Optogenetic Disruption of Memory-Driv~, Oculomotor Beha~the Non-Human Pnmate ~ by . ~ASS~~"~~~~~;~~~,~JT~2~,---~:~""!"'§!"""'(I1~-U~TE~. I ~~,~ -;-~2014 ] Leah C. Acker LiBRARJES S.M. Electrical Engineering and Computer Science Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2009 SUBMITTED TO THE HARVARO-MlT PROGRAM IN HEALTH SCIENCES AND TECHNOLOGY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PlflLOSOPHY AT THE MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY September 2014 © 2014 Leah C Acker.. All rights reserved. The author hereby grants to MIT permission to reproduce and to distribute publicly paper and electronic copies of this thesis document in whole or in part in~edium now known or hereafter created. Signature redacted Sign.ature of Autltor ..........~ __~...................................................... "..................... . . Harvard-MIT Pro2I'am in Health Sciences and Technology Signature redacted May23,2014 Certified by ...... . Robert Desimone ~oris ~y ProfeSsor of Neuroscience . . S ig natu re redacted Certified .by.. .. ... .... .. . . ......................... . Ed Boyden Associate Professor and AT&tT Chair Sig natu re redacted C/ ................................................... Accepted by.......... . Thesis Supervisor ThesisSupervisor .. ····E~~N B~~;iID:PhD Professor of Computational Neuroscience and Health Sciences and Technology Director, Harvard-MIT Program in Health Sciences and Technology Optogenetic Disruption of Memory-Driven, Oculomotor Behavior in the Non-Human Primate by Leah C. Acker Submitted to the Harvard-MIT Division of Health Sciences and Technology on May 23, 2014 in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Biomedical Engineering Abstract Pharmacological inactivation studies have shown that the frontal eye field (FEF) is critical for executing saccades to remembered locations. FEF neurons increase their firing rate during the three epochs of the memory-guided saccade task: visual stimulus presentation, the delay interval, and motor preparation. It is unclear, though, whether FEF activity during each epoch is necessary for memory-guided saccade execution. To address this question, techniques for millisecondprecise optical inactivation of the primate brain were invented. A red-shifted halorhodopsin (Jaws) and a novel large-volume tissue illuminator were used in two rhesus macaques to inactivate part of the FEF at different times during the memory-guided saccade task. Neuronal recordings showed that the inactivated tissue volume (i.e., the volume where the firing rate of > 80% of neurons decreased by >80%) spanned several cubic millimeters, which is consistent with histological findings. When the target was in the inactivated receptive field, error rates (i.e., failures to execute memory-guided saccades to the proper target location) increased in both monkeys with inactivation during either the target, delay, or motor period. This implies that FEF neuronal activity contributes to performance throughout the memory-guided saccade task. 2 Acknowledgments First, I would like to thank Robert Desimone. Bob taught me how to ask hard questions, how to design experiments, how to interpret results, and how to present the findings-in short, he taught me how to think deeply and clearly about science. Just as importantly, Bob welcomed me into a lab that feels like a family, where he is a kind of father-figure and where the "siblings" remain close throughout their careers. I would like to thank Ed Boyden for his support, help, and constructive feedback. Ed helped me to obtain whatever resources I needed for my thesis, and he challenged me to think broadly about research and the impact of my work. I would like to thank Roger Mark, my committee chair, for all of the kind, honest, and practical advice he has given me as a housemaster, professor, and mentor. Clearly, I have had the honor of knowing him for many years in many different circumstances. I would like to thank Dr. Ann Graybiel for serving on my committee with such enthusiasm and dedication. Ann has long been an inspiration to me, but as we have gotten to know each other better, I have discovered a kindred spirit, a mentor, and a friend. I am thankful for the support of my parents, my brother, and especially, my husband, Jared. I am grateful to my colleagues in the Desimone and Boyden Labs, from whom I have learned so much. Specifically, I want to acknowledge: Amy Chuong, whose opsin, Jaws, was critical for this study; Erica Pino, who assisted with the rodent experiments and fiber fabrication; Narcisse Bichot and Huihui Zhou, who patiently taught me much of what I know about primate neurophysiology; Fumiaki Yoshida, from whom I learned to be a good surgeon; Bob Schafer and Bruno Gomes, my delightful officemates who taught me a lot about statistics and made it a joy to work in 46-6121; Tim Buschman, Grant Mulliken, Reza Rajimehr, and Ying Zhang, who generously shared their wisdom regarding everything from data analysis to experimental design to career planning; the Desimone Lab technicians Ellen DeGennaro, Matt Heard, and Jonathan Winkle-without their support this work would not have been possible; and, finally, Azriel 3 Ghadooshahy, Harbi Sohal, Min Xu, Daniel Baldauf, Yasaman Bagherzadeh Bioki, Rogier Landman, Ben Lu, Justin Kinney, Annabelle Singer, Anthony Zorzos, Brian Allen, Jake Bernstein, Aimei Yang, Brian Chow, Ian Wickersham, Xue Han, Christian Wentz, Nathan Klapoetke, Suhasa Kodandaramaiah, and Giovanni Talei Franzesi for their friendship, support, feedback, and encouragement. I also would like to thank my 61 floor "neighbors": Jeetu Sharma, Robert Ajemian, Margo Cantor, Henry Hall, Christine Keller, and Jannifer Lee. I am grateful to Kostas Tomadakis for fabricating many of my designs over the years, and for the veterinary and animal care staff here at MIT, who make our research possible. Finally, despite my best effort above, it is impossible for me to properly thank everyone who has encouraged, supported, and inspired me here at MIT, in general, and at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research at MIT, in particular. I have been humbled by the interest that has been shown in my work and by the generosity with which so many here have shared their time, equipment, expertise, and insight. The McGovern Institute is a truly special place, and it has been an honor to work here not just among colleagues, but among friends. 4 Table of contents Acknowledgments..................................... . 3 ................ Table of contents .................................-...-.---.------------------------------..................... 5 List of Figures..........................----......--------------------.---...-.........-----Chapter 1: Introduction .. ....................... ....---- 00-...--....--------------------------------- 1.1 Frontal Eye Field ......................--.............----.-..-...............................................- 11 1.1.1 Anatomy ......................................................................................................... 11 1.1.2 Frontal Eye Field Inactivation ................................-........................................... 15 1.1.3 Significance of studying the frontal eye field.......................................17 1.1.4 The pre-motor theory of attention....................................................18 1.1.5 Local FEF 18 -- -----------............................................... circuitry ..................................................................------- 1.1.6 FEF in the context of larger oculomotor and attentional circuits....................................................... 18 19 -----------------------------............................................... 1.2 Optogenetics ......................................... 21 1.2.1 Expressing the opsin in a large enough fraction of neurons................................................................ 1.2.2 Opsin expression in rhesus macaques..............................................................21 1.3 Illumination and challenges with current light propagation models......................................22 1.4 "Backward" effects: activation of neurons with inactivating opsins..................23 24 1.5 Other challenges with primate optogenetics ................................. 1.5.1 Absence of behavioral change......................................................... 1.5.2 Inconsistent behavioral changes ............................................................. 1.6 Motivation for the present study .............................. 24 ................ ---- .... 25 26 .................... Chapter 2: Visible light propagation in the living brain: implications for optogenetics........27 28 2.1 Introduction .........................................................---------------............................................... 2.2 Materials and Methods........................................................................30 2.2.1 Anim al Care........................................................... .................--- ----- ---- 2.2.2 Isometric light measurements ..........................................................................-----2.2.3 Illum inator calibration ....................................................--.................... --- 30 ----............................................. ... 31 .....-------------------........................... 33 2.2.4 Aligner placement and isometric probe insertion ................................ 2.2.5 In vivo light measurements..............................................................................35 5 ... 34 2.2.6 Determining absorption and reduced scattering coefficients ............................................................... 36 2.2.7 Diffusion model of light propagation in tissue ..................................................................................... 37 2.2.8 Deriving absorption and reduced scattering coefficients from light measurements............................ 37 2.2.9 Geometric limit of light propagation in tissue...................................................................................... 38 2.2.10 Kubelka-Munk model...............................................................................................................................39 2.2.11 Internal geometric model ........................................................................................................................ 39 2.3 Results ................................................................................................................................... 40 2.4 Discussion .............................................................................................................................. 45 2.4.1 Comparison of measured absorption coefficients and those estimated based on the hemoglobin content of the b rain ......................................................................................................................................................... 45 2.4.2 Comparison of absorption coefficients reported with values in the literature .................................... 46 2.4.3 Kubelka-Munk model.................................................................................................................................47 2.4.4 Implications for the selection of opsins................................................................................................ 48 Chapter 3: Illuminatorfor optogeneticmodulation oflarge brain volumes........................50 3.1: Introduction .......................................................................................................................... 50 3.2 Materials and Methods...........................................................................................................54 3.2.1 Illuminator construction ............................................................................................................................ 54 3.2.2 Interfacing the illuminator with a laser ............................................................................................... 57 3.2.3 Illuminator profiling and calibration.................................................................................................... 58 3.2.4 Subjects and surgical procedures ......................................................................................................... 61 3.3 Results ................................................................................................................................... 3.3.1 Optical artifact in local field potential .................................................................................................. 63 63 3.3.2 Electrophysiology.......................................................................................................................................65 3.4 Discussion .............................................................................................................................. 67 3.4.1 Potential challenges...................................................................................................................................67 3.4.2 Extensions and further applications .................................................................................................... 68 Chapter 4: Optogenetic modulation of memory-guided saccades reveals that visual, delayperiod and motor-related FEF firingactivitycontributesto behavior..................................70 4.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................................... 4.2 Methods.................................................................................................................................72 4 .2.1 Su bjects......................................................................................................................................................72 6 70 - -... 73 -------------------...............................-- 4.2.2 Surgical procedures ...........................................................--........ 73 4.2.3 Localizing injection targets in behavior monkeys ................................................................................. . 4.2.5 Behavioral testing ..............................................................-............ .... 76 ...... 4.2.4 Virus injections in behavior primates ................................................................. ----------------------.......................... 81 4.2.6 Simultaneous recording and optical inactivation ................................................................................. ....... 82 4.2.7 Classification of behavioral errors .......................................................................... 82 4.2.8 Electrophysiological analysis ....................................................................... ... 4.2.9 Quantification of rebound ................................................................ .---. --------.................... 83 84 4.2.10 Viral vector screening ....................................................................................................------................ 4 .2.11 H istology ................................................................. .......... ... ----------------------------------------------....-------... 85 -------..---------------------------------------------...-. 4.3 Results .....................................................-.---........--. 78 88 4.3.1 Histology.....................................................------------------------------------------------...............................................88 4.3.2 FEF was identified using micro-stimulation ......................................................................................... 89 4.3.3 Spatial specificity of neuronal responses.............................................................................................. 90 4.3.4 Neurons with visual, delay, and motor activity were recorded........................................................... 92 4.3.5 FEF neurons were optogenetically inactivated in both monkeys ......................................................... 94 4.3.6 Overall population level firing-rate analysis ................................................................................. 94 4.3.7 Silencing of individual neurons............................................................................ .. ... 95 ....... 4.3.8 All subtypes of neurons were silenced in both monkeys .................................................................... 4.3.9 Rebound activity ................................................................................. ... . ---...--------------------.............. 102 102 4.3.10 Behavioral results ......................................................................................------..................................... 103 4.4 Discussion ....................................................................................--------------........................ ..... 4.4.1 Significance of the frontal eye field ............................................................................. 4.4.2 Technical and scientific significance ...................................................................................... Appendixes ..................................................................-----..-... 96 103 104 .................-....................... 108 Appendix 1: Shutter characterization plots.................................................................................-108 Appendix 2: Supplemental micro-stimulation and neuronal recording data. ................... 110 Appendix 3: Viral vector selection.......................................................................... ............ 110 Appendix 4: Behavioral data analysis.................................................................................. Appendix 5: List of injected viral vectors. ............ ........ ............. .....................-----.-- 113 Appendix 6: Fluorescence probe leads to isometric response ...................................................... 7 112 115 Appendix 6.1: Geometric estimations of isometric probe shadowing ............................................................. 116 Appendix 6.2: Supplemental methods for ruby sphere calibration ................................................................. 118 Appendix 6.3: Internal geometric model..........................................................................................................123 Appendix 7: Heating measurements in tissue .............................................................................. 125 Appendix 7.1: Possible negative effects of heating in the brain ...................................................................... 125 Appendix 7.2: Neuronal firing rate increases with increased brain temperature ............................................ 125 Appendix 7.3: Heating may play a role in driving behavior in primate visual cortex ....................................... 125 Appendix 7.4: BOLD fMRI activation results from heating alone ..................................................................... 126 Appendix 7.5: Results of temperature measurements in vivo ......................................................................... 127 Works Cited ............................................ ...................................................................... 8 130 List of Figures Figure 1: The Frontal Eye Field in Humans and Macaque Monkeys............................................12 14 Figure 2: Response profiles of FEF neurons. ............................................................................... Figure 3: Light-sensitive proteins, opsins, can modulate neuronal firing...................................20 Figure 4: Absorption coefficients of hemoglobin as a function of wavelength..........................30 Figure 5: Isometric light probe ............................................................................. 33 . --.. . . . . . . . 35 Figure 6: Custom aligner .......................................................................... ------------ ... 36 Figure 7: Experimental setup for in vivo light measurements...................................................... Figure 8: Average light decrease with distance from illuminator.....................................................41 42 Figure 9: Light fall off for all anim als ...................................................................................... Figure 10: Blue light propagation.......................................................................................43 Figure 11: Green light propagation..............................................................................................44 45 Figure 12: Red light propagation ........................................................................ Figure 13: Comparison of large-volume illuminator and standard optical fiber.......................53 56 Figure 14: Etched tip of the large-volume illuminator.................................................................. Figure 15: Optical setup interfacing the large-volume illuminator with a laser........................58 Figure 16: Setup of integrating sphere and illuminator that allow for measurements of the light emission profile along the length of the illuminator.......................................................................60 Figure 17: Locations of the illuminator and recording sites in cortex. ........................................ 62 Figure 18: Local field potential recordings of representative neurons.........................................64 Figure 19: LFP and instantaneous spike values for Monkey L measured using a multi-contact u. probe............................................................................... -- ..... - ....------------------............................ 66 75 Figure 20: Circuit used for electrical micro-stimulation ................................................................ Figure 21: Task used during testing...................................................................................... .......... 78 Figure 22: Injection fields and target presentation locations for both monkeys.........................79 Figure 23: AAV8-Jaws-GFP injection of 1 uL / site at two sites in a histology primate. ............ 89 Figure 24: The end points of saccades ............................................................................................. Figure 25: Firing profiles ............................................................................................................... 9 90 . --91 Figure 26: A neuron with only a visual response ........................................................................... 92 Figure 27: Two neurons with visual, delay, and motor-related activity. ..................................... 93 Figure 28: Distribution of response types among task-responsive FEF neurons.......................94 Figure 29: Decrease in firing rate with illumination for all FEF neurons in both monkeys. ......... 95 Figure 30: Raster plot of optogenetically-inactivated FEF neuron.............................................. 96 Figure 31: Firing rate profile of visually-responsive neurons during target presentation. ...... 97 Figure 32: Firing rate decrease in visually responsive neurons in both monkeys......................98 Figure 33: Firing rate profile of delay - responsive neurons during the delay period .............. 99 Figure 34: Firing rate decrease in delay-responsive neurons in both monkeys............................100 Figure 35: Firing rate profile of motor-related neurons during the motor period........................101 Figure 36: Firing rate decrease in motor-related neurons in both monkeys..................................101 Figure 37: Error rates for both monkeys at different silencing times..............................................102 Figure 38: Characterization of shutter opening ................................................................................. 108 Figure 39: Characterization of shutter closure...................................................................................109 Figure 40: Examples of micro-stimulation evoked saccades............................................................110 Figure 41: Ruby absorption coefficients for red and green light.....................................................115 Figure 42: Demonstration of angle independence of isometric light probe...................................116 Figure 43: Beam profiler used for absolute light power measurements.................118 Figure 44: Water bath for isometric probe calibration. ..................................................................... 119 Figure 45: Beam profile of planar illuminator....................................................................................122 Figure 46: Internal geom etric.............. . --....................................... .............................................. 123 Figure 47: Illustration of how rays can internally reflect and yield a uniform beam in the center. ................................................................ ..-...... . . . ................................................................................. 124 Figure 48: In vivo temperature measurements with the primate specific illuminator for a single laser p u lse ................................................................................................................................................ 128 Figure 49: Additive heating with frequently spaced laser pulses ................................................... 129 10 Chapter 1: Introduction Abstract This thesis uses a novel neuro-modulation technique, optogenetics, to reversibly inactivate the primate frontal eye field (FEF) at different times during the memory-guided saccade task. The scientific contribution of this thesis is a better understanding of the time-dependent contributions of FEF to oculomotor behavior and to short-term memory. The technical contributions include adapting optogenetics to a scientific study in non-human primates and providing methods that allow other groups working with other species (e.g., squirrel monkeys, ferrets, cats, etc.) to adapt optogenetics to their specific needs. The following chapter has two parts, consistent with the dual scientific-technological character of this thesis. The first section describes the frontal eye field and significant questions about this area that can be answered optogenetically. The second section reviews optogenetics and the challenges with its use in non-human primate models. 1.1 Frontal Eye Field By definition, the FEF is the part of the primate prefrontal cortex in which low-current electrical stimulation (as low as 10 tA, <50 paA threshold) reliably evokes saccadic eye movements (Bruce, Goldberg et al. 1985, Murphey and Maunsell 2008). FEF plays a major role in planning and executing saccades and in controlling visual attention. The anatomy of FEF and the firing responses of its neurons indicate that FEF may be critical for sensory, motor, and working memory functions, among others; however, it can be difficult to determine how brain regions like FEF, contribute to larger neuronal circuits. For example, we still cannot pinpoint why inactivating FEF prevents subjects from making memory-guided saccades, a simple task that is extensively used in both primate cognitive neuroscience and clinical neurology. 