (Allada et al. 1998; Rutila et al. 1998; Kaneko and Hall 2000). Another example involves the behaviorally arrhythmic disco flies, which show normal molecular rhythms of clock-gene expression when peripheral tissues are examined but a reduction of LN cell number combined with abolished PER expression within the remaining neurons (Zerr et al. 1990; Hardin et al. 1992; Blanchardon et al. 2001). Components of the DD clock The players Which factors comprise the clock that drives lifelong rhythmic behavior in the absence of external zeitgebers (which can be observed for up to 8 weeks depending on the experimental setup and the care of the researcher)? The most straightforward way to identify such factors would be to search for mutants that behave arrhythmically or show altered periods under these conditions using forward genetic screens. Indeed, in addition to finding new mutated versions of per (Dushay et al. 1992; Konopka et al. 1994; Hamblen et al. 1998), this simple strategy led to the discovery of several novel clock factors: timeless (tim), Clock (Clk), cycle (cyc), and doubletime (dbt). Whereas the original tim01 mutation was isolated as an aperiodic eclosion variant (Sehgal et al. 1994), the ClkJrk (Allada et al. 1998) and cyc01 (Rutila et al. 1998) mutants emerged as arrhythmic strains from screens for altered locomotor activity rhythms. Subsequent testing revealed that (as in the case of per) arrhythmic behavior was also observed for the respective other clock output. Therefore it was concluded that the loci identified by these mutations also encode crucial clock factors. Subsequently, period-lengthening and -shortening alleles of tim were isolated as well as one novel lossof-function allele each for the tim and cyc loci (Rutila et al. 1996; Rothenfluh et al. 2000a, 2000b; Stempfl et al. 2002; Park et al. 2000a). In the case of dbt, originally two period-altering alleles were isolated in a screen where locomotor activity rhythms were measured: one with short free-running rhythms (18 h; dbtS), and a long variant (27 h; dbtL). Again, eclosion rhythms were affected in the same way by both mutations, suggesting that the locus encodes a clock factor (Price et al. 1998). Subsequently, two additional period-lengthening alleles, dbth and dbtg, were isolated, exhibiting free-running periods of 29 h (dbth) or 28.5 h (dbtg/+) (Suri et al. 2000). When dbtg was placed over a deletion of the locus the resulting flies were behaviorally arrhythmic (Suri et al. 2000): the same phenotype that was observed for homozygous dbtar flies (Rothenfluh et al. 2000c). It turned out that completely eliminating dbt gene function resulted in lethality, pointing to an additional role of the dbt gene product during development (Price et al. 1998; Zilian et al. 1999). This 14 also indicates that even the two arrhythmia-inducing dbt alleles are not loss-of-function alleles. Different approaches uncovered rhythm-related functions for the shaggy (sgg) and vrille (vri) genes, which had previously been shown to be developmental factors (e.g., Siegfried et al. 1992; George and Terracol 1997). Sgg was reidentified in a genetic screen where individual genes covering a fair fraction of the whole Drosophila genome were specifically overexpressed in all clockgene-expressing cells including the LNs (Martinek et al. 2001). In the case of sgg this resulted in shortenings of the free-running period (20–21 h) and reducing sgg gene function led to period lengthenings (26 h). Due to the vital function of this gene during development it was not possible to study the behavioral consequences of a total loss of function mutant (Martinek et al. 2001). However, the specific effects of altered sgg expression on the expression of other clock genes within the LNs (discussed below) suggest that this gene also encodes an important clock factor. Finally, vri was identified in a molecular screen aimed at isolating genes that are rhythmically expressed and controlled by the circadian clock (Blau and Young 1999). Like sgg, vri has a vital function during development, militating against behavioral analysis of vri loss-of-function mutants. But reduction of vri gene dosage resulted in mild period-shortenings (23 h), and experimentally induced constant expression in the LNs resulted in lengthened locomotor periods or even arrhythmic behavior (Blau and Young 1999). In addition, vri is normally expressed in the LNs (LNv and LNd) and overexpression in these cells blunts the expression of other clock genes therein (Blau and Young 1999). The behavioral and molecular phenotypes just described suggest that the per, tim, Clk, cyc, dbt, sgg, and vri genes each comprise a crucial part of the Drosophila clock. In the following I will summarize what is known about their gene products and how they interact to generate self-sustained oscillations. Interactions among clock factors The transcription factors CLOCK and CYCLE The genes Clk and cyc both encode transcription factors containing a PAS protein-dimerization domain (for PERARNT-SIM; the founders of the PAS-protein family) and a basic helix-loop-helix (bHLH) domain involved in DNA binding (Crews and Fan 1999). Both genes show a broad RNA expression pattern in heads including the photoreceptor cells and the brain cortex, including the region where the different LN groups are located (So et al. 2000). Clk and cyc expression in the LNs is not surprising given the behavioral phenotypes of the respective mutants described above. However, a subset of the LNs, the l-LNv (Fig. 1), likely does not express those two genes given that the ClkJrk and cyc01 mutations do not affect expression of per and tim within these cells (Kaneko and Hall 2000). This suggests that other factors drive Fig. 2 Model describing the daily patterns of activity, cellular distribution, and interactions of important clock genes under constant conditions. Transcriptional activity is indicated by wavy lines, reduced RNA production by dotted lines. Protein concentrations are coded by the intensity of the color chosen for a certain protein (e.g., light blue indicates low TIM abundance, dark blue refers to high TIM levels). Colored dots indicate progressive degradation of a given protein. The phosphorylation status of PER and TIM is depicted by flags. CLK, CYC, PER and TIM proteins are abbreviated with single letters (C, B, P, and T, respectively) due to space constraints. For other abbreviations, see text. Regulation of vri RNA is identical to that of per and tim and was therefore not included. Levels and function of the VRI protein are entirely hypothetical, and the activator of Clk transcription is also not known (for details, see text) clock-gene expression within the l-LNv. The following description of the temporal expression pattern of both genes and their interactions is based on experiments that were performed with crude RNA or protein extracts from whole fly heads, and may not necessarily reflect the processes in the pacemaker neurons. While Clk expression is itself circadianly regulated – resulting in cycling Clk mRNA and CLK protein levels – cyc is constitutively transcribed, leading to high, steadystate cyc RNA and CYC protein levels (Bae et al. 1998, 2000; Lee et al. 1998; Rutila et al. 1998). Both Clk RNA and protein show a similar temporal profile, reaching peak levels around subjective dawn (the time when in nature or in an LD cycle the lights would go on) and their lowest levels in the early subjective night (Fig. 2). 15 In addition, CLK (like the per and tim gene products, see below) is post-translationally modified by phosphorylation (Lee et al. 1998). Yet, it is not clear whether the phosphorylation status is circadianly regulated as in the case of PER and TIM. CLK and CYC proteins heterodimerize and bind to so-called E-Box sequences – six consensus nucleotides that are a target for bHLH transcription factors (reviewed by Kyriacou and Rosato 2000) – in the promoters of the per and tim genes, thereby activating their transcription (Darlington et al. 1998; Lee et al. 1999; Fig. 2). The interaction of CLK and CYC has been shown to occur in fly heads (Bae et al. 2000), whereas the binding of the heterodimer to the per and tim promoter sequences has been inferred from cell culture experiments (Darlington et al. 1998) or from the low levels of PER and TIM proteins in ClkJrk or cyc01 mutant backgrounds (Allada et al. 1998; Rutila et al. 1998). Direct binding has so far only been shown with in vitro translated CLK and CYC proteins (Lee et al. 1999). Since CLK levels vary throughout the day and CYC is constitutively present in high amounts, it is likely that CLK dictates the number of CLK-CYC heterodimers in the cell (Bae et al. 2000). This in turn would explain why activation of the per and tim promoters occurs rhythmically. A caveat of this model results from the fact that levels of the CLK-CYC heterodimer are decreasing rapidly during the time per and tim transcription is activated in the middle of the day (Bae et al. 2000; So and Rosbash 1997). These two studies followed the temporal profile of both events only in LD, but the temporal CLK expression is similar in LD and DD (Lee et al. 1998), suggesting that this is the case for the dimer too. Thus, some mechanism has to exist that prevents the CLK-CYC dimer from activating per and tim at earlier times (Fig. 2 and see below). The multiple roles of PERIOD and TIMELESS PER, a founding member of the PAS protein family, is an odd one, since it lacks the bHLH DNA-binding domain. The PAS domain functions as a binding site for PER’s partner TIM (Gekakis et al. 1995), although the latter protein itself has no such protein-protein interaction domain. Instead TIM contains three ARMADILLOlike dimerization domains, two of which overlap with the region that can bind to PER in cultured Drosophila