THE PURSUIT OF POSITIVE AFFECT IN TASK ADVICE NETWORKS: EFFECTS ON INDIVIDUAL PERFORMANCE Tiziana Casciaro University of Toronto March 2014 Working draft. Please do not circulate or cite. THE PURSUIT OF POSITIVE AFFECT IN TASK ADVICE NETWORKS: EFFECTS ON INDIVIDUAL PERFORMANCE ABSTRACT Research has demonstrated that people in organizations form advice networks not only for access to task knowledge, but also for the positive affect they derive from social interaction, so much so that they are willing to trade off task competence for the personal liking, pleasantness and energy experienced in interactions with colleagues. The effects of such relational choices on individual performance are poorly understood, however. I propose a theory of affect in advice networks according to which an individual’s propensity to form task-related advice ties based on positive affect experienced when interacting with a colleague enhances individual task performance. I argue that this effect changes depending on the level of activation (a feeling of being energized) generated by those interactions. Compared to forms of positive affect with neutral-to-moderate levels of activation (feelings of enjoyment), the preferential selection of work partners that elicit high-activation positive affect enhances task performance by stimulating an individual to engage in task-oriented effort. Social-network and individual performance data on 430 technology salespeople in a global IT corporation provide support for this theory. I discuss implications for network research and rational action in organizations. 1 INTRODUCTION As social entities whose raison d’être is enabling the collective performance of tasks that individuals cannot perform in isolation, organizations are purposefully designed to induce interactions between members with complementary expertise. Organizational theorists have therefore been concerned with how to facilitate desired patterns of interaction to integrate the differentiated competencies of organization members (Barnard, 1938; Blau & Schoenherr, 1971; Lawrence & Lorsch, 1967; Mintzberg, 1979; Thompson, 1967). The social network of taskrelated advice that an individual forms with other organization members is a central mechanism for such knowledge integration and the execution of interdependent tasks (Baldwin, Bedell, & Johnson, 1997; Gargiulo, Ertug, & Galunic, 2009; Sparrowe, Liden, Wayne, & Kraimer, 2001). How people go about seeking colleagues out for advice, therefore, is relevant to their ability to carry out assigned tasks. Much evidence indicates that, in forming advice networks to perform their job, people tend to seek coworkers out not only for task knowledge, but also for affective inputs related to the hedonic rewards of social interaction (Bales, 1958; Hinds, Carley, Krackhardt, & Wholey, 2000; Homans, 1961; Krackhardt, 1999; Krackhardt & Stern, 1998; Roethlisberger & Dickson, 1939; Slater, 1955). The coexistence of affective and instrumental motivations in advice networks is so prevalent that, across organizational contexts and tasks, people have been shown to trade off task competence for the personal liking, pleasantness and energy experienced in interactions with colleagues (Casciaro & Lobo, 2008). How does the tendency to pursue positive affect in task-advice networks influence individual performance? Existing theory offers ambivalent arguments in this regard. The intertwining of instrumental and affective motivations in how people in organizations seek colleagues out for task advice has been portrayed as a deviation from desired task-oriented 2 behaviour, whereby a logic of sentiments supplements and erodes the logic of cost and efficiency represented by task requirements (Barnard, 1938; Merton, 1957; Roethlisberger & Dickson, 1939). This is because positive affect in social relationships tends to develop for reasons—such as attribute similarity—that are tangential to task execution (Ibarra, 1992; Lincoln & Miller, 1979). Consistent with these arguments, performance benefits that individuals derive from their position in organizational networks have been documented primarily for instrumental networks, rather than expressive ones (for reviews, see Borgatti & Foster, 2003; Brass, Glaskiewicz, Greve, & Tsai, 2004). For these reasons, people’s tendency to form task advice ties with colleagues based on the positive emotions they feel for them may leave a reservoir of knowledge untapped in organizations, potentially dampening performance (Casciaro & Lobo, 2008). Yet, network research has also demonstrated that the affective content of social relationships can facilitate organizational processes relevant to task execution, including the transfer of useful knowledge (Levin & Cross, 2004) and the adaptive response to organizational crises (Krackhardt & Stern, 1998). Although these studies did not investigate how forming task-related advice ties based on affective considerations influences individual performance, they pose a fundamental question about relational choice in organizations. Does forging task advice ties for the increase in positive affect experienced in interactions with colleagues hinder or benefit individual performance? In this paper, I argue that an individual’s propensity to form task-related advice ties based on positive affect experienced when interacting with a colleague enhances individual task performance. This prediction reflects the central role of affect and motivation in performance: affect serves a fundamental motivational function by stimulating engagement with and effort toward assigned tasks (Buck, 1988), thus increasing the likelihood that they will be successfully performed. Not all forms of positive affect that people draw from social interactions enhance 3 performance, however. I argue that the effect of positive affect experienced in interactions with colleagues changes depending on the level of activation (i.e., a feeling of being energized) generated by those interactions. It is specifically the choice of social interactions that induce a state of high-activation positive affect—excitement (Izard, 1991), emotional energy (Collins, 1993) or energetic activation (Thayer, 1989)—that enhances task performance by stimulating task-oriented engagement and effort. I test this argument with social network and individual performance data on 430 technology salespeople operating in a multinational computer, technology and IT consulting corporation headquartered in the United States. POSITIVE AFFECT, ADVICE SEEKING, AND PERFORMANCE The formal roles individuals perform in organizations require patterns of interaction with colleagues that organization members may not willingly choose (Barnard, 1938; Blau & Schoenherr, 1971; Lawrence & Lorsch, 1967; Mintzberg, 1979; Thompson, 1967). Within these formal constraints, however, emergent task advice networks also reflect people’s discretionary choice as to whom they wish to seek out as a source of task knowledge. Such choices are driven by an individual’s subjective assessment of the resources embedded in interactions with different coworkers (Merton, 1957). Network scholars have identified several criteria people follow in forming and maintaining task advice relationships. These criteria concern cognitive evaluations of the expected instrumental value of task-advice interactions with a colleague (as indicated by a colleague’s perceived task competence), and the expected accessibility of task advice (as indicated by colleagues’ perceived willingness to share their knowledge and by their physical proximity) (Borgatti & Cross, 2003; Hinds et al., 2000; Nebus, 2006). In addition to these 4 cognitive judgments of a colleague’s ability and willingness to provide task-relevant input, affective judgments related to the affective rewards of a social relationships have also been linked to the formation of task advice ties, such that people tend to seek out for task-related input those with whom they experience positive affect (Bales, 1958; Hinds et al., 2000; Homans, 1961; Krackhardt, 1999; Krackhardt & Stern, 1998; Roethlisberger & Dickson, 1939; Slater, 1955). While we have clear theory and evidence regarding the criteria people tend to use as they develop task advice relationships in an organization, we lack understanding of the consequences for performance of building advice ties based on the positive emotions experienced in a social relationship with a colleague. I propose that using positive affect as a criterion for task-advice ties can be beneficial for individual performance. More precisely, I argue that seeking out for task-related advice colleagues with whom an individual experiences an emotional state of positive activation may increase the probability of successful task execution by stimulating the advice seeker to engage in task-oriented effort. Theoretical foundations for this argument rest in the microsociology of social interaction advanced by Collins (1981). Collins argues that emotional energy is the main motivating force in social life. It provides the impetus for working, consuming, investing, and any other human activity. An individual experiences an increase or a decrease in emotional energy based on how social encounters unfold through interaction rituals. This is how the network of social relationships in an organization drives the energy that people feel at work (Cross, Baker, & Parker, 2003). An individual who negotiates social interactions successfully acquires an increment of positive emotional energy (Collins, 1981). “High levels of emotional energy in an individual consist