1.1.1 Anatomy FEF, which includes Brodmann's areas 8Ac and 45 and is located on the rostral bank of the arcuate sulcus near dorsal prefrontal and ventral premotor cortex. Figure 1 shows the locations of FEF in the human and macaque monkey brains. The anatomical relationship of FEF and the other eye 11 fields of the primate frontal cortex are preserved between macaques and humans (Amiez and Petrides 2009), and FEF generally is believed to be present only in higher species although this is debated (Bahring, Meier et al. 1994). Spa 6 - Ew ? Human Macaque Monkey -f-OV Figure 1: The Frontal Eye Field in Humans and Macaque Monkeys Human (left) and macaque frontal eye field (PEP) is highlighted above. Notice the similarity in brain structure between the two species. FEF includes areas 8Ac and area 45 on the rostral bank of the arcuate sulcus (AS) in macaque. FEF is not present in the mouse brain (not shown). Adapted from (Wise, Boussaoud et al. 1997). 1.1.1.1 Cytoarchitecture At the cellular level, FEF has characteristics of both motor and prefrontal cortex. For example, FEF contains many more large Layer V pyramidal neurons, which are typically seen in primary motor areas, than other nearby cortical areas contain (Stanton, Deng et al. 1989). Conversely, even though primary motor cortex lacks granular Layer IV neurons, frontal cortical regions and FEF both contain these granular Layer IV neurons (Stanton, Deng et al. 1989). The cytoarchitecture of FEF hints at its functional responses. 12 1.1.1.2 Receptive fields FEF neurons exhibit spatially selective visual responses, or receptive fields, similar to neurons in primary visual cortex. However, FEF receptive fields are larger and less defined (Mohler, Goldberg et al. 1973) than those in primary visual cortex. Populations of FEF neurons also exhibit so-called motor fields -spatially tuning of firing rate in preparation for a saccade to a particular part of the visual field (Bizzi 1968). Visual receptive fields and motor fields generally overlap within FEF, but they are not necessarily a perfect match, particularly at the level of single neurons (Bruce and Goldberg 1985, Bruce, Goldberg et al. 1985). This thesis studies the visual receptive and motor fields in FEF, which may help us to better understand the interplay between FEF's sensory and motor functions. 1.1.1.3 Firing rate characteristics When Bruce and Goldberg (1985) recorded from single units in macaque FEF during visuallyand memory- guided saccades, they identified three kinds of pre-saccadic activity (visual, motor, and delay-period / anticipatory activity). Forty percent of neurons had visual activity but no motor activity. Twenty percent had little or no visual response, but a discharge prior to movement, and the remaining 40% of neurons had both visual- and motor-related activity. One in five visuomotor and motor neurons showed anticipatory motor activity, but only 2% of visual neurons showed anticipatory activity (Bruce and Goldberg 1985). Hanes et al. (1998) also observed these three classes of neurons in FEF (Hanes, Patterson et al. 1998). The discharge rates during a memory-guided saccade task are shown in Figure 2. A detailed description of the memory-guided saccade task can be found in Chapter 4, Section 4.2.5. 13 A ATF 100 Trigger B 100. C 100. 200 400 -4W -200 0 0 Time from Time from target (mu) saccade (ms) D FT u 100 C# ~ 1 0 200 -200 0 Time from Time from fixation (mg) maccado (mS) Figure 2: Response profiles of FEF neurons. A) a neuron with a primarilyvisual response;B) a neuron with a visual response and delay period activity; C) a neuron with a slight visual response and delay period activity, but primarily a motor response; d) a very rare motor suppression cell. From Hanes et al., 1998 (Hanes, Patterson et al. 1998). The F bar corresponds to "fixation." The T bar corresponds to "target" presentation, and the word "trigger" corresponds to the end of "fixation,"which cued the monkey to preparea saccade. 14 1.1.2 Frontal Eye Field Inactivation Inactivation demonstrates FEF's necessary role in mediating behavior. Conventional techniques for inactivation (i.e., ablation, pharmacological inactivation, and cooling) are either permanent or act over timescales much longer than the actions of the neural circuit (e.g., hours v. milliseconds). Optogenetic inactivation occurs at the millisecond timescale, which is appropriate for dissecting neural circuit function. Earlier studies using cruder inactivation methods, however, have informed the pioneering experiments with optogenetic inactivation in primate FEF as described in Chapter 4. 1.1.2.1 Ablation FEF ablation has modest, and generally temporary, effects on saccade behavior; however, saccade defects become more profound as larger volumes of FEF are affected (Schiller and Tehovnik 2003). For example, three weeks after lesioning the left FEF in a rhesus macaque, Schiller and Chou (1998) demonstrated that the latency of visually-guided saccades to right-sided targets increased by 45 ms, the latency of saccades to left-sided targets decreased by 13 ms, and saccadic peak velocities decreased for right-sided saccades from 534*/s to 459*/s (Schiller and Chou 1998). After four months, though, both the latency and differences decreased, and after a year, no deficit remained (Schiller and Chou 1998). In contrast to the post-ablation compensation seen in saccade tasks, Schiller and Chou (2000) found that an FEF lesion caused a permanent bias in a two-target, temporal asynchrony task. Prior to ablation, the macaque would make a saccade to whichever of the two targets appeared first; however, after unilateral ablation of left FEF, the macaque showed a lasting bias for leftsided targets, even if the right target appeared more than 80 ms before the left target did (Schiller and Chou 2000). 15 1.1.2.2 Pharmacological inactivation Because the brain can compensate for FEF ablation with time, reversible pharmacological inactivators, such as Lidocaine or Muscimol produce more dramatic behavioral defects than ablation does. Dias and Segraves (1996) hypothesize, "Acute inactivation of the FEF of monkeys.. .produced much more severe oculomotor impairment [than ablation did]. This difference is probably due to the acute nature of the Muscimol effect, which does not allow time for reorganization of the control of eye movements before testing begins." One would expect even less reorganization with optogenetics than with pharmacological inactivation because the duration of silencing is even shorter with optogenetics. 1.1.2.2.1 Muscimol inactivation Dias et al., (1995) found that injections of Muscimol in FEF prevented monkeys from making either visually or memory-guided saccades to targets represented by neurons at the center of the injection site. They also reported longer saccade latencies to targets adjacent to the area represented by the injection site. For memory-guided saccades, Dias et al., (1995) reported that saccade latencies to targets in the opposite direction of inactivated receptive field decreased and that the saccades often were initiated prior to the "go-cue" -something we have termed a "premature saccade." Later Dias and Segraves (1999) reported that monkeys initiated fewer saccades to the retinotopic representation of the inactivated site. Further memory-guided saccades to targets in the inactivated site become even less accurate and had decreased peak velocities with longer delay periods (Dias and Segraves 1999). The primates also were less accurate in their fixation to the central cue (Dias, Kiesau et al. 1995, Dias and Segraves 1996, Dias and Segraves 1999). 1.1.2.2.2 Lidocaine inactivation Sommer and Tehovnik (1997) reported similar deficits in memory-guided saccades when they pharmacologically inactivated part of FEF with Lidocaine (Sommer and Tehovnik 1997). Specifically, they found that saccade latency, error rate, and "no saccade" frequency increased for 16 targets represented by the inactivated receptive field (Sommer and Tehovnik 1997). Further, Sommer and Tehovnik (1997) reported an increased frequency of premature saccades to target locations opposite of the inactivated receptive field. Unlike Dias et al., (1995), Sommer and Tehovnik (1997) did not find a consistent effect on saccade peak velocity. In order to estimate the minimum volume of tissue that must be silenced optogenetically to disrupt memory-guided saccades, we consider the Tehovnik and Sommer (1997) studies of Lidocaine in macaque FEF, which are reviewed in more detail in Chapter 2. 1.1.2.3 Significance of pharmacological inactivation Overall, the Lidocaine and Muscimol results could point to deficits in motor activity, perception, working memory, impulse control, saccade initiation, and/or fixation control. The firing activity of FEF neurons implies that the area responds differently during different times in the task, but since pharmacological inactivation encompasses the entire task, we cannot separate different aspects of FEF inactivation from the observed behavior deficits. However, with optogenetic silencing during specific epochs of the memory-guided saccade task, we were better able to tease apart the role of FEF in generating memory-guided saccades, as described in Chapter 4. 1.1.3 Significance of studying the frontal eye field Even, in a straightforward, classic, extensively-used task like the memory-guided saccade, conventional inactivation techniques do not allow us to pinpoint why a particular brain region, (i.e., FEF) is critical for behavior. Therefore, this thesis uses optogenetics to answer the following questions about the role of FEF in memory-guided saccades. Does FEF allow the subject: * To "see" the target through its visual response? STo recall the target through the delay period activity? To execute the saccade? *All of the above? 17 In addition to the basic questions on which this thesis focuses, the results presented in Chapter 4 provide a better understanding of FEF, which is critical to the field of cognitive neuroscience because FEF lies in the center of many open questions, some of which are described below. 1.1.4 The pre-motor theory of attention The pre-motor theory of attention suggests that latent saccade commands give rise to covert spatial attention (i.e., attention to a part of the visual field outside of direct gaze) even if the eye movement is never executed (Thompson, Biscoe et al. 2005). While the pre-motor theory remains controversial, most agree that a better understanding of FEF, which plays a role in generating eye movements (Bruce and Goldberg 1985, Bruce, Goldberg et al. 1985, Hanes, Patterson et al. 1998, Tehovnik, Sommer et al. 2000) and which is involved in spatial attention allocation (Moore and Armstrong 2003, Moore and Fallah 2004), could help improve understanding of the extent to which saccadic control and attentional control share common neuronal mechanisms. 1.1.5 Local FEF circuitry Moreover, the local FEF circuitry, with its diverse neuronal subtypes, is poorly understood. We know that, in addition to the visual-, delay-, and motor-related firing rate changes described above, FEF neurons can be selective for everything from visual features (Zhou and Desimone 2011) to decision signals (Ding and Gold 2012) to auditory responses (Schall, Morel et al. 1995, Kirchner, Barbeau et al. 2009). However, we do not know how or if FEF neurons modulate one another's responses. Understanding FEF better and developing the optogenetics tools to probe functionally heterogeneous brain regions, such as FEF, provide new tools for understanding local microcircuits. 1.1.6 FEF in the context of larger oculomotor and attentional circuits FEF is part of the larger oculomotor and visual attention circuits, and it shares functional similarities with connected brain regions, such as the superior colliculus (SC) and the lateral interparietal area (LIP). The interplay among these areas is an area of active research. McPeek et 18 al. (2003) showed that sub-threshold electrical micro-stimulation of the superior colliculus (McPeek, Han et al. 2003) and FEF (McPeek and Takahashi 2006) resulted in eye movements curved toward the area of the visual field corresponding to the location of micro-stimulation, implying shared control of motor planning. Schafer and Moore (2011) suggested that FEF plays a stronger role in top-down attention while Buschman and Miller (2007) pointed to LIP playing a stronger role in bottom-up attention (Buschman and Miller 2007, Schafer and Moore 2011). In Chapter 4, 1 transiently remove FEF from the oculomotor circuit during motor planning or during the delay period of a memory-guided saccade, which provides some clues about how much shared functions, such as motor preparation, depend on FEF. 1.2 Optogenetics In addition to its scientific contribution, this work makes two key technological contributions. First, this thesis adapts optogenetics, which modulates neuronal firing on the time-scale of the brain, to a scientific study in a primate. Second, it details tools and methods so that researchers who work with other species can tailor optogenetic parameters for their specific experiments. Optogenetics genetically incorporates light sensitive proteins, called opsins, into neurons and subsequently modulates neuronal firing rate with light. Neuronal firing can be driven with excitatory opsins (such as Channel-rhodopsin 2, ChR2) or suppressed by inhibitory opsins (such as ArchT or eNpHR), by illuminating with a particular color of light (Figure 3). In addition, it is possible to target opsins to genetically-defined cell populations (Boyden, Zhang et al. 2005, Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Gradinaru, Thompson et al. 2007, Han and Boyden 2007, Gradinaru, Thompson et al. 2008, Zhang, Prigge et al. 2008, Zhao, Cunha et al. 2008, Chow, Han et al. 2010, Gradinaru, Zhang et al. 2010, Gunaydin, Yizhar et al. 2010, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Yizhar, Fenno et al. 2011, Chow, Han et al. 2012). 19 B A archaerhodopsins and bacterorhodopsins (e.g., Arch, Mac, BR) C halorhodopsins (e.g., Halo/NpHR) channelirhodopsins (e.g., ChR2) H+, Na+, K+, Ca 2 Ci1i H. D E 100% neural sen in cortlcal neurons of awake mio mediated by Arch y -A I -4 - I F Halo expreselng neuron in vitro, quieted by yellow light 1 5 1 5 10 15 20 15 W two different ChR2-expressing neurons in vitro, responding to the samne train of blue lit pulse LLUlUI I 1~ml W 10 50nVI1 a iIJmiL1UJaOD M Is Figure 3: Light-sensitive proteins, opsins. can modulate neuronal firing A) Archaerhodopsin, Bacteriorhodopsins and B) Halorhodopsin are membrane proteins that can be incorporatedinto neurons to silencefiring, D) and E). C) Channelrhodopsinsare also membrane proteins, but illuminating these proteins can drive neuronalfiring, as shown in F). Adapted from (Boyden 2011). Optogenetic manipulations of rodent brain circuits have provided insight into functional physiology (Adamantidis, Zhang et al. 2007, Zhang, Holbro et al. 2008, Airan, Thompson et al. 2009, Cardin, Carlen et al. 2009, Claridge-Chang, Roorda et al. 2009, Sohal, Zhang et al. 2009, Tsai, Zhang et al. 2009, Cardin, Carlen et al. 2010, Carter, Yizhar et al. 2010, Ciocchi, Herry et al. 2010, Haubensak, Kunwar et al. 2010, Johansen, Hamanaka et al. 2010, Lee, Durand et al. 2010, Llewellyn, Thompson et al. 2010, Witten, Lin et al. 2010, Carter and de Lecea 2011) and disease states (Bi, Cui et al. 2006, Alilain, Li et al. 2008, Claridge-Chang, Roorda et al. 2009, Gradinaru, Mogri et al. 2009, Tonnesen, Sorensen et al. 2009, Zhang, Ivanova et al. 2009, Covington, Lobo et al. 2010, Kravitz, Freeze et al. 2010, Tomita, Sugano et al. 2010, Witten, Lin et al. 2010); however, the optogenetics have not yet been applied to scientific questions in primates. Because the macaque brain is 200x larger than the mouse brain and contains almost 100x as many neurons 20 (Herculano-Houzel 2009), two key challenges for primate optogenetics are 1) expressing the opsin in a large enough fraction of neurons over a large enough volume of tissue to impact behavior and 2) illuminating a large enough cortical volume to reliably change behavior with minimal light delivery. 1.2.1 Expressing the opsin in a large enough fraction of neurons While transgenic mice expressing opsins are the model of choice for rodent optogenetics studies, transgenic, opsin-expressing animals do not exist in another other species (except rat) (Tomita, Sugano et al. 2009, Tomita, Sugano et al. 2010). Transgenic macaque models are rare, (Chan 2009, Chan and Yang 2009, Niu, Yu et al. 2010), and none of those reported to date would be viable for primate optogenetics. Further, in utero electroporation (Gradinaru, Thompson et al. 2007, Huber, Petreanu et al. 2008) can introduce opsins into embryos, but this is not practical for primate research given the long gestation period (164 days v. 19 days in mice), low number of offspring per pregnancy (1 v. 7.4 / litter for C57 black mice (Little and Pearsons 1940)), and lengthy time to full maturity (-8 years for male rhesus macaques (Dixson and Nevison 1997) v. 6-8 weeks for mice). While there are three standard ways of incorporating opsins in whole animalstransgenics, in utero electroporation, and viral vectors-only viral vectors are currently practical and feasible in adult macaques. 1.2.2 Opsin expression in rhesus macaques Opsins have been introduced into the primate brain using lentivirus (Han, Qian et al. 2009, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011), AAV1 (Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012), AAV5 (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012) and AAV8 (Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012) with both ubiquitous promoters (Han, Qian et al. 2009, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012) and with neuron-specific promoters (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012). Yet, a particular virus and promoter combination often does not yield consistent neuronal expression levels across species, brain regions, or research groups. For example, a marmoset study of AAV8 found expression of a reporter gene in up to 91% of neurons (Masamizu, Okada et al. 2010), while 21 Cavanaugh et al. (2012) reported transfection of <40% of cells with AAV8 using a ubiquitous CAG promoter and a GFP-tagged inhibitory opsin (ArchT) in the macaque superior colliculus (Cavanaugh, Joiner et al. 2012). Even within the same species, Han et al. (2009) reported that more than half of neurons in macaque cortex expressed ChR2 with a lentiviral construct, but when Gerits et al. (2012) injected the same virus in the same brain area, they found the virus to be ineffective (Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012). With so few reports of opsin expression in primates and disagreement among those reports, there was no obvious choice of virus. Therefore, I systematically screened several viral vectors in the rhesus monkey brain in order to determine the best vector for this particular project and for the general benefit of the field of primate optogenetics. Specifically, I evaluated whether lentivirus or AAV gave better expression, which serotype of AAV gave the best expression, and whether any of the viruses appeared to damage neurons over time. These results are summarized in Chapter 4 and detailed reports are available upon request. 1.3 Illumination and challenges with current light propagation models In addition to better characterization of viral vectors, the optogenetics community lacks (yet desperately needs) systematic measurements of light propagation in the living brain. Without these measurements, optogenetics researchers cannot determine the light power needed to illuminate a given tissue volume. The solution has been to err on the side of over-illumination but increasing the input light power is more likely to result in heating, which 1) could damage tissue and 2) may confound results by increasing neuronal firing rates or driving increases in the fMRI BOLD signal. The potential for tissue heating is described in detail in Appendix 7. Measurements from ex vivo tissue specimens (Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Huber, Petreanu et al. 2008) inform popular irradiance predictors used within the optogenetics community (e.g., (Yizhar, Fenno et al. 2011) and optogenetics.org) while scattering and absorption coefficients taken from ex vivo tissue samples (Yaroslavsky, Schulze et al. 2002) underlie the Monte Carlo models of light propagation published in papers introducing new opsins (Chow, Han et al. 2010) or new illuminators (Bernstein, Han et al. 2008). Unfortunately, models based on in vitro or ex vivo 22 measurements do not reflect in vivo light propagation because in vitro and ex vivo tissue samples differ geometrically from living tissue and contain less blood than living tissue does. Current light propagation models do not show significant wavelength-dependent light propagation differences, but one would expect substantial wavelength-dependent light propagation differences in vivo since all wavelengths of visible light expect for red are absorbed much more readily by the hemoglobin than red light is (Eggert and Blazek 1987, Robles, Chowdhury et al. 2010). Chapter 2 of this thesis describes techniques for in vivo light propagation measurements and presents the results of these measurements in the living brain. These measurements allowed us to design a primate-specific illuminator to deliver light to the desired volume of FEF, which is described in Chapter 4. 1.4 "Backward" effects: activation of neurons with inactivating opsins All of the previous studies that used inhibitory opsins in primate cortex reported a subpopulation of neurons that were activated by the illumination rather than inhibited. Han et al. (2011) expressed an inhibitory opsin (ArchT) in the parietal cortex (area 7a) of one macaque and the visual cortex (area V1) of another macaque (Han, Chow et al. 2011). The primates were awake and freely viewing during the recordings, in which Han et al. (2011) reported a significantly reduced firing rate in 60% of neurons (45 / 74) relative to baseline and a significantly increased firing rate in 10% (7 / 74) of neurons during illumination (Han, Chow et al. 2011). Diester et al. (2012) found no change in movements elicited by electrical stimulation with simultaneous illumination in an eNpHR2.0-injected area of motor cortex despite eNpHR2.0 expression in 70% of neurons, based on histology. In contrast to the histology, Diester et al. (2012) reported that only 38% of neurons (55/144) were silenced in this area of the cortex and -10% of neurons (15/144) increased their firing rate in response to illumination (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011). Han et al. (2011) and Diester et al. (2012) both hypothesized that these heterogeneous neuronal responses resulted from inactivating inhibitory neurons. Results from Ohayon et al., (2013) seem to be consistent with this hypothesis. Ohayon et al., (2013) used a lower light power density (190 23 mW/mm2 ) than Diester et al, (2012) or Han et al., (2011), yet Ohayon et al., (2013) still reported increased firing with inhibitory opsins (ArchT and eNpHR2.0). It is possible, though, that the "backward" effect in Ohayon et al. (2013) may have resulted from heating (even at the lower light power), from an interaction between light delivered to these sites and adjacent locations that were injected with ChR2, or possibly from the network effects and inactivation of interneurons that Han et al., (2011) and Diester et al, (2012) proposed. Regardless of the source of these heterogeneous effects, activating some cells while simultaneously inhibiting others could negate the overall effects of modulation, particularly if the goal is to modulate behavior. Thus, this work tackles this potential problem both by achieving high levels of neuronal expression across all layers of cortex (limiting potential network effects) and by carefully controlling for possible effects of heating. 1.5 Other challenges with primate optogenetics The challenges discovered in previous primate optogenetics studies (e.g., varying virus expression levels, inverse modulation, and little behavioral modulation) raised questions about whether optogenetics could be meaningfully applied to a scientific study in non-human primates. Chapter 4 of this thesis presents results that affirm the viability of optogenetics in scientific studies using non-human primates. Specifically, Chapter 4 shows consistent, optogeneticallydriven behavioral changes in two monkeys. Below, there is a brief review the groundwork that others have laid for primate optogenetics and the challenges that they have identified. 