cells (Kyriacou and Hastings 2001; Saez and Young 1996). The spatial expression pattern of per and tim is very similar (see above and Fig. 1), although in general tim seems to be active in more cells compared to per (Kaneko and Hall 2000). The temporal profiles of gene activity as well as the protein-protein interactions between PER, TIM and other proteins discussed below were mainly extracted from “whole head” preparations. PERIOD and TIMELESS as repressors CLK- and CYC-mediated transcription of per and tim starts in the middle of the day and peak levels of each mRNA are reached in the early evening (Hardin et al. 1990; Sehgal et al. 1995; So and Rosbash 1997). The PER and TIM proteins accumulate with a ca. 4- to 6-h delay – reaching peak levels late in the night – compared to their mRNAs. This delay is a potentially crucial feature of the clock mechanism (Zerr et al. 1990; Zeng et al. 1994, 1996; Myers et al. 1996; and see below). Interestingly, already in the early stage of clock-molecule studies, a difference in the cyclical PER expression profile was found between a peripheral tissue (the photoreceptors) and the LNs, where peak and trough levels of PER were reached later compared to the photoreceptors (Zerr et al. 1990). PER and TIM accumulate in the cytoplasm of photoreceptor cells, Malpighian tubules, and LNs and eventually enter the nucleus probably as a heterodimer (Vosshall et al. 1994; Curtin et al. 1995; Hege et al. 1997; Fig. 2). In fact, heterodimerization with TIM is likely to be a prerequisite for PER accumulation (cf. Price et al. 1995) since newly synthesized monomeric PER is destabilized by the dbt-encoded kinase (Price et al. 1998; Kloss et al. 1998; Fig. 2). DBT function therefore delays accumulation of the PER-TIM dimer until sufficient amounts of TIM are present in the cytoplasm, allowing per and tim transcription to continue. This delay is further increased by temporal gating of PER and TIM nuclear entry, a process likely to involve dbt and sgg gene functions (see below). The temporal gap between transcriptional activity and nuclear entry of the repressors is probably important to maintain robust molecular oscillations (e.g., Price et al. 1998), although the relevance of this delay is still under debate (e.g., Suri et al. 2000; see below). Once in the nucleus PER and TIM are thought to repress transcription of their own genes by interfering with their activators CLK and CYC (Lee et al. 1998, 1999; Bae et al. 2000; Fig. 2). Most likely this occurs via a direct binding of the PER-TIM dimer to the CLK-CYC dimer in the late night and early morning (Bae et al. 2000). But this would not explain why transcription of per and tim is turned off ca. 4 h before the PER-TIM dimer enters the nucleus (see above). It is more likely that the initial deactivation of per and tim expression is caused by the trough levels of CLK reached around the same time (at subjective dusk; Fig. 2). Later, the repressor activity of PER and TIM prevents the CLK-CYC dimer from reinitiating per and tim transcription during the rising phase of CLK in the late night and its initial decline in the morning. Only after PER and TIM have disappeared from the nucleus around midday can a new round of transcription be initiated by the CLK-CYC dimer. PERIOD and TIMELESS as activators In addition to negatively regulating their own expression, PER and TIM are part of a second, interlocked 16 feedback-loop, which further regulates Clk gene expression. The initial finding was that in per01 and tim01 mutant animals levels of Clk RNA and protein were low, suggesting that PER and TIM positively influence Clk activity (Bae et al. 1998; Lee et al. 1998). Surprisingly, in per01 ClkJrk and per01 cyc01 double-mutant, levels of Clk RNA were at peak levels throughout the circadian day, suggesting that the CLK-CYC dimer normally represses Clk expression and that this repression is released by the PER-TIM dimer (Glossop et al. 1999). This scenario raised the hypothesis that Clk expression would be activated after the nuclear entry of PER and TIM in the middle of the night, consistent with what is observed experimentally (Bae et al. 1998; Fig. 2). Since CLK and CYC are positively acting transcription factors, it is hard to imagine that they can act as repressors as well. Instead of directly repressing Clk, it seems more likely that they upregulate a yet unknown repressor in a mode similar to per and tim. This factor would then also be downregulated by nuclear PER and TIM, an assumption that jibes with the observation that Clk RNA levels are low in per01 and tim01 (because the repressor is up) and high in per01 ClkJrk and per01 cyc01 double mutants (because the repressor is down due to missing transcriptional activation). A potential candidate molecule for this function is discussed in the next paragraph, but yet another question remains open: what activates Clk expression in the absence of functional CLK, CYC, PER, and TIM proteins? The role of vrille Vri encodes a basic-Zipper transcription factor and is expressed with the same temporal profile as per and tim (Blau and Young 1999). Its spatial expression pattern in the fly head is similar to that of per and tim (including the LNs) although vri seems to be present in more CNS cells compared to the other two clock genes (Blau and Young 1999). Since vri function is required for normal clock-gene oscillations, its function was placed within the core of the circadian pacemaker mechanism. Moreover vri is involved in post-transcriptional regulation of PDF, indicating that it also plays a role in clock output (Blau and Young 1999). There is also circumstantial evidence that VRI indeed could be the Clk repressor and in the following I will present some arguments in favor of this hypothesis. First, vri is activated by CLK and CYC, since vri RNA levels are low in ClkJrk or cyc01 mutants. This activation is likely to be a consequence of direct CLK-CYC binding to an E-Box in the vri promoter, since both the dimer and the promoter element are crucial for activating vri transcription in vitro (Blau and Young 1999). The low repressor-encoding RNA levels in ClkJrk or cyc01 would be consistent with the high levels of Clk RNA in the same genetic backgrounds (Glossop et al. 1999; see above). Second, in per01 or tim01 mutant flies vri RNA levels are intermediate, similar to the per and tim steady-state RNA amounts in the same mutant backgrounds. This indicates that vri is indeed regulated by the same mechanism as per and tim (Blau and Young 1999). The predicted constant and intermediate VRI amounts would explain why Clk RNA is low in per01 and tim01. Third, vri overexpression reduces per and tim RNA levels (Blau and Young 1999), probably as a result of reduced Clk activity. If VRI is indeed the repressor, one would expect its phase of expression to be just the opposite to that of Clk, whose gene products reach their trough levels around dusk, just about when vri RNA is at its peak. This implies that – unlike for per and tim – there would be no delay expected to exist between vri RNA and protein accumulation and also no gated nuclear entry of VRI. As a result (and as in the case of Clk) the RNA and protein profiles of vri would be very similar. VRI would be made and enter the nucleus closely following the temporal vri RNA pattern, repressing Clk from afternoon until midnight when PER and TIM enter the nucleus and block VRI’s function (Fig. 2). What gene product activates Clk transcription? Arguments favoring certain candidates are even more handwaving than those for VRI being a repressor. Nevertheless, microarray analysis aimed at identifying direct target genes of CLK revealed two transcription factors that are normally rhythmically expressed in the fly head, with a similar temporal profile to per and tim (McDonald and Rosbash 2001). The two genes in question, pdp1 and sticky ch1, encode basic-Zipper and bHLH transcription factors, respectively, and could therefore be involved in CLK regulation via positive feedback regulation. Alternatively, especially in case VRI turns out to be a rhythmically active Clk repressor, production of its activator could be temporally flat (Fig. 2). The kinases encoded by double-time and shaggy DOUBLE-TIME regulates PERIOD at several points throughout the day Dbt encodes the Drosophila ortholog of the mammalian casein kinase Iε (Kloss et al. 1998). Considering the behavioral phenotypes of dbt mutations discussed above, the kinase function of the DBT enzyme could be responsible for the circadian fluctuation of the phosphorylation status observed for both the PER and TIM proteins (Edery et al. 1994; Zeng et al. 1996) or even CLK (Bae et al. 1998). It turned out that dbt function influences PER post-translationally, although at present it cannot be ruled out that TIM and CLK are also substrates of this enzyme (Kloss et al. 1998; Price et al. 1998; Blau 2001). Dbt RNA and protein are expressed constitutively in the photoreceptor cells and the brain cortex including the LNs (Kloss et al. 1998, 2001). Yet, the spatial dbt expression, at least during development, is probably much broader compared to per and tim because strong hypomorphic alleles (dbtP) result in pupal lethality and altered brain architecture (Price et al. 1998; Zilian et al. 1999). 