of feelings of enthusiasm and confidence; low levels are manifested as apathy and depression” (Collins, 1993: 211). People preferentially pursue social 5 encounters that increase their emotional energy, and the confidence and enthusiasm an individual derives from social interactions stimulates action based on the expectation that a desirable future can be brought into the present (Barbalet, 1998; Collins, 1993). Collins’ notion of emotional energy portrays relational choice and human activity as the result of a subjective feeling of confidence and enthusiasm that an individual derives from social interactions. Applied to advice seeking and individual performance, this argument implies that people will seek colleagues out for task-related advice based on the feeling of excitement experienced in interaction with them. Because emotional energy is synonymous with motivation for action, in the context of relationships formed to seek out task-related advice, emotional energy may serve as a stimulus for task-oriented action, hence boosting task performance, under the assumption that engagement and effort increase the likelihood of executing tasks successfully. Research on the psychology of affect adds precision to Collins’ definition of emotional energy. Affect refers to consciously accessible feelings (Fredrickson, 2001), and can be defined in terms of two dimensions: valence and arousal (Diener & Emmons, 1984; Russell, 1979, 1980; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). Valence (or hedonic tone) is a subjective feeling of pleasantness and unpleasantness. Arousal is a subjective state of feeling activated or deactivated. The positive affective resources an individual draws from interactions with another person, therefore, can be characterized in terms of varying degrees of activation. Positive valence with high activation represents an excited, enthusiastic mood. This is the affective state that Collins (1981, 1993) refers to as emotional energy. A positive mood with high activation corresponds to a discrete emotion of excitement (Cropanzano, Weiss, Hale, & Reb, 2003; Izard, 1991), which is a subjective state of “feeling energized” based on the expectation of future rewards (Izard, 1991; 6 Lawler & Yoon, 1996). Positive valance with neutral-to-moderate activation represents instead a pleasant, contented, happy mood. A positive mood with neutral-to-moderate activation corresponds to a discrete emotion of enjoyment (Cropanzano et al., 2003; Izard, 1991), which is a subjective state of “feeling gratified” due to rewards received through the interaction (Izard, 1991; Lawler & Yoon, 1996). I propose that the level of activation in the positive affect that an individual draws from social interactions is consequential for task performance due to its effects on task engagement and effort. Collins emphasis on emotional energy—rather than any generic form of positive emotion—as an engine of human activity is echoed by research in psychology and organizational behavior. In general, high-activation emotions (both positive and negative) are associated with higher levels of engagement and effort than low-activation emotions (Carver, 2004). More specifically, positive emotions with neutral levels of activation, such as pleasantness, signal that a goal has been reached and further effort is unnecessary; by contrast, positive high-activation emotions, such as excitement, imply eagerness to achieve (Buck, 1988; Carver, 2003). In work contexts, job engagement is thus defined as a motivational state of high levels of energy (Rich, Lepine, & Crawford, 2010), whereby “goals are chosen and behavior is energized and directed in relation to envisaged positive outcomes” (Warr & Inceoglu, 2012: 618), with energetic activation serving as an antecedent of intrinsic motivation (Quinn, Spreitzer, & Lam, 2012). Consistent with these insights, Seo, Bartunek and Feldman Barrett (2004; 2010) proposed theoretically and documented empirically a direct effect of emotional activation on the amount of effort exercised by an individual in pursuit of work goals.1 Adding further nuance to this result, Foo, Uy and 1 The evidsence Seo, Bartunek and Barrett (2010) provided for the effect of pleasantness on motivation is problematic, due primarily to construct validity issues. In that study, persistence (one of three components of motivation) was measured in terms of how long an investor held on 7 Baron (2009) found high-activation positive affect (measured as “enthusiastic,” “attentive,” “proud,” “interested,” and “inspired”) to predict individual orientation toward the future and effort toward tasks not immediately required. This result is consistent with the notion of excitement as a motivational state based on the expectation of future rewards (Izard, 1991; Lawler & Yoon, 1996), and the idea that confidence and enthusiasm an individual derives from social interactions stimulates action based on the expectation that a desirable future can be brought into the present (Barbalet, 1998). Applied to task-advice relationships, these arguments paint a consistent picture in which advice ties eliciting high-activation positive affect result in higher levels of task engagement and effort than task-related relationships eliciting positive emotions with lower levels of activation. Enjoyable task-advice relationships may boost psychological resilience and emotional well-being (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005), but do not necessarily stimulate an individual to take task-oriented action, because pleasantness, contentment and happiness may signal that a desired end state has been reached and no further action is required. By contrast, engagement and effort have clearer theoretical implications for job performance (Rich et al., 2010). Together, these insights suggest that the tendency to seek out for advice colleagues that elicit feelings of excitement (a propensity I label excitement-based advice seeking) will stimulate greater task engagement and effort than the tendency to seek out for advice colleagues that elicit enjoyment (enjoyment-based advice seeking). To the extent that engagement and effort have to a stock position. This variable could measure disengagement from the task, rather than persistence. Indeed, persistence was negatively predictive of effort, signaling a potentially negative indirect association between pleasantness and engagement with the task. 8 greater likelihood of producing performance gains than gratification and contentment, these predictions follows: Hypothesis 1: Ego’s propensity to seek advice from alters based on how exciting social interactions with them are (excitement-based advice seeking) is positively associated with ego’s task performance. Hypothesis 2: Excitement-based advice seeking has a stronger association with ego’s task performance more than ego’s propensity to seek advice from alters based on how enjoyable social interactions with them are (enjoyment-based advice seeking). METHOD Site and sample To test these hypotheses, I conducted a web-based survey of information technology salespeople in a large, multinational computer and IT consulting corporation headquartered in the United States. The study concerned the performance of salespeople operating in teams that the company created with the explicit purpose of integrating hardware, software, and other technology services to meet wide-ranging client needs in a coordinated and comprehensive manner. Clients represented a wide range of industries, including public sector not-for-profit organizations, as well as retail, service, and manufacturing companies. Salespeople serving these clients were typically engineers, by training. They performed a variety of roles, ranging from information technology specialists to relationship managers. Providing clients with integrated products and services depended on effective knowledge sharing and coordination among team members. Consequently, although salespeople’s performance was evaluated based on attaining 9 their individual sales quotas, doing so required them to draw from the specialized expertise and client knowledge of their teammates. The need to integrate others’ knowledge to accomplish individual goals provided the rationale for creating these particular team structures. Because of the technical complexity of products and services and the size of clients requesting them, the sales teams had standing supplier relationships with clients, rather than short-term project-based engagements. Salespeople serving a client were not necessarily co-located, although most were located within the same geographical area within the United States. The study was sponsored and supported by senior management within the organization, including the executive vice president of sales operations, who provided detailed information regarding the structure, dynamics, and composition of sales teams. Their insights helped to select and word the survey items so that respondents would accurately and consistently interpret them, given their work context. The selection process for participants in the survey included several steps. First, three executive vice presidents of regional sales in the United States generated a list of sales teams that they deemed to be representative of either high-performing and lowperforming teams in their region. Out of the population of 800 sales teams, the executive vice presidents selected 60 teams for the study, with equal proportions of high- and low-performing teams. Rather than outliers in the distribution of team performance at the company, high- and