1.5.1 Absence of behavioral change To date, there have been nine published reports of optogenetics in rhesus macaques (Han, Qian et al. 2009, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012, Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012, Ohayon, Grimaldi et al. 2013, Ruiz, Lustig et al. 2013, Dai, Brooks et al. 2014). Behavioral modulation or subthreshold "optical micro-stimulation" has been reported in primates, but only with excitatory opsins (Gerits, Farivar 24 et al. 2012, Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012, Ohayon, Grimaldi et al. 2013, Dai, Brooks et al. 2014) or by targeting structures in the brainstem (Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012). Three studies reported optogenetic inactivation in primate cortex, but reported behavioral changes. Diester et al. (2012) injected viruses containing ChR2 in somatosensory cortex in two macaques and in motor and pre-motor cortex of a single macaque. Despite expression in >65% of neurons in motor cortex, no behavior changes were evoked (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011). Han et al. (2011) reported inactivation but no behavioral results. Ohayon et al, (2013) did not report any behavioral changes with ArchT or eNphR2.0 (inhibitory opsins) in FEF. 1.5.2 Inconsistent behavioral changes Cavanaugh et al. (2012) expressed ArchT in the superior colliculus of three rhesus macaques and studied the effect of inactivation during visually-guided saccades in two of the primates and during free viewing in the third. Despite large light power densities (650 - 1600 mW/mm 2 ), there were only modest effects. Visually-guided saccades showed a shift in saccade end points (7.3% and 4.6% of saccade magnitude in the two primates, respectively), a reduced peak velocity (decreases of 79*/s and 9.6*/s, in the two monkeys, respectively), and increased latencies (7 ms and 7.9 ms). Cavanaugh et al. (2012) reported that neuronal response decreased 68.2% on average in one macaque and 27.4% in the second, but it is not clear whether there were inverse effects (i.e., excitation with illumination) included in this average. Regardless, Cavanaugh et al. (2012) suggest that effects were much stronger in one primate than the other due differences in the extent of neuronal silencing, but they do not provide insight into what methodological improvements could be made to ensure consistent silencing across primates (Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012). In the Gertis et al. (2012) study, rhesus macaques injected with ChR2 in FEF performed a visuallyguided saccade task during an fMRI. The group reported that saccade latencies were shorter to ipsilateral targets in cases of optical stimulation than in non-stimulation trials, but in one rhesus macaque (M2), the difference between stimulation and non-stimulation trials was present for both ipsilateral and contralateral targets, implying an attentional increase in subject M2 coincident with the illumination alone. Further, the authors suggested that the other monkey (Ml) may have 25 sustained unilateral FEF damage, which alone may explain an ipsilateral, but not contralateral, effect. Better methodological controls (e.g., confirming whether the primate sustained a lesion prior to testing, controlling for light leakage during illumination, running sham illumination in non-injected areas, etc.) may have been sufficient to explain inconsistent behavioral results. Ohayon et al., (2013) reported a few evoked saccades with optical stimulation using ChR2 in FEF, but the effect was not replicable across sessions, monkeys, or even nearby locations within FEF. 1.6 Motivation for the present study FEF plays a critical role in executing memory-guided saccades. Using optogenetics, we can determine which of these firing rate increases-visual, delay, or motor-are behavior-critical by inactivating FEF on the millisecond time scale of the neural circuit. In order to apply optogenetics to a scientific study in the primate, I had to overcome specific technical challenges. Chapter 2 presents measurements of light propagation and heating in the living brain. Chapter 3 describes a specialized illuminator that delivers light over a large volume of the frontal eye field without overheating the cortical tissue. Chapter 4 describes the identification of viral vectors sufficient for primate physiology and details techniques for monitoring viral vector expression in real-time in the living brain. Finally, Chapter 4 also details the successful manipulation of memory-guided saccades in the rhesus macaque through optogenetic FEF inactivation. From these results, I determine that FEF critically contributes to saccades through its visual activity, its delay-period activity, and its motor activity because silencing FEF at any of these times during the task increases the saccade error rate. 26 Chapter 2: Visible light propagation in the living brain: implications for optogenetics Abstract The distribution of red, green, and blue light was measured in the anesthetized rodent brain using a novel approach designed for in vivo, visible light measurements. 12 A 1.5 mm-diameter illuminator was placed on the surface of the cortex. Fluence rates were measured at depths of 0.5 to 2.5 mm using an isometric light probe. Mathematical models based on diffusion theory were applied to these measurements, and the absorption and effective coefficients in the living brain were derived for red, green, and blue light. The in vivo absorption coefficients for green and blue light are substantially higher than the in vitro and ex vivo coefficients typically used in the literature to estimate light propagation in optogenetics studies; however, the values presented here closely match the absorption coefficients that one would expect based on the amount of blood in the living brain and the absorption coefficient of oxygenated blood. These results imply a substantial advantage for red-light sensitive opsins in terms of lower light power requirements, decreased risk of heating, and larger potential illumination volumes. Keywords: optogenetics, tissue optical properties, absorption, scattering, brain optical properties, isometric, Glossary: KM-Kubelka-Munk, a model of light loss as a function of distance. NA - numerical aperture 1Yellow light was not tested because a suitable laser at the light powers needed for these experiments and the heating experiments (>100 mW total light power after coupling) was not commercially available. Further, at the time of this study, evidence to suggest that neuronal silencing with ArchT (green-light responsive) was substantially better than eNpHR (yellow-light responsive) had already been published, e.g., Han et al., 2011 and Chow et al., 2010. Thus, the true comparison was whether to use green-light sensitive ArchT or a new red-light sensitive halorhodopsin, Jaws. 2 Blue light was tested, even though no blue-light sensitive opsin was considered for the primate study, because ChR2 (a blue light sensitive excitatory opsin) is used more than any other opsin. Thus, information about blue light is generally valuable to the field. 27 2.1 Introduction While optogenetics is widely used in vivo, current models of visible light propagation use tissue properties derived from in vitro or ex vivo specimens (Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Huber, Petreanu et al. 2008, Yizhar, Fenno et al. 2011). These measurements inform popular irradiance predictors used within the optogenetics community (e.g., the "Stanford" predictor (Yizhar, Fenno et al. 2011) and optogenetics.org), which estimate light propagation based exclusively on scattering and geometry, ignoring absorption. Even when groups have attempted to include absorption coefficients via Monte Carlo models, e.g., (Bernstein, Han et al. 2008, Chow, Han et al. 2010), they had resort to ex vivo-derived coefficients (Yaroslavsky, Schulze et al. 2002) because in vivo absorption and scattering coefficients for non-red, visible light have not been determined. In vivo preparations contain flowing, oxygenated blood unlike either ex vivo or in vitro preparations. The hemoglobin in blood may prove critical to understanding the optical properties of the brain because hemoglobin attenuates visible light more than other tissue components. Further, hemoglobin does not just scatter light, it absorbs light in a wavelength-dependent manner. One would expect red light to propagate much further in vivo than any other color of visible light because, as Figure 4 shows, all wavelengths of visible light expect for red are absorbed much more readily by the hemoglobin than red light is (Eggert and Blazek 1987, Robles, Chowdhury et al. 2010). It is likely more important to account for light absorption than any other optical property of tissue. Unlike scattering and geometric factors, which spread light over larger volumes, absorption converts light energy to heat. Temperature impacts neuronal firing and excessive illumination can damage tissue, as is reviewed in Appendix 7. In vivo light propagation measurements are feasible, as the photodynamic therapy community has demonstrated, but these measurements are only practical with current techniques for longer wavelengths that propagate far through tissue (such as those in the deep red and infra-red range). The parts of diffusion theory used to estimate absorption and scattering in these experiments require the light source to approximate an isotropic point source. This assumption can only be 28 made when the light measurement probe is far from the light source. These distant measurements cannot be made with visible light of the wavelengths typically used in optogenetics because that light does not propagate that far in living, blood-perfused tissue. Here, techniques for in vivo light measurements used in the photodynamic therapy community are adapted to visible wavelengths in a novel experimental preparation. Effective and absorption coefficients were determined in the living mouse brain for red (635 nm), green (532 rum) and blue (473 nm) light. These values can be used directly in models of light propagation (e.g., Monte Carlo models) or heating (via the bioheat equation). Recently, the optogenetics community has expanded to include many different species of animal models (Gradinaru, Zhang et al. 2010, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012, Roberts, Gobes et al. 2012), wavelengths of light (Zhang, Prigge et al. 2008, Chow, Han et al. 2010, Gunaydin, Yizhar et al. 2010), illumination sources (Zorzos, Boyden et al. 2010, Anikeeva, Andalman et al. 2012, Stirman, Crane et al. 2012), and tissue targets-both in the brain and in other parts of the body (Arrenberg, Stainier et al. 2010, Cheng, Zhang et al. 2012). The techniques presented here are flexible enough to be adapted to any of these recent developments and to continue to grow with the field. Experimenters can use these techniques to directly measure light propagation in their specific experimental preparations, reducing the risk of heating, over-illumination, and tissue damage regardless of the wavelength, illuminator or animal model. 29 400 HbO 2 Hb deoxy 350 300 _U 250 U 1200 8 1 .a 4 15 IVV I - 50 ' 45 0 0~' 500 ------------ " " - 0 550 600 Wavelength (nm) 650 700 Figure 4: Absorption coefficients of hemoglobin as a function of wavelength Based on data provided courtesy of Scott Prahl, Oregon Medical Laser Center 2.2 Materials and Methods 2.2.1 Animal Care Sixteen male C57BL/6J mice (Taconic) aged 8-16 weeks were used for light power measurements (n = 5 for 473 nm; n = 6 for 532 nm; and n = 5 for 635 nm). All procedures were approved by the MIT Committee on Animal Care and were in accordance with the NIH Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Prior to surgery, analgesics (buprenorphine, 0.1 mg/kg, IP; Meloxicam, 2 mg/kg, IP) were administered. An anti-inflammatory corticosteroid (Dexamethasone, 1 mg/kg, IP) was administered to prevent edema. Under isofluorane anesthesia (1-4% isoflurane in oxygen), the top of the head was shaved and a depilatory (Nair) was then applied to ensure complete fur removal. The field was scrubbed 3x with betadine and isopropyl alcohol prior to opening. A 3 mm (medial-lateral) x 2.5 mm (rostral-caudal) craniotomy was made in the parietal 30 bone. For consistency, the top left corner of the craniotomy was always 0.5 mm caudal and 0.5 mm lateral to bregma. To keep the cortical surface free from blood, a thin coat of transparent silicone (Kwik-Sil, WPI) was placed in a ring around the bony margins of the craniotomy, as needed, to prevent bleeding. The craniotomy was kept moist with saline. Once the craniotomy surgery was complete, pentobarbital (50mg/kg, IP) was administered, and 3 isoflurane anesthesia was removed 3-5 minutes later, depending on the depth of anesthesia. Mice remained anesthetized in the stereotax and on the heating pad during testing. Reflexes were tested regularly to ensure adequate depth of anesthesia. Supplementary pentobarbital (25 mg/kg, IP) was administered as needed. Euthanasia was performed at the conclusion of testing. If bleeding on to the surface of the craniotomy was detected either pre- or post-experiment, the animal was excluded from this study. 2.2.2 Isometric light measurements Because opsin-expressing cells can be modulated by photons approaching from all directions, an isometric light probe is needed to measure how much light is reaching a particular point in the brain. Isometric light measurements also allow tissue optical properties to be derived from diffusion theory. The ruby-tipped probes used in this study are isometric because fluorescence is an inherently isometric process. The incident angle of the excitatory blue, green, or red photon has no bearing on the emission angle of the measured ruby-colored photon (Skinner 1964). Appendix 6 describes fluorescence and isometric theory in more detail. 3 Isofluorane was used to quickly induce anesthesia in the test mice and maintained during surgery. It was not continued during testing, though, because isofluorane increases cerebral perfusion which can lead to bleeding or cerebral edema (Sicard, Shen et al. 2003), both of which would complicate the light measurements. Cerebral blood flow with pentobarbital is close to what is seen in the awake animal (Goldman and Sapirstein 1973). 31 2.2.2.1 Isometric probe construction Isometric, ruby-tipped light probes similar to those presented by Bays et al. (Bays, Wagnieres et al. 1997) were constructed (Figure 5a). A 300 pm diameter spherical ruby ball lens (NT46-223, Edmund Optics) was centrally affixed to the flat-cleaved, polished end of a 400 tm diameter multimode optical fiber (ThorLabs, BFH48-400, NA = 0.48) with optically-transparent, UV curable adhesive (NT37-322, Edmund Optics). An SMA connector on the other end of the multimode optical fiber allowed the probe to interface with the spectrometer. 2.2.2.2 Spectrometer set up The fluorescence emitted from the isometric light probe was measured using a CCD spectrometer (HR2000, Ocean Optics) and recorded using SpectraSuite software (Ocean Optics). Prior to testing, a dark spectrum measurement was taken and subtracted out of all subsequent measurements. A non-linearity correction was applied using the default linearity coefficients of the spectrometer. The integration duration of the spectrometer was adjusted to prevent saturation in the ruby wavelength range (690 nm to 695 nm). Integration times ranged from 3 ms to 3 s with most integration times in the 0.1 to 1 s range. All recorded spectra were saved in their raw form without averaging. Figure 5b shows an example spectrum with peaks representing both the incident light and the ruby fluorescence. Only the ruby peak was used in analysis, and all photon counts were converted to fluence rates with units of photons/second in offline processing. 2.2.2.3 Isometric probe calibration The probe was calibrated in water by applying collimated light. Prior to calibration the beam of the collimated light was profiled with a beam profiler calibrated for absolute light power measurements (BC106-Vis, ThorLabs). The linear relationship between incident light power density at the test wavelength and the emitted fluorescence at the peak ruby emission wavelength (-695 nm, defined as the 0.47 nm-wide bin with peak amplitude between 693 nm and 697 nm) was determined for each probe. A linear relationship between the applied light power density and the ruby photon emission rate was generated as shown in Figure 5c. Two small corrections 32 were applied to the slope prior to using it for testing. The first accounted for the difference in the refractive indices of the brain, n = 1.37, (Binding, Ben Arous et al. 2011) and water, n = 1.33. The second accounted for the difference in the angle between the light source during (900) and testing (720). See Appendix 6 for detailed probe calibration methods and theoretical explanation of the calibration process. Output from photon counter Ruby sphere 3am dlamtr Sample calibration curve relating photon count to light power - 4000 :~3500- 4 3.5 3000 2000 r 3 Applied light wavelength 5r o 1000 ( a) b) y5.57710E-05 2 1500- Ruby wavelength 500 400 500 800 700 800 Wavelength(nm) 900 RS9.95746E-01- 0 1000 0 20000 40000 60000 80000 100000 c) Figure 5: Isometric light probe Isometric light probe (a), spectrometer output from probe (b), and calibrationcurve (c) 2.2.3 Illuminator calibration The light power output from the planar illuminator was measured prior to each testing session. For red (635 nm) and green (532 nm) light tests, an analog, fiber-coupled DPSS laser (Shanghai Laser Optics Company) was connected to a TTL-controlled optical shutter (OzOptics, SH-200-33470/700-M-0-0-SP) via a 200 tm diameter (numerical aperture = 0.22) multimode FC/PC terminated fiber. For 473 nm light, a fiber-coupled diode laser (Vortran) was used. The output from the shutter was FC coupled to the planar illuminator-a 1.5 mm diameter (numeric aperture = 0.5) plastic optical fiber (CK-60, Industrial Fiber Optics, Inc.). The planar illuminator, which was fixed in the aligner, as shown in Figure 6, was lowered into an integrating sphere (S142C, ThorLabs) with attached power meter (PM100D, ThorLabs). 33 For the red and green lasers, a 15 minute warm up was required for stable output. Afterward, the laser power was manually adjusted to achieve total light powers from the planar illuminator of 0.44, 0.88, 1.77, 3.53, and 8.83 mW. When these quantities are divided by the area of the planar 2 , illuminator (1.77 mm2 ), they yield light power densities of 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, and 5mW/mm respectively. For blue light tests, total light powers of 1.17, 1.57, 2.35, 4.03, and 8.83 mW were , used. These correspond to, light power densities of 0.66, 0.885, 1.33, 2.275, and 5 mW/mm 2 respectively. 4 2.2.4 Aligner placement and isometric probe insertion To ensure alignment between the planar illuminator and isometric probe, a custom aligner (Figure 6) was fabricated. The aligner was attached to a stereotactic arm and lowered on to the surface of cortex using a micromanipulator (Siskiyou). 5 The isometric probe was mounted to stereotactic holder (Kopf) and lowered at a 28' angle through the custom aligner. Ruby fluence 4 The blue light powers differ from those of red and green because the integrating sphere used initially consistently misreported low powers of 473nm light. This was identified after the first three blue-light tests. The first integrating sphere reported light powers of 0.44, 0.88, 1.77, 3.53, and 8.83 mW yielding light power densities of 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, and 5mW/mm 2, but measurement with a second, new integration sphere reported light powers of 1.17, 1.57, 2.35, 4.03, and 8.83 mW yielding light power densities of 0.66, 0.885, 1.33, 2.275, and 5 mW/mm 2, respectively. Because the difference in the reported values between the two integrating spheres was consistent and replicable, data from the three mice already tested was included and subsequent mice were tested with the powers that had already been used with blue light. In these subsequent tests, the new integrating sphere reported the values listed above and the old integrating sphere reported the incorrect values, as expected. Calibrations were repeated after testing as well. The actual light powers used do not matter for the final normalized result. s Despite prophylactic dexamethasone administration, some mice had slight cerebral edema. To address the variability of tissue height relative to the skull, a low intensity of light was coupled into the planar illuminator during lowering. This created a well-defined spot on the surface of the brain that changed color when the illuminator was fully in contact with the cortex (similar to the change in the color of green laser pointer when one shines a green laser pointer on his thumb). This change in color allowed experimenters to ensure that the planar illuminator was fully on the surface of the cortex. In a few cases, the laser light became visible to the experimenter during the shallowest testing depth, presumably because the dexamethasone had taken effect only after the aligner had been placed. In those cases, the aligner could not be lowered further as this would risk damaging the isometric probe. Thus, an adjustment was made to the depths if they were with 0.1 mm of a 0.5 mm increment and if the isometric probe monitoring confirmed that the brain had sunken between aligner placement and the first measurements. 34 was monitored in real time during lowering and retraction using the spectrometer and SpectraSuite software. Planar illumina 1.5mm diameter The ruby sphere probe will be lowered into cortex across the beam path of the planar \ illuminator. Planar illuminator Figure 6: Custom aligner Custom alignerviewed from the top (left) andfrom the side (right). 2.2.5 In vivo light measurements Fluence rates were measured in 0.5 mm increments from 0.5 mm to 2.5 mm deep to the surface of cortex. At each depth, continuous light pulses were applied at power densities of 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, and 5 mW/mm2 via the planar illuminator. Note that for blue light these light power densities were slightly different as described in the calibration section above. Integration times ranged from 3 - 3000 ms, with higher powers requiring shorter integration times. During a single pulse, the spectral output from the light probe was recorded at least 25 times for each applied light power density. The pulse durations ranged from 5-20 s, depending on the maximum integration time possible without saturating the spectrometer measurement (i.e., without exceeding the maximum photon count at the ruby wavelength). 6 Figure 7 shows a diagram with the 6 The total pulse length necessary to achieve 25 repeats was a bit longer than the sum of the 25 integration periods due to transfer time between the spectrometer and the computer running the SpectraSuite software. 35 experimental set up for in vivo light measurements. Note that the hardware set up is nearly identical to that used for calibration. The probe was not retracted until the end of testing to minimize bleeding. Offline analysis and plotting were performed in MATLAB. Photon count Information from spectrometer - Laserandshu mo eter Loser S * enablel power, signal S I.1ser Cofto fiberprb fiber al Light measurement optical optical Lae Light probe Aesthetized mouse Iflum, .- Figure 7: Experimental setup for in vivo light measurements 2.2.6 Determining absorption and reduced scattering coefficients In the photodynamic therapy literature, in vivo light propagation measurements of red and infrared light are made several millimeters away from a small, narrow light source. This paradigm allows for absorption and scattering coefficients to be estimated using simple diffusion theory equations for isotropic point sources. For visible light of sub-red wavelengths, it is not 36 feasible to use point source estimates because too little light reaches these distant points to get accurate measurements. Thus, this work uses a wide, collimated-beam estimate to determine scattering and absorption coefficients. For this work, a planar illuminator was used to approximate a wide, collimated-beam source (Appendix 6). 2.2.7 Diffusion model of light propagation in tissue According to diffusion theory, in the limit where Ita, the absorption coefficient, is much larger than ts', the reduced scattering coefficient, a wide-beam collimated irradiance incident on a thick slab with refractive index matched boundaries has the following fluence rate, PDt(z): (Pt(z) (1) = (5/(1 + 2*a/peff))*exp(-peff *z)-2*exp(-pt*z) where z is the vertical distance from the illumination source to the point measured, teff is the effective attenuation coefficient, and tt is the total attenuation coefficient. Assuming ia / [s <<1, no sources deep within tissue, and a semi-infinite medium with isometric scattering, the effective and total attenuation coefficients can be related to one another as follows: leff~ (2) sqrt(3*Ia* lit) Equation 2 can be rearranged to solve for the total attenuation coefficient, ut = je/ .tt: (3) (3* p.) Equation 3 can be substituted into Equation 1 to yield Equation 4 Pt(z) =(5/(1 + 2*p/i /jeff))*exp(-eff *z)-2*exp(-pe# *z/3/p,) (4) A full derivation of all of the equations shown above can be found in (Star 2011). 2.2.8 Deriving absorption and reduced scattering coefficients from light measurements Two manually calibrated spectrometers (HR2000, Ocean Optics) were used in this study, and their peak ruby wavelengths differed slightly, leading to different ruby wavelength ranges. The 37 ruby wavelength range was 695.7 to 698nm for the older device and 692-695 nm for the newer spectrometer. All tests with a given ruby sphere probe were performed using the same spectrometer that was used for calibration without disconnection. In SpectraSuite, photons were measured across a range of "ruby" wavelengths. Offline, in MATLAB, the photon count was normalized by the integration time to yield a fluence rate (photons/ second). Fluence rates for each condition in each mouse were averaged across all trials at every wavelength (omitting the first trial due to possible software lag). The calibration curve was used to convert the mean fluence rates (photons/second) into light powers (mW). The light power reaching a given depth was divided by the applied light power to yield a normalized fluence rate (or fraction of light power remaining). The normalized fluence rates for all light powers at a given depth in a given mouse were averaged to yield mean normalized fluence as a function of distance from the illuminator for each mouse. The normalized fluence functions for every mouse were fit to Equation 4 using a least-squares approach. All available depths were included in the fit for each mouse. All fits had an R2 value > 0.95. From those fits, aand Leff were determined for every mouse. Those absorption and effective coefficients were then averaged across all mice of a given color to yield the estimated absorption and effective coefficients for a given color. 2.2.9 Geometric limit of light propagation in tissue Light power density in tissue decreases as a function of distance from the optical fiber due to geometry. Light clearly spreads out as it exits the tip of a multi-mode optical fiber so it makes sense that there would be some geometric limit to how much light could reach a given depth in tissue even in the absence of scattering or absorption. The Kubelka-Munk (KM) model (Kubelka 1948) has been proposed as the geometric limit for light reaching a given point in tissue, both in the light dosimetry literature (Eggert and Blazek 1987, Star, Marijnissen et al. 1987) and in the optogenetics literature (Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Yizhar, 38 Fenno et al. 2011). This study puts forth an alternative geometric limit: the internal geometric model. 2.2.10 Kubelka-Munk model The most commonly used estimate for geometric light decrease is the Kubelka-Munk model (Equations 5 and 6). I (z) _ I (z=O) p2 (z+p) (5) 2 where (6) p r- Here I(z) is the flux at depth z, I (z =0) is the initial flux, r is the radius of the optical fiber, n is the refractive index of the medium, and NA is the numerical aperture of the optical fiber. Arvanis et al., 2007 expanded the KM model to include a wavelength dependent scattering factor, S, as shown in Equation 7. This has now become the basis of the Stanford predictor. I (z) I;(z=o) _ - p2 (sz+1)(z+ p) 2 2.2.11 Internal geometric model In a multi-modal optical fiber, many modes of light are coupled into a single optical fiber. The modes exit the fiber at some point on the surface of the tip and at some angle relative to the fiber surface. This model of geometric loss was developed to estimate the light due to light spread as different modes of light exited the tip of the fiber at different locations and angles. For a light mode to spread maximally away from the multi-mode optical fiber, it would have to exit the fiber tip at the farthest distance possible from the center of the fiber (r = rfiber) along the trajectory of the maximum divergence angle, 0&iv, which is calculated in Equation 8. (8) Odv = sin- 1 NAfiber ntissue 39 where ntissue is the refractive index of the tissue and NAfiber is the numerical aperture of the fiber. This maximally spreading mode crosses the center of the optical fiber at a distance, zfiber above the tip. Zfiber is calculated in Equation 9. Zfiber - (9) rfiber tan(Odiv) At a distance zfiber away from the tip of the fiber, all of the modes crossing the exact center will be emitted at some angle ranging from - Odiv to Ouv. None of those modes will be internally reflected because they all have a trajectory angle, 0 5 1 Odiv I. For points off of the center axis at distance zfiber from the tip, some of the modes crossing through those points will be internally reflected while others would be emitted from the tip (Appendix 6). The launch angle and point along the fiber can be calculated in each case. The code for this function is available upon request. By integrating across the longitudinal axis of the fiber, a profile of modes at the tip of the fiber can be generated with each mode having a launch angle, and a launch distance from the center of the fiber. Those profiles at the tip can be fit to a model which projects where they will go at various distances from the tip of the illuminator. 