17 Although constitutively expressed, DBT undergoes daily changes in subcellular localization in photoreceptor cells and the LNs at least under LD conditions (Kloss et al. 2001). One role of DBT is to destabilize cytoplasmic PER in the absence of sufficient amounts of TIM that normally stabilize PER by forming a heterodimer. This can be inferred from the accumulation of high amounts of PER in the strong dbtP allele, although only very little TIM is expressed in this mutant. Moreover, PER isolated from dbtP larvae is hypophosphorylated, indicating that DBT destabilizes PER by phosphorylation (Price et al. 1998; Fig. 2). Evidence for this also comes from a different set of experiments, involving flies expressing per-lacZ fusion genes. One of these (SG-lacZ), encoding the N-terminal half of PER, is constitutively expressed although its encoding RNA exhibits per-like oscillations (Zwiebel et al. 1991; Dembinska et al. 1997). Adding the next 231 PER amino acids in the C-terminal direction – encoded by BG-lacZ – not only results in robust protein oscillations, but also in daily changes of the phosphorylation pattern (Dembinska et al. 1997; Stanewsky et al. 1997a). This implies that PER phosphorylation is required for rhythmic turnover and defines a likely DBT target within the PER protein. This view is further supported by the direct physical interaction between DBT and PER, which can be observed both in vitro and in vivo (Kloss et al. 1998, 2001). The interaction occurs throughout the day, resulting in the observed daily rhythms in subcellular localization of DBT (Fig. 2). It is likely that DBT just sticks to PER when the latter protein is newly synthesized in the cytoplasm, leading to substantial amounts of cytoplasmic DBT in the early night. After the shift of the PER-TIM complex into the nucleus around midnight, DBT is also observed predominantly in this compartment, indicating that the three proteins enter the nucleus as a complex (Kloss et al. 2001). Strikingly, in per01 flies DBT is mainly nuclear, suggesting that this is its default localization, and that cytoplasmic PER is needed to accumulate DBT in the cytoplasm. However, the same nuclear localization is observed in tim01 flies, but since TIM accumulates to rather high amounts in the cytoplasm of per01 flies (Myers et al. 1996) and PER levels are very low in tim01 flies (Price et al. 1995, 1998), it is much more likely that PER is responsible for cytoplasmic accumulation of DBT (Kloss et al. 2001). PER phosphorylation is not restricted to the cytoplasm since the highest molecular weight forms are observed at times when PER is in the nucleus (Edery et al. 1994; Curtin et al. 1995). Since PER persists in nuclei of dbtP larvae in the absence of TIM, it was suggested that DBT affects the stability of PER also in this compartment (Price et al. 1998). That this is likely to be the case was shown by applying yet another clock gene mutation, the timUltraLong (timUL) allele. Flies carrying this variant exhibit free-running periods of 33 h (!) caused by a dramatically increased stability of the nuclear TIMUL-PER complex (Rothenfluh et al. 2000b). In agreement with this finding PER is hypophosphorylated (and therefore probably stable) for an extended period of time in timUL flies (Rothenfluh et al. 2000b; Kloss et al. 2001). When TIMUL is removed from this complex by light (see next section), PER becomes rapidly phosphorylated, indicating that TIM normally blocks excessive PER phosphorylation as long as the proteins are part of the same complex (Kloss et al. 2001; Fig. 2). Blocking DBT function might indeed be a crucial function of TIM in the nucleus: Using the same timUL flies and the same strategy of clearing PER from the mutant TIM protein, it was shown that monomeric PER can act as a potent repressor of CLK-CYC-induced per and tim expression (Rothenfluh et al. 2000b). Consistent with this idea, a mutant version of PER that constitutively localizes to the nucleus disrupts behavioral rhythms even in the presence of a wild-type per gene copy. Moreover, in vitro expression of this nuclear form of PER in the complete absence of TIM resulted in potent inhibition of CLK-CYC-induced per expression (Rothenfluh et al. 2000b). Taken together this suggests that nuclear TIM retards phosphorylation of PER, thereby prolonging the cycle. An important time cue for setting the period length of the molecular cycle would then be the dissociation of TIM from PER and DBT in the late night. Regulation of nuclear PER stability by DBT (at least under LD conditions) was also suggested after analysis of period-lengthening and arrhythmia-inducing dbt alleles: dbtL, dbtg, and dbth/+ all show a prolonged presence of nuclear PER in the early morning. Although in these alleles PER also accumulates a bit later in the day compared to wild type, peak levels of PER are reached around the same time (Price et al. 1998; Suri et al. 2000). Therefore this delayed onset of PER accumulation in the mutants cannot explain the significantly slower decay of nuclear PER in the morning (Price et al. 1998; Suri et al. 2000). In LD conditions the extended repressing activity of monomeric PER results in a delayed onset of per and tim expression, thereby eliminating the delay usually observed between transcripts and proteins (Suri et al. 2000). Although the importance of “the delay” for feedback regulation has been questioned after those results, the same study shows that it is not eliminated in constant conditions, implying its importance in maintaining freerunning molecular oscillations. Strikingly, the stable monomeric forms of PER observed in the period-lengthening alleles were hyperphosphorylated (Price et al. 1998; Suri et al. 2000), and even in the arrhythmia-inducing dbtar mutants temporally flat PER molecules represented a mixture between hypo- and hyperphosphorylated molecules (Rothenfluh et al. 2000c). This indicates that other kinases are also involved in PER regulation. On the other hand, the dbtar mutant underscored the specificity of DBT for PER: A double mutant between perS and dbtar restored behavioral and molecular rhythms, indicating that the increased turnover of nuclear PERS proteins (Curtin et al. 1995) 18 compensates for the increased stability of nuclear PER in dbtar flies (Rothenfluh et al. 2000c). Interestingly, DBT nuclear and cytoplasmic function seems to have opposing effects on cycle progression. In the cytoplasm PER accumulation is inhibited by DBT and can only occur after enough TIM is present, resulting in a delay of PER increase compared to its RNA. In contrast, nuclear DBT function seems to set the endpoint for each molecular cycle by phosphorylating monomeric PER and turning it into a potent repressor before it is degraded (Fig. 2). DBT seems to affect yet another step in the circadian cycle, i.e., the nuclear translocation of PER or the PERTIM complex. Evidence for this stems from a detailed analysis of the period-shortening dbtS allele (Price et al. 1998; Bao et al. 2001). Although turnover of nuclear PER and its cytoplasmic accumulation in photoreceptor cells of this mutant is accelerated, the nuclear entry of PER is delayed by several hours compared to wild type (Bao et al. 2001). Since this effect was analyzed only in LD conditions, it is not clear if it describes an additional clock function of DBT, or its involvement in entrainment to such environmental cycles (see below). SHAGGY affects TIMELESS phosphorylation and nuclear translocation of the PER-TIM dimer Shaggy encodes the ortholog of the mammalian glycogen synthase kinase-3 (GSK-3) and is important for the daily changes in phosphorylation pattern of TIM, which is likely to be a substrate of this kinase (Martinek et al. 2001). Although the spatial and temporal expression pattern of sgg in adult flies is not known, its overexpression in all tim-expressing cells results in an advanced nuclear entry of PER and TIM proteins in the LNs correlated with shorter free-running periods of locomotor rhythms (Martinek et al. 2001). Reducing sgg expression results in hypophosphorylated TIM throughout the circadian day, whereas overexpression has the opposite effect. Together with the observation that mammalian GSK-3 in vitro phosphorylates fly TIM, this shows that SGG is crucial for TIM phosphorylation. Although it was proposed that SGG mainly targets cytoplasmic TIM during early subjective night, SGG also affects the onset of TIM phosphorylation in the middle of subjective night (Martinek et al. 2001). This event could normally trigger the dissociation of TIM from the TIM-PER-DBT complex, leading to DBT-dependent phosphorylation of PER, thereby turning it into a potent monomeric repressor of CLK-CYC-induced per and tim expression until the 24-h cycle is completed (Fig. 2). Since completely eliminating sgg gene function results in lethality, it is not certain if SGG is the only kinase involved in TIM phosphorylation. There is evidence that a tyrosine kinase is linked to degradation of TIM by the proteasome (Naidoo et al. 1999) and TIM has also been discussed to be a substrate of DBT (Blau 2001). In summary it seems clear that both DBT and SGG are involved in post-translational modifications of PER and TIM, respectively. Their functions regulate the period of the circadian clock and seem to be a potential target for other regulatory events determining day length, as indicated by the opposing effects both kinases have on nuclear entry of the PER-TIM complex. The LD-clock: light as a potent amplifier Poor molecular rhythms in the absence of light as zeitgeber Although it is important to study the functions that generate sustained molecular and behavioral oscillations under constant conditions, it is at least equally important to study clock mechanisms under more natural conditions, like LD cycles. It should be noted that the LD cycles applied in most laboratories aimed at simulating the natural change of night and day are far from appropriate. In nature, organisms synchronize their clock mainly to twilight conditions occurring at dawn and dusk, which are characteristic of dramatic changes in both light quantity and quality (Roenneberg and Foster 1997). Moreover, light:dark changes in nature usually go in hand with temperature changes that also can act as potent zeitgebers (e.g., Wheeler et al. 1993). We are only starting to understand