low-performing teams in the sample were representative of large swaths of the company’s team population. In this organization, differences in team-level performance stemmed primarily not from differences in the level of competence of team members, but rather from exogenous characteristics of the client organization, such as industry competitive dynamics and the lifecycle of the client relationship, with new clients in fast-growing industries providing a better platform for sales than long-standing clients in mature industries. Stratifying the sample of 10 salespeople by current team performance was intended therefore to yield a sample representative of the variability of team environments in which salespeople operated in the company. The leaders of the sampled teams were contacted and 53 agreed to participate, ranging in size from 4 to 47 (mean = 17.01, S.D. = 10.93). Team leaders, in turn, identified a total of 898 salespeople in their teams. An invitation to participate in the web-based study was emailed to all sampled salespeople, who received a personalized access code to the survey website with a guarantee of confidentiality of individual survey responses. This invitation was followed up by emails from the team leader as well as phone calls by senior researchers within the organization. Of the sampled salespeople, 693 returned at least partially completed questionnaires. I excluded from statistical analyses respondents who did not rate any team member. Salespeople who filled out the questionnaire but left the organization before the end-of-year performance evaluation were also excluded from the analyses. The final sample included 430 salespeople. There were no significant differences between respondents and non-respondents along characteristics such as tenure in the organization, formal role, or gender. Likewise, among those who did fill out the survey, there were no significant differences, along either survey or non-survey measures, between respondents who stayed at the company and those who left before the year-end performance evaluation. Network data The web-based survey provided respondents (egos) with a roster of all members of their sales team (alters). Respondents were asked to check off members of their team that they knew. This question was particularly relevant to large teams, where not all salespeople knew each other. Teammates that a respondent did not know were excluded from subsequent survey questions. For each team member that salespeople had checked off as someone they knew, 11 respondents were asked to indicate their agreement or disagreement with each of several statements on a five-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). Task-related advice was measured with the item “I turn to this person for information or advice before making an important decision about this account”. The items “Interactions with this person are enjoyable” and “Interactions with this person are energizing” measured, respectively, the extent to which ego experienced positive affect with neutral-to-moderate activation (enjoyment) and highactivation positive affect (excitement) when having social interactions with each alter. Company executives and salespeople who pre-tested the survey preferred the term “energizing” over the term “exciting” because the latter could be associated with the notion of arousal, which was deemed as potentially awkward and misleading in a professional context. To account for people’s demonstrated tendency to seek out for advice colleagues they deem competent at the task (Borgatti & Cross, 2003; Casciaro & Lobo, 2008; Hinds et al., 2000), I also included the question “This person is very competent at his/her job,” which measured the respondent’s competence evaluations of each member of the team. As with the other network questions, the wording was determined based on input from company executives, who requested the addition of the qualifier “very” to prevent, or at least limit, the censoring of responses at the high end of the response scale. I also wished to account for the known effect of perceived accessibility on the likelihood of seeking a colleague out for task advice (Borgatti & Cross, 2003; Nebus, 2006). To measure a salesperson willingness to share knowledge, company executives suggested the item “This person volunteers information that helps the team better serve the client.” Survey respondents provided a total of 8327 ratings of team members. Overview of modeling strategy 12 Testing this study’s hypotheses entailed two estimation steps. Simply put, the first estimation step computes four regression lines (slopes) for each ego based on the association between ego’s four ratings of his teammates (enjoyment, excitement, competence, and willing to share knowledge) and ego’s report on turning to each teammate for task advice. Figure 1 illustrates this estimation step. This respondent, Bob, rated each of his 12 teammates for how exciting and enjoyable he found interactions with them, and how competent and willing to share knowledge he perceived them to be. Bob also reported on how much he turned to each of those 12 teammates for work advice. The first estimation step uses these ratings to estimate four regression lines for Bob, where the y axis is the rating for turning to a teammate for advice, and the x axis is, respectively, the rating for excitement, enjoyment, competence, and willingness to share that Bob gave to each teammate. Figure 2 illustrates how variability in the slope coefficients this estimation produces for different respondents in the sample. The model estimating these regression lines is a three-level model. Level 1 is the dyadic level. It includes all ratings provided by the 430 salespeople in the sample about their teammates and estimates overall (not individual) slopes for excitement, enjoyment, competence and willingto-share ratings as predictors of advice seeking across all respondents. Level 2 is the individual level. For example, Bob is the individual within which Bob’s 12 ratings of alters are nested. Level 2 estimates both Bob’s intercept for advice seeking (which account for Bob’s general tendency to go to colleagues for task advice) and Bob’s slope for each of the four ratings of alters (for example, the slope for excitement represents the tendency for Person’s A advice ties to be exciting relationships). Level 3 in the model is the team in which each individual was nested. In step 2 of the estimation, the four individual-level slopes for each of the 430 sampled individuals are used as independent variables predicting individual performance. The model 13 estimating individual performance is a two-level model. Level 1 is the individual. Level 2 is the team within which individuals are nested. Below, I detail this analytical approach. First estimation step: Modeling advice-seeking propensities I used the 8327 dyadic ratings that respondents provided about their teammates to construct individual-level measures of the propensity to form advice ties with alters that ego perceived as, respectively, enjoyable, exciting, competent, and willing to share knowledge. These four measures were estimated for each salesperson in the sample. To that end, I ran mixed models with random intercepts and random slopes for each ego, as well as fixed effects for each alter, an approach equivalent to Snijders and Kenny’s (1999). The random slopes for each ego represent the competence-, excitement-, enjoyment- and sharing- based advice seeking displayed by each individual, which are the relative tendency of an actor to seek others out for task-related input (i.e., ego’s responses to the “turn to” survey item) based on ego’s subjective assessment of their competence and their willingness to share knowledge, and of how enjoyable and exciting interacting with them is, respectively. I estimated random-slope models because they yield the best linear unbiased predictions for the slope coefficients for each ego. The random intercept for ego and fixed effects for alter control for perceiver effects (an individual average tendency to seek colleagues out for advice) and target effects (an alter’s average tendency to be sought out for advice), consistent with Kenny’s (1994) social relations model. The mixed models have the following functional form: P(Yij= y)= a0 + at + ai + b1ix1ij + b2ix2ij + b3ix3ij + b4ix4ij + b5ix5ij +( ∑19 π=6 ππ π₯ππ + π’π )+ eij where: yij is the “turn to” rating given by the ith ego to the jth alter, 14 (1) a0 is the overall intercept at is the random portion of the intercept for the tth team, ai is the random portion of the intercept for the ith ego, b1i is the slope on the competence rating for the ith ego (competence-based advice seeking), x1ij is the competence rating given by the ith ego to the jth alter, b2i is the slope on the excitement rating for the ith ego (excitement-based advice seeking), x2ij is the excitement rating given by the ith ego to the jth alter, b3i is the slope on the enjoyment rating for the ith ego (enjoyment-based advice seeking), x3ij is the enjoyment rating given by the ith ego to the jth alter, b4i is the slope on the willingness-to-share-knowledge rating for the ith ego (sharing-based advice seeking), x4ij is the willingness-to-share-knowledge rating given by the ith ego to the jth alter, b4i is the slope on face-to-face communication for the ith ego, x4ij is the face-to-face communication rating given by the ith ego to the jth alter, ∑13 π=1 ππ π₯ππ + π’π are the fixed effects for ego’s formal job role eij is the random error, and ai ~ N(0,ο³ai2); at ~ N(0,ο³aj2); b1i ~ N(b10, ο³b12); b2i ~ N(b20, ο³b22); b3i ~ N(b30, ο³b32); b4i ~ N(b40, ο³b42); ul ~ N(0,ο³u2). The b1i, b2i, b3i, and b4i coefficients are the measures of competence-, excitement-, enjoyment- and sharing- based advice seeking, respectively. The intercepts ai and at and the slopes b1i- b4i are random because ego and team are random effects. b10 - b40 are the overall slopes on the competent, excitement, enjoyment, and willingness-to-share-knowledge ratings. The covariance of the random effects is unconstrained, which allows the random effects parameters to be correlated. The estimation of enjoyment-, excitement-, competence-, sharing-based advice seeking included controls for the formal role performed by each salesperson in their team ( 15 ∑19 π=6 ππ π₯ππ + π’π in equation 1), which might affect their patterns of interaction with other team members due to varying degrees of task interdependence between formal roles. The company defined fourteen formal roles in sales teams, including Client Executive/Manager, Client Services Manager, Software Sales Representative, Business Development Executive, Sales Representative, IT Architect, and Technical Specialist. The first estimation step includes thirteen dummy variables for the formal role played by each ego in the sample, treating the “Other” response option as the omitted category in the estimation. In supplementary analyses, I also included dummy variables for the formal role of alters. The coefficients of dummy variables for alters’ formal role were not significantly different from zero in any of the models, and their inclusion substantially decreased sample size, because of missing data on the formal role of alters who did not fill out the survey. To prevent inflating standard errors and increasing the probability of Type II errors, I excluded dummies for alters’ formal role from the estimation. I also wished to control for the impact of physical proximity on the probability of seeking a colleague out for advice (Nebus, 2006). The data collection did not include measures of physical or geographical proximity in the organization, partly because salespeople travelled extensively, and had opportunities to meet each other at their clients’, making measures of proximity in the confines of their organization less meaningful. But the network survey did ask respondents to indicate which of the following methods of communication they used with each alter: face to face (planned or unplanned); instant messaging; email; phone for 1-1 conversations; multi-person conference call; and shared repository. I used a dummy variable for face-to-face communication (planned or unplanned) as a proxy for physical proximity between ego and alter. Overall, the first estimation step controls for four primary forms of non-independence of observations: row effects (with ego random intercepts), column effects (with alter fixed effects), 16 dyad effects (with fixed effects for ego’s and alter’s formal role that account for unobserved task interdependence), and cluster effects (with team random intercepts). Second estimation step: Predicting individual performance The dependent variable is this study was individual performance as measured by yearend performance ratings assigned to each salesperson by a line manager outside of the team structure. These performance ratings were based mainly on the extent to which a salesperson achieved individual sales-quota targets for the year. A minor component of performance evaluations was a line manager’s assessment of how well a salesperson represented the products and services of his division. As such, performance evaluations were based primarily on objective financial results, and only marginally on subjective supervisor ratings, providing a relatively unbiased measure of sales effectiveness at the individual level. Individual performance ratings were expressed in integers, ranging from 1 to 4, with high values indicating better performance. The enjoyment-, excitement-based advice seeking measures calculated with equation (1) were independent variables in ordered logistic regressions predicting individual performance. Competence- and sharing-based advice seeking measures from equation (1) served as two of the controls in this second estimation step. I used an ordinal logistic model specification to test the hypotheses because the dependent variable was categorical, ordered, and had four possible outcomes (Long, 1997). With this specification, an underlying score is estimated as a linear function of the independent variables and a set of cut points. The probability of observing outcome Z corresponds to the probability that the estimated linear function, plus random error, is within the range of the cut points estimated by the outcome: πΎππ§ =P (κz−1 < β1b1i + β2b2i + β3b3i + β4b4i + …+ βkxki + ui ≤ κZ) 17 (3) where b1i, b2i, b3i, and b4i are the estimates for competence-, excitement-, enjoyment- and sharing-based advice seeking from equation (1), and ui is assumed to be logistically distributed. Using maximum likelihood, the coefficients β1, β2,…, βk are estimated along with the cut points κ1, κ2, …, κz−1, where Z is the number of possible outcomes. To control for team-level non-independence of observations, I performed the ordered logistic regression in equation (3) within a two-level generalized linear latent and mixed models (GLLAMM) with the following functional form: (π§) π¦π = log( (π§) πΎπ (π§) 1−πΎπ ) = πΌ (π§) - (∑49 π‘=1 π½π‘ π₯π‘π + π’π ), π’π ~π(0, ππ’2 ) (4) The GLLAMM model in equation (4) estimates individual performance with an ordered logistical regression nested in team-level random effects. In alternative specifications, I ran ordered logistic models predicting performance estimating either team-clustered robust standard errors or including team-level fixed effects. The results of these analyses were entirely comparable with those obtained with the models presented in the table below. The GLLAMM model included a number of control variables. In addition to controlling for the formal job role performed by salespeople on their team, I used the dyadic ratings to construct measures to account for how each salesperson was perceived by his or her teammates, on average, along the criteria of competence, enjoyment, excitement, and willingness to share knowledge. For each of those network items, the measure ∑π π=1 πππ (5) π represents the average evaluation that each salesperson, i, received from his or her n team-mates j. These measures account for the possibility that a salesperson’s performance may be a function of her individual characteristics, as perceived by others, rather than her relational behavior within 18 the team. To account for the possibility that newer members of the organization or of the team may be at a disadvantage in terms of both task-related knowledge and social capital, I controlled for the length of time individuals had been members of the team (team tenure) with a four-level variable: less than 1 year, 1–2 years, 2–3 years, and more than 3 years. Similarly, I controlled for tenure in the company (company tenure) with a five-level variables: less than 1 year, 1–2 years, 2–3 years, 3–4 years, and more than 4 years.2 Because a number of salespeople supported multiple teams simultaneously, I controlled for the percentage of time a salesperson devoted to a team included in the sample (time percentage on team) with a four-level variable (less than 20%, 20% to 50%, more than 50%, and 100%), and with a count of the number of clients served by the salesperson (number of clients). I also wished to consider the possibility that women may achieve levels of performance systematically different from those of men in a technology company such as the one in which the study was conducted, where 75 percent of the sales force was male. For that reason, the models predicting individual performance also included a dummy variable for gender (female). RESULTS First estimation step: Modeling advice-seeking propensities Table 1 presents descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients for the variables included in the mixed models estimating the propensity of respondents to seek advice from colleagues based on their perceived competence, willingness to share knowledge, and the experience of excitement and enjoyment when interacting with them. The correlation 2 Company management provided data on team and company tenure in the format described above, rather than as continuous variables. Alternative specifications of the team tenure and company tenure variables, including dummies for each level, did not alter the results. 