2.3 Results Using the methods described above, normalized fluence rates were determined as a function of distance from the illuminator for red, green, and blue light. Figure 8 shows that red light propagates much further in tissue than either green or blue light does and that very little of the applied green and blue light remains at distances 1 mm or more from the light source. Curves based on Equation 4 were fit to the average normalized fluence rate for each mouse (Figure 9). All fits had R-squared values > 0.95, typically > 0.99. For red (635 nm) light, the mean best fit for the absorption coefficient was found at ta = 2.14 cm-1 (standard error = 0.15 cm1). For the effective coefficient, the best fit was Ideff = 6.61 cm 1 (standard error = 0.53 cm-1). For green (532 nm) light, the mean best fit was found at Ida = 13.7 cm- 1 (standard error = 1.33 cm-1 ) and 4eff = 41.1 40 cm-1 (standard error = 4.04 cm-1 ). For blue (473 nm) light, the mean best fit was found at lta = 10.6 cm-1 (standard error = 0.486 cm-') and e = 31.8 cm-1 (standard error = 1.46 cm-'). Mean light fall off with standard error -- 0.9 - 473 nm 532 nm 635 nm - 1 0.8 E 0.7 0.6 0.5 0 0.4 LL 0.3 - 0 C. 0.2 0.1 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 Depths (mm) Figure 8: Average light decrease with distance from illuminator Mean normalizedfluence rates representing the fraction of applied light power reaching a given depth for blue (473 nm), green (532 nm) and red (635 nm) light as a function of distancefrom the illuminatorwith standard error bars 41 Light fall off with distance 1 473 nm 532nm 0.9 - - - 0.7 a - -% t% 0.6 \~%% 4-. 0.5 0. - %% A '4*4'4"b OA 0 % *4t*f* C 0.3 X4"W f 0.2 0.1 0 ) *4 *4 % .0 635nm 0.8 4, 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 Depths (mm) Figure 9: Light fall off for all animals Normalizedfluence ratesfor each animal are plotted (markers) and curves for each mouse were fit using Equation 4. From these curve fits values for the absorptionand effective coefficients at each wavelength were determined. Next, the light powers found here for the three colors of light were plotted against two commonly used estimates for light propagation (Figure 10, 11, and 12). First, the Stanford prediction was plotted. Recall that the Stanford online calculator of irradiance combines the KM estimate with scattering estimates to predict light fall with distance (Equation 7). Next, the KM estimation was plotted to represent the widely-assumed geometric limit which light powers should not exceed even in the absence of scattering and/or absorption. Finally, the new "internal geometric" estimate of the geometric limit of light propagation was plotted as an alternative geometric limit, contrasting with KM. The in vivo measurements for red light exceed the KM model, which is not supposed to be possible if KM is truly the geometric limit. The in vivo measurements never exceed the 'internal geometric" 42 estimate, which unlike the KM estimate, accounts for the internal reflections within multi-mode optical fibers. For blue light, the in vivo measurements generally match with the Stanford estimate at 473 nm (Figure 10). For green light, in vivo measurements are lower than predicted by the Stanford estimate. Some part of this difference between the Stanford estimate and the in vivo data may result from a slight difference in the wavelength used for the estimate (560 nm) and the actual measurements (532 nm). For red light, the in vivo and estimate wavelengths are much closer (630 nm for the estimate, 635 nm for the tests), and the difference is even greater. Measurements and estimates of blue light propagation x x xxx x x xxxxxxx x xxxxx .x X X 0.8 x x x AA -I Measured light powers: x x (in vivo) 0.6 -A A Predictions: A -------- Stanford A A -r Current study - A Kubelka Munk AA 0.4 (geometric limit) AAAA x IAAAAAAAAA& U- Internal geometric (geometric limit) AAAA 0.2 AAAAAAAA ............ 0 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 Depths (mm) Figure 10: Blue light propagation Blue light propagationin this study compared with the widely-used Stanford predictor and two estimates of the geometric limit (Kubelka Munk and the internal limit presented here). 43 Measurement and estimates of green light propagation I 1 -x xx x I x x x III x xx x xx xXXxxx x x A Measured light powers: - 'A x x A A x AA 0.6 Predictions: A A A A a A AA AA OA A x A A AA AA AAA A A Kubelka Munk (geometric limit) Internal geometric (geometric limit) A A U- Current study (in vivo) (532 nm) xx Ax 01 -- - 0.8 A A A AA A A A A 0.2t OL 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 Depths (mm) Figure 11: Green light propagation Green light propagationin this study compared with two estimates of the geometric limit (Kubelka Munk and the internal limit presented here). Data from the Stanford predictor is omitted because the closest available value is 560 nm, whereas these measurements were taken at 532 nm. 44 Measurement and estimates of red light propagation 1 x xx xx xxx xx xx xx x x x x~ x .Ax X -A 1E 0.8 S -- A A Measured light powers: - Current study X x (invivo) . .Predictions: A '.- -------- Stanford . Kubelka Munk x (geometric limit) internal geometric (geometric limit) SOA 0 -.. LA. 0.2 -*-**--.*-mea 0I 0 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 Depths (mm) Figure 12: Red light propagation Red light propagationin this study compared with models based on ex vivo measurements, the widelyused Stanford predictor and two estimates of the geometric limit (Kubelka Munk and the internal limit presented here). 2.4 Discussion 2.4.1 Comparison of measured absorption coefficients and those estimated based on the hemoglobin content of the brain Since, to our knowledge, there are no other in vivo measurements of light propagation at 473 nm and 532 nm, it may prove useful to consider the physiology of the cortex in the context of the green and blue light absorption estimates. At 473 and 532 nm, the only major source of absorption in tissue is hemoglobin. In particular, most absorption is due to oxygenated hemoglobin because oxygen saturations are typically >95% in healthy individuals (Koeppen and Stanton 2009). Within the cranium, blood and CSF each comprise 10% of the total volume while the brain makes up the 45 other 80% (Rengachary 2005). Assuming that 3-5% of the intracranial blood is in large vessels (e.g., the Circle of Willis) and that the rest is in tiny vessels throughout the brain tissue itself, it is reasonable to expect that the blue and green absorption coefficients in brain tissue would be 57% of the absorption coefficients for oxygenated hemoglobin in whole blood. In Figure 4, the absorption coefficient for oxygenated hemoglobin 235 cnr1 at 532 nm. Thus, at 473 nm, our value of aH _oxy = 165.35 cm.- at 473 nm and a = 10.6 IaHb-oxy = cm1 (standard error = 0.486 cm-1) is 6.4% of the absorption coefficient of oxygenated hemoglobin. At 532 nm, our value of 4a = 13.7 cm- 1 (standard error = 1.33 cm- 1) is 5.8% of the absorption coefficient for oxygenated hemoglobin at that wavelength. The absorption coefficients determined here are consistent with physiology of the brain, and imply that absorption is the main source of light loss for green and blue light in the living brain. 2.4.2 Comparison of absorption coefficients reported with values in the literature Even though, we know of no other in vivo reports of absorption coefficients at 532 nm and 473 nm blue light comparisons with in vitro and ex vivo data may provide insight into how close these two types of measurements are. At 473 nm, this paper reports Via = 10.6 cm1 (standard error = 0.486 cm'). Gottschalk (Gottschalk 1992) reported Ia= 9 cm-1 at 456 nm for in vitro for gray matter (reprinted in (Mobley 2003)). Mesradi et al., 2013 reported Va Z 4.5 cnrl for fresh slices of rat brain and 4a = 3 cm1 for frozen slice of rat brain (Mesradi, Genoux et al. 2013). Yaroslavsky et al., 2002 reported Via = 0.7 cm1 and Ia = 1.4 cm-1 for gray and white matter, respectively, based on measurements taken in post mortem human tissue (Yaroslavsky, Schulze et al. 2002). The Yaroslavsky et al. values have been used widely in the optogenetics literature (Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Bernstein, Han et al. 2008, Gutierrez, Mark et al. 2011, Kahn, Desai et al. 2011, Yizhar, Fenno et al. 2011, Han 2012, Jing, Fabien et al. 2012, Osawa, Iwasaki et al. 2013) despite being an order magnitude lower than the in vivo measurements presented here. At 532 nm, this paper reports Va = 13.7 cm-1 (standard error = 1.33 cm-1). Again this value is similar to what Gottschalk (Gottschalk 1992) reported for in vitro gray matter at 514 nm (ha = 11.7 cm-1). The Yaroslavsky et al., values are even lower for green light (reported at 510 nm) than they were 46 for blue with ta = 0.4 cmr 1 and ta = 1 cm-1 for gray and white matter, respectively. Mesradi et al., 2013 report comparable values for green light as well as for blue (Mesradi, Genoux et al. 2013). In vitro measurements of human gray matter taken 24 hours post mortem yielded Jta = 19.5 cm- 1 at 514 nm; however, that paper notes that a separate sample from this patient's brain contained melanoma (Sterenborg, Saarnak et al. 1996). Thus, the tissue yielding the measurement was not from a healthy brain, and it is even possible that some melanin expressing cells were present in the sample that was taken as "gray matter." In contrast to the lower two wavelengths, the literature offers some, limited in vivo light measurements of red (635 nm) light. Our value of mice is a bit higher than was reported in rats Jta= La = 2.14 cm- 1 (standard error = 0.15 cn- 1) in 0.57 cm-' at 632 nm (Angell-Petersen, Hirschberg et al. 2007); however, this difference may result from our measurements being taken in gray matter while the other measurements were likely taken in gray matter and the thalamus in order to keep the probe far enough from the source to use the point-source approximation in diffusion theory. Overall values from extracted tissue have a wide range of absorption coefficients from ha = 0.20 to 4.5 cm-1 (Wilson, Patterson et al. 1986, Flock, Wilson et al. 1987, F. 1995, Sterenborg, Saarnak et al. 1996, Yaroslavsky, Schulze et al. 2002, Angell-Petersen, Hirschberg et al. 2007, Mesradi, Genoux et al. 2013). Our value for red light falls squarely within that range. 2.4.3 Kubelka-Munk model Even though the Kubelka-Munk (KM) model has been applied to biological tissues and in estimates of light propagation for optogenetics, its primary and more appropriate application is in the paint industry (Star, 2011). This model assumes completely diffuse light (Kubelka, 1948), which is appropriate for the ambient room lighting incident on a painted wall, but not for the output of a laser-coupled optical fiber in tissue. Further, the Kubelka-Munk model essentially assumes a one dimensional process in which scattering decreases flux with distance. This differs from transport theory, where the three dimensional radiance decreases due to geometric spread, scattering, and absorption. In addition, the KM model assumes that the radius of the fiber, r, is much smaller than the distance between the optical fiber and the light source, z. In optogenetics, 47 researchers often use optical fibers with diameters on the order of 0.1 mm, and they couple their electrodes and optical fibers so that they can record from targets only a few hundred microns from the light source. Thus, the KM model assumption that r << z does not hold for in vivo optogenetics experiments, like the ones in this study. The KM model seems to have become popular for use with optogenetics for three reasons: 1) it is simple mathematically, 2) when the radius of the optical fiber is much smaller than the measurement distance the source begins to resemble the isotropic, diffuse, uniform source assumed in the KM model, and 3) the in vitro and ex vivo preparations used for light measurement contain little blood, and, thus, have little absorption due to hemoglobin. Often these ex vivo preparations are purposefully exsanguinated, but even if they are merely excised, much of the blood is lost in the excision process and any remaining blood would contain only deoxygenated hemoglobin. Thus, in these ex vivo preparations, scattering is the dominant cause of light loss while in an in vivo preparation, absorption is the dominant cause of light loss. The fact that our measured red light values exceed the maximum predicted values possible (in the absence of absorption and scattering) suggests that the KM model is inappropriately applied as a geometric limit and that a model such as the internal geometric model presented in Appendix 6.3 would be more appropriate. 2.4.4 Implications for the selection of opsins Red light propagates much farther in tissue than green light or blue light does. Models based only on scattering and/or geometric loss fail to demonstrate just how dramatically absorption attenuates light in the living brain. When models fail to account for absorption, the difference in red and blue/green light propagation is less apparent. The in vivo measurements presented here show the dramatic advantage of using red light in the brain rather than other visual light colors. Now, that the difference is better understood, the choice of opsin for neuroscience studies may shift to reflect this finding. In particular, new red-light sensitive opsins like Crimson (Klapoetke, 48 Murata et al. 2014) and Jaws (Chuong 2014) are likely even better choices for most neuroscience studies than ChR2, eNpHR, Arch, or ArchT. Jaws differs from previous inactivating opsins because it has a red shoulder, meaning that in addition to having a peak sensitivity in the yellow range (like other halorhodopsins) it is also sensitive to light at higher, red wavelengths, which is sometimes called a red shoulder. Further, these red-light sensitive opsins are great candidates for primate neuroscience or even for translational applications. Thus, for this particular study, we chose to use Jaws, a red-light sensitive opsin that inhibits neuronal firing rather than Arch / ArchT or eNpHR, which are sensitive to green and greenish-yellow light, respectively. 49 Chapter 3: Illuminator for optogenetic modulation of large brain volumes Abstract A large-volume illuminator was developed for optogenetic manipulations in the non-human primate brain. The light-emitting surface of this illuminator is two orders of magnitude larger than a conventional optical fiber of equal diameter, which allows for substantially greater light delivery to brain tissue with no greater penetration damage than a conventional fiber. This illuminator delivers light in the macaque cortex over a tissue volume of ~ 10 mm3, which is at least 2 mm in diameter and at least 3 mm in length. When used in conjunction with a red-light sensitive halorhodopsin, Jaws, neuronal silencing spanning a 3 mm depth was observed 1 mm away from the light source. This illuminator may prove to be an enabling technology for widespread optogenetics use in non-human primate studies. Keywords: primate optogenetics, large-volume illumination, halorhodopsin, channelrhodopsin, illuminator Glossary: Jaws-a red-shifted halorhodopsin LFP - local field potential 3.1: Introduction While optogenetics is widely used in the rodent neuroscience community (Boyden 2011, Bernstein, Garrity et al. 2012), this technique has been used only a handful of times in non-human primates, primarily in proof-of-concept studies (Han, Qian et al. 2009, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012, Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012, Dai, Brooks et al. 2014). The size of the macaque brain is a challenge for optogenetics. Because the macaque brain is 200x larger than the mouse brain, the tissue volume comprising a region of interest in the macaque brain may be significantly larger than an analogous tissue volume in a rodent (Herculano-Houzel 2009). For example, macaque tracer studies have found single axons that span distances longer than the entire mouse brain (Amir, 50 Harel et al. 1993), and electrophysiological studies show that the point image spread in macaques is up to 10 mm in certain brain areas (such as 7a) and at least a few millimeters in most other visual areas (Hubel and Wiesel 1974, Dow, Snyder et al. 1981, Gattass, Gross et al. 1981, Van Essen, Newsome et al. 1984, Tootell, Switkes et al. 1988, Andersen, Asanuma et al. 1990, Boussaoud, Desimone et al. 1991, Felleman and Van Essen 1991). Thus, in order to silence or drive a neuronal population in the primate brain corresponding to a particular function or representation using optogenetics, tissue volumes on the order of cubic millimeters may need to be illuminated. Previous optogenetics studies in the primate have illuminated cortical tissue in a manner similar to what Aravanis et al., (2007) presented for rats (Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Gradinaru, Zhang et al. 2010, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Dai, Brooks et al. 2014). In this "dual-pronged optrode," a solid-state laser diode is coupled to a flat-cleaved glass or silica multimodal optical fiber, which is glued to an electrode and inserted into the brain as a single unit (Aravanis, Wang et al. 2007, Han, Qian et al. 2009, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012, Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012). Other groups have identified the need to illuminate larger tissue volumes in the primate brain and proposed possible solutions. Diester et al., (2011) coupled multiple flat-tipped fibers to a single electrode (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011), while Tamura et al., (2012) enclosed several flattipped optical fibers and a tungsten microelectrode together in glass (Tamura, Ohashi et al. 2012). Unfortunately, these approaches double or triple the diameter of the illuminator that is inserted into the brain and have been noted to cause damage (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011), perhaps due to the large brain volumes that must be displaced for a given degree of illumination achieved. Because penetration damage is proportional to the diameter of whatever is inserted into the brain (Sharp, Ortega et al. 2009), the large-volume illuminator described here was designed to increase the volume of light delivery without increasing the fiber diameter or the surface light power density. 51 As shown in Figure 13, the large-volume illuminator has the same diameter as a standard optical fiber (70 -250 microns), but its light emitting surface area is on the order of cubic millimeters, rather than tens of microns. When placed in a brain phantom (such as the 1" cube of 1.75% agarose shown in Figure 13) and coupled to lasers with equal total input light power, the large-volume illuminator delivers light over a larger volume than the traditional fiber does. The surface light power density of the large-volume illuminator is approximately two orders of magnitude smaller than the light power density at the tip of the traditional fiber because its light emitting surface area is approximately 5 mm 2 compared with 0.05 mm 2 for the traditional fiber. Even so, the large volume illuminator distributes light more broadly. This broad distribution allows both light and heat to spread more evenly through the tissue without creating a "hot spot" near the fiber tip. Beyond its tip geometry, the large-volume illuminator differs from conventional optical fibers because it is made of a flexible, biocompatible plastic rather than glass or silica. Further, it is made of a single material without any glue, doping, or surface asymmetry. As Ozden et al., (2013) noted, the axial asymmetry of "dual-pronged optrodes" induces cortical damage and increases the risk of optical fiber breakage in the brain (Ozden, Wang et al. 2013). While coupling the illuminator and electrode is still feasible, this illuminator delivers light to a large to enough volume that optogenetic silencing may be recorded with a separately inserted electrode placed a millimeter or more away from the illuminator. Thus, this fiber has a reduced risk of breaking both because it is made of plastic and because it is not necessary to couple it to an electrode. While this illuminator allows for broad tissue illumination regardless of wavelength, it is most powerful when used with red light, which is less readily absorbed by tissue than any other color of visible light. The illuminator presented here delivered light over a 3 mm depth of macaque cortex (>10 mm 3) and silenced neurons expressing the red light-sensitive halorhodopsin, Jaws, (Chuong 2014) over the same distance. 52 Figure 13: Comparison of large-volume illuminator and standard optical fiber. The large-volume illuminator(left) and a standardopticalfiber with equal diameter and equal total input light powers are shown in a block of 1.75% agarviewed from the top (top) and side (bottom). 53 3.2 Materials and Methods 3.2.1 Illuminator construction Illuminator construction began with etching the light emitting surface of the fiber, near the tip. Figure 14a illustrates the result of tip etching by comparing the cross section of the tip of a traditional optical fiber with the cross section of the tip of the large volume illuminator. A 22gauge wire stripper (StripMaster) was used to remove the polyethylene jacket 15- 20 cm from the end of a 250 tm diameter plastic optical fiber (Industrial Fiber Optics). A table vise clamp (Wilton) was locked onto the stripped end of the fiber while the experimenter held the jacketed end taut and parallel to the floor (perpendicular to the vise). The stripped section of the fiber was thinned to a diameter of 60-100 pm using the lower setting of a dual temperature (570 / 1000'F) heat gun (Milwaukee). While still holding the fiber taut, the experimenter removed heat and allowed the fiber to cool, which prevents the fiber from curling. Once the fiber cooled, the experimenter gripped the fiber about an inch to either side of its thinnest point and pulled the fiber apart to create a tapered tip. While this pulling technique requires some practice, particularly in regulating the heat to prevent the fiber from melting or curling, several members of the lab have mastered the technique in less than a day. Next, the tapered tip of the fiber was examined under a 4x dissection microscope (VistaVision). Figure 14b shows the tip of the illuminator grossly and Figure 14c shows the tip under a microscope. Fibers with forked or curled tips were discarded. To etch the tip, the length of desired light emission (typically 3-7 mm) was measured up from tip and lab tape (VWR) was applied above that level. This left only the desired length of light emission exposed and protected the rest of the fiber during etching. The exposed tip of the fiber was then uniformly etched on all sides using a 5 tm silicon carbide lapping sheet (ThorLabs) followed by a 3 .tm aluminum oxide lapping sheet (ThorLabs). At the other end of the fiber, the 22-gauge wire stripper was used to remove about 5 mm of the jacket. This end was cut flat using a hot knife (Industrial Fiber Optics) and polished to a smooth finish using successively smaller fiber polishing sheets ranging from 5 tm to 0.3 tm (ThorLabs). 54 A fiber microscope (ThorLabs) was used to ensure a smooth and uniform polish. The flat end of the polished fiber was inserted into a stainless steel ferrule with 260 urm inner diameter (Precision Fiber Products) until it was flush with the coupling end of the ferrule. The fiber was then secured in the ferrule with plastic epoxy (Industrial Fiber Optics). Excess epoxy was removed after 12 to 24 hours of curing. A dust cap (ThorLabs) was placed on the ferrule to protect the fiber. Figure 14d summarizes the fabrication process. 55 Standard fiber IlII uminator Cladding Cladding Core b) a) c) I d) Figure 14: Etched tip of the large-volume illuminator (A) Cartoon cross section showing how both the core and cladding are etched to spread light broadly out of the tip. (B) The tip of the illuminatortapersfrom a diameter of 250 ym to 60-100 pm. (C) Cladding is removed along the last 5 mm of the penetrating illuminator. When a laser is applied to the far end of this illuminator, light is emitted over the 5 mm long etched tip. (D) This flow chart describes the fabrication processfor etched large-volume illuminators. 56 3.2.2 Interfacing the illuminator with a laser A diode-pumped solid state (DPSS) laser (Shanghai Laser Optics Co.) was coupled to a mechanical shutter (Oz Optics) via a 200 lim diameter glass optical fiber (Thor Labs). A PC running MonkeyLogic software (University of Chicago) and a DS8000 digital stimulator (World Precision Instruments, Inc.) controlled the shutter opening and laser output. A 200 tm diameter glass optical fiber (ThorLabs) coupled the output of the shutter to the illuminator with a ceramic mating sleeve (Precision Fiber Products). This setup (Figure 15) was used for both the calibration and in vivo testing. 57 Laserandshutter controlsgn 2 Laser senable TTL Laser DPSSopen power 10speca sign tt r or opbicaoptical 'Ollr fier fber Mating Illuminator a) Ceramic Patch cable with FC connector mating Illuminator b) Figure 15: Optical setup interfacing the large-volume illuminator with a laser. (A) The full optical setup is shown here in block diagramformat. (B) A close-up of the opticalfiber mating sleeve / illuminatorinterface. 