how light and temperature interact to mediate stable entrainment to environmental cycles in flies (e.g., Tomioka et al. 1998; Majercak et al. 1999). For a detailed description of light inputs into the circadian clock the reader is referred to more specific articles dealing with this subject (e.g., Foster and HelfrichFörster et al. 2001; Shafer 2001; Zordan et al. 2001). Here, I will only summarize how light resets the molecular clock-gene oscillations in flies. Indeed, light does probably more than that, because molecular oscillations dampen rapidly in DD. Robust gene-product rhythms in flies can be observed for a maximum of 4 days in DD, which is in stark contrast to the lifelong behavioral rhythms. Therefore, the mechanisms described in the previous chapter are likely not sufficient to explain sustained rhythmic behavior. Several arguments have been presented trying to “rescue” the dogma that rhythmic gene expression drives behavioral rhythms. Initially DD-dampening was attributed to interindividual drift of clock-gene oscillations. In this context it is noteworthy that usually at least ca. 30 fly heads are required to determine the RNA or protein content at one given timepoint, which could result in flattening of a molecular rhythm if the individuals had drifted out of phase from each other. Introducing the luciferase reporter allowed real-time measurement of clock-gene expression in individual flies (Brandes et al. 1996). But even here rapid dampening of per-driven luciferase expression occurred in DD, ruling out the desynchronization hypothesis (Stanewsky et al. 1997b). In turn, this pointed to the 19 presence of multiple oscillators within an individual, which could rapidly run out of phase with each other in the absence of synchronizing time cues. The existence of brain-independent oscillators and moreover their ability to be synchronized by light was demonstrated in several studies (e.g., Plautz et al. 1997; Giebultowicz et al. 2000), further nourishing the intraindividual desynchronization hypothesis. Alternatively, however, it is possible that these “peripheral oscillators” (which we generally use as a source to generate our models: see above) are not able to maintain sustained oscillations in DD. The observed dampening oscillations of per- and timdriven luciferase expression in isolated body tissues favor the latter hypothesis (Plautz et al. 1997; Giebultowicz et al. 2000). Moreover, the amplitude of the only known biological rhythm controlled by a peripheral oscillator – circadian olfactory responses in the fly antennae – is rather low (both in terms of sensitivity changes and of clock-gene expression) and it is at least questionable whether it persists for more than a week in constant conditions (Krishnan et al. 1999, 2001). This brings us back to the fly’s activity rhythm, which we know persists in DD. It is controlled by the LNs, and therefore at least in these cells the molecular cyclings should continue without zeitgebers. This is difficult to prove and would require the behavior of a fly to be monitored, before sacrificing it to determine the amount of a certain clock-gene product. If molecular rhythms indeed underlie the behavioral ones, a certain behavioral activity phase should always be correlated with a certain expression level of a clock gene. One hopes that this kind of experiment will indeed show “lifelong” clock-gene oscillations. Robust fluctuations of TIM protein and vri RNA have been observed in the s-LNv after flies were kept for 2 days in DD (Yang and Sehgal 2001). Strikingly, in the same animals oscillations had dampened to arrhythmicity in the l-LNv, supporting the crucial role for the smaller neurons in controlling locomotor rhythms, whereas the large cells may at best behave like peripheral oscillators. How light interacts with clock-gene products As discussed, the natural light:dark changes keep the different clock-gene-expressing tissues synchronized and they are probably responsible for maintaining rhythmicity as well. How is light interacting with the clock works? TIM is a key player, since it is rapidly degraded in response to light, both in light:dark cycles and after light pulses (Hunter-Ensor et al. 1996; Lee et al. 1996; Myers et al. 1996; Zeng et al. 1996). PER is stabilized by TIM and this observation led to a model of how light resets molecular oscillations. Late in the night, light causes advanced degradation of nuclear TIM (and subsequently PER), resulting in the release of CLK-CYC repression by the PER-TIM dimer (or monomeric PER), so that per and tim transcription can be activated again (Fig. 3). In the early evening, light also causes a reduction in TIM Fig. 3 Light effects on clock molecules. The example shown here illustrates how light pulses in the late night result in a phase advance of clock-gene cyclings. The natural equivalent would be the day by day earlier occurring sunrise in winter and spring. The biological significance of the CRY-PER interaction is not known. In any event, the earlier than usual disappearance of TIM probably results in an earlier decline of PER. As a consequence per and tim transcription start earlier each day. A similar CRY-dependent mechanism is responsible for phase delays induced by light pulses in the early night (or consecutive longer days in winter and spring), and might also contribute to the maintenance of clockgene oscillations under the light-dark condition (for details, see text) (NF postulated “nuclear factor,” TK postulated tyrosine kinase, U ubiquitin). Other symbols and abbreviations are described in the legend to Fig. 2 levels, but here tim RNA levels are high so that the depleted proteins can be replaced by newly synthesized TIM, resulting in phase delays. Since light pulses given at these times have the same qualitative effect on the phase of behavioral rhythms (e.g., Saunders et al. 1994), it was tempting to speculate that the TIM response is connected to the behavioral one. That this is indeed the case was demonstrated by Suri et al. (1998) and Yang et al. (1998), who independently showed that both TIM degradation and behavioral resetting require a similar light intensity and quality. In addition, the latter study showed that this is true for the LNs too, and that the eyes and the canonical phototransduction cascade are not required for TIM degradation within these neurons. This suggested the existence of an extraocular, non-opsinphotoreceptor mediating circadian photoreception, which turned out to be CRYPTOCHROME (Emery et al. 1998, 2000; Stanewsky et al. 1998). Although the eyes – and probably other extraocular photoreceptors – also contribute to entrainment of molecular and behavioral rhythms (Stanewsky et al. 1998; Helfrich-Förster et al. 2001), CRY is the main mediator for light-induced TIM degradation in peripheral tissues and in most of the LN cell types. Strikingly, however, the behaviorally important sLNvs are the exception. In cryb mutant flies, which are devoid of functional CRY, TIM oscillations in these cells and behavioral rhythms can be nicely entrained by LD cycles (Stanewsky et al. 1998; Helfrich-Förster et al. 2001). This suggests that light-dependent, but CRY-inde- 20 pendent mechanisms exist that are able to synchronize biologically meaningful TIM oscillations (cf. Yang et al. 1998). Nevertheless, in peripheral oscillators CRY is clearly crucial for light-induced TIM disappearance: In LD cycles TIM is stable in photoreceptors of cryb flies (Stanewsky et al. 1998) and this phenotype can be reversed by expressing functional CRY in these cells (Emery et al. 2000). In cryb mutant Malpighian tubules and whole-head protein extracts, TIM does not respond to light pulses, again indicating the importance of functional CRY for light-dependent TIM degradation (Ivanchenko et al. 2001; Lin et al. 2001). In fact, lightdependent interactions between TIM and CRY (Ceriani et al. 1999) and also between PER and CRY (Rosato et al. 2001) probably occur in the nuclei of yeast cells. Furthermore, interactions between these proteins are observed in the cytoplasm of Drosophila cells cultured in darkness (Ceriani et al. 1999; Rosato et al. 2001), leading to the model that a nuclear factor binds to CRY, preventing it from binding the two clock proteins in the dark. Light activation of CRY probably results in the dissociation or inactivation of the nuclear factor, allowing the CRY-TIM and CRY-PER interactions to occur (Rosato et al. 2001; Fig. 3). However, mutations in the yeast genome could be induced (probably eliminating the proposed “nuclear factor”), which rendered the nuclear PER-CRY interaction light independent (Rosato et al. 2001). Therefore, it is more likely that PER (and maybe TIM too) and CRY can always bind to each other, unless a light-sensitive nuclear factor abolishes their interaction. A likely result of the CRY-TIM interaction is the phosphorylation of TIM by a tyrosine kinase and subsequent ubiquitination of this clock protein. As a consequence TIM is degraded via the proteasome pathway (Naidoo et al. 1999; Fig. 3). This was further investigated by Lin et al. (2001), who showed that CRY is also degraded by the same pathway in response to light (cf. Emery et al. 1998). CRY degradation requires electron transport, mediated by one of the CRY cofactors, flavin adenine dinucleotide (FAD). Specifically blocking this transfer probably results in the accumulation of activated CRY protein since TIM ubiquitination is increased (Lin et al. 2001). This suggests that light-activated CRY indeed promotes TIM turnover. Whether the CRY-dependent mechanism proposed above is also involved in TIM turnover under constant conditions is unknown. It should be kept in mind that, in addition to the light-dependent tyrosine kinase function, SGG is involved in TIM phosphorylation under constant