19 coefficients among ratings of competence, willingness to share knowledge, enjoyment, and excitement that an individual gave his teammates are relatively high, consistent with the wellknown halo effect, according to which interpersonal judgments along different dimensions tend to co-vary, so that individual characteristics, such as intelligence, likeability, warmth, and competence, tend to be highly correlated in people’s judgments of others (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1993; Dion, Walster, & Berscheid, 1972). The random intercepts for ego and fixed effects for alter, as well the simultaneous estimation of the effects of all four ratings (Equation 1) partial out sources of this common variation. Table 2 shows that ratings of competence, excitement, enjoyment and willingness to share knowledge all have positive and statistically significant effects on the probability of seeking a teammate out for task advice. These results indicate that the tendency to seek advice from a colleague is positively associated with how ego perceives a colleague’s competence and willingness to share knowledge, as well as the excitement and enjoyment experiences by ego in interactions with alters. Face-to-face communication—a proxy for physical proximity to a colleague—was also positively related to advice seeking. This reliance on competence, positive affect, willingness to share knowledge, and proximity as criteria for selecting sources of task advice is consistent with existing theory and evidence (Casciaro & Lobo, 2008; Collins, 1993; Hinds et al., 2000; Nebus, 2006), providing evidence for the internal and external validity of this study. In supplemental mixed models, I also tested the possibility that competence, excitement, enjoyment and willingness-to-share ratings might have multiplicative, as well as additive, effects on advice seeking. To that end, I estimated the effect of six two-way interaction terms between, respectively, ratings of competence, excitement, enjoyment and willingness to share knowledge. 20 These two-way interactions were not statistically significant in any of the mixed models predicting advice seeking. Second estimation step: Predicting Individual Performance In the second estimation step, I used ego’s random slope coefficients associated with ratings of excitement, enjoyment, competence, and willingness to share knowledge obtained with the mixed model in equation (1) to measure ego’s tendency to seek task advice from colleagues ego based on feelings of excitement (excitement-based advice seeking) and enjoyment (enjoyment-based advice seeking) experienced when interacting with colleagues. Table 3 provides descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients for these predictors, as well as all control variables, including competence-based advice seeking and sharing-based advice seeking. Among the four measures of advice-seeking propensities, excitement-seeking has the highest correlation with individual performance (r=.11, p<.05). The four propensity measures also vary with regard to their mean, with sharing-based advice seeking more common (μ=.45) than competence-based advice seeking (μ=.12). The standard deviations for the four measures of advice-seeking propensities also indicate variability across respondents. For instance, the standard deviation for the individual slopes measuring excitement-based advice seeking (coefficient b2i in equations 1 and 3) was .12, ranging from a minimum of -.18 to a maximum of .51. This variability in individual slopes indicates that, while on average salespeople in the sample treated the excitement experienced in interactions with a colleague as a reason to seek them out for advice, some salespeople valued that excitement more than others. The correlation coefficients among the average evaluations of competence, willingness to share knowledge, enjoyment, and excitement that an ego received from teammates (i.e., alters’ rating of ego) are high, consistent with the halo effect discussed above (Ambady & Rosenthal, 21 1993; Dion et al., 1972). These correlation patterns notwithstanding, the four team-level evaluations of an individual have different patterns of association with performance: only an individual’s competence, as perceived by team mates, correlates significantly with individual performance (r=.17, p<.01). I also ran t-tests comparing the average performance and competence evaluations of salespeople in high- and low-performing teams. There were minimal differences in the individual performance of salespeople operating in high-performing teams (μ=2.88) and in low-performing teams (μ=2.71) (t = 2.07, p < .05), and no difference in average ratings of competence received by salespeople in the two groups, indicating that the selection of teams for the study did not unduly bias the distribution of individual performance and competence in the sample. Table 4 presents the results of the two-level GLLAMM model estimating individual performance with an ordered logistical regression nested in team-level random effects. The coefficient for excitement-based advice seeking was positive and statistically significant. Consistent with hypothesis 1, the greater a person’s tendency to seek out for task advice colleagues with whom ego experiences feelings of excitement was positively associated with individual performance. By contrast, the results provide no evidence for enjoyment-based advice seeking having a significant impact on task performance. A test of two linear hypotheses rejected the null that the coefficients for excitement-seeking advice seeking and enjoyment –seeking advice seeking are equal (p < .05), supporting Hypothesis 2, which predicted that excitementbased advice seeking would have a stronger positive association with task performance than enjoyment-based advice seeking. These results indicate that having advice ties with colleagues who elicit feelings of excitement—emotional energy—is distinctly predictive of individual performance, even after 22 accounting for an individual’s tendency to seek advice from colleagues based on their competence, willingness to share knowledge and enjoyment of interactions with them. In supplementary models not included in Table 2, I tested for the possibility that the four adviceseeking variables might interact with the other three advice-seeking propensities to influence task performance. None of these interaction terms was statistically significant. The lack of a significant interaction effect between excitement-based and competence-based advice seeking, in particular, indicates that the performance improvement induced by positive high-activation positive affect was not contingent on the amount of task knowledge accessed through the interaction. Yet, it is difficult to draw firm conclusions from null results concerning second-order effects that are difficult to detect in multilevel models that include a high number of predictors. Only one control variable had a significant effect on task performance: the average competence ratings received by a salesperson from her team mates. This effect was to be expected, as there should be consistency between teammates’ ratings of how knowledgeable a colleague is about her job and that colleague’s job performance. By contrast, the average enjoyment, excitement and generosity ratings received by a salesperson from his team mates did not predict individual performance. The lack of a significant effect of average ratings of excitement received by an individual rules out of possibility that the effect of excitement-based advice seeking on individual performance may be an artefact of that individual’s own ability to generate excitement in social interactions. Creating excitement does not predict individual performance; seeking excitement does. This pattern of results held when the measures for alters’ ratings of ego along the competence, enjoyment, excitement and generosity dimensions were tested one at a time, indicating that, in spite of the high correlation among these four group-level assessments of an individual, it is specifically the team’s average evaluation of a team member’s 23 competence that predicts individual performance. The Variance Inflation Index (VIF) of these four control variables ranged between 4.05 and 4.25, well below conventionally used rules of thumb that indicate excessive or serious multicollinearity if any of the VIF are greater than 10 (Aiken & West, 1991; O'Brien, 2007). In supplemental analyses, I checked the robustness of these findings by performing the estimation using a conservative sample that included respondents who had provided a minimum of five ratings of alters and whose ratings displayed a standard deviation greater than 0.5. Restricting the sample to those who had provided at least five ratings accounted for potential bias introduced by variability in the amount of relational data available for different individuals. By restricting the sample to those whose ratings had a standard deviation greater than 0.5, I addressed the possibility that invariant ratings might signal low response quality. The results obtained with the full and the restricted sample were entirely comparable. In additional models, I tested for team-level effects not with a two-level GLLAMM model with random effects for team, but rather with a dummy variable denoting whether the team a salesperson operated in was designated as high-performing by sales executives in the company. The results of these model specifications were consistent with those in Table 4. Finally, I wished to address the possibility that a salesperson’s extraversion may underlie both excitement-based advice seeking and task performance. To that end, in supplemental models, I included ego’s average rating of alters as enjoyable and exciting. These variables serve as proxies for extraversion, since extraverts tend to enjoy interacting with people and draw energy from social interactions. The inclusion of these measures did not alter the findings. The lack of an effect of ego’s average ratings of alters as enjoyable and exciting also 24 addresses the possibility that strong performers may simply find colleagues more exciting, and thus it rules out a potential form of reverse causality. DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to investigate the performance implications of people’s tendency to choose colleagues as sources of task advice based on the positive affect experienced in interactions with them. I started from the premise that advice networks in organizations reflect not only