3.2.3 Illuminator profiling and calibration To ensure uniform light emission along the etched fiber tip, a lowering test was performed. Specifically, the illuminator was lowered with a micromanipulator (Siskiyou) into an integrating sphere (ThorLabs) in 500 mrincrements. Using light absorbing foil (ThorLabs) and alumuminumn 58 foil (Reynolds), custom shielding, with non-reflective material on one side and reflective material on the other, was created. The custom shielding placed over the top of the integrating sphere, and the large volume illuminator passed through a 26-gauge hole in the shielding. No ambient light or light from parts of the fiber above the sphere could enter the integrating sphere because the diameter of the hole in the shielding matched the diameter of the penetrating illuminator. The total light power output was measured along the length of the illuminator and plotted as a function of how far the illuminator had been lowered into the integrating sphere (Figure 16). Some novel tip geometries (e.g., diffuser tips) introduce an optical mismatch between the optical fiber and a secondary material, which decreases total light power output and causes some light energy to be dissipated as heat. To confirm that this illuminator does not lose light energy due to its geometry, light power measurements from a flat cleaved optical fiber and the illuminator were compared under identical input conditions. After initial tests showed identical total light power output for the current illuminator and flat-cleaved fibers, this testing was not continued. 59 Cladded optical fiber Cadded optical fiber / .-- \ '4 4$ fiber I In' fiber Iris Integratin sphere a) .......... ... ........................................ Illumination profile of representative illuminator with standard error bars Light absorbing coating 0.8 ht 0.6 F 0 S0.4 0.2 0 b) c) 1 2 3 4 5 Length of illuminator in integrating sphere (mm) Figure 16: Setup of integrating sphere and illuminator that allow for measurements of the light emission profile along the length of the illuminator. (A) External view of profiling set up. (B) Cross-sectional view of illuminator profiling set up. (C) Representative illuminatorprofile with the fraction of total light emission measured as a function of the length of the illuminatorthat is in the integratingsphere to be measured. 60 6 3.2.4 Subjects and surgical procedures Two male rhesus macaques weighing 13 kg and 16 kg were tested. The animals were cared for in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the guidelines of the MIT Animal Care and Use Committee. All surgical procedures were carried out under anesthesia. Antibiotics and analgesics were administered postoperatively, as needed. In each monkey, a recording well (19 mm inner diameter, Crist Instruments, Co.) over the frontal eye field and a titanium head post were implanted surgically under aseptic conditions. After electrophysiological and micro-stimulation mapping, a viral vector containing Jaws (AAV8-hSyn-Jaws-GFP) was stereotactically injected into cortices of both monkeys. Illumination testing and electrophysiological recording began six weeks after virus injection. 3.2.4.1 In vivo illumination During all testing, the animal was seated in an ergonomically-designed chair facing a computer screen with its head held. Two micromanipulators (NAN Instruments, Inc.) were mounted on the recording well. A custom grid was placed in the recording well to ensure that the illuminator and electrode were lowered in parallel one millimeter apart from one another. The grid was locked into place relative to the implanted recording well such that the virus injected sites could be accurately targeted months after injection. The illuminator was lowered to the desired depth through a 25-gauge stainless steel guide tube held by one of the drives, while an electrode was lowered in parallel 1 mm away through another guide tube using the second drive. In monkey C, a single-contact, parylene-coated tungsten microelectrode with approximately 1 MQ resistance (WeSense) was used, while in monkey L, a 16-channel multi-contact electrode (u-probe, Plexon) was used. The illuminator and u-probe remained in the same location throughout a recording session, but the single-contact electrode moved along its insertion trajectory, to allow for recording at different depths along the length of illumination. Prior to testing, the experimenter visually confirmed that the primate was not exposed to any stray laser light. The entire implant and optics setup remained shielded with light-absorbing foil (ThorLabs) throughout testing. 61 Figure 17 shows the recording, illumination, and virus injection locations on an MRI image for both monkeys. Subject C Subiect L a) c) b) Figure 17: Locations of the illuminator and recording sites in cortex. (A) Coronal MRI imagefrom Monkey C showing the grid through which electrodes and the illuminator are lowered and the target region of interest (white box). Yellow lines denote the sides of the recording chamber. (B) Enlarged view of the region of interest with the trajectories of the illuminatorand electrode shown as well as the virus injection region and the locations of the sample neurons shown in Figure 18 and 19. Note for spatial reference that the electrode and illuminatortrajectories are separatedby 1 mm. (C) and (D) Same as (A) and (B), respectively, except with the imagingfor Monkey L. 62 3.2.4.2 Recording and analysis The electrode input was coupled into a pre-amplifier (Plexon) via a headstage (Plexon) and electrical connector (Onnetics). The pre-amplifier split the data into spike channels (0.25 -8 kHz) sampled at 40 kHz and local field potential channels (0.7 - 170 Hz) sampled at 1 kHz. Preamplifier output went into a multi-channel acquisition processor, or MAP box, (Plexon) where it was filtered and acquired using the Rasputin (Plexon) software. Spikes were sorted offline using principal component analysis and manual waveform shape analysis (Offline Sorter, Plexon). MATLAB (Mathworks) was used for electrophysiological data analysis and plotting. 3.3 Results 3.3.1 Optical artifact in local field potential An optical artifact results when metal electrodes are illuminated with light (Ayling, Harrison et al. 2009, Han, Qian et al. 2009, Cardin, Carlen et al. 2010, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Ozden, Wang et al. 2013) presumably due to the Becquerel effect, as per Han, 2012, The presence of a light artifact in the local field potential (LFP) indicates that light is reaching a particular cortical location, and because the light artifact is even observed in saline, it is an opsin-independent marker of tissue illumination. However, as others have noted, the light artifact varies across locations and between recording sessions (Ayling, Harrison et al. 2009, Han, Qian et al. 2009). Here, the optical artifact is used only as a digital marker that light has reached a particular point in the tissue, not as an analog readout of how much light has reached that point. The optical artifact was observed over 3 mm depth of cortex, which coincided with neuronal silencing in areas that had previously been injected with the halorhodopsin, Jaws. Figure 18 shows the actual LFP deflection that resulted during illumination in both animals. 63 Light artifact at 0.5 mm Instanteous firing rate at 0.5 mm A0 I. C aIL -J A)l) -500 0 1I91 Tkm (n"s)50 A)lI) 0 iTme (rns) Light artifact at 1.5 mm 1 0 Instariteous firing rats at 1.5 rm 112C C 6C 0. -J IN B)) 5 1000 Tme (mn) 0 We 0n) O 1000 Instanteous firing rate at 2.5 mm Light art actat 2.5 m f120 so -J C)I) -500 6 0 Om)3oTim C)ii) too I Light artfact at 3.5 mm 0 Tom (m) 300 OW Instanteous firing rate at 3.5 mm .30 D)I) -400 500 s1mm D)li) 0 . Is a. So0 Time (Msr0 s0 Figure 18: Local field potential recordings of representative neurons. Local field potential recordingsof representativeneurons at the locations shown in Figure17 (left column) and instantaneous spike time histograms (right). The red bar denotes when the laser is on. Note that the virus injections were performed at depths of 1 mm relative to the surface of cortex to 3.5 mm relative to the surface of cortex in 0.5 mm intervals. All of this data comes from single contact recordingsin Monkey C. 64 3.3.2 Electrophysiology Illuminating neurons that express a halorhodopsin with light of the appropriate wavelength suppresses neuronal firing (Han and Boyden 2007, Gradinaru, Thompson et al. 2008, Arrenberg, Del Bene et al. 2009, Sohal, Zhang et al. 2009, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011) and often leads to rebound excitation after illumination ceases (Arrenberg, Del Bene et al. 2009, Tonnesen, Sorensen et al. 2009, Tsunematsu, Kilduff et al. 2011, Brown, Tan et al. 2012, Madisen, Mao et al. 2012, Schone and Burdakov 2012, Smith, Virkud et al. 2012). When the previously-injected area was illuminated, both the expected neuronal suppression during the light pulse and the rebound excitation post-illumination were observed. This is consistent with light reaching these neurons and corroborates the LFP findings for the same locations. The left column of Figure 18 shows the LFP at four locations spaced one millimeter apart from one another along the recording trajectory for Monkey C shown in Figure 17. The right column of Figure 18 shows the instantaneous firing rate for four example neurons recorded in Monkey C at the same locations. Figure 19 shows single unit recordings and LFPs from Monkey L taken using a multi-contact electrode (Plexon, u-probe) at depths denoted between the 'A' and 'B' points in Figure 17d. 65 Mean LFP Values for lilmitnated Channels L*SWr on an uwrobe 13 400 a) 4m, 0w sic loco 4O0100 Tbm (Ins) rel. to lser onset kitanteous spke rats for Contact 11 Instenteous spike rat lo Contact 10 30 Lawe - Lawan I20 0. 4 mo e"IS) rel b) m Go to Ia~n" e m we m C) Instentuous spike rate for Contact 12 reF -Law Tnetms oalownset lnstataous spike rate for Contact 13 LAW o Lwon 30 20o Ito 110 - M0 d) a 200 raw 400 400 ON 1400 12W200 to la assnt t)m%)retDIsw~ a ma a m logo ima Instanteous spike rate for Contact 15 Iistenteous spike rate for Contact 14 -cor" n Sin 9) am oes) ra etoWo Tm, (ts) rettotow onset Figure 19: LFP and instantaneous spike values for Monkey L measured using a multi-contact u-probe. 66 (Caption continuedfrom Figure 19 on previous page) Only channels 6-16 are included because channels 1-5 were superficial to the illuminatorand, therefore, not illuminated. (A) Mean LFP values for channels 6-16. All illuminated channels have a light artifactregardlessof whether there was neuron (colored traces) or no single unit neuron present (gray traces) on the channel. (B) - (F) The instantaneous spike ratesfor illuminated channels with a single unit neuron present. Note that the neurons on all channel were either partly or completely silenced during illumination. The spacing between the contacts is 0.2 mm. 3.4 Discussion This chapter presents a novel illuminator with diameter equal to that of a conventional fiber and with a light-emitting surface area two orders of magnitude larger than that of a conventional illuminator. This illuminator allows for dramatically increased illumination without increasing the penetration diameter. Further, this illuminator can be used in the primate cortex to illuminate tissue along a length of at least 3 mm and a volume of -10 mm 3.Most importantly, this illuminator can silence neurons expressing a halorhodopsin (here, Jaws) over this same span, thus, facilitating optogenetic silencing of tissue volumes relevant in primate neuroscience. 3.4.1 Potential challenges The main challenge with this illuminator is mastering the fabrication technique. The experimenter must learn what tension needs to be applied to the fiber during the heating process. Applying too much tension will cause the fiber to snap while applying too little allows the fiber to curl upon itself. Additionally, the experimenter must practice applying heat to the fiber in order to stretch it enough to taper it, but not enough to curl or break it. Even after mastering fiber heating, the experimenter must check the tip of each fiber to ensure that it is not curled or forked before placing it into the brain. While the authors have experimented with automated methods of heating and tapering the fiber (e.g., a pipette puller), none of these have provided results as consistent as a practiced individual can achieve manually. A second challenge involves maintaining the mechanical integrity of the illuminator after fabrication. Unlike glass or silica optical fibers which break when they are mechanically overstressed or bent too sharply, plastic optical fibers curl or kink. If the illuminator presented here is forcefully advanced toward the brain before the guide tube has penetrated the dura, the tip will 67 curl on itself. It is apparent when this occurs because the fiber will not enter the brain and will not advance further through the guide tube. This curling destroys the tip of the fiber, but spares the tissue. If the same force were applied to a glass fiber, it is possible that shards of glass would be driven into granulation tissue, the dura and/or the brain. Secondly, kinks can develop in plastic optical fibers due to overly sharp bending. Just as light leaks out of glass fibers when they are bent or crushed, light will leak out from the kinked region of a damaged plastic optical fiber. Both plastic optical fiber illuminators (such as this one) and glass fibers should be handled carefully and examined regularly to ensure that they have not sustained mechanical damage. 3.4.2 Extensions and further applications While this chapter presents illuminators with etched, light-emitting surfaces 3-5 mm in length, longer etched tips are possible for studies requiring illumination over longer trajectories and shorter tips could be manufactured for more focal illumination. Though this study uses 250 im diameter fibers, other fiber diameters could be used depending on the desired application. Further, one could use non-uniform etching to create "dead spots" where no light is emitted. This would allow for light to be delivered selectively to one layer of cortex. Because acrylic is more biocompatible than glass (Williams 2001) this illuminator could be placed chronically in the brain with fewer complications than one would expect with traditional optical fibers. Further, there is some evidence that more flexible electrodes (e.g., Tungsten micro-wires) cause less tissue damage than rigid electrodes because there is less shear stress and tearing as the electrode moves relative to the brain (Freire, Morya et al. 2011). Similarly, this more flexible plastic illuminator may cause less damage chronically than a rigid glass fiber would. Although the fiber was tested in the cortex, it is reasonable to assume that it could be used successfully in deeper brain areas, such as the thalamus, amygdala, or subthalamic nuclei. While neuronal silencing is demonstrated here using Jaws, this illuminator could be used with many different opsins, including excitatory opsins, such as the recently developed Chrimson molecule that is 45 nm red-shifted beyond previous opsins (including VChR2, C1V1, and ReaChR), and has a spectrum similar to that of Jaws (Klapoetke, Murata et al. 2014). Finally, this illuminator 68 geometry has the potential to eventually be used in human applications where there is an even greater need to illuminate larger brain volumes, should optogenetics be used in a clinical scenario (Chow and Boyden 2013). 69 Chapter 4: Optogenetic modulation of memory-guided saccades reveals that visual, delay-period and motor-related FEF firing activity contributes to behavior Abstract The previous chapters lay the groundwork for applying optogenetics to behavioral study in nonhuman primates. Light propagation in living tissue was measured to choose the best color for large volume illumination (red) and, thus, the best opsin (Jaws). An illuminator specifically for large volumes was developed. In this chapter, virus expression was evaluated while tissue heating was addressed in Appendix 7. With the technology optimized, optogenetics could finally be applied to more than just a proof-of-concept study. In this chapter, we present optogenetic silencing of nearly every task-relevant neuron in a large region of frontal eye field (FEF). Error rates increase with FEF inactivation during the memory-guided saccade task regardless of when the inactivation occurred during the task. This demonstrates that visual, delay, and motor-related firing in FEF all contribute to the proper execution of memory-guided saccades. Keywords: primate optogenetics, frontal eye field, saccade, memory-guided, oculomotor, halorhodopsin, Glossary: Jaws-a red-shifted halorhodopsin FEF - frontal eye field RF -receptive field 4.1 Introduction The frontal eye field (FEF) is critical for executing saccades to remembered target locations. When FEF is pharmacologically inactivated, subjects can no longer perform the memory-guided saccade task (Dias, Kiesau et al. 1995, Dias and Segraves 1996, Sommer and Tehovnik 1997, Dias and Segraves 1999); however, it is unclear from the firing patterns of FEF neurons why the saccade cannot be executed. FEF neurons exhibit spatially selective visual responses (i.e., receptive fields), 70 which are larger and less well-defined than those in primary visual cortex (Mohler, Goldberg et al. 1973). Populations of FEF neurons also exhibit motor fields, which correspond to the spatial tuning of firing rate in preparation for a saccade to a particular part of the visual field (Bizzi 1968). Visual receptive fields and motor fields generally overlap within FEF (Bruce and Goldberg 1985, Bruce, Goldberg et al. 1985). In addition to visual and motor-related activity, FEF neurons often have sustained, spatially-specific increases in their firing rate throughout the delay period of a memory-guided saccade task (Bruce and Goldberg 1985, Hanes, Patterson et al. 1998). Taking these results together, it is unclear if FEF inactivation prevents subjects from executing memoryguided saccades due to a lack of visual activity (inability to see the target), a lack of delay activity (inability to remember the target location), a lack of motor activity (inability to move one's eyes to the target), or some combination of these. This study used temporally-precise optogenetic techniques to inactivate neurons FEF during only one epoch of the memory-guided saccade task in each trial. This approach is conceptually straightforward, and it disambiguates the behavioral contributions of FEF neuronal firing at various times during the memory-guided saccade task. However, this approach is technically challenging because much larger tissue volumes must be inactivated than are typically modulated with optogenetics in other species (e.g., rodents), as discussed in Chapter 1. Acute inactivation of a large tissue volume seems to best allow for observation of FEF-related saccade deficits. With either ablation or pharmacological inactivation, saccade deficits become more profound as larger volumes of FEF are affected (Schiller and Tehovnik 2003); however, over time, saccades return to normal even after ablation (Schiller and Chou 1998). Dias and Segraves (1996) write, "Acute [pharmacological] inactivation of the FEF of monkeys.. .produced much more severe oculomotor impairment [as compared with ablation]. This difference is probably due to the acute nature of the Muscimol effect, which does not allow time for reorganization of the control of eye movements before testing begins." (Dias and Segraves 1996) Optogenetic inactivation is orders of magnitude more temporally precise than pharmacological inactivation (i.e., on the order of milliseconds rather than hours); however, inactivating a large enough fraction of neurons over a large enough tissue volume is a technical challenge for primate optogenetics. 71 Pharmacological inactivation studies suggest that in order to disrupt memory-guided saccades nearly every FEF neuron in a tissue volume of -10 mm 3 must be inactivated (i.e., the firing rate must be reduced by > 80%). When Tehovnik and Sommer (1997) studied the effects of Lidocaine in macaque FEF, they determined that silencing nearly all 100% of neurons within a 1.5 mm radius of the injection site (-14 mm3) prevented saccade execution to the effected receptive field (Sommer and Tehovnik 1997, Tehovnik and Sommer 1997). Martin et al., (1999) and Dias et al. (1999) both reported that silencing 100% of neurons in a 1 - 1.5 mm (4.2 - 14 mm2 ) sphere of FEF with either Lidocaine or Muscimol is sufficient to disrupt memory-guided saccades as well. Inactivating -80% of neurons by cooling part of FEF also can disrupt saccades (Chafee and Goldman-Rakic 2000). Thus, the technical goal for this study was to reduce the firing rate of at least 80% of neurons by >80% over at volume of ~10mm 3, as determined using micro-stimulation, the standard technique for mapping receptive fields in FEF, and confirming this with electrophysiology. A novel illuminator (Chapter 3) was invented for this study and paired with a red-light sensitive halorhodopsin, Jaw, (Chuong 2014). A red-light sensitive opsin was selected rather than conventional green-light sensitive silencer based on in vivo light propagation measurements (Chapter 2). Together these allowed for inactivation of volumes of tissue comparable to those reported in pharmacological inactivation studies. 4.2 Methods 4.2.1 Subjects Two healthy, male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) weighing 13-16 kg were used for the behavioral study. Two male rhesus monkeys weighing (Macaca mulatta) 10-11 kg were used for histological studies. The histology monkeys were selected on the basis of pre-existing, worsening medical conditions (e.g., metastatic cancer) for which veterinarians recommended euthanasia within a few months. All procedures and animal care were in accordance with the NIH guidelines and were approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Animal Care and Use Committee. 72 4.2.2 Surgical procedures Anatomical MRI images taken prior to surgery were used determine the stereotactic coordinates for chamber placement in behavior subjects. Both monkeys already had a titanium head post surgically affixed to the skull. Under Sevoflurane anesthesia, a recording chamber (Crist) was stereotactically placed over FEF in each monkey based on the each monkey's MRI. After surgically affixing the chamber to the skull, a craniotomy was made inside the chamber. 4.2.3 Localizing injection targets in behavior monkeys An ultem grid (custom) with 1 mm hole spacing was placed in the recording chamber during awake testing. A screw on the side of the chamber fit into a notch on one side of the grid to ensure that it was oriented consistently across recording sessions. An anatomical MRI was performed with the grid in the well, which was filled with a surgical lubricant (SurgiLube). This MRI image was processed offline in software (Amide) and rotated such that estimates of the distance from the top of the grid to various points in the brain could be made along the grid holes. An x-y stage (NAN) was placed over the recording chamber and fixed in a consistent orientation. Micro-drives (NAN) were mounted on the x-y stage and used for micro-stimulation, electrophysiological recording, and, later, for in vivo illumination. A 220 tm, parylene-coated, tungsten microelectrode (Nimer Lab) was lowered through a 25 gauge guide tube to MRI-derived depths spanning the thickness of cortex, based on the MRI image. One grid hole per day was tested in order to isolate the exact location of the frontal eye field within the craniotomy. 4.2.3.1 Eye tracking Subjects performed a central fixation task while seated 57 cm in front of a CRT computer monitor (resolution of 1024 x 768 pixels and refresh rate of 75 Hz). The MATLAB-based MonkeyLogic software suite (Asaad and Eskandar 2008, Asaad and Eskandar 2008, Asaad, Santhanam et al. 2013) controlled the task and recorded the eye position, which was monitored at 250 Hz using a video-based eye tracking system (Eye Link II, SR Research). 73 4.2.3.2 Micro-stimulation Between trials, the experimenter manually-triggered electrical stimulation via a TTL input to a digital stimulator (DS8000, WPI) and stimulus isolation unit (DSL 100, WPI). The current output was constantly monitored via an oscilloscope (Tektronix). Figure 20 shows the electrical circuit used for stimulation. A train of 100 bipolar pulses was applied with a frequency of 250 Hz, and a total pulse width of 0.2 ms (0.1 ms depolarization followed by 0.1 ms hyperpolarization) via a 220 tm, parylene-coated, tungsten microelectrode (Nimer Lab, WeSense). Applied currents ranged from 10 - 300 tA. Eye position was monitored during stimulation via an Eye Link II system and recorded in MonkeyLogic. The frontal eye field was defined as the region of cortex in the vicinity of the principal and arcuate sulci where fixed-vector, saccades could be evoked with current < 150 FtA at least 50% of the time, a standard that other groups have employed (Bruce, Goldberg et al. 1985, Moore and Fallah 2004, Maunsell and Treue 2006, Murphey and Maunsell 2008). Typically, thresholds were lower, though, around 50 tA or less. This study specifically sought to find areas of FEF in which micro-stimulation evoked saccades with eccentricities of approximately 10 visual degrees. 74 Manual trigger 10 k Wl Oscilloscope output across 10kD Connects to ground (guide tube) Connects to electrode * voltL 500 ps Figure 20: Circuit used for electrical micro-stimulation 4.2.3.3 Recording frontal eye field neurons Neurons in FEF have a well-characterized range of firing responses during a memory-guided saccade task (Bruce and Goldberg 1985), as shown in Figure 2. Neuronal recordings were made in the vicinity of FEF coordinates identified during micro-stimulation while the monkey performed a memory guided saccade task. These recordings confirmed that the proposed 75 injection sites identified during micro-stimulation7 and contained neurons with the firing profiles expected in FEF (e.g., with visual, delay period, and motor activity), and that opsin injection and subsequent silencing should be able to affect all of the three main neuronal responses. 4.2.3.3.1 Recording set up Spikes and LFPs were recorded using a multi-channel acquisition processor system, or MAP box (Plexon) via a single contact, 220 rim, parylene-coated, tungsten microelectrode (Nimer Lab or WeSense) coupled to a pre-amplifier (Plexon) via a headstage (Plexon) and electrical connector (Omnetics). The pre-amplifier split the data into spike channels (0.25 -8 kHz) sampled at 40 kHz and local field potential channels (0.7 - 170 Hz) sampled at 1 kHz. Pre-amplifier output went into a MAP box, (Plexon) where it was filtered and acquired using the Rasputin (Plexon) software. Spikes were sorted offline using principle component analysis and manual waveform shape analysis (Offline Sorter, Plexon). MATLAB (Mathworks) was used for further analysis and plotting. 4.2.4 Virus injections in behavior primates The grid holes and depths for virus injections were determined using micro-stimulation and recording during a memory guided saccade task, as described above. The results of microstimulation are found in section 4.3.2 and shown in Figure 24. Monkey C was injected along trajectories in two grid holes while Monkey L was injected along trajectories in three grid holes. The injected locations were determined solely by the physiological map (determined via microstimulation and electrophysiology) in each monkey. receptive fields and motor fields generally overlap within FEF, but they are not necessarily a perfect match, particularly at the level of single neurons Bruce, C. J. and M. E. Goldberg (1985). "Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades." I Neurophysiol 53(3): 603-635, Bruce, C. J., M. E. Goldberg, M. C. Bushnell and G. B. Stanton ibid."Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements." 54: 714-734.. We did not necessary see a perfect match here either, but the response were to the same side and fit within the range of point we would expect. 7Visual 76 Virus injections were performed under general (Sevoflurane) anesthesia. Dexamethasone was administered prior to virus injection to prevent brain swelling and to potentially improve virus uptake into neurons. The grid used during recording and micro-stimulation was placed in the well during surgery. 4.2.4.1 Virus preparation The virus (AAV8-hSyn-Jaws-GFP) was thawed on wet ice, diluted 1:10 with sterile phosphate buffered saline (pH 7.4, Life Technologies), centrifuged and loaded into the syringe as described in the histology section below (Section 4.2.11). The virus-loaded syringe was placed in the microsyringe injector pump on a stereotactic arm and aligned such that its trajectory moved directly along the desired grid hole. The depth when the injection needle reached the top of the grid was recorded so that the depth for injection could be calculated. 4.2.4.2 Injection sites For Monkey C, cortical trajectories along two adjacent grid holes were injected. Along one trajectory, 0.8 iL of diluted virus was injected at each of five sites, spaced 0.6 mm apart. Along a parallel trajectory 1 mm away, 0.8 VL of diluted virus was injected at each of four sites spaced 0.6 mm apart. The deepest site along each trajectory was injected first. The injection needle remained at each injection site for 2 minutes before it was retracted 0.6 mm to the next injection site. For the last, most superficial injection site along a given trajectory, the injection needle remained in place for at least 20 minutes post-injection. For Monkey L, trajectories of cortex along three grid holes were injected. Along the first trajectory (G-4), 0.4 1iL were injected at each of 8 sites spaced 0.6 mm apart. The amount of virus per site was reduced in order to maintain an equivalent total volume of virus across both behavior monkeys. Along the second trajectory (H-3), 0.4 iL of virus was injected at each of 5 sites spaced 0.6 mm apart. Along the third trajectory (H-2), 0.4 tL were injected at each of 5 sites spaced 0.6 mm apart. As in the Monkey C, the injection need remained in place for at least 20 minutes before being removed from the brain. 77 4.2.5 Behavioral testing Each monkey was were seated in front of a CRT monitor which was controlled by the MonkeyLogic software package, as described above. Prior to each testing session, an eye calibration was performed using the built-in function in the MonkeyLogic software (Asaad and Eskandar 2008, Asaad and Eskandar 2008, Asaad, Santhanam et al. 2013). Figure 21 summarizes the primary behavioral task used in this study. 1) Visual? 2) Delay? 3) Motor? Dirpto 114MIS Initial fixation: 300400 ms Target: looms Delay: 500-1000 ms Saccade: <Sooms Figure 21: Task used during testina 4.2.5.1 Memory-guided saccade Both behavior monkeys were trained to perform a memory-guided saccade task. The overall task setup is shown in Figure 21. The trial began with central fixation on a white spot (radius 0.50) for a randomly determined duration of 300-400 ms. Next, while the central fixation spot remained on the screen, a peripheral stimulus (also a white dot with a 0.5* radius) flashed for 100 ms in one of four pre-determined locations, all with an eccentricity of 10 visual degrees. Three possible stimulus locations were located on the side contralateral to the virus injection. The other target 78 was located ipsilaterally. The target locations with both laser and sham trials are shown in Figure 22. Target selection for a given trial was random. Non-laser sham locations were not tested in Monkey L because when these were used, Monkey L refused to make any saccades to the injected target location, presumably recognizing that saccades to the injected location were more difficult to execute than saccades to other sites (due to inactivation). Both monkeys maintained central fixation during the stimulus flash and for another 500-1100 ms until the central spot was extinguished. The disappearance of the central spot served as a "go-cue" for the monkey to make a saccade to the remembered location where the stimulus had flashed. A reward was given for a saccade that fell within 30 of the flashed stimulus. Monkey L Monkey C Figure 22: Injection fields and target presentation locations for both monkeys. All possible target locationsare shown above; however, only one target appearedper trial. The central fixation dot appearedon every trial. Both the target and the fixation dot were shown as white 1 degree diameter circles. In the diagram above, targets that were presented only with sham illumination are shown in white; those with both sham and laser illumination are shown in red. All targets had an eccentricity of 10 degrees relative to the centralfixation point. 4.2.5.2 Laser and sham shutter opening during the task At one of three possible points in each trial, a shutter (either the laser shutter or an identical "sham" shutter) opened for 300 ms. In a third of trials, one of the two shutters opened at the same 79 time as the peripheral cue appeared. In a different third of trials, one of the shutters opened 200 ms after the peripheral cue disappeared (during the delay period). In the final third of trials, one of the shutters opened at the same time as the central fixation spot disappeared. The "sham" shutter was three times as likely to open as the laser shutter. This both increased subject morale and allowed a large enough average time between laser trials to prevent tissue heating (as discussed in Appendix 7). 4.2.5.3 Optical hardware set up A 635 nm, 500 mW DPSS laser (SLOC) and two identical mechanical shutters (Oz Optics) were controlled via TTL pulses from a pulse generator (DS8000, WPI) with timing controlled by a computer running the MonkeyLogic software package. The laser was coupled to one of the electrically-controlled fiber optics shutters via 200 im diameter optical fiber (provided with the laser by SLOC). The output of the shutter was coupled to a different 200 pm diameter optical fiber (ThorLabs). Any part of the optical fiber that extended into the primate box was shielded with light-absorbing, black electrical tape (3M) to prevent any light from being visible to the primate through the shielding. The optical fiber was coupled to a large-volume illuminator (described in Chapter 3) with a ceramic sleeve (Precision Fiber Products, Inc.). This coupling was located behind the primate's chair and shielded with optically absorbing foil (ThorLabs) to prevent any light loss at the coupling from becoming visible to the monkey. The drives and stage mounted on the recording chamber were shielded with optically absorbing foil as well. The large-volume illuminator was lowered into the brain via guide tube that penetrated the dura but remained just above the cortex. The experimenter inserted the largevolume illuminator into the brain by hand because the illuminator is too flexible to be lowered into the brain by either an electro-mechanical or screw drive. The large volume illuminator was affixed to a micro-drive (NAN), which retracted the fiber retracted at the end of experiments. While the opening of the shutter was clean (albeit with a slight delay) the physical closing of the shutter was not immediate and seemed to "bounce" shut, meaning that it would close fully then slightly reopen, only to close again as shown in Appendix 1. To ensure a laser pulse with crisp 80 onset and offset, the laser was disabled for 150 ms at the end of the pulse. Thus, sharp light offsets were maintained even if there was some "bounce" in the shutter. Appendix 1 describes the shapes of the laser pulses in more detail. All of the hardware that interfaced with the optical fiber was shielded with optically-absorbing, black-painted foil (ThorLabs), and the setup with visually inspected before every testing session to make sure that no stray light would be visible to the monkey. Further, as an additional prevention against visual light cues, a bank of 5 red LEDs (DigiKey) was placed in the testing box and flashed continuously at 2.5 Hz (50% duty cycle). This prevented the monkey's eyes from adjusting to darkness and was an extra precaution against any light leakage during testing. The monkeys were trained with this flashing light prior to any kind of illumination to ensure that it did not impact their behavior. 4.2.6 Simultaneous recording and optical inactivation In Monkey C, a single contact electrode was lowered into the cortex with a micro-drive (NAN) through 25 gauge guide tube that penetrated the dura. In Monkey L, a 16 channel multi-site, linear electrode (Plexon U-Probe) was used instead of the single contact electrode to allow for better characterization of neuronal populations with fewer penetrations. The same method of lowering and driving the electrode was used in both primates. The electrode was always lowered through a grid hole adjacent and parallel to (i.e., 1 mm away from) the grid hole through which the optical fiber was lowered. The output from each electrode was passed through a high-impedance headstage prior to extracting the spike and LFP components via the pre-amp (Plexon). The spike channel ranged from 250 Hz to 8 kHz, and was sampled at 40 kHz. The LFP channel is defined as 0.7 Hz to 170 Hz. Spikes were sorted offline in software (Offline Sorter, Plexon Inc.). Data analysis was performed using MATLAB. 81 4.2.7 Classification of behavioral errors A saccade error occurred when the monkey failed to make a saccade to the correct target. We specifically considered trials in which the monkey maintained fixation until the central spot disappeared. If the monkey failed to move his eyes out of the central fixation window within 500 ms of the go-cue, a "no saccade" error occurred. If the primate attempted to make a saccade, but did not move his eyes to the correct target location, an "incorrect" error occurred. 8 If the trials did not end in an error or correct saccade, the monkey either did not initiate the trial or broke fixation at some point during the task. 4.2.8 Electrophysiological analysis This section describes the statistical measures and terms that were used to analyze the firing rate data presented in the subsequent section. 4.2.8.1 Population-level inactivation For each neuron, the average firing rate (spikes / second) was calculated during the illumination period in laser trials and during the equivalent time period in control trials for every neuron. A paired sample student's t-test was performed on the average laser and control firing rates for all neurons (or subgroup of neurons) for each monkey. 4.2.8.2 Inactivation of individual neurons The change in firing rate with illumination was assessed in individual neurons as follows. The number of spikes during the illumination period (or control period) was calculated for each trial in each neuron. The firing rate was normalized based on the location of the target and the period of the trial in which the firing rate was measured (i.e., target v. delay v. go-cue periods). 8 These "incorrect" saccades can be further sub-classified into a) saccades to one of the other possible target sites, b) saccades that approached, but failed to reach, the correct target, and c) saccades that veered off into space, not approach any of the possible targets. 82 Specifically, the average firing rate for the target location and task period for control trials was subtracted from the firing rate calculated for during the test period for each trial. This difference was divided by the standard deviation of the control trial firing rates for this testing period and target location. The distribution of normalized trial-by-trial firing rates for given neuron without illumination was compared with the normalized trial-by-trial firing rates for a given neuron during illumination. An f-statistic was calculated for each comparison to determine whether the variances were equal at the a = 0.95 level and the appropriate student's t-test was applied to the distributions at a 95% confidence level. 4.2.8.3 Classification of neuronal subpopulations based on firing rate response profiles Neurons were classified based on whether their firing rate increased at different task times. The firing rate of "visually-responsive" neurons increased significantly when a target was presented in the associated receptive field. Specifically, in visually-responsive neurons, the firing rate in the period 50 ms to 200 ms after the presentation of the target was significantly higher than the baseline firing rate, 200 to 50 ms prior to target presentation. The firing rate of "delay-responsive" increased significantly during the delay period (relative to the baseline). Specifically, delayresponsive neurons had a significant increase in firing rate for the period 350 to 500 ms after the target offset as compared with the baseline firing rate (again determined from 200 ms to 50 ms prior to target presentation). A paired sample student's t-test was used once again with a significance level of 0.95. The firing rate of "Motor-responsive" increased during motor preparation. Each neuron was tested for a significant increase in firing at its preferred target location at each task time using a paired sample student's t-test at a 95% confidence level. Motor responsive neurons had a significant increase in the firing rate during the 100 ms window prior to the start of the saccade. The rate during this motor preparation period was compared against the firing rate calculated for the 100 ms window prior to the go-cue. 4.2.9 Quantification of rebound The number of spikes in the 50 ms period from 20 ms after the end of the laser pulse to 70 ms after the end of the laser pulse was measured and average across all trials for each condition, 83 neuron, and monkey. This time period was used to exclude silencing while fully encapsulating the rebound for all neurons. The average number of spikes in this period was calculated for control trials that used a sham shutter as well. For each neuron, the average number of spikes in the rebound period across conditions for laser and control trials was taken by weighting the percondition averages by the frequency of each condition and calculating the weighted mean. The average number of spikes during this period in the laser trials was subtracted from the average number of spikes during the rebound period in control trials to yield the average increase in the number of spikes in the rebound period for each neuron. 4.2.10 Viral vector screening A large screen of opsin-containing viral vectors was performed to identify which virus(es) maximized neuronal opsin expression without compromising neuronal health. Two injections of each of 12 different viral vectors were performed in the cortex of monkey SM under aseptic surgical conditions. All 12 of the vectors were injected in duplicate in distant locations. To prevent expression overlap, each injection site was separated from all other injection sites by at least 10 mm in all directions. 4.2.10.1 Surgical procedures for viral vector screening Anesthesia was induced with Telazol, and then continued with Sevoflurane. Prior to surgery, Dexamethasone and Famotidine were administered. IV fluids (5% Dextrose) were infused throughout surgery, Antibiotics and painkillers (Buprenorphine) were administered intraoperatively. Craniotomies were performed bilaterally at stereotactic coordinates determined from a pre-op anatomical MRI. The bone cap of the craniotomy was preserved in antibiotic and replaced at the end of surgery. 4.2.10.2 Intra-operative injection preparation Injection syringes (10 1iL gas-tight, Hamilton) were pre-loaded with 5 1 iL of sterile silicone oil (Sigma). Each syringe was mounted on a UMP3 micro-syringe injector pump (WPI Inc.). The plunger was depressed until a bubble of silicone oil formed at the tip of the injection needle. Next, 84 aliquots of virus were removed from dry ice, quickly thawed on wet ice, and centrifuged at 40 C and 5k rmp for at least 5 minutes. Immediately after centrifugation, the injection needle was lowered into the aliquot and 2.5 tL of thawed, centrifuged virus was drawn up at a rate of 1 1iL / minute. The syringes were visually inspected for bubbles during and after loading the virus. 4.2.10.3 Virus injection The micro-syringe injector pump with virus-loaded syringe was mounted on a stereotactic arm (Kopf). The tip of the needle was moved to the appropriate location on the surface of the cortex. The plunger of the syringe was depressed prior to the needle penetrating the cortex, and a small amount of virus -0.1 - 0.3 tL was forced out. A droplet formed on the tip of the injection needle, forcing out any bubbles present in the needle prior to injection. Next, the injection needles were each observed to enter the brain. The coordinate at which the needle penetrated the brain was recorded. For each test injection, 2 VL of virus were infused into the brain at either one site (if cortex were thin in this region) or two sites along the same trajectory. When two sites were injected, the deeper site was injected first with 1 iL. The needle was retracted 1 mm and the second site was injected with 1 iL. Virus was injected at a rate of 0.1 iL/minute at all sites. Injection needles remained in the brain for at least 10 minutes post-injection. The sites were injected four at a time, using four different stereotactic arms, pumps, and syringes. 4.2.11 Histology Five weeks after the virus injections, Monkey SM was sedated with Ketamine (10 mg/kg, subcutaneous) and then transported to the perfusion space where a pentobarbital overdose (>10 mg / kg, IV) was administered. Trans-cardiac perfusion was performed with 2 liters of phosphate buffered saline (pH 7.4, room temperature) followed by 4 liters of 4% paraformaldehyde (prepared the day of the perfusion in PBS, room temperature). A gravity perfusion system (1 meter height) and 18 gauge perfusion cannula were used to match the perfusion solution flow rate to that of normal blood flow. The brain was removed and blocked immediately after fixation 85 and then soaked in 4% paraformaldehyde for ~36 hours. Next, the tissue was blocked and cryoprotected in a glycerol sinking solution (25% glycerol + 0.1% sodium azide in O.1M phosphate buffer) and kept on a shaker at 4C for 3-4 days. Coronal slices (80 Im) thickness were made on a sliding microtome and stored in 0.1M phosphate buffer + 0.1% sodium azide at 4C. All tissue processing occurred at room temperature on a shaker unless otherwise noted. 4.2.11.1 DAB staining and imaging Every 611 section (every 480 rim) was washed and permeabilized 3 times (10 min / rinse) in PBSTX (0.01M PBS + 0.2% triton X-100). The sections were incubated in 3% hydrogen peroxide in PBSTX for 10 minutes, and then rinsed 3 more times (10 min / rinse) in PBS-TX. The sections were blocked for 1 hour in blocking solution (10% normal goat serum in PBS-TX) and incubated overnight at 4*C in a 1:2000 dilution of polyclonal rabbit GFP antibody (Invitrogen) in blocking solution. The next day, the sections were rinsed 3 times (10 min / rinse) with PBS-TX and incubated, at room temperature for 1 hour, in a 1:500 dilution of biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (Vector) in blocking solution. The sections were subsequently rinsed 3 times (10 min / rinse) in PBS-TX and incubated in an avidin/biotin (Vectastain ABC) solution for 1 hour. After 3 more (10 min / rinse) with 0.1M phosphate buffer, the sections were incubated with 3,3'-diaminobenzidine, hydrogen peroxide, and a nickel solution (DAB Peroxidase Substrate Kit, Vector Labs) until just exposed. The sections were rinsed 3 more times (10 min / rinse) with 0.1 M phosphate buffer. The sections were then mounted onto gel-subbed slides and allowed to dry. Once dried, the slices were placed in serial dehydrations of 70%, 95%, and 100% ethanol each for 2 minutes. Next, the slides progressed through three washes with Xylenes, each lasting 5 minutes. Finally, the slides were cover-slipped with Eukitt. DAB stained sections were visualized under light microscopy. The DAB staining allowed for each of the virus injection sites to be mapped to a range of tissue sections, based on both the anatomy of the sections and the presence of DAB label. 86 4.2.11.2 Fluorescent immunohistochemistry Sections adjacent to those that stained strongly with DAB were selected for fluorescent immunohistochemistry. Sections were double-stained. An anti-GFP stain was used to assay for virus expression while an anti-NeuN stain was used to label neurons. Selected sections were rinsed 3 times (10 min / rinse) with PBS-TX and then blocked for 1 hour in 10% normal goat serum in PBS-TX. Next, the sections were incubated at 4C overnight in primary antibody. The primary antibody consisted of both a 1:2000 dilution of polyclonal rabbit anti-GFP (Invitrogen) and a 1:5000 dilution of monoclonal mouse anti-NeuN (Invitrogen) mixed together in PBS-TX + 10% normal goat serum. Next, sections were rinsed 3 times (10 min / rinse) with PBS-TX at room temperature. The well plates were covered in aluminum foil (Reynolds) to block out ambient light. The sections were incubated in secondary antibody for 3 hours on a shaker at room temperature. The secondary antibody consisted of 1:250 dilutions of Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-rabbit (Invitrogen) and Alexa Fluor 633 goat anti-mouse (Invitrogen) mixed together in PBS-TX + 10% normal goat serum. With the aluminum foil in place, the sections were rinsed in 0.1 M phosphate buffer 3 times (10 minutes / rinse) and mounted on to gel subbed slides under dim light. The slides were left to dry in complete darkness. They were then cover slipped in dim light using Vectasheild + DAPI and the edges of each slide were sealed with clear nail polish. The slides were stored in an opaque container at 4C. 4.2.11.3 Fluorescent imaging and cell counting Confocal imaging was performed on slides that underwent fluorescence immunohistochemistry. Images were taken in 3D over a depth of 50 tm in 2 Itm increments with a 20x lens (0.45 mm x 0.45 mm area). Cells with neuronal label (NeuN) or GFP label were counted offline using ImageJ (NIH). In the counting process, two researchers independently counted cells and digitally noted their labeling. Afterward, the researchers compared their digital labels to reach a consensus on cell classification. Using these classifications, the spatial extent of labeling, the fraction of neurons 87 labeled, the specificity of each viral vector for neurons, and the health of cells infected with different viral vectors was determined. The anterior-posterior spread of expression along the surface of the cortex was determined by measuring the number of coronal sections that had expression in cells in the vicinity of an injection site. The lateral-medial spread of virus was measured based on the area of expression on a single slide at the center of the injection site. The fraction of labeled neurons at the center of the injection site was determined by dividing the total number of neurons by the number of neurons expressing GFP. Further, these counts were broken down by layer of cortex. To assess for cell death, the number of total neurons per unit area in the injection site was compared against the total number of neurons per unit area in adjacent sites that was not injected. If there were fewer neurons in the injection site, it was assumed that some kind of cell death had occurred. In the case of frank necrosis, tissue may take on a "Swiss cheese" appearance. Other signs of poor cell health include blebbing or granular inclusions in the soma or along dendrites. To control for possible variability in surgical technique, injections were performed with the same techniques and equipment by the same experimenter, and each virus was injected in two distinct sites. 4.2.11.4 Histology matched to viral vector and brain region A second histology monkey, R, was injected in the frontal eye field with AAV8-hSyn-Jaws-GFP in a surgical procedure similar to that described for the behavior monkeys. Tissue was collected 8 weeks after the injection in the manner described above. Tissue processing and analysis also was the same as described previously. 4.3 Results 4.3.1 Histology Virus injections with AAV8-Jaws-GFP yielded expression in > 80% neurons across layers II - V (Figure 23). Virus injection did not lower the number of neurons per unit area, based on 88 comparisons of the neuronal density within the injection site with the neuronal density of adjacent, un-injected sites. Figure 23: AAV8-aws-GFP injection of 1 uL / site at two sites in a histology primate. 4.3.2 FEF was identified using micro-stimulation FEF was functionally identified in both primates at sites with micro-stimulation thresholds 150 VA. The evoked saccades resembled those in the literature, e.g., (Bruce and Goldberg 1985). Examples of evoked saccades trajectories are shown in Appendix 2. Micro-stimulation was performed every 0.25 mm along two trajectories (each spanning -4 mm) in monkey C and along three trajectories (each spanning 3-4 mm) in monkey L. The end points of evoked saccades at locations spanning all injection trajectories in both primates are shown in Figure 24. Specifically, each dot in Figure 24 corresponds to the mean end point of saccades evoked at a single site along the injection trajectories for each primate. The starting point of these saccades was normalized to be (0,0). Figure 24 illustrates the receptive fields that were injected with virus and subsequently inactivated in both primates. 89 Evoked saccade endpoints 20 * Monkey C 15 [ RF M onkey C . * Monkey L I f~ L F m 15 05 0 -5 F I-S 0 RF Monkey L &10 -15F -20 -15 -10 -5 0 5 10 Degrees of visual angle 15 20 Figure 24: The end points of saccades The end points of saccades evoked using micro-stimulationin the injection sites in both monkeys. All end points are relative to a saccade that startedin the centralfixation location (0, 0) denoted with the cross. The possible target locations, each with an eccentricity of 10 degrees are shown as black circles. The general receptivefields (RFs)for both monkeys are circled and shaded in red (monkey C) or blue (monkey L). 4.3.3 Spatial specificity of neuronal responses The neuronal responses recorded in the two monkeys were specific for the receptive fields that were determined using micro-stimulation prior to viral vector injection. Figure 25 shows an example of the firing profile of a neuron recorded during a memory-guided saccade task session in Monkey C. In this task, the direct left and direct right target locations (y = 0) were used for Monkey C and Monkey L, respectively, as the "injected" sites. Visual receptive fields and motor fields generally overlap within FEF, but they are not necessarily a perfect match, particularly at the level of single neurons (Bruce and Goldberg 1985, Bruce, Goldberg et al. 1985). For Monkey 90 C, the receptive fields tended to be a bit lower than the motor fields. Further, by placing both the "injected" target location and the "opposite" target location along the horizontal axis, we illuminated the possible confounding factor of perceptual asymmetries in the upper v. lower visual field (Thomas and Elias 2011). bw I 180 deg (anti-RF) + I 0 deg (RF) -A E M 5 400 ms, Target on -I IdwAMWOR Saccade start Figure 25: Firing profiles Firingprofiles for targets at the 8 locations shown (all with eccentricity of 10 degrees relative to central fixation point, denoted by cross). The receptivefield (RF) demonstrates an elevatedfiring rate throughout the delay period and ending just prior to the start of the saccade. 