conditions (Martinek et al. 2001). Nevertheless, in several peripheral tissues a function for CRY under free-running conditions was demonstrated. The circadianly regulated olfactory responses and the clock gene cyclings normally observed in fly antennae become arrhythmic in cryb mutant animals (Krishnan et al. 2001). To rule out that the observed arrhythmicity was due to the lack of light-induced synchronization (caused by cryb) amongst the antennal cells comprising this peripheral oscillator, the animals were entrained by temperature cycles prior to their release into constant conditions. As shown previously, PER and TIM oscillations in the eyes of cryb mutant flies can be entrained by this zeitgeber (Stanewsky et al. 1998). Again, antennae isolated from cryb mutant animals exhibited neither molecular nor sensitivity rhythms in constant conditions, indicating that CRY is a part of the clock in this tissue (Krishnan et al. 2001). A similar result was obtained for another peripheral oscillator, ticking in the Malpighian tubules. Here the cryb mutation also led to constitutive clock gene expression under constant conditions (Ivanchenko et al. 2001). This study also beautifully demonstrated the difference between the clock mechanisms in LN and peripheral tissues: In DD, both PER and TIM cycled with normal amplitude and phase in the LN of cryb mutant larvae in agreement with the normal free-running locomotor period of adult cryb flies (Ivanchenko et al. 2001; cf. Stanewsky et al. 1998). In summary, the CRY-mediated turnover of TIM is likely to be the driving force of the high-amplitude rhythms of clock-gene cyclings observed in peripheral oscillators under LD conditions. No other known clock genes or any of their products are influenced by light directly, but since the different feedback loops are interconnected (see above) the TIM light-response probably helps to amplify molecular oscillations in general. For example, the amplitude of both Clk RNA and protein cycling is enhanced by light (Lee et al. 1999; Bae et al. 1998). Other mechanisms contributing to overall molecular oscillations of clock gene products Transcriptional mechanisms The cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) has been implicated in circadian rhythms both in mammals and flies (Ginty et al. 1993; Ding et al. 1997; Belvin et al. 1999). The Drosophila CREB2 gene encodes a protein, whose activity is circadianly regulated (Belvin et al. 1999). A functional per gene is required for CREB2 rhythmicity, but at the same time CREB2 function is needed for normal rhythmic transcriptional activity of per and normal behavioral rhythmicity (Belvin et al. 1999). That CREB2 indeed affects transcription was indicated by the differential effects a CREB2 mutant had on the real-time expression pattern of transgenic flies transformed with two different per-luciferase reportergene fusions. Cycling of a reporter gene reflecting per’s transcriptional activity was severely blunted, whereas that of a construct which in addition mimics post-transcriptional regulation of per mRNA (see below) exhibited quite robust oscillations in the face of the mutant (Belvin et al. 1999; cf. Stanewsky et al. 1997b). This suggests that CREB2 is part of the regulatory feedback loops, and in addition to CLK and CYC influences per 21 transcription. In fact, three potential CREB-binding sites are present in the per promoter region (Belvin et al. 1999) and one in the tim promoter (Okada et al. 2001). It is not known whether this transcription factor also regulates any of the other clock genes discussed above, but there is evidence that transcriptional regulation by CREB2 contributes to rhythmic gene expression downstream of the central clock (see below). Post-transcriptional regulation of period RNA The first hint that temporal regulation of per RNA involves more than transcriptional regulation mediated by promoter sequences came from a study where the daily profile of endogenous per mRNA was compared with that of a reporter gene transcript driven by per-promoter sequences (Brandes et al. 1996). Surprisingly, the daily upswing of the reporter-RNA was advanced by several hours as compared to that of per RNA, suggesting that the transcriptional and mRNA profiles of per differ. That this is indeed the case was shown by So and Rosbash (1997): the comparison of the temporal profiles of per transcription with those of per mRNA accumulation revealed the same advanced rising phase for the transcript relative to the mature mRNA. This demonstrated that per RNA is post-transcriptionally regulated during its rising phase, most likely by temporal stabilization of the mRNA (So and Rosbash 1997). Further reporter-gene analyses of per-luciferase fusion genes containing various amounts of the transcribed region of per in addition to the promoter revealed that this additional form of regulation is mediated by sequences in per’s 5’-transcribed (but untranslated) region, probably within the first intron (Stanewsky et al. 1997b, 2002). In addition, it was shown that this event depends on functional PER and TIM proteins. In tim01 mutant flies, a pulse of wild-type TIM resulted in a temporally fairly normal accumulation and decline of PER (Suri et al. 1999). This was mediated by a purely post-transcriptionally regulated rise of per RNA, which was also dependent on a functional per gene. Thus, the PER-TIM dimer – in addition to its multiple roles in transcriptional regulation – may also mediate RNA regulation at the post-transcriptional level (Suri et al. 1999). The biological significance of this additional level of regulation is further implied by altered free-running periods of flies containing a per gene lacking intron-1 (Liu et al. 1991; Cooper et al. 1994). Moreover, a per-encoding transgene, completely lacking the promoter region – but retaining parts of intron-1 – is able to restore rhythmic behavior in per01 mutant flies (Frisch et al. 1994), and rhythmic RNA expression within these flies is regulated exclusively post-transcriptionally (So and Rosbash 1997). The mechanism of this regulation is not known, but a different – PER and TIM independent – mechanism of post-transcriptional per mRNA turnover involves an alternative splicing event in per’s 3′-untranslated region (UTR) (Majercak et al. 1999). At low temperatures it results in a phase advance of per mRNA compared to warmer temperatures and is likely to be responsible for adjusting the behavioral pattern to seasonal changes in the daily temperature cycles (Majercak et al. 1999). In contrast, post-transcriptional regulation mediated by the 5′-UTR of per is not influenced by temperature (Stanewsky et al. 2002). Although this suggests a different mechanism, regulation of splicing events involving intron-1 may participate in this form of PER- and TIMdependent RNA regulation (as discussed in Stanewsky et al. 2002). Reaching the end of our discussion concerning “clock mechanisms” in the fly, one is left stunned by the complexity of known processes contributing to clockgene oscillations. It is difficult, and probably unwise, to judge which of the mechanisms is more important than the other. That is because it has repeatedly been demonstrated that a particular mechanism on its own (e.g., post-translational regulation) can mediate behavioral rhythmicity, but in no case neither the molecular nor the behavioral rhythms obtained were even close to normal (Ewer et al. 1988; Frisch et al. 1994; Vosshall and Young 1995; Cheng and Hardin 1998; Yang and Sehgal 2001). A common theme of these studies involves pegging per or tim RNA (or both: Yang and Sehgal 2001) at constant levels by driving their expression with constitutively active promoters in per01 or tim01 mutant animals. Although transcription and overall mRNA levels are probably flat in these flies, rhythmic behavior was observed, likely driven by rhythmic turnover of PER and TIM proteins in the LNs (Ewer et al. 1988; Frisch et al. 1994; Vosshall and Young 1995; Yang and Sehgal 2001). Nevertheless, the fraction of rhythmic flies was far from being 100% and free-running periods were abnormal, usually substantially longer than the typical 24h rhythm. In summary this suggests that all the different clock mechanisms discussed above contribute and are necessary to generate normal behavioral rhythmicity. In the following final part I will summarize our current knowledge about how the temporal information generated by all these processes might be transferred from clock cells to downstream target cells expressing overt circadian rhythms. For detailed discussions regarding clock-output the reader is referred to special reviews dealing with this subject (Williams and Sehgal 2001; Jackson et al. 2001). Mechanisms controlling clock-output Two of the three biological rhythms mentioned in this review, pupal eclosion and adult locomotor activity, depend on clock-gene expression in certain brain neurons (LNs). In contrast, the clock gene-dependent sensitivity rhythm in the fly antennae is probably independent of these neurons, and the rhythm-generating cells are unknown (Krishnan et al. 1999, 2001). Therefore in the lat- 22 ter case it is possible that the rhythm-generating clock cells are identical with those expressing the biological rhythm so that it seems unnecessary to transmit temporal information spatially. But how is the time-of-day information relayed from the LNs to the cells that directly regulate eclosion and activity of the fly? Rhythmicity of both events is regulated by the neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor (PDF), which is expressed within LNs of larvae and LNv of adults. Ectopic PDF expression during development disrupts sustained eclosion rhythms (Helfrich-Förster et al. 2000). In adults, a loss-of-function mutation in the pdf gene results in