the instrumental resources someone can contribute to a task (e.g., knowledge), but also the affective resources that people derive from social interaction (Bales, 1958; Hinds et al., 2000; Homans, 1961; Krackhardt, 1999; Krackhardt & Stern, 1998; Roethlisberger & Dickson, 1939; Slater, 1955). Although it is evident that task-oriented social interaction involves the pursuit of both instrumental and affective resources, the effects of building advice networks based on the affective content of social relationships with colleagues on people’s task performance have been poorly understood. The sole contributor to individual performance emerging from this study of adviceseeking behavior is an individual’s tendency to have task-advice ties that elicit high-activation positive affect. This result persisted after accounting for the potential performance boost an individual might gain by seeking out for advice especially competent colleagues or gaining privileged access to their knowledge. Caution should be used, however, in interpreting the lack of evidence for positive effect of competence- and sharing-based advice seeking (Cortina & Dunlap, 1997; Cortina & Folger, 1998). Finding no support for the role of competence-based advice seeking in individual performance, for instance, may be due to the specific distribution of competence in the organization studied. In this complex and interdependent task environment, 25 the baseline of individual competence was high, and seeking advice from teammates with greater competence relative to this selected set of colleagues may not have significantly increased the task knowledge an actor got access to. Moreover, although we partly accounted for unobserved task interdependence with fixed effects for ego’s and alter’s formal role, company management did not provide us with a direct measure of the degree of task interdependence between a team’s formal roles. As a result, our measure of competence-based advice seeking may not accurately reflect the complementarity of competencies, or lack thereof, in a given dyad. This interpretative caution noted, the documented effect of excitement-based advice seeking on an individual’s job performance has three primary implications. First, it suggests that the affective content of social relationships can serve a fundamental motivational function for task-related action in organizations. Research in sociology, psychology and organizational behavior consistently documents how a positive affective state of energized activation motivates engagement and effort, and goal-oriented action toward achieving envisaged positive outcomes (Buck, 1988; Carver, 2003, 2004; Collins, 1993; Foo et al., 2009; Quinn et al., 2012; Seo et al., 2004; Seo et al., 2010; Warr & Inceoglu, 2012). In the context of task advice relationships, highactivation positive affect can thus stimulate engagement and effort toward assigned tasks (Rich et al., 2010), and therefore increase the likelihood that they will be successfully performed. Network research on the strength of ties has noted the motivational role of affect in task interaction, evoking affective constructs, such as emotional closeness and trust, to explain what induces social actors to invest time and effort to transfer useful and complex knowledge to their colleagues (Hansen, 1999; Levin & Cross, 2004). These accounts portray affect as motivating social actors to provide valued resources to others, but they largely leave to the background the question of how affect experienced in social interaction may, in turn, stimulate actors to take 26 action toward work tasks. The present study underscores this very aspect of advice seeking in organizations: affect as a motivational force that can enhance individual performance by encouraging an actor to engage with and exercise effort toward work tasks, independent of the task knowledge gained through the advice network. Second, different forms of positive affect experienced in social interaction influence task performance differently. Network research has typically treated relational affect as generally positive or negative (Heider, 1958; Homans, 1961; Labianca & Brass, 2006; Labianca, Brass, & Gray, 1998; Sampson, 1968), and has characterized the affective experience with constructs— such as friendship, closeness, and liking—that lend themselves to ambiguous interpretation. Friendship, for instance, takes many forms, ranging from enjoyable socializing to deep emotional bonds of trust, which are mechanisms with potentially different outcomes. To address this ambiguity, I measured directly two emotional responses with theoretical relevance to my argument and clear definitions in psychological models of the affective experience. This approach adds precision to traditional network measures of expressive ties, and it alleviates concerns with the single-item measures to which network studies are often bound. Moreover, the present findings indicate that a finer characterization of the emotional content of social relationships is necessary to understand how positive affect may enhance task performance. In this study, the pursuit of high-activation positive affect yielded unique performance advantages over forms of positive affect with lower levels of activation. The documented link between emotional activation and engagement and effort (Carver, 2004; Collins, 1993; Quinn et al., 2012; Seo et al., 2010; Warr & Inceoglu, 2012) helps to explain the effects of excitement-based advice seeking on performance. Positive emotions with moderate or neutral levels of activation, such as enjoyment, signal that a goal has been reached and, therefore, further effort is unnecessary; by 27 contrast, positive high-activation emotions signal that a desirable end-state is possible, triggering eagerness to achieve (Barbalet, 1998; Carver, 2003). Theories of affect in organizational networks should therefore be sensitive to the motivational foundations of social interaction between organization members. A failure to do so may yield misleading conclusions about the relevance, or irrelevance, of affect for task-related action in organizations. Third, these findings demonstrate that eschewing the pursuit of task competence for the sake of positive affect may not be as irrational as it may appear at first glance. Prior research has documented people’s tendency to form task-related ties based on their personal feelings for coworkers, raising the possibility that such behaviour might prevent a significant reservoir of task knowledge from being tapped in organizations (Casciaro & Lobo, 2008). I find that, at the individual level, preferentially seeking for advice exciting colleagues may be rational, because increasing one’s motivation to carry out work tasks can enhance an actor’s performance independent of seeking out and accessing superior task competence. This may be the case for at least two reasons. First, to the extent that organizations hire and retain people based on their possessing basic abilities to perform assigned roles, choosing task partners with greater competence relative to this selected set of coworkers may not increase significantly how much task knowledge an actor gets access to. Second, seeking advice from colleagues with significantly greater competence relative to other organization members may not stimulate an actor toward applying their superior knowledge to task-oriented effort. For instance, particularly knowledgeable colleagues may trigger upward comparisons that lower the advice seeker’s confidence and energy (Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988). The relevant question for organizations, then, is: what makes a social interaction energizing? A number of theories in sociology, psychology and organizational behavior have 28 tackled this question (for a review, see Quinn et al., 2012). Extant theorizing and empirical research indicate that it is not simply the existence and identification of social resources, but rather the perception that desirable rewards are achievable that generates a motivational state of energetic activation (Collins, 1993; Cross et al., 2003; Higgins, 1997; Izard, 1991). This characterization of excitement as sparked by the prospect of future rewards begs further questions concerning which social relationships are more likely to be associated with such potential rewards. Classic social psychology suggests that task interdependence among organizational actors may be predictive of high-activation positive affect, because the need to work closely together to achieve superordinate goals enhances the intensity of emotional responses within social interactions (Sherif, 1966; Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, & Sherif, 1961). Research on the emergence of affective commitment and micro orders has confirmed the link between higher levels of interdependence and emotional attachment (Lawler, Thye, & Yoon, 2008). The recurrence and exclusivity of social interactions may increase the emotional energy they generate (Collins, 1981, 1993). Individual differences may also explain variability in relational excitement (Cross et al., 2003). The hallmark of extraversion, for instance, is lively sociability—the enjoyment of others' company (McCrae & Costa, 1987). In the present study, manifestations of extraversion were controlled for with measures of ego’s average rating of alters as enjoyable and exciting, since extraverts tend to enjoy interacting with people and draw energy from such social interactions. But while the results of this study cannot straightforwardly be attributed to traits like