91 4.3.4 Neurons with visual, delay, and motor activity were recorded Specifically, we sought to confirm the presence of three classes of FEF neurons: a) neurons with firing rate increases in response to visual stimulation in the target area, b) neurons with firing rate elevation during the delay period, and c) neurons with firing rate elevation during saccade preparation and execution. These are referred to as visual, delay, and motor neurons subsequently, and the classifications are not mutually exclusive as described in section 4.2.8.3. Figure 26 shows the response profile of a "visual" neuron while Figure 27 shows two neurons that would both be classified as having "visual," "delay", and "motor" activity. This illustrates the variability of response profiles within neuronal subclasses. Fixatior Target I Target not in RF 50 Target 40 in RF 30 -20 10 p 'a ::: y-vvvY~ -0.5 ___________________ 0 Time from target presentation (s) 0.5 // " ~ -0.5 11me from saccade start (s) Figure 26: A neuron with only a visual response. 92 0.5 Fixation Fixatio r Target Targe t t 25 - 20 Target not In RF 2 Target InRF 20 5 10 0 5 5 0 Time from target presentation (s) 0.5 -05 Time from saccade start (s) 0.5 Target in IF , 5not 15 -0.5 E -0.5 - 0 Time from target presentation (s) .S"0.0 Timefrom saccade start (s) Targe t inRF 0.5 Figure 27: Two neurons with visual, delay, and motor-related activity. In Monkey L, 67 FEF task-modulated neurons were recorded. In Monkey C, 52 task-modulated units were recorded. In Monkey L, 62 units had an increase in firing associated with visual stimulus presentation, 60 units had an increase in firing associated with the delay period, and 39 units had a motor-related increase in firing rate. In Monkey C, 39 units had a visually-related increase in firing rate, 36 units had a delay-related increases in firing rate, and 33 units had a motor-related increase in firing rate. These ratios are summarized in Figure 28. 93 100 C 0 : 90 Monkey L 80 Monkey C 7070 W 60- 4-.. 0 ) 50 'J 30Q- 20- 10 0 Visual Delay Motor Figure 28: Distribution of response types among task-responsive FEF neurons. A given neuron may respond to more than one type of stimulation (e.g., "visual" and "motor"). Percentage of task-responsive neurons that increase theirfiring rate during each epoch of the memory-guided saccade task. 4.3.5 FEF neurons were optogenetically inactivated in both monkeys The firing rate for neurons in the injected region of FEF decreased significantly with illumination in both monkeys. The decrease in firing rate was significant for all subtypes of neurons (i.e., visual, delay, and motor) at all task times (i.e., during the target, delay or go-cue periods) regardless of whether or not the target was in the receptive field or opposite to it. 4.3.6 Overall population level firing-rate analysis On the population-level, the decrease in firing rate with illumination was significant at a level of p < Ie -10 using a paired sample student's t-test for each monkey. Figure 29 shows the distribution of firing rate decreases for both monkeys in all task-modulated neurons. In both monkeys, the firing rate in > 2/3 of units decreased by > 80%. Previous pharmacological studies, defined a 94 neuron as "inactivated" if its firing rate decreases by >80%. None of FEF neurons increased their firing rate during illumination. 70 70 Monkey L En 60 60 C 50 so C 40 IV 40 30 30 20 20 10 10 Monkey C &A #A 0 IV. C 0 0 20 40 60 8 0 100 Percent decrease in firing rate 0 8 60 40 Percent decrease in firing rate 20 100 Figure 29: Decrease in firing rate with illumination for all FEF neurons in both monkeys. 4.3.7 Silencing of individual neurons In addition to population-level analyses, the difference in the distribution of instantaneous firing rates for laser v. control trials at equivalent trial times were compared for every neuron. This more rigorous test evaluates what fraction of individual neurons had a significant decrease in firing rate during illumination based on the distribution of instantaneous firing rates from all trials. A significance level of a = 0.95 was used throughout. Of the 67 units recorded in Monkey L with visual, motor, or delay period activity, 60 units (89.6%) had a significant decrease in firing rate. Of the 52 units recorded in Monkey C with visual, delay or motor activity, 36 units (69.2%) had a significant decrease in firing rate with illumination. An example of a neuron that was optogenetically inactivated is shown in Figure 30. 95 I I Tarqet in RF * S e a * so 80 70 60 E C I- I- 50 a0 0 - Target outside RF 40, 40 a ' I. .c0 30 -iw-n-. % 20 C # a, * a- .0 10 0 - -400 -200 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 Laser on 1400 1600 1800 Time relative to laser pulse onset (ms) Figure 30: Raster plot of optogenetically-inactivated FEF neuron Note that the trial number only reflects the relative trial number within each condition, not the absolute ordering in which these recordings were made. Trials were re-orderedhere to help the reader visualize the effects remain regardless of whether or not the target is inside the receptivefield or opposite to it. 4.3.8 All subtypes of neurons were silenced in both monkeys Because all subtypes of neurons were recorded in both monkeys (see Figure 28), neuronal silencing could be further evaluated within these neuronal subpopulations. 4.3.8.1 Visual neurons Visually-responsive neurons were inactivated in both monkeys. When the target was in the receptive field, the population of visually-driven neurons showed a smaller, later increase in firing rate relative to the non-illuminated trials, as shown in Figure 31. 96 Fixation Target Laser 3 Fn Monkey L Control trials Target opposite to RF =62 2.s F Control trials Target In RF 0.5 2 OA .aser trials Target In RF 1.51 0.3 0 I 0.2 z U. F dP%00$W 0 0.1 0 -200 0 200 400 Time from target presentation (s) 000 -200o 0 200 40 600 Time from target presentation (s) Figure 31: Firing rate profile of visually-responsive neurons during target presentation. The top panel shows the response profile when the target was in the RF and the bottom panel shows the response profile when the target was opposite to the RE. There arefewer neurons for Monkey C here than in Figure 32 because only a subset of neuronsfrom Monkey C were recorded during target inactivation. The increase in firing rate immediately after illuminationcorresponds to the rebound effect discussed later in the chapter. As Figure 32 shows, the firing rate in the vast majority of visually-responsive neurons in both monkeys decreased by > 80%. The laser-on and laser-off firing rates of the population of visuallyresponsive neurons were evaluated using a paired samples student's t-test. In both monkeys, there was a significant decrease in the average firing rate of the visually-responsive neuron population during illumination (Monkey L: n = 62, p < le-12; Monkey C: n = 39, p < le-5). 97 S70- MSnao- Monkey L 60.- Visual units 60 11 70 50 so- Monkey C Visual units 60- so- 140- L 4030 S30- > 20o % 2010~ o 20 40 60 80 Percent decrease in firing rate 100 0 20 40 60 so 100 Percent decrease in firing rate Figure 32: Firing rate decrease in visually responsive neurons in both monkeys. Further, when individual visually-responsive units were considered, the majority of units in both monkeys had a significant decrease in the trial-by-trial spike probability. A student's t-test was used to compare the distribution of spikes/test period for all control trials against the distribution of spikes/test period for all laser trials for a given unit. Monkey L had a significant decrease in firing rate with illumination in 55 of 62 units (88.7%) while Monkey C demonstrated a significant decrease in 28 of 39 units (72.8%). 4.3.8.2 Delay neurons Neurons with elevated firing rates during the delay period were inactivated in both monkeys. During delay period illumination, there was some breakthrough firing toward the end of the laser pulse in delay-responsive neurons. Figure 33 shows the firing profile for delay-responsive neurons with delay-period illumination for when the target is in the injected receptive field and opposite to it. The majority of delay-responsive neurons decreased their firing rate by >80% as shown in Figure 34. As a population, delay-responsive neurons had a significant decrease in firing rate with illumination (Monkey L: n = 60, p < le-11; Monkey C: n = 36, p < le-5). When considered individually, the firing rate of most delay-responsive neurons significantly decreased 98 with illumination. Monkey L had a significant decrease in 53 of 60 delay-responsive neurons (86.7%) while Monkey C had a significant decrease in 28 of 36 delay-responsive neurons (77.8%). Fixation Target Laser I 3 r Monkey L Monkey C 0.9 n -60 2s Control trials Target opposite to RF n= 36 0.8 Control trials Target in RF 0.7 2 0.6 'a Laser trials Target in RF 0.5 1.5 l- 0A I 0.3 z ;4e 0.s 0 -200 -100 0 Time rel. to laser start (ms) k 02 0.1 0 100 200 300 Time rel. to laser end (ms) IW"W I -200 -100 0 Time rel.to laser start (ms) 0 - -, 100 200 300 Time rel. to laser end (ms) Figure 33: Firing rate profile of delay - responsive neurons during the delay period. The top panel shows the response profile when the target was in the RF and the bottom panel shows the response profile when the target was opposite to the RF. 99 S70 ga 60 80 Monkey L Delay units II C z C SO. a, 'A 40- C 0 ~30. Eu Monkey C 70 - Delay units 60 50 -- 40 a, 30 '4- o 20 0 20 4- 10 C a,U 10 0~ 0 20 40 60 8 100 Percent decrease in firing rate 0 0 80 20 40 60 Percent decrease in firing rate 100 Figure 34: Firing rate decrease in delay-responsive neurons in both monkeys. 4.3.8.3 Motor units Neurons with elevated firing rates during the pre-motor and motor period were inactivated in both monkeys. Figure 38 shows the firing profile for motor-responsive neurons with go-cue illumination when the target is in the injected receptive field and when the target was opposite to the injected receptive field. On average, these neurons eventually elevated their firing rate in preparation for movement (albeit it later and to a lesser extent than during inactivated trials), but the overall population of motor-responsive neurons in both monkeys had a significant decrease in firing rate with illumination (Monkey L: n = 39, p < le-9; Monkey C: n = 33, p < le-7). Figure 39 shows the percent decrease in firing rate for motor neuron populations in both monkeys. When the distribution of neuronal firing rates across trials was calculated for each individual unit, Monkey L had a significant decrease in 34 of 39 motor-responsive neurons (87.2%) while Monkey C had a significant decrease in 25 of 33 motor-responsive neurons (75.7%). 100 - Fixation Laser 0.8 3 Monkey L n- 39 0.71 Monkey C n= 13 Control trials Target opposite to RF 2.s Control trials Target In RF 0.6 .2! M. 2 0.5 Laser trials Target In RF .A 1.5 1- 02 z 0 0 -200 -100 0 ime rel.to laser start (ms) - 0.5 0 -200 -100 0 Timemel.to 100 200 300 Timerel.to laser end (ms) laser start (ms) laser end (ms) 100 200 300 Time l. to Figure 35: Firing rate profile of motor-related neurons during the motor period. The top panel shows the response profile when the target was in the RF and the bottom panel shows the response profile when the target was opposite to the RE. There arefewer neurons for Monkey C here than in Figure 36 because only a subset of neurons from Monkey C were recorded during pre-saccadic inactivation. =80 70 II Monkey L Motor units 80 70 Monkey C Motor units 60- -60 S50 so so- Ih1.40 04.0 0 404.' 0 930 60 920 20[ 10 110 U 0 30- 20 40 60 8 0I 100 0 20 40 60 Percent decrease in firing rate Percent decrease in firing rate Figure 36: Firing rate decrease in motor-related neurons in both monkeys. 101 100 4.3.9 Rebound activity Not during, but immediately after illumination, a rebound effect was observed (see Figure 30, 31, 33, and 35). On average less than one additional spike occurred per trial during rebound period in both monkeys (Monkey L: mean +/- std. dev. = 0.965 +/- 0.878 spikes / pd; Monkey C: mean +/std. dev. = 0.350 +/- 0.825 spikes / pd.). The extent of the rebound was correlated with the extent of silencing in Monkey L, but not in Monkey C (p < le-10 in Monkey L, p > 0.1 in Monkey C). To put the rebound effect into perspective, note that a single additional spike over a 50 ms period yields a transient spike rate increase of 20 Hz. The raster plot in Figure 30 most clearly illustrates the extent of the rebound. 4.3.10 Behavioral results Error rates (e.g., failures to execute memory-guided saccades to the proper target location) increased in both monkeys with optogenetic inactivation during either the target, delay, or motor period for targets in the inactivated receptive field. Figure 37 shows the rate of errors made after the go-cue. Data was collected over four testing sessions in each primate. Because errors were relatively uncommon and relatively few trials with each condition at each target location were performed on a given day, trials for all sessions were combined at the end of testing and x2 analysis was performed on the overall error counts. ** ** 40A 30- 0 o ** Monkey C Monkey L 8 No Illumination Target 6- Delay Go-Cue 4 I * p <0.05,)etest ** p<0.05/12 (Bonferroni correction) 2- Opposite Injected 0 Opposite Figure 37: Errorrates for both monkeys at different silencing times. 102 Injected 4.4 Discussion Optogenetic inactivation of visual, delay, and motor-related activity was observed in both monkeys. Error rates for saccades to targets in the injected receptive field increased with optogenetic inactivation at during any of the epochs of the memory-guided saccade task. Thus, neuronal firing in the frontal eye field activity contributes to performance in all three epochs (visual, delay, and motor preparation) of the memory-guided saccade task. Overall, 80.7% of neurons (96/119) had a significant decrease in firing rate with illumination. Silencing is often reported as inhibition of baseline activity compared with baseline activity itself, but the firing rates (illuminated v. control) here were measured during the same epochs of the task in which these neurons typically increased their firing rate. Therefore, we report silencing of task-relevant, driven activity which reflects the extent to which optogenetic inactivation can block drive activity in an active brain region. When this driven activity was inhibited at any time during the task, the rate of errors increased specifically for the targets in the inactivated receptive field. 4.4.1 Significance of the frontal eye field The frontal eye field is a critical region for both oculomotor and visual attention circuits in the primate brain. This study demonstrates that silencing of the frontal eye field can impact simple task behavior, and it allows for the possibility of inactivating FEF to study its role in these larger circuits. One aspect to consider is how momentary inactivation of FEF with simultaneous recordings in other brain areas could be used to study the connections between these areas. FEF's close connections to the superior colliculus (McPeek, Han et al. 2003, McPeek and Takahashi 2006), the lateral intraparietal area (Buschman and Miller 2007, Schafer and Moore 2011), pre-frontal cortex, and the other eye fields are well known (Parthasarathy, Schall et al. 1992), but longer-range connections between FEF and areas such as V4 (Gregoriou, Gotts et al. 2009, Anderson, Kennedy et al. 2011, Zhou and Desimone 2011), and the pulvinar (Stanton, Bruce et al. 1995) are active areas 103 of research. In future work, synchrony between FEF and these more distant regions could be disrupted with optogenetics to study global phenomena, such as attention. 4.4.2 Technical and scientific significance While previous primate optogenetics studies have shown decreased firing rates when inactivating opsins were expressed in the cortex (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Ohayon, Grimaldi et al. 2013), none demonstrated a behavioral change with optogenetic inactivation. The fraction of neurons with a significant decrease in firing rate was 81.5% (97/119) in this study, compared with 38% (55/144) with eNpHR2.0 (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011), 46.5% (45/74) with ArchT (Han, Chow et al. 2011), 68.1% (96/ 141) with ArchT and 52.9% (54/102) with eNphR3.0 (Ohayon, Grimaldi et al. 2013). In addition to silencing a larger fraction of neurons, this study is different from earlier work because none of the neurons in this study increased their firing rate during illumination. In each of the previous primate optogenetics studies with inactivating opsins, a subpopulation of neurons (-10-25% of the total neuronal population) significantly increased their firing rate during illumination (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Ohayon, Grimaldi et al. 2013). The coordinated, population-level silencing and homogenous modulation demonstrated in the current study presumably allowed for optogenetically-driven behavioral changes. The rebound effect, which was described in Sections 3.3.2 and 4.3.9, is not the same the as the heterogeneous neuronal effects that other groups reported. With heterogeneous activity, some neurons are driven while others are simultaneously inhibited. Here, the response of the population is homogenous in that nearly every neuron is inhibited during illumination and very transiently driven once illumination ends. The rebound effect is a known consequence of optogenetic inactivation with halorhodopsins (Arrenberg, Del Bene et al. 2009, Tonnesen, Sorensen et al. 2009, Tsunematsu, Kilduff et al. 2011, Brown, Tan et al. 2012, Madisen, Mao et al. 2012, Raimondo, Kay et al. 2012, Schone and Burdakov 2012, Smith, Virkud et al. 2012). Halorhodopsins inactivate neurons by allowing an influx of chloride ions to lower the membrane potential beyond baseline and, thus, block the generation of action potentials. Raimondo et al. (2012) suggested that 104 intracellular chloride accumulation may alter the extracellular chloride concentration and drive an efflux of chloride at the end of illumination, which manifests itself as the rebound effect. The rebound occurs before the trial is over in two of the test conditions (visual and delay period); however, it is unlike that the rebound effect impacted behavior. On average, less than one extra spike per trial was induced by the rebound effect. Compared to micro-stimulation, which used 100 pulses at 250 Hz (inducing >50 spikes) to induce saccades in this study, the single extra spike is negligible. For trials with illumination during the motor period, illumination typically continued through the end of the trials because the mean saccade latencies for both monkeys were 40-80 ms shorter than the illumination period. The overall optogenetic modulation during each task epoch demonstrated that visual, delay, and motor firing all contribute to performance in the memory guided saccade task. Chuong et al., 2014 showed that the rebound effect with Jaws than with ArchT, but that light pulse sculpting (i.e., ramping down the intensity of illumination over hundreds of milliseconds) can ameliorate the amplitude of the post-inhibition rebound. Pulse sculpting was not used in this study because 1) temporally-precise light pulse onsets and offsets were needed at specific task times. This could not be achieved with the durations needed to "ramp down" the light pulse; 2) this study sought to disrupt a particular kind of neuronal signal during the task (i.e., the visual signal, the delay signal, the motor preparation signal). Silencing was not strictly necessary as a meaningless rebound spike could also serve to disrupt the signal without changing the interpretation of the results; and 3) the temporal specificity of the task and the monitoring allowed for monitoring of rebound-driven behavior changes, and there were no rebound-driven changes. When rebound spikes occurred, they occurred in a specific time period after the end of the pulse. This timing allowed us to look for time-locked events that may have occurred as a result of the rebound as a control. If we used a ramping pulse, we would not be able to disambiguate spikes that resulted from some kind of rebound (if any) from spikes that resulted as normal firing resumed. While we did not use sculpted pulses in this initial study, in the future, others may find that sculpted pulses work well for different studies or that different techniques for sculpting pulses may be identified. 105 This study was able to achieve homogenous inactivation during illumination because a large, functionally-relevant region of cortex was inactivated uniformly. First, virus was injected over a large, functionally determined volume of cortex. The full receptive field was mapped in FEF prior to injection and the injection sites were selected based on the physiology of each monkey. In contrast to injection sites selected based on anatomical MRIs or to those in close proximity to sites where other opsins were injected, a single inhibitory opsin was injected at all chosen sites in a given primate. We ensured that the whole tissue volume corresponding to our task was targeted. Other studies relied on MRI images alone or injected excitatory opsins in close proximity to the silencing opsin injection site. Second, we delivered light evenly over a large volume and intentionally sought to avoid heating, which can lead to increased firing rates or damage. In order to achieve this broad illumination, though, a primate-specific illuminator was developed for this study. Finally, histologically-evaluated viruses with high expression levels and no evidence of toxicity were used. These viruses were specifically determined for our monkeys, prep, and brain area. In previous studies, low expression levels or small tissue volumes were consistently cited as a possible reason for the small or absent behavioral changes with optogenetic inactivation (e.g., Cavanaugh et al., 2012), the sources for these small or absent effects. In contrast, the volume of tissue inactivated in this study (see Chapter 3) is comparable with that of the pharmacological experiments (Sommer and Tehovnik 1997, Tehovnik and Sommer 1997), as is the extent of silencing (Chafee and Goldman-Rakic 2000). Here, -70% of task-modulated neurons had firing rate reductions of >80%. In the Muscimol literature, >80% of neurons had firing rate reductions of > 80% (Keating and Gooley 1988, Dias, Kiesau et al. 1995, Dias and Segraves 1996, Sommer and Tehovnik 1997, Tehovnik and Sommer 1997, Dias and Segraves 1999, Tehovnik, Sommer et al. 2000, Sommer and Wurtz 2002, Schiller and Tehovnik 2003, Keller, Lee et al. 2008). Pharmacological inactivation may affect memory-guided saccades not only because FEF neurons are inactivated during the task, but because FEF neurons are inactivated for a long enough time that reciprocal connections (e.g., among inactivated and non-affected FEF neurons or between FEF and other structures, such as the superior colliculus or parietal cortex) are disrupted. For 106 example, FEF sends information related to movement, memory, and vision to the superior colliculus (Sommer and Wurtz 2001) while the superior colliculus reciprocally activates FEF (Sommer and Wurtz 1998). Disrupting the FEF input to the SC may in turn weaken the SC input to the FEF over the time course of Muscimol inactivation. With optogenetic inactivation, it is unlikely that adaptation would occur given how infrequently the FEF is inactivated and the brevity of the illumination. Unlike pharmacological inactivation, in which the brain may begin to adapt to this change and in which the short-time scale compensatory mechanism may not be present, optogenetic inactivation disrupts the "normal" state. Ultimately, simultaneous recordings in multiple brain areas during optogenetic and pharmacological inactivation could help to answer this question, From this study, we learned that the visual, delay, and motor-related increases in FEF neuronal firing contribute to performance in the memory-guided saccade task. 107 Appendixes Appendix 1: Shutter characterization plots Onset Lght power measured -Aa kntegratIng sphere 10 8 6 E 4 5 ms 2 1.07 1.0705 1.071 1.0715 I 1.072 I 1.0725 1.073 1.0735 1074 1 0745 1.0735 1.074 1-0745 Time Ons) 1.C 75 XlO0 Enable sig"ul to shiter 5 43 2 1rms 0 I -- 1.07 - -I1 0 1.0705 1. 71 1.0715 1.072 1.0725 Time 91s) Figure 38: Characterization of shutter opening 108 1.073 105 X10 4 Offset U t power measured va Irtegring sphere 108- E 4- 2- - 0 2.06 2,065 2.07 2.075 2.08 2.005 2.08 2.085 Time (ms) 2.09 x10 4 Enable slgna to shutter 4 -N 148 ms 3- 18 MS 1- 0-1 2.06 2.065 2.07 2.075 Time Figure 39: Characterization of shutter closure. 109 (ms) 2.09 X104 Appendix 2: Supplemental micro-stimulation and neuronal recording data. Examples of evoked saccades at injection sites used in this study (monkey L) Eye m Iid giuitshole G4 at adupth (5 mm muM to the eute ofoactia wDt cuneit o!50 iaA it Eye moamn ta relaftt to1 b a 2 lI tde G4 at a depth of6 mm ate t with cutrent of 100 uA 1614- 12 0 810 0 8[- -1 6-2 Eye pieh Eye---- * 0 -2 0 2 4 Endnt 6 a locationEnigWao Degrees e to rftt) 10 12 14 0 2 4 6 a 10 Depues let to rght) 12 14 16 Figure 40: Examples of micro-stimulation evoked saccades. Appendix 3: Viral vector selection A wide variety of opsin-containing viruses have been injected in the primate brain, including lentivirus (Han, Qian et al. 2009, Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Han, Chow et al. 2011), AAV1 (Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012), AAV5 (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012) and AAV8 (Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012). Ubiquitous (Han, Qian et al. 2009, Han, Chow et al. 2011, Cavanaugh, Monosov et al. 2012, Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012) and neuron-specific promoters (Diester, Kaufman et al. 2011, Jazayeri, Lindbloom-Brown et al. 2012) have been used, but ultimately, virus expression still varies across different groups depending on injection techniques, target region, and which batch of virus was used. In this study, we identified the batch of virus that worked for us and used a consistent injection technique (an intra-operative preparation). Some viruses failed to work for us (e.g., AAV1) while others worked much better for us than for others. For example, we tested the same lot of AAV8-CAG-ArchT that Cavanaugh et al. (2012) used. We found expression in -80% of cortical neurons while (Cavanaugh, Joiner et al. 2012) reported expression in <40% of cells. In another example, Han et al. (2009) reported 110 expression in more than half of neurons with ChR2 in a lentiviral construct while Gerits et al. (2012) reported that the same construct in the same brain area was ineffective (Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012). 