short-period activity rhythms that gradually become arrhythmic (Renn et al. 1999). Expression of pdf is both transcriptionally and post-transcriptionally regulated by clock genes: In the larval LNs and the adult s-LNv pdf RNA levels are low or undetectable in the face of the cyc01 and ClkJrk mutations, respectively (Park et al. 2000a; Blau and Young 1999), suggesting that CLK and CYC activate pdf transcription. In fact, the pdf promoter contains an E-box consensus-binding site for CLK and CYC, but surprisingly neither do pdf mRNA levels exhibit daily fluctuations (Park and Hall 1998), nor is the E-box required for normal amounts and spatial expression of pdf (Park et al. 2000a). This shows that the transcriptional regulation by CLK and CYC is probably indirect. RNA levels of pdf are unaffected by the per01 and tim01 mutations (Park and Hall 1998; Park et al. 2000a) as well as by overexpression of vri within the larval LNs (Blau and Young 1999). Nevertheless all three gene products affect PDF expression post-transcriptionally. A circadian rhythm of PDF accumulation in the nerve terminals of the s-LNv in the dorsal brain is disrupted in per01 and tim01 mutant flies (Park et al. 2000a), and overall PDF levels in larval LNs are severely reduced in vrioverexpressing animals (Blau and Young 1999). The role of PDF in timing pupal eclosion is not known, but it seems likely that timed release of the neuropeptide serves as a signal for downstream cells expressing other factors involved in eclosion (cf. Jackson et al. 2001). One of those is encoded by the lark gene, which is expressed in a group of crustacean cardioactive peptide (CCAP)-positive cells that are involved in eclosion (for other CCAP cells: see Zhang et al. 2000). Mutations in lark were shown to specifically alter the eclosion rhythms without affecting adult activity rhythms (reviewed by Jackson et al. 1998). Although lark RNA is expressed constitutively, LARK protein expression is circadianly regulated in CCAP cells of late pupae, consistent with its role in regulating rhythmic eclosion (Jackson et al. 1998; Zhang et al. 2000). Surprisingly, in the adult brain PDF is required for accumulation of normal levels of MAP kinase, suggesting that the Ras/MAPK-signaling pathway is the next downstream target of the clock and activated by PDF (Williams et al. 2001). In fact, mutations in the Neurofibromatosis-1 (Nf1) gene, which encodes a Ras-GTPaseactivating protein, result in arrhythmic behavioral activi- ty. MAP kinase levels are upregulated in Nf1 mutants and the behavioral phenotype can be rescued by mutations that downregulate MAP kinase signaling, further establishing a role for this signaling pathway in the output of the circadian clock (Williams et al. 2001). In addition, signaling by PKA is involved in the output controlling adult activity rhythms since mutations in the catalytic subunit of PKA (encoded by the DCO gene), or its regulatory subunit RII, result in arrhythmic behavior (Levine et al. 1994; Majercak et al. 1997; Park et al. 2000b). The cells in which these signaling pathways act (and probably interact: Williams et al. 2001) to control activity rhythms are unknown, but future studies will certainly address these questions. In summary, similar to the feedback loops comprising the central clockworks, the known output mechanisms also involve regulatory processes at different levels. Microarray analyses as well as molecular and genetic approaches aimed at determining the extent of circadianly regulated gene expression suggested that about 1–6% of all Drosophila genes are rhythmically expressed (Van Gelder et al. 1995; Claridge-Chang et al. 2001; McDonald and Rosbash 2001; Stempfl et al. 2002; Ueda et al. 2002). One way to generate these rhythms would be via direct transcriptional regulation through clock genes, but the few examples discussed above indicate that this is rather unlikely. In fact McDonald and Rosbash (2001) found only seven genes (in addition to tim and vri) that seem to be direct targets of CLK, indicating that the vast majority of transcript oscillations are controlled indirectly. Moreover, Stempfl et al. (2002) showed that the rhythmic activity of certain circadianly regulated enhancers and expression of oscillating transcripts can be affected differentially by the two clock mutants per01 and tim01, pointing to novel mechanisms of gene regulation in the clock output. Similar conclusions were drawn from a study of per01, tim01, and ClkJrk effects on RNA levels of usually rhythmically expressed genes. Considering the transcriptional feedback loops discussed above, it was expected that levels of a given transcript were either low in per01 and tim01 mutant backgrounds and high in ClkJrk, or vice versa. Those cases were found, but in addition a similar number of genes were equally influenced by all three clock mutations (either up- or downregulated) (Claridge-Chang et al. 2001). Nevertheless, the enrichment of binding sites for transcription factors with a known function in the circadian clock (e.g., Eboxes and CREB-binding sites) in the promoters of clock-regulated genes argues in favor of widespread transcriptional control (Claridge-Chang et al. 2001; Stempfl et al. 2002). This does not imply that the same molecules and mechanisms as in the central clock are used in the output pathway. In fact, the transcriptional regulation of pdf and the rare direct CLK targets argue against this view (see above). Importantly, the “mass identification” of novel circadianly regulated genes revealed insight into the diversity of processes likely to be regulated by the clock. These involve functions like vision, olfaction, mechanorecep- 23 tion, detoxification, learning and memory, ion-channel activity, and certain metabolic events (Claridge-Chang et al. 2001; McDonald and Rosbash 2001; Stempfl et al. 2002; Ueda et al. 2002). Moreover, reproduction has been shown to be regulated by the clock, in terms of both mating behavior and physiological processes underlying overall reproductive fitness (Sakai and Ishida 2001; Beaver et al. 2002). Studying the temporal aspects of these processes will certainly help to understand how the circadian clock regulates these rhythms in order to optimize the overall fitness of organisms. Acknowledgements I thank Charlotte Helfrich-Förster and the members of my laboratory for critical comments on the manuscript, and Patrick Emery for discussions. References Allada R, White NE, So WV, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1998) A mutant Drosophila homolog of mammalian Clock disrupts circadian rhythms and transcription of period and timeless. Cell 93:791–804 Bae K, Lee C, Sidote D, Chang K-Y, Edery I (1998) Circadian regulation of a Drosophila homolog of the mammalian Clock gene: PER and TIM function as positive regulators. Mol Cell Biol 18:6142–6151 Bae K, Lee C, Hardin PE, Edery I (2000) dCLOCK is present in limiting amounts and likely mediates daily interactions between the dCLOCK-CYC transcription factor and the PERTIM complex. J Neurosci 20:1746–1753 Bao S, Rihel J, Bjes E, Fan J-Y, Price JL (2001) The Drosophila double-timeS mutation delays the nuclear accumulation of period protein and affects the feedback regulation of period mRNA. J Neurosci 21:7117–7126 Beaver LM, Gvakharia BO, Vollintine TS, Hege DM, Stanewsky R, Giebultowicz JM (2002) Loss of circadian clock function decreases reproductive fitness in males of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:2134–2139 Belvin MP, Zhou H, Yin JCP (1999) The Drosophila dCREB2 gene affects the circadian clock. Neuron 22:777–787 Blanchardon E, Grima B, Klarsfeld A, Chelot E, Hardin PE, Préat T, Rouyer F (2001) Defining the role of Drosophila lateral neurons in the control of activity and eclosion rhythms by targeted genetic ablation and PERIOD overexpression. Eur J Neurosci 13:871–888 Blau J (2001) The Drosophila circadian clock: what we know and what we don’t know. Semin Cell Dev Biol 12:287–293 Blau J, Young MW (1999) Cycling vrille expression is required for a functional Drosophila clock. Cell 99:661–671 Brandes C, Plautz JD, Stanewsky R, Jamison CF, Straume M, Wood KV, Kay SA, Hall JC (1996) Novel features of Drosophila period transcription revealed by real-time luciferase reporting. Neuron 16:687–692 Brett WJ (1955) Persistent diurnal rhythmicity in Drosophila emergence. Ann Entomol Soc Am 48:119–131 Ceriani MF, Darlington TK, Staknis D, Más P, Petti AA, Weitz CJ, Kay SA (1999) Light-dependent sequestration of TIMELESS by CRYPTOCHROME. Science 285:553–556 Cheng Y, Hardin PE (1998) Drosophila photoreceptors contain an autonomous circadian oscillator that can function without period mRNA cycling. J Neurosci 18:741–750 Claridge-Chang A, Wijnen H, Naef F, Boothroyd C, Rajewsky N, Young MW (2001) Circadian regulation of gene expression systems in the Drosophila head. Neuron 32:657–671 Cooper MK, Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Liu X, Rutila JE, Hall JC (1994) Dosage compensation of the period gene in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 138:721–732 Crews ST, Fan C-M (1999) Remembrance of things PAS: regulation of development by bHLH-PAS proteins. Curr Opin Genet Dev 9:580–587 Curtin KD, Huang ZJ, Rosbash M (1995) Temporally regulated nuclear entry of the Drosophila period protein contributes to the circadian clock. Neuron 14:363–372 Darlington T, Wager-Smith K, Ceriani MF, Staknis D, Gekakis N, Steeves T, Weitz C, Takahashi J, Kay SA (1998) Closing the circadian loop: CLOCK-induced transcription of its own inhibitors, per and tim. Science 280:1599–1603 Dembinska ME, Stanewsky R, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1997) Circadian cycling of a period-lacZ fusion protein in Drosophila: evidence for an instability cycling element in PER. J Biol Rhythms 12:157–172 Ding JM, Faiman LE, Hurst WJ, Kuriashkina LR, Gillette MU (1997) Resetting the biological clock: mediation of nocturnal CREB phosphorylation via light, glutamate, and nitric oxide. J Neurosci 17:667–675 Dushay MS, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1989) The disconnected visual system mutations in Drosophila drastically disrupt circadian rhythms. J Biol Rhythms 4:1–27 Dushay MS, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1992) Mapping the Clock mutation rhythm mutation to the period locus of Drosophila melanogaster by germline transformation. J Neurogenet 8:173– 179 Edery I, Zwiebel LJ, Dembinska ME, Rosbash M (1994) Temporal phosphorylation of the Drosophila period protein. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 91:2260–2264 Emery P, So WV, Kaneko M, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1998) CRY, a Drosophila clock and light-regulated cryptochrome, is a major contributor to circadian rhythm resetting and photosensitivity. Cell 95:669–679 Emery P, Stanewsky R, Helfrich-Förster C, Emery-Le M, Hall JC, Rosbash M (2000) Drosophila CRY is a deep brain circadian photoreceptor. Neuron 26:493–504 Ewer J, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1988) An inducible promoter fused to the period gene in Drosophila conditionally rescues adult per-mutant arrhythmicity. Nature 333:82–84 Ewer J, Frisch B, Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1992) Expression of the period clock gene within different cell types in the brain of Drosophila adults and mosaic analysis of these cells’ influence on circadian behavioral rhythms. J Neurosci 12:3321–3349 Foster RG, Helfrich-Förster C (2001) The regulation of circadian clocks by light in fruitflies and mice. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 356:1779–1789 Frisch B, Hardin PE, Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1994) A promoterless period gene mediates behavioral rhythmicity and cyclical per expression in a restricted subset of the Drosophila nervous system. Neuron 12:555–570 Gekakis N, Saez L, Delahaye-Brown A-M, Myers MP, Sehgal A, Young MW, Weitz CJ (1995) Isolation of timeless by PER protein interaction: defective interaction between timeless protein and long-period mutant PERL. Science 270:811–815 George H, Terracol R (1997) The vrille gene of Drosophila is a maternal enhancer of decapentaplegic and encodes a new member of the bZIP family of transcription factors. Genetics 146:1345–1363 Giebultowicz JM, Stanewsky R, Hall JC, Hege DM (2000) Transplanted Drosophila excretory tubules maintain circadian clock cycling out of phase with the host. Curr Biol 10:107–110 Ginty DD, Kornhauser JM, Thompson MA, Bading H, Mayo KE, Takahashi JS, Greenberg ME (1993) Regulation of CREB phosphorylation in the suprachiasmatic nucleus by light and a circadian clock. Science 260:238–241 Glossop NRJ, Lyons LC, Hardin PE (1999) Interlocked feedback loops within the Drosophila circadian oscillator. Science 286:766–768 Hall JC (1995) Tripping along the trail to the molecular mechanisms of biological clocks. Trends Neurosci 18:230–240 Hamblen MJ, White NE, Emery PTJ, Kaiser K, Hall JC (1998) Molecular and behavioral analysis of four period mutants in 24 Drosophila melanogaster encompassing extreme short, novel long, and unorthodox arrhythmic types. Genetics 149:165–178 Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Wheeler DA, Rutila JE, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1992) Behavior of period-altered circadian rhythm mutants of Drosophila in light:dark cycles. J Insect Behav 5:417–446 Hardin PE, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1990) Feedback of the Drosophila period gene product on circadian cycling of its messenger RNA levels. Nature 343:536–540 Hardin PE, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1992) Behavioral and molecular analyses suggest that circadian output is disrupted by disconnected mutants in D. melanogaster. EMBO J 11:1–6 Hege DM, Stanewsky R, Hall JC, Giebultowicz JM (1997) Rhythmic expression of a PER-reporter in the Malpighian tubules of decapitated Drosophila: evidence for a brain-independent circadian clock. J Biol Rhythms 12:300–308 Helfrich-Förster C (1995) The period clock gene is expressed in CNS neurons which also produce a neuropeptide that reveals the projections of circadian pacemaker cells within the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:612–616 Helfrich-Förster C (1998) Robust circadian rhythmicity of Drosophila melanogaster requires the presence of lateral neurons: a brain-behavioral study of disconnected mutants. J Comp Physiol A 182:435–453 Helfrich-Förster C (2002) The neuroarchitecture of the circadian clock in the Drosophila brain. Microsc Res Tech (in press) Helfrich-Förster C, Täuber M, Park JH, Mühlig-Versen M, Schneuwly S, Hofbauer A (2000) Ectopic expression of the neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor alters behavioral rhythms in Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurosci 20:3339– 3353 Helfrich-Förster C, Winter C, Hofbauer A, Hall JC, Stanewsky R (2001) The circadian clock of fruit flies is blind after elimination of all known photoreceptors. Neuron 30:249–261 Hunter-Ensor M, Ousley A, Sehgal A (1996) Regulation of the Drosophila protein timeless suggests a mechanism for resetting the circadian clock by light. Cell 84:677–685 Ivanchenko M, Stanewsky R, Giebultowicz JM (2001) Circadian photoreception in Drosophila: functions of cryptochrome in peripheral and central clocks. J Biol Rhythms 16:205–215 Jackson FR, Zhang X, McNeil GP (1998) Oscillating molecules and circadian clock output mechanisms. Mol Psychiatry 3:381–385 Jackson FR, Schroeder AJ, Roberts MA, McNeil GP, Kume K, Akten B (2001) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of circadian control in insects. J Insect Phys 47:833–842 Jin X, Shearman L, Weaver D, Zylka M, DeVries G, Reppert SM (1999) A molecular mechanism regulating output from the suprachiasmatic circadian clock. Cell 96:57–68 Kaneko M, Hall JC (2000) Neuroanatomy of cells expressing clock genes in Drosophila: transgenic manipulation of the period and timeless genes to mark the perikarya of circadian pacemaker neurons and their projections. J Comp Neurol 422:66–94 Kaneko M, Helfrich-Förster C, Hall JC (1997) Spatial and temporal expression of the period and timeless genes in the developing nervous system of Drosophila: newly identified pacemaker candidates and novel features of clock gene product cycling. J Neurosci 17:6745–6760 Kaneko M, Park JH, Cheng Y, Hardin PE, Hall JC (2000a) Disruption of synaptic transmission or clock-gene-product oscillations in circadian pacemaker cells of Drosophila cause abnormal behavioral rhythms. J Neurobiol 43:207–233 Kaneko M, Hamblen MJ, Hall JC (2000b) Involvement of the period gene in developmental time-memory: effect of the perShort mutation on phase shifts induced by light pulses delivered to Drosophila larvae. J Biol Rhythms 15:13–30 Kloss B, Price JL, Saez L, Blau J, Rothenfluh A, Young MW (1998) The Drosophila clock gene double-time encodes a protein closely related to human casein kinase Iεe. Cell 94:97–107 Kloss B, Rothenfluh A, Young MW, Saez L (2001) Phosphorylation of PERIOD is influenced by cycling physical associations of DOUBLE-TIME, PERIOD, and TIMELESS in the Drosophila clock. Neuron 30:699–706 Konopka RJ, Benzer S (1971) Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 68:2112–2116 Konopka RJ, Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Jamison CF, Hall JC (1994) An ultrashort clock mutation at the period locus of Drosophila melanogaster that reveals some new features of the fly’s circadian system. J Biol Rhythms 9:189–216 Krishnan B, Dryer SE, Hardin PE (1999) Circadian rhythms in olfactory responses of Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 400: 375–378 Krishnan B, Levine JD, Lynch KS, Dowse HB, Funes P, Hall JC, Hardin PE, Dryer SE (2001) A new role for cryptochrome in a Drosophila circadian oscillator. Nature 411:313–317 Kyriacou CP, Hastings M (2001) Keystone clocks. Trends Neurosci 24:434–435 Kyriacou CP, Rosato E (2000) Squaring up the E-box. J Biol Rhythms 15:483–490 Lee C, Bae K, Edery I (1998) The Drosophila CLOCK protein undergoes daily rhythms in abundance, phosphorylation and interactions with the PER-TIM complex. Neuron 21:857– 867 Lee C, Bae K, Edery I (1999) PER and TIM inhibit the DNA binding activity of a Drosophila CLOCK-CYC/dBMAL1 heterodimer without disrupting formation of the heterodimer: a basis for circadian transcription. Mol Cell Biol 19:5316–5325 Lee CG, Parikh V, Itsukaichi T, Bae K, Edery I (1996) Resetting the Drosophila clock by photic regulation of PER and a PERTIM complex. Science 271:1740–1744 Levine JD, Casey CI, Kalderon DD, Jackson FR (1994) Altered circadian pacemaker functions and cyclic AMP rhythms in the Drosophila learning mutant dunce. Neuron 13:967–974 Lin F-J, Song W, Meyer-Bernstein E, Naidoo N, Sehgal A (2001) Photic signaling by cryptochrome in the Drosophila circadian system. Mol Cell Biol 21:7287–7294 Liu X, Yu Q, Huang Z, Zwiebel LJ, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1991) The strength and periodicity of D. melanogaster circadian rhythms are differentially affected by alterations in period gene expression. Neuron 6:753–766 Majercak J, Kalderon D, Edery I (1997) Drosophila melanogaster deficient in protein kinase A manifests behavior-specific arrhythmia but normal clock function. Mol Cell Biol 17:5915– 5922 Majercak J, Sidote D, Hardin PE, Edery I (1999) How a circadian clock adapts to seasonal decreases in temperature and day length. Neuron 24:219–230 Martinek S, Inonog S, Manoukian AS, Young MW (2001) A role for the segment polarity gene shaggy/GSK-3 in the Drosophila circadian clock. Cell 105:769–779 McDonald MJ, Rosbash M (2001) Microarray analysis and