extraversion, it is possible that individual differences may underlie how social interactions operate as sources of emotional energy. And, certainly, “goals have an energizing function” (Locke & Latham, 2002: 706). To the extent that goal-setting occurs through social interaction with coworkers, the choice of interaction partners can influence self29 regulation in service of task performance (Latham & Locke, 1991). Despite these insights, the distinct importance of excitement-based advice seeking demonstrated by this study demands a deeper exploration of the relational and structural dynamics that determine the emergence of high-activation positive affect in organizations. Future research can also investigate the negative sphere of the affective experience in intra-organizational networks. I focused on positive affective responses because my theory concerned the social resources—and not the liabilities—that people seek out from interactions with coworkers. Nevertheless, negative relational content can hinder individual and group performance (Labianca & Brass, 2006; Sparrowe et al., 2001), and the tendency to either avoid or seek out relationships that trigger negative high-activation emotion, such as fear and anger, may play an important role in sustaining motivation. The results of this study also reflect the specific distribution of task competence in the organization I studied. It is conceivable that, in organizations populated with employees with limited task competence, competence-seeking behaviour would emerge as essential to ensuring adequate task performance. To the extent that organizations tend to select employees that have basic task-relevant skills, however, widespread incompetence is both improbable and unsustainable in organizations. The results of this study are therefore likely to emerge in other common organizational contexts. The external validity of these findings is further corroborated by research showing that interpersonal affective evaluations—such as personal liking, pleasantness or energy—contribute to the formation of task-advice networks independent of either organizational context or task characteristics, suggesting that these basic interpersonal responses may not vary significantly by organizational and task environment (Casciaro & Lobo, 2008). The results of this study also replicate patterns of advice network formation documented 30 in a number of organizational contexts (for a review, see Nebus, 2006), suggesting that the context I studied shared basic characteristics with other organizations (Johns, 2006). These avenues for future research notwithstanding, this study sheds new light on the motivational function that relational affect plays in task interaction and performance. Managers often design organizational structures and systems to encourage interactions among coworkers with the greatest amount of task competence. Without accounting for the affective content of a social interaction, however, recognizing that a colleague has desirable task resources may not be sufficient to enhance performance. In this sense, the affective motives that people apply to task interactions do not simply “contaminate” their rational pursuit of work tasks. Rather, they contribute to an individual’s ability to achieve task goals, and should therefore be considered an indispensable component of any theory of task-oriented social action in organizations. 31 REFERENCES Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. 1991. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. 1993. Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(3): 431-441. Baldwin, T. T., Bedell, M. D., & Johnson, J. L. 1997. The social fabric of a team-based M.B.A. program: Network effects on student satisfaction and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 40(6): 1369-1397. Bales, R. F. 1958. Task roles and social roles in problem-solving groups. Readings in social psychology, 3. Barbalet, J. M. 1998. Emotion, Social Theory, and Social Structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Barnard, C. I. 1938. The Functions of the Executive. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Blau, P. M., & Schoenherr, R. A. 1971. The Structure of Organizations. New York: Basic Books. Borgatti, S. P., & Cross, R. 2003. A relational view of information seeking and learning in social networks. Management Science, 49(4): 432-445. Borgatti, S. P., & Foster, P. C. 2003. The network paradigm in organizational research: A review and typology. Journal of Management, 29(6): 991-1013. Brass, D. J., Glaskiewicz, J., Greve, H. R., & Tsai, W. P. 2004. Taking stock of networks and organizations: A multilevel perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 47(6): 795817. Buck, R. 1988. Human motivation and emotion (2nd. ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Carver, C. S. 2003. Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cognition & Emotion, 17(2): 241-261. Carver, C. S. 2004. Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system. Emotion, 4(1): 3-22. Casciaro, T., & Lobo, M. S. 2008. When competence is irrelevant: The role of interpersonal affect in task-related ties. Administrative Science Quarterly, 53: 655-684. Collins, R. 1981. On the microfoundations of macrosociology. American Journal of Sociology, 86(5): 984-1014. 32 Collins, R. 1993. Emotional energy as the common denominator of rational action. Rationality and Society, 5(2): 203-230. Cortina, J. M., & Dunlap, W. P. 1997. On the logic and purpose of significance testing. Psychological Methods, 2(2): 161-172. Cortina, J. M., & Folger, R. G. 1998. When Is It Acceptable to Accept a Null Hypothesis: No Way, Jose? Organizational Research Methods, 1(3): 334-350. Cropanzano, R., Weiss, H. M., Hale, J. M. S., & Reb, J. 2003. The structure of affect: Reconsidering the relationship between negative and positive affectivity. Journal of Management, 29(6): 831-857. Cross, R., Baker, W., & Parker, A. 2003. Whats creates energy in organizations? Mit Sloan Management Review, 44(4): 51-+. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. 1984. The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47: 1105-1117. Dion, K., Walster, E., & Berscheid, E. 1972. What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(3): 285-290. Foo, M. D., Uy, M. A., & Baron, R. A. 2009. How Do Feelings Influence Effort? An Empirical Study of Entrepreneurs' Affect and Venture Effort. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4): 1086-1094. Fredrickson, B. L. 2001. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology - The broadenand-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3): 218-226. Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. 2005. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3): 313-332. Gargiulo, M., Ertug, G., & Galunic, C. 2009. The Two Faces of Control: Network Closure and Individual Performance among Knowledge Workers. Administrative Science Quarterly, 54(2): 299-333. Hansen, M. T. 1999. The search-transfer problem: The role of weak ties in sharing knowledge across organization subunits. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(1): 82-111. Heider, F. 1958. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Higgins, E. T. 1997. Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12): 1280-1300. Hinds, P. J., Carley, K. M., Krackhardt, D., & Wholey, D. 2000. Choosing work group members: Balancing similarity, competence, and familiarity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 81(2): 226-251. 33 Homans, G. C. 1961. Social Behavior: Its Elementary Forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World. Ibarra, H. 1992. Homophily and differential returns: Sex differences in network structure and access in an advertising firm. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37: 422-447. Izard, C. E. 1991. The Psychology of Emotions. New York: Plenum Press. Johns, G. 2006. The essential impact of context on organizational behavior. Academy of Management Review, 31(2): 386-408. Kenny, D. A. 1994. Interpersonal Perception: A Social Relations Analysis: The Guilford Press. Krackhardt, D. 1999. The Ties that Torture: Simmelian Tie Analysis in Organizations. Research in the Sociology of Organizations, 16: 183-210. Krackhardt, D., & Stern, R. 1998. Informal Networks and Organizational Crises: An Experimental Simulation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51: 123-140. Labianca, G., & Brass, D. J. 2006. Exploring the social ledger: Negative relationships and negative asymmetry in social networks in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 31(3): 596-614. Labianca, G., Brass, D. J., & Gray, B. 1998. Social networks and perceptions of intergroup conflict: The role of negative relationships and third parties. Academy of Management Journal, 41(1): 55-67. Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. 1991. Self-regulation through goal-setting. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2): 212-247. Lawler, E. J., Thye, S. R., & Yoon, J. 2008. Social exchange and micro social order. American Sociological Review, 73(4): 519-542. Lawler, E. J., & Yoon, J. 1996. Commitment in exchange relations: Test of a theory of relational cohesion. American Sociological Review, 61: 89-108. Lawrence, P. R., & Lorsch, J. W. 1967. Differentiation and integration in complex organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 12(1): 1-47. Levin, D. Z., & Cross, R. 2004. The strength of weak ties you can trust: The mediating role of trust in effective knowledge transfer. Management Science, 50(11): 1477-1490. Lincoln, J. R., & Miller, J. 1979. Work and friendship ties in organizations - comparative analysis of relational networks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24(2): 181-199. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. 2002. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation - A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57(9): 705-717. 34 Long, J. S. 1997. Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Beverly HIlls, CA: Sage. Merton, R. K. 1957. The role-set: problems in sociological theory. British Journal of Sociology, 8: 106-120. Mintzberg, H. 1979. The structure of organizations. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Nebus, J. 2006. Building collegial information networks: A theory of advice network generation. Academy of Management Review, 31(3): 615-637. O'Brien, R. M. 2007. A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality & Quantity, 41(5): 673-690. Quinn, R. W., Spreitzer, G. M., & Lam, C. F. 2012. Building a Sustainable Model of Human Energy in Organizations: Exploring the Critical Role of Resources. Academy of Management Annals, 6: 337-396. Rich, B. L., Lepine, J. A., & Crawford, E. R. 2010. Job engagement: Antecedents and effects on job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 53(3): 617-635. Roethlisberger, F. J., & Dickson, W. J. 1939. Management and the Worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Russell, J. A. 1979. Affective space is bipolar. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(3): 345-356. Russell, J. A. 1980. A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39: 1161-1178. Sampson, F. 1968. A Novitiate in a Period of Change: An Experimental and Case Study of Social Relationships. Cornell University: Doctoral dissertation. Seo, M. G., Barrett, L. F., & Bartunek, J. M. 2004. The role of affective experience in work motivation. Academy of Management Review, 29(3): 423-439. Seo, M. G., Bartunek, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. 2010. The role of affective experience in work motivation: Test of a conceptual model. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31(7): 951-968. Sherif, M. 1966. In common predicament: Social psychology of intergroup conflict and cooperation. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, J., Hood, W., & Sherif, C. 1961. Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robber's Cave Experiment. Institute of Intergroup Relations: Norman: University of Oklahoma. 35 Slater, P. E. 1955. Role differentiation in small groups. American Sociological Review, 20(3): 300-310. Snijders, T. A. B., & Kenny, D. A. 1999. The social relations model for family data: A multilevel approach. Personal Relationships, 6(4): 471-486. Sparrowe, R. T., Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., & Kraimer, M. L. 2001. Social networks and the performance of individuals and groups. Academy of Management Journal, 44(2): 316325. Tesser, A., Millar, M., & Moore, J. 1988. Some affective consequences of social-comparison and reflection processes - The pain and pleasure of being close. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(1): 49-61. Thayer, R. E. 1989. The Biopsychology of Mood and Arousal. New York: Oxford University Press. Thompson, J. D. 1967. Organizations in action. New York: McGraw-Hill. Warr, P., & Inceoglu, I. 2012. Job Engagement, Job Satisfaction, and Contrasting Associations With Person-Job Fit. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 17(2): 129-138. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. 1985. Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2): 219-235. 36 FIGURE 1. Modeling advice-seeking propensity based on ego’s evaluations of alters Advice seeking Bob Criterion Legend: Each dot represents how Bob rated each of her 12 teammates along one of four criteria (enjoyment or excitement experienced in interaction with that teammate, or the teammate’s competence or willingness to share knowledge) FIGURE 2. Variability of advice-seeking behavior Advice seeking Michael Bob Anne Joel Criterion Legend: Each regression line represents a respondent’s tendency to seek out for advice colleagues with a given rating for a given criterion. For example, if the criterion is the excitement experienced in interactions with teammates, the line represents a person’s excitement-based advice seeking. 37 TABLE 1 Means, standard deviations, and correlations of variables in first estimation step 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Variable Mean s.d. 1 2 3 4 5 Advice seeking 3.05 1.14 Competence rating 3.91 .96 .52 Excitement rating 3.55 1.00 .55 .70 Enjoyment rating 3.89 1.00 .54 .72 .73 Willingness to share knowledge rating 3.72 1.05 .60 .69 .73 .73 Face-to-face communication .15 .36 .11 .12 .13 .14 .13 Formal role 1 .11 .31 .16 .10 .06 .09 .08 Formal role 2 .09 .29 -.08 -.01 -.04 -.02 -.04 Formal role 3 .04 .19 .03 -.02 .01 -.02 -.02 Formal role 4 .03 .18 -.04 -.04 -.05 -.04 -.04 Formal role 5 .03 .18 .00 -.04 -.03 -.02 -.02 Formal role 6 .08 .26 -.05 .05 .02 .05 .04 Formal role 7 .05 .21 .04 .04 .03 .05 .04 Formal role 8 .03 .16 -.01 -.03 -.01 -.01 .00 Formal role 9 .01 .11 -.01 -.01 -.01 -.02 .01 Formal role 10 .05 .21 -.04 -.02 .01 -.01 -.01 Formal role 11 .02 .12 .03 .00 .03 .01 .04 Formal role 12 .06 .24 -.05 -.04 -.05 -.05 -.06 Formal role 13 .01 .08 .09 .06 .08 .06 .09 9 -.04 -.04 -.06 -.04 -.03 -.02 -.04 -.02 -.05 -.01 10 10 Formal role 4 11 Formal role 5 -.03 12 Formal role 6 -.05 13 Formal role 7 -.04 14 Formal role 8 -.03 15 Formal role 9 -.02 16 Formal role 10 -.04 17 Formal role 11 -.02 18 Formal role 12 -.05 19 Formal role 13 -.01 n = 8327. Coefficients greater than .021 are significant at p<.05; Coefficients greater than .031 are signficant at p<.01 38 11 12 13 14 -.05 -.04 -.03 -.02 -.04 -.02 -.05 -.01 -.06 -.05 -.03 -.06 -.04 -.07 -.02 -.04 -.02 -.05 -.03 -.06 -.02 -.02 -.04 -.02 -.04 -.01 15 6 7 8 .08 -.03 -.02 -.02 .00 .03 .00 .08 -.01 -.06 .06 .02 .00 -.11 -.07 -.06 -.06 -.10 -.08 -.06 -.04 -.08 -.04 -.09 -.03 -.06 -.06 -.06 -.09 -.07 -.05 -.04 -.07 -.04 -.08 -.02 16 17 18 -.02 -.01 -.03 -.03 -.06 -.03 -.01 -.02 -.01 -.02 TABLE 2 Three-level mixed model with random intercepts and random slopes for egos, random effects for teams, and fixed effects for alters, predicting advice-seeking propensities Model 1 Predictors Competence rating Excitement rating Enjoyment rating Willingness-to-share-knowledge rating Face-to-face communication Formal role 1 Formal role 2 Formal role 3 Formal role 4 Formal role 5 Formal role 6 Formal role 7 Formal role 8 Formal role 9 Formal role 10 Formal role 11 Formal role 12 Formal role 13 .12 .17 .21 .44 .11 .26 -.14 .14 -.08 .18 -.25 .04 -.05 -.12 -.10 -.07 -.07 .61 Log restricted-likelihood -9532.38 *** n = 8372 Standard errors are in parentheses *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 39 (.02) (.02) (.02) (.02) (.03) (.08) (.09) (.12) (.11) (.12) (.08) (.10) (.13) (.17) (.11) (.18) (.09) (.29) *** *** *** *** *** ** * TABLE 3 Means, standard deviations, and correlations of variables in second estimation step Mean 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 Individual performance Competence-based advice seeking Excitement-based advice seeking Enjoyment-based advice seeking Sharing-based advice seeking Alters' rating of ego's competence Alters' rating of ego as exciting Alters' rating of ego enjoyable Alters' rating of ego as wiling to share knowledge Female Team tenure Time percentage on the team Number of clients Organizational tenure 27 Alters' rating of ego enjoyable 28 Alters' rating of ego as wiling to share knowledge 29 Female 30 Team tenure 31 Time percentage on the team 32 Number of clients 33 Organizational tenure n = 430 Coefficients greater than .10 are significant at p<.05; Coefficients greater than .12 are signficant at p<.01 40 s.d. 20 21 22 23 24 25 .78 .81 .79 -.12 .27 .02 -.23 .13 2.79 .12 .17 .21 .45 3.95 3.61 3.95 3.81 .27 2.74 2.47 3.54 4.73 .83 .16 .12 .15 .19 .49 .48 .46 .51 .45 1.16 1.41 1.27 .66 -.08 .11 .00 .02 .17 .09 .08 .07 .04 .06 -.06 -.05 .02 .00 -.43 -.19 -.02 .02 .04 .04 -.01 .03 .03 -.04 .06 -.32 -.02 .05 .07 .07 .06 -.11 -.07 -.04 -.14 .03 -.33 .04 .00 .00 .00 .05 .04 .01 .09 -.11 -.10 -.03 -.08 -.08 .01 -.01 .00 .08 .01 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 .80 .79 -.10 .16 .11 -.21 .04 .83 -.09 .20 .08 -.22 .10 -.06 .20 .16 -.27 .11 -.07 -.04 .09 -.01 .11 -.05 .31 -.18 -.07 -.10 TABLE 4 Two-level generalized linear latent and mixed model (GLLAMM) estimating individual performance with an ordered logistical regression nested in team-level random effects Predictors Model 2 Excitement-based advice seeking Enjoyment-based advice seeking Competence-based advice seeking Sharing-based advice seeking Alters' rating of ego's competence Alters' rating of ego as exciting Alters' rating of ego enjoyable Alters' rating of ego as wiling to share knowledge Female Team tenure Time percentage on the team Number of clients Organizational tenure Formal role 1 Formal role 2 Formal role 3 Formal role 4 Formal role 5 Formal role 6 Formal role 7 Formal role 8 Formal role 9 Formal role 10 Formal role 11 Formal role 12 Formal role 13 Log likelihood n = 430 Standard errors are in parentheses *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p<.001 41 .73 -.13 -.41 .09 .53 .10 -.14 -.26 .15 .03 -.02 .00 -.01 -.21 -.04 -.05 -.05 -.09 -.09 -.23 .18 -.02 -.18 .48 .11 .36 -503.244 *** (.37) * (.35) (.30) (.24) (.16) ** (.16) (.18) (.16) (.09) (.04) (.04) (.04) (.06) (.17) (.19) (.19) (.27) (.18) (.14) (.17) (.28) (.27) (.20) (.34) (.16) (.56)