organization of circadian gene expression in Drosophila. Cell 107:567–578 Myers MP, Wagner-Smith K, Wesley CS, Young MW, Sehgal A (1995) Positional cloning and sequence analysis of the Drosophila clock gene, timeless. Science 270:805–808 Myers MP, Wager-Smith K, Rothenfluh-Hilfiker A, Young MW (1996) Light-induced degradation of TIMELESS and entrainment of the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 271:1736– 1740 Naidoo N, Song W, Hunter-Ensor M, Sehgal A (1999) A role for the proteasome in the light response of the timeless clock protein. Science 285:1737–1741 Newby LM, Jackson FR (1993) A new biological rhythm mutant of Drosophila melanogaster that identifies a gene with an essential embryonic function. Genetics 135:1077–1090 Okada T, Sakai T, Murata T, Kako K, Sakamoto K, Ohtomi M, Katsura T, Ishida N (2001) Promoter analysis for daily expression of Drosophila timeless gene. Biochem Biophys Res Comm 283:577–582 Park JH, Hall JC (1998) Isolation and chronobiological analysis of a neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor gene in Drosophila melanogaster. J Biol Rhythms 13:219–228 25 Park JH, Helfrich-Förster C, Lee G, Liu L, Rosbash M, Hall JC (2000a) Differential regulation of circadian pacemaker output by separate clock genes in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:3608–3613 Park SK, Sedore SA, Cronmiller C, Hirsh J (2000b) Type II cAMP-dependent protein kinase-deficient Drosophila are viable but show developmental, circadian, and drug response phenotypes. J Biol Chem 275:20588–20596 Plautz JD, Kaneko M, Hall JC, Kay SA (1997) Independent photoreceptive circadian clocks throughout Drosophila. Science 278:1632–1635 Price JL, Dembinska ME, Young MW, Rosbash M (1995) Suppression of PERIOD protein abundance and circadian cycling by the Drosophila clock mutation timeless. EMBO J 14:4044–4049 Price JL, Blau J, Rothenfluh A, Abodeely M, Kloss B, Young MW (1998) double-time is a new Drosophila clock gene that regulates PERIOD protein accumulation. Cell 94:83–95 Renn SCP, Park JH, Rosbash M, Hall JC, Taghert PH (1999) A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell 99:791–802 Roenneberg T, Foster RG (1997) Twilight times: light and the circadian system. Photochem Photobiol 66:549–561 Rosato E, Codd V, Mazzotta G, Piccin A, Zordan M, Costa R, Kyriacou CP (2001) Light-dependent interaction between Drosophila CRY and the clock protein PER mediated by the carboxy terminus of CRY. Curr Biol 11:909–917 Rothenfluh A, Abodeely M, Price JL, Young MW (2000a) Isolation and analysis of six timeless alleles that cause short- or long-period circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Genetics 156: 665–675 Rothenfluh A, Young MW, Saez L (2000b) A TIMELESS-independent function for PERIOD proteins in the Drosophila clock. Neuron 26:505–514 Rothenfluh A, Abodeely M, Young MW (2000c) Short-period mutations of per affect a double-time-dependent step in the Drosophila circadian clock. Curr Biol 10:1399–1402 Rutila JE, Zeng H, Le M, Curtin KD, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1996) The timSL mutant of the Drosophila rhythm gene timeless manifests allele-specific interactions with period gene mutants. Neuron 17:921–929 Rutila JE, Suri V, Le M, So WV, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1998) CYCLE is a second bHLH-PAS clock protein essential for circadian rhythmicity and transcription of Drosophila period and timeless. Cell 93:805–814 Saez L, Young MW (1996) Regulated nuclear localization of the Drosophila clock proteins PERIOD and TIMELESS. Neuron 17:911–920 Sakai T, Ishida N (2001) Circadian rhythms of female mating activity governed by clock genes in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:9221–9225 Saunders DS, Gillanders SW, Lewis RD (1994) Light-pulse phase response curves for the locomotor activity rhythm in period mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Physiol 40: 957–968 Sehgal A, Price J, Young MW (1992) Ontogeny of a biological clock in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:1423– 1427 Sehgal A, Price JL, Man B, Young MW (1994) Loss of circadian behavioral rhythms and per RNA oscillations in the Drosophila mutant timeless. Science 263:1603–1606 Sehgal A, Rothenfluh-Hilfiker A, Hunter-Ensor M, Chen Y, Myers MP, Young MW (1995) Rhythmic expression of timeless: a basis for promoting circadian cycles in period gene autoregulation. Science 270:808–810 Siegfried E, Chou T-B, Perrimon N (1992) wingless signaling acts through zeste-white 3, the Drosophila homolog of glycogen synthase kinase-3, to regulate engrained and establish cell fate. Cell 71:1167–1179 Shafer OT (2001) Blind clocks reveal elusive light input pathway in Drosophila. Trends Neurosci 24:627–628 So WV, Rosbash M (1997) Post-transcriptional regulation contributes to Drosophila clock gene mRNA cycling. EMBO J 16: 7146–7155 So WV, Sarov-Blat L, Kotarski CK, McDonald MJ, Allada R, Rosbash M (2000) takeout, a novel Drosophila gene under circadian clock transcriptional regulation. Mol Cell Biol 20: 6935–6944 Stanewsky R, Frisch B, Brandes C, Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1997a) Temporal and spatial expression patterns of transgenes containing increasing amounts of the Drosophila clock gene period and a lacZ reporter: mapping elements of the PER protein involved in circadian cycling. J Neurosci 17:676–696 Stanewsky R, Jamison CF, Plautz JD, Kay SA, Hall JC (1997b) Multiple circadian-regulated elements contribute to cycling period gene expression in Drosophila. EMBO J 16:5006– 5018 Stanewsky R, Kaneko M, Emery P, Beretta B, Wager-Smith K, Kay SA, Rosbash M, Hall JC (1998) The cryb mutation identifies cryptochrome as a circadian photoreceptor in Drosophila. Cell 95:681–692 Stanewsky R, Lynch KS, Brandes C, Hall JC (2002) Mapping of elements involved in regulating normal temporal period and timeless RNA-expression patterns in Drosophila melanogaster. J Biol Rhythms (in press) Stempfl T, Vogel M, Szabo G, Wülbeck C, Liu J, Hall JC, Stanewsky R (2002) Identification of circadian-clock regulated enhancers and genes of Drosophila melanogaster by transposon mobilization and luciferase reporting of cyclical gene expression. Genetics 160:571–593 Suri V, Qian Z, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1998) Evidence that the TIM light response is relevant to light-induced phase shifts in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron 21:225–234 Suri V, Lanjuin A, Rosbash M (1999) TIMELESS-dependent positive and negative autoregulation in the Drosophila circadian clock. EMBO J 18:675–686 Suri V, Hall JC, Rosbash M (2000) Two novel doubletime mutants alter circadian properties and eliminate the delay between RNA and protein in Drosophila. J Neurosci 20:7547–7555 Tomioka K, Sakamoto M, Harui Y, Matsumoto N, Matsumoto A (1998) Light and temperature cooperate to regulate the circadian locomotor rhythm of wild type and period mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Physiol 44:587–596 Ueda HR, Matsumoto A, Kawamura M, Iino M, Tanimura T, Hashimoto S (2002) Genome-wide transcriptional orchestration of circadian rhythms in Drosophila. J Biol Chem 277: 14048–14052 Van Gelder RN, Bae H, Palazzolo MJ, Krasnow MA (1995) Extent and character of circadian gene expression in Drosophila melanogaster: identification of twenty oscillating mRNAs in the fly head. Curr Biol 5:1424–1436 Vosshall LB, Young MW (1995) Circadian rhythms in Drosophila can be driven by period expression in a restricted group of central brain cells. Neuron 15:345–360 Vosshall LB, Price JL, Sehgal A, Saez L, Young MW (1994) Block in nuclear localization of period protein by a second clock mutation, timeless. Science 263:1606–1609 Wheeler DA, Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Dushay MS, Hall JC (1993) Behavior in light-dark cycles of Drosophila mutants that are blind, arrhythmic, or both. J Biol Rhythms 8:67–94 Williams JA, Sehgal A (2001) Molecular components of the circadian system in Drosophila. Ann Rev Physiol 63:729–755 Williams JA, Su HS, Bernards A, Field J, Sehgal A (2001) A circadian output in Drosophila mediated by neurofibromatosis-1 and Ras/MAPK. Science 293:2251–2256 Yang Z, Sehgal A (2001) Role of molecular oscillations in generating behavioral rhythms in Drosophila. Neuron 29:453– 467 Yang Z, Emerson M, Su HS, Sehgal A (1998) Response of the timeless protein to light correlates with behavioral entrainment and suggests separate pathways for visual and circadian photoreception. Neuron 21:215–223 26 Zeng H, Hardin PE, Rosbash M (1994) Constitutive overexpression of the Drosophila period protein inhibits period mRNA cycling. EMBO J 13:3590–3598 Zeng H, Qian Z, Myers MP, Rosbash M (1996) A light-entrainment mechanism for the Drosophila circadian clock. Nature 380:129–135 Zerr DM, Hall JC, Rosbash M, Siwicki KK (1990) Circadian fluctuations of period protein immunoreactivity in the CNS and the visual system of Drosophila. J Neurosci 10:2749–2762 Zhang X, McNeil GP, Hilderbrand-Chae MJ, Franklin TM, Schroeder AJ, Jackson FR (2000) Circadian regulation of the Lark RNA-binding protein within identifiable neurosecretory cells. J Neurobiol 45:14–29 Zilian O, Frei E, Burke R, Brentrup D, Gutjahr T, Bryant PJ, Noll M (1999) double-time is identical to discs overgrown, which is required for cell survival, proliferation and growth arrest in Drosophila imaginal discs. Development 126:5409– 5420 Zordan MA, Rosato E, Piccin A, Foster R (2001) Photic entrainment of the circadian clock: from Drosophila to mammals. Semin Cell Dev Biol 12:317–328 Zwiebel LJ, Hardin PE, Liu X, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1991) A posttranscriptional mechanism contributes to circadian cycling of a per-βb-galactosidase fusion protein. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88:3882–3886