111 Appendix 4: Behavioral data analysis Saccades made after the disappearance of the target were analyzed and classified as correct or as errors as described above. When no saccade was made, no further analysis was performed. Saccadic latency was defined as the time from the disappearance of the central fixation dot to the beginning of pursuit. The instantaneous velocity was calculated from 30 ms prior to when the eyes left the central fixation window until the eyes reached the desired target location. The peak instantaneous velocity was determined. The acceleration was calculated for all times from the first instantaneous velocity calculated until the peak velocity. The beginning of pursuit was defined as the time of peak acceleration. Trials in which the peak velocity failed to reach 85 degrees / second were excluded. If the monkey failed to make a saccade 500 ms after the go-cue, the saccade was counted as a "no saccade" trial. Saccadic error distance is the vector distance between the end-point of the saccade (adjusted linearly by the starting point) and the center of the target location. 112 Appendix 5: List of injected viral vectors. Two of the injections failed during surgery due to leaky syringes, which were discarded. Table 1 lists the viral vectors tested and the injection sites. Table 1: List of all viral vectors injected and their injection locations. All vectors had a GFP tag. Virus X (mm, lateral to Y (mm, anterior to ear midline) bars) Side AAV9-CAG-ArchT L 20 19 AAV8-CAG-Jaws Kir2.1 R 12 14 AAV1-CAG-ArchT L 10 12 AAV5-CAG-ArchT L 20 10 AAV8-CAG- Jaws Kir2.1 R 5 15 AAV1-CAG-ArchT L 4 12 AAV8-CAG- Jaws Kir2.1 L 30 15 lenti Jaws -ER2 L 20 2 AAV8-CAG- Jaws R 9 14 FCK-eNpHR3.0 L 10 5 EIAV-ArchT L 37 0* AAV8-CamKII-ArchT L 2 20* AAV9-CAG-ArchT R 8 3 113 AAV8-CAG-ArchT R 19 0* lenti of Jaws -ER2 L 37 18 AAV8-CAG- Jaws Kir2.1 L 11 8 AAVDJ-CAG-ArchT R 25 25* EIAV-ArchT R 12 25* FCK-ArchT L 30 2 AAVDJ-CAG-ArchT L 3 8 AAV8-CamKIl-ArchT R 17 0* AAV8-CAG-Halo57 Kir2.1 R 9 11 114 Appendix 6: Fluorescence probe leads to isometric response Because an opsin-expressing cell can be activated by light coming from any direction, the light measurement probe must be as isotropic as possible. Fluorescence is an inherently isotropic process because the incident angle of the excitatory photon has no bearing on the emission angle of the emitted photon(Skinner 1964). In our probe, we couple a 0.3 mm diameter ruby sphere to a 0.4 mm diameter optical fiber using optically transparent adhesive (Figure 5a). The necessary attachment to the optical fiber only obstructs about 20% of the ruby sphere surface area (see Appendix 6.1 on the next page). We chose to use a ruby as the fluorescent source in our probe because rubies absorb photons at wavelengths across most of the visible light range (Cronemeyer 1966). Figure 41 shows the absorptive spectrum of rubies. Bayes et al., 1995 tested the angular response of a similar probe and found a flat response for the range of unobstructed incident angles (-130* to 1300), reprinted below in Figure 42 (Bays, Wagnieres et al. 1995). 4 40 agreen 7' P 5? 1.1 33 cm ared = 0.15 / cm 20 2 =4/ 2 a- 9 PINKROT 07I SLES 6. 7'. AMO7 6.1 I F25 30 0 1.06 X 10'1CM-3 1 I I 40 4" a :i s VAVELMTH UOP I we -o , FIG. 2. Absorption coefficient a(cm-') and absorption cross section w(10-" c0 at 300"K as functions of wavelength ?(9) for E.,c and EIc for Linde pink-ruby boule B-9 samples3 6, 7 and 7" with an average Cr-concentration of 1.86X 109 m- . The points designated by circles refer to ae, e, those by triangles to a,, g,. The transmittance data were measured with the Beckman B spectrophotometer. Figure 41: Ruby absorption coefficients for red and green light. 115 Cronemeyer 1966 S, ..a. "SO 0.4,441 - 19 -126 0. -~60 IIt 6 0 2 4II 0* I"a*.s.a......l. I -IS0 .12 40 0 Oreutation 60 120 1o0 [degreel Figure 42: Demonstration of angle independence of isometric light probe. From Bays et al. Appendix 6.1: Geometric estimations of isometric probe shadowing Calculations showing how much coupling between the optical fiber and the ruby sphere limits light acceptance in the optical probe 116 d=0.2mm 8 Glass optical fiber D = 0.15mm Ruby sphere The ruby sphere surface defined by the angle 0 is blocked by the glass optical fiber and cannot accept light. 0 = arctan(d/D) Recall that the surface integral of a sphere in spherical coordinates is sine de d<p surface Let fiblocked be the acceptance angle that is blocked by the optical fiber Let flid.a be the acceptance angle of a ruby sphere that is not attached to a fiber 2 [Tt 01JOsinedeldv arctan( 2 7t = -2n = sin9de cos Iartan 21r2ir ld..i = .1[ 0 -cosO)i 1 2uI + ( = ) fiblocked = . 0 sinede]dp = 4n Thus, the fraction of light that the attached ruby sphere collects compared with an ideal sphere is 4r- 21-2n 1 (ne. - flbloekd) / fideol 1+(d)/ h 47T= 2w+ 2n 1+ 1+ 1+ 471 2 - _+(__r 2 =0.8 The ruby sphere, even when attached to the optical probe, collects 80% of the light Incident for all directions. 117 Appendix 6.2: Supplemental methods for ruby sphere calibration Appendix 6.2.1: Beam profiler set up For each color of visible light (473 nm, 532 nm, and 635 nm), a beam of collimated light was generated by coupling a DPSS (532 nm and 635 nm) or diode (473 nm, Vortran) laser to a largediameter collimator (F810FC-A, ThorLabs) via a 20 0 tm diameter (NA = 0.22) multimode FC/PC terminated fiber (ThorLabs). The collimated beam passed through a custom-built iris with lightabsorbing coating. The total power light power and beam uniformity were measured with a beam profiler (BC106-Vis, ThorLabs) that had been calibrated to take absolute light power measurements. The set up for this calibration is shown in Figure 43. Output from beam profiler with absolute light power values fibe r -1 .0.5 0 5 1 X ("04 Figure 43: Beam profiler used for absolute light power measurements. Appendix 6.2.2: Fluence measurements in water bath Next, the collimated beam, with measured, uniform light power density, was applied to the isometric probe via a water bath. Figure 44 shows the experimental set up. A relationship between 118 incident light power density at the test wavelength and the emitted fluorescence at the peak ruby emission wavelength (-695 nm, defined as the 0.47 nm-wide bin with peak amplitude between 693 nm and 697 nm) was determined for each probe as shown in Figure 5c. OpticalI fiber Water bath Figure 44: Water bath for isometric probe calibration. Appendix 6.2.3: Determination of correction factors for calibration Equation A.6.2.3.1 is determined by plotting the light power density (mW /mM 2 ) on the surface of the ruby sphere as a function of fluence rate (photons / second), as shown in Figure 5. cPrs = m*x I C (A.6.2.3.1) Where Ors is light power density on the surface of the ruby sphere probe, x is fluence rate (photons / second) at the ruby wavelength(s), C is a constant correction factor determined to be 3.08 as shown below, and m is the probe- and wavelength-specific slope of the linear curve fit during the isometric probe calibration. Note that the y-intercept for that linear curve fit is set to 0. The constant correction factor, C, is determined by Equation A.6.2.3.2. C= (1 +fH2o-cortex)* Across section I (Asurface *(1 -fiocked)) (A.6.2.3.2) Where fmo-cortex is the fraction by which calibration in water over estimates photon counts in cortex, Across secion is the cross-sectional area of the ruby sphere (n r 2 ), the area over which 119 collimated light is applied, Asurface is the surface area of the ruby sphere over which it (theoretically) can absorb photons (471 r2), the area over which light is applied in vivo, and fblocked is the fraction of the ruby sphere probe surface area that is blocked by the attached optical fiber and cannot absorb photons. Across section, Asurface, and fblocked are all geometric properties of the isometric probe; however, the derivation of fmo-cortex is a bit more nuanced and, thus, explained below. The difference in the refractive indices of water (nm2o = 1.33) and the ruby (nruby = 1.77, provided by Edmund optics) is slightly larger than the difference in the refractive indices of cortex (ncortex = 1.37) (Lue, Bewersdorf et al. 2007) and the ruby. Thus, the critical angle in for the ruby in water is slightly smaller than the critical angle in cortex. The critical angle for total internal reflection of photons inside the ruby in both air and water was determined using Equations A.6.2.3.3 and A.6.2.3.4. OcriticalH2o= arc(n2o / nruby) (A.6.2.3.3) (9 critical_cortex = arc(ncortex / nruby) (A.6.2.3.4) Then Snell's law was used to derive an expression for the angle of transmission, Eh, relative to the incident angle & (Equations A.6.2.3.5 and A.6.2.3.6). (A.6.2.3.5) t_H20 = arcsin( nruby sin((9d nH2o) &-cortex = (A.6.2.3.6) arcsin(nruby * sin(&) I ncortex) Fresnel's Equations were rearranged to get reflection and transmission coefficients as a function of the incident angle for both the parallel and perpendicular cases as shown in Equations A.6.2.3.7 - A.6.2.3.10. The terms parallel and perpendicular are relative to the plane of incidence. t) R 11 = tan(9itan(ei+at) (A.6.2.3.7) R_= (A.6.2.3.8) sin(Oi - 9t) sin(Oi + 9t) 120 T, '-= 2*si_n(Ot)* cos(9i) (A.6.2.3.9) = sin(Oi+ Ot)* cos(9i - et) T.= 2*sin(Ot)* cos(Oi) sin(Oi+ (A.6.2.3.10) et) The percentages of light reflected and transmitted for a given incident angle were determined using the conservation of energy and then integrated over all possible incident angles to yield an overall percentage of photons emitted by the ruby sphere and then transmitted out of the ruby sphere back to the medium. In Equation A.6.2.3.11 below, the first term represents the fraction of fluoresced photons that are reflected back into the ruby sphere when emitted angles are less than the critical angle. The second term represents the angles over which 100% of photons remain in the ruby sphere. A=Oc A6=90* f r(0)dO + Jd0 -=0* (A.6.2.3.11) A-=& These equations were solved empirically in MATLAB using dA = 10. It was determined empirically that water yields a photon count 3.8% higher than expected in cortex. This procedure was repeated for measurements and estimations in air v. water as well to confirm the correction. Theoretically, air should give a photon count 29.3% higher than expected in water and the measurements were 28.2 +/- 2.6% (standard error) higher in water than in air, which empirically confirms the validity of this theoretical correction. Appendix 6.2.4: Planar illuminator as a wide-beam collimated source A planar illuminator was used to approximate a collimated wide beam source. Use of an actual collimator was not feasible because in order to get a collimator with uniform light power output over a 1.5 mm diameter area, the collimator itself would need to be much larger than the mouse's head and only the very center of the collimated beam would be uniform enough for testing. Thus, an iris with a very small aperture and large physical footprint would be needed between the collimator and the brain. Even though this is almost exactly the preparation used for ruby sphere 121 calibration, this was not physically feasible in the in vivo preparation. A beam profile of the output from the planar illuminator showed a uniform output over a centrally located one mm square area in the center of the beam even at a distance 8 mm away (Figure 45). Further, geometric decrease models accounting for internal reflection of multi-model coupled rays show that the total light power directly under the planar illuminator remains at 100% up to 2.5 mm away from the illuminator surface (Figure 46). -1.5 0.12 -1 -0.5 0.1 0 E 1 0.06 1.5 0.04 2 2.5 3 -25 -2 -1.5 -1 -0.5 0 x (Mm) 0.5 Figure 45: Beam profile of planar illuminator Beam profile of planarilluminatorat 8 mm away. 122 1 1.5 Light power density (mW/mm 2 ) E 0.0 0.5 Fraction of light reaching a gien depth based on geometric decrease only 1 0.5 0.9 1 0.8 E 1.5 0.7 2 0.6 2.5 0.5 3 0.4 3.5 0.3 4 0.2 4.5 0.1 E TL CS 5 -4 2 3 -1 0 1 -2 -3 Distance from center of planar illuminator (mm) 4 0 Figure 46: Internal geometric Geometric decrease model for planarilluminatorover a distance of 8 mm. Appendix 6.3: Internal geometric model Models of geometric light power decrease with distance were generated in MATLAB. These models used the numerical aperture of the planar illuminator and its diameter to calculate a decrease in light power density due to the beam "spreading out" as it exited the illuminator. They showed that the light power in the center of the beam directly under the planar illuminator does not begin to decrease until about 2.5 mm below the illuminator surface. In addition, beam profiles of the planar illuminator showed a uniform light power density across its surface and this uniformity in the middle of the beam was even present 8 mm away from the illuminator surface. 123 rfilber rfZfibr rfa, Zfiber Figure 47: Illustration of how rays can internally reflect and yield a uniform beam in the center. 124 Appendix 7: Heating measurements in tissue Appendix 7.1: Possible negative effects of heating in the brain Cooling the brain after a traumatic injury reduces brain metabolism and limits the extent of injury (Busto, Dietrich et al. 1987, Brengelmann 1993, Cabanac 1993, Brown, Bae et al. 2007). While cooling the brain impairs task performance in healthy subjects by reducing neuronal firing rate (Horel, Voytko et al. 1984, Gregory Keating and Gooley 1988, Keating and Gooley 1988, Quintana, Fuster et al. 1989, Chafee and Goldman-Rakic 2000), locally increasing brain temperature also can disrupt saccade behavior (Bajada, Mastaglia et al. 1980). In the extreme, raising the temperature of the brain by >50 C (e.g., during the course of a methamphetamine overdose or an extreme fever) may cause permanent brain damage and at times, death (Kiyatkin 2004, Kiyatkin and Brown 2004, Kiyatkin 2005, Kiyatkin 2007). Appendix 7.2: Neuronal firing rate increases with increased brain temperature In vitro studies have shown that heating increases neuronal firing rate and the probability of neurotransmitter release (Volgushev, Kudryashov et al. 2004). Some populations of neurons increase their firing rate in response to small (>20 C) increases in baseline temperature (Burgoon and Boulant 2001, Guatteo, Chung et al. 2005). Further, the brain endogenously contains transient receptor potential (TRP) channels that increase neuronal firing as the brain temperature increases (Xu, Ramsey et al. 2002, Story, Peier et al. 2003, Moran, Xu et al. 2004, Bernstein, Garrity et al. 2012). Appendix 7.3: Heating may play a role in driving behavior in primate visual cortex Synaptic transmission in the visual cortex is temperature dependent, according to in vitro studies which showed increased excitability of rat visual cortical neurons with increased temperature (Hardingham and Larkman 1998). Jazeyeri et al. (2012) introduced ChR2 into the primary visual cortex (V1) of a rhesus macaque and reliably shifted the primate's free gaze during the inter-trial interval with illumination. With the given total light powers (20 - 50 mW) and assuming a 125 standard optical fiber (100 - 200 pm diameter), total light power densities used in this study were - likely high, between 1500 - 6500 mW/mm2 . Even though Jazeyeri et al. (2012) used short (100 250 ms) pulses with a low duty cycle (5-10%), no temperature measurements. Instead, the control for possible heating was to place the fiber in a different un-injected site. Similarly, Dai et al., (2014) reported a response bias during a visuospatial discrimination task with illumination of a greenlight sensitive excitatory opsin, C1V1 that had been introduced into the lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP). This effect is similar to what had been previously reported with sub-threshold microstimulation (Hanks, Ditterich et al. 2006), but the source of the stimulation could be related to both the optogenetic inactivation and heating. Even though Dai et al. (2014) only used a total light power of 1 mW, the tip of their fiber was very small (10 tm). Delivering 1mW over a 10 pm diameter surface yields light power densities in excess of 10 W/mm 2, which is two orders of magnitude greater than the light power densities used in this study. Illuminating an un-injected area of cortex does not fully control for heating-effects with excitatory opsins for several reasons. First, it is possible that the effects of the excitatory opsin were enhanced by increased temperature. This synergy would not be present at the control site. Second, the control area of the cortex may not evoke behavior or network activation as readily as the injection site does. In fact, researcher typically map brain regions prior to injection and chose the area in which neurons are the most task responsive and the most easily modulated by external stimuli or microstimulation. Finally, repeated penetrations into the testing location likely changed the composition of the extracellular matrix in that location over time. The tissue in the uninjected area is likely healthier and may have different thermal properties as a result. Appendix 7.4: BOLD fMRI activation results from heating alone Heating can drive neuronal firing and cause spurious fMRI activation. Recently, Christie et al., (2012) drove profound fMRI responses in the naive rat brain by delivering blue light and inducing a subsequent increase in baseline temperature (Christie, Wells et al. 2012). Activations in the fMRI BOLD signal reflect increased blood flow consistent with increased brain metabolism, but blood flow can increase for reasons other than brain activity, such as heat dissipation. Rat studies report 126 brain temperature increases locally and transiently with increased neuronal activity, thus, triggering increased blood flow which is detected in the fMRI BOLD signal (Kiyatkin, Brown et al. 2002, Trubel, Sacolick et al. 2005, Vanhoutte, Verhoye et al. 2006, Kiyatkin 2007). Gertis et al. (2012) injected a virus containing ChR2 into the ventral pre-motor area (F5) and FEF of two macaques and reported changes in fMRI activation patterns with illumination, although the results were not fully consistent among subjects (Gerits, Farivar et al. 2012). Revising the methodology (light powers of 80 - 300 mW / mm2 and 8 ms pulses repeated at 40 Hz for 16 s) and findings from Gertis et al. (2012) in light of the Christie et al. (2012) study published subsequently, it is difficult to attribute the fMRI changes in Gertis et al. (2012) exclusively to optogenetic activation, particularly in the absence of electrophysiological or histological data. As described above, the control of illuminating an un-injected site is not a sufficient control with activating opsins. Overall, this demonstrated the need for additional thermal controls in newer primate optogenetics studies. Appendix 7.5: Results of temperature measurements in vivo A primate-specific illuminator as described in Chapter 3 was inserted into the mouse brain longitudinally such that all of the illuminator remained in the cortex. Four needle thermocouples (Omega) were placed 1 mm away from the illuminator and the largest temperature increase from all probes with illumination was used for the heating measurements shown below in Figure 48 and Figure 49. Minimal temperature increase was observed with a 1 s pulse of either 50 mW /mm2 or 100 mW/mm2 ; however if these pulses were repeated for long durations additive heating results, as shown in 100 mW/mm 2 . The relaxation time for tissue was determined to be approximately 20x the pulse duration so a pulse spacing of 20x pulse duration on average was used between laser pulses in this study to prevent additive heating. 127 1 4-, 100 mW/mm2 i I 11 .1.1- 0.5F .A.1 . I I.. I I, I.1 I . Q) I E I IF I II 1,111 -1 0 I F. -1 II I III I ''III I I I 50 mW/mm 2 4-J Laser on -1 0 i I I 1 2 3 I I 4 5 6 Time after laser onset (s) Figure 48: In vivo temperature measurements with the primate specific illuminator for a single laser pulse 128 4 100 mW/mm 2 3CD 2- 0 1s a) 0 laser pulses repeated 25x 10 20 30 40 50 60 Time after pulse train onset (s) Figure 49: Additive heating with frequently spaced laser pulses 129 70 Works Cited Adamantidis, A. R., F. Zhang, A. M. Aravanis, K. Deisseroth and L. de Lecea (2007). "Neural substrates of awakening probed with optogenetic control of hypocretin neurons." Nature 450(7168): 420- 424. Airan, R. D., K. R. Thompson, L. E. Fenno, H. Bernstein and K. Deisseroth (2009). "Temporally precise in vivo control of intracellular signalling." Nature 458(7241): 1025-1029. Alilain, W. J., X. Li, K. P. Horn, R. Dhingra, T. E. Dick, S. Herlitze and J. Silver (2008). "Light-induced rescue of breathing after spinal cord injury." J Neurosci 28(46): 11862-11870. Amiez, C. and M. Petrides (2009). "Anatomical organization of the eye fields in the human and nonhuman primate frontal cortex." Progress in Neurobiology 89(2): 220-23 0. Amir, Y., M. Harel and R. Malach (1993). "Cortical hierarchy reflected in the organization of intrinsic connections in macaque monkey visual cortex." The Journal of Comparative Neurology 334(1): 19-46. Andersen, R. A., C. Asanuma, G. Essick and R. M. Siegel (1990). "Corticocortical connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior parietal lobule." J Comp Neurol 296(1): 65-113. Anderson, J. C., H. Kennedy and K. A. Martin (2011). "Pathways of attention: synaptic relationships of frontal eye field to V4, lateral intraparietal cortex, and area 46 in macaque monkey." J Neurosci 31(30): 10872-10881. Angell-Petersen, E., H. Hirschberg and S. J. Madsen (2007). "Determination of fluence rate and temperature distributions in the rat brain; implications for photodynamic therapy." J Biomed Opt 12(1): 014003. Anikeeva, P., A. S. Andalman, I. Witten, M. Warden, I. Goshen, L. Grosenick, L. A. Gunaydin, L. M. Frank and K. Deisseroth (2012). "Optetrode: a multichannel readout for optogenetic control in freely moving mice." Nat Neurosci 15(1): 163-170. Aravanis, A. M., L. P. Wang, F. Zhang, L. A. Meltzer, M. Z. Mogri, M. B. Schneider and K. Deisseroth (2007). "An optical neural interface: in vivo control of rodent motor cortex with integrated fiberoptic and optogenetic technology." J Neural Eng 4(3): S143-156. Arrenberg, A. B., F. Del Bene and H. Baier (2009). "Optical control of zebrafish behavior with halorhodopsin." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(42): 17968-17973. Arrenberg, A. B., D. Y. R. Stainier, H. Baier and J. Huisken (2010). "Optogenetic Control of Cardiac Function." Science 330(6006): 971-974. Asaad, W. F. and E. N. Eskandar (2008). "Achieving behavioral control with millisecond resolution in a high-level programming environment." J Neurosci Methods 173(2): 235-240. Asaad, W. F. and E. N. Eskandar (2008). "A flexible software tool for temporally-precise behavioral control in Matlab." J Neurosci Methods 174(2): 245-258. Asaad, W. F., N. Santhanam, S. McClellan and D. J. Freedman (2013). "High-performance execution of psychophysical tasks with complex visual stimuli in MATLAB." J Neurophysiol 109(1): 249260. Ayling, 0. G., T. C. Harrison, J. D. Boyd, A. Goroshkov and T. H. Murphy (2009). "Automated lightbased mapping of motor cortex by photoactivation of channelrhodopsin-2 transgenic mice." Nat Methods 6(3): 219-224. Bahring, R., R. K. Meier and N. Dieringer (1994). "Unilateral ablation of the frontal eye field of the rat affects the beating field of ocular nystagmus." Exp Brain Res 98(3): 391-400. Bajada, S., F. L. Mastaglia, J. L. Black and D. W. Collins (1980). "Effects of induced hyperthermia on visual evoked potentials and saccade parameters in normal subjects and multiple sclerosis patients." J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 43(9): 849-852. 130 Bays, R., G. Wagnieres, D. Robert, J. F. Theumann, A. Vitkin, J. F. Savary, P. Monnier and H. van den Bergh (1997). "Three-dimensional optical phantom and its application in photodynamic therapy." Lasers Surg Med 21(3): 227-234. Bays, R., G. A. Wagnieres, D. Robert, J. C. Mizeret, D. Braichotte, H. van den Bergh, J.-F. Savary and P. Monnier (1995). "Clinical measurements of tissue optical properties in the esophagus." 39-45. Bernstein, J. G., P. A. Garrity and E. S. Boyden (2012). "Optogenetics and thermogenetics: technologies for controlling the activity of targeted cells within intact neural circuits." Curr Opin Neurobiol 22(1): 61-71. Bernstein, J. G., X. Han, M. A. Henninger, E. Y. Ko, X. Qian, G. T. Franzesi, J. P. McConnell, P. Stern, R. Desimone and E. S. Boyden (2008). "Prosthetic systems for therapeutic optical activation and silencing of genetically-targeted neurons." Proc Soc Photo Opt Instrum Eng 6854: 68540H. Bi, A., J. Cui, Y.-P. Ma, E. Olshevskaya, M. Pu, A. M. Dizhoor and Z.-H. Pan (2006). "Ectopic Expression of a Microbial-Type Rhodopsin Restores Visual Responses in Mice with Photoreceptor Degeneration." Neuron 50(1): 23-33. Binding, J., J. Ben Arous, J. F. Leger, S. Gigan, C. Boccara and L. Bourdieu (2011). "Brain refractive index measured in vivo with high-NA defocus-corrected full-field OCT and consequences for two-photon microscopy." Opt Express 19(6): 4833-4847. Bizzi, E. (1968). "Discharge of frontal eye field neurons during saccadic and following eye movements in unanesthetized monkeys." Exp Brain Res 6(1): 69-80. Boussaoud, D., R. Desimone and L. G. Ungerleider (1991). "Visual topography of area TEO in the macaque." J Comp Neurol 306(4): 554-575. Boyden, E. S. (2011). "A history of optogenetics: the development of tools for controlling brain circuits with light." F 1000 Biol Rep 3: 11. Boyden, E. S. (2011). "A history of optogenetics: the development of tools for controlling brain circuits with light." F 1000 Biology Reports 3(11). Boyden, E. S., F. Zhang, E. Bamberg, G. Nagel and K. Deisseroth (2005). "Millisecond-timescale, genetically targeted optical control of neural activity." Nat Neurosci 8(9): 1263-1268. Brengelmann, G. L. (1993). "Specialized brain cooling in humans?" FASEB J 7(12): 1148-1152; discussion 1152-1143. Brown, M. T., K. R. Tan, E. C. O'Connor, I. Nikonenko, D. Muller and C. Luscher (2012). "Ventral tegmental area GABA projections pause accumbal cholinergic interneurons to enhance associative learning." Nature 492(7429): 452-456. Brown, P. L., D. Bae and E. A. Kiyatkin (2007). "Relationships between locomotor activation and alterations in brain temperature during selective blockade and stimulation of dopamine transmission." Neuroscience 145(1): 335-343. Bruce, C. J. and M. E. Goldberg (1985). "Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades." J Neurophysiol 53(3): 603-63 5. Bruce, C. J., M. E. Goldberg, M. C. Bushnell and G. B. Stanton (1985). "Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements." J Neurophysiol 54(3): 714-734. Burgoon, P. W. and J. A. Boulant (2001). "Temperature-sensitive properties of rat suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons." Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 281(3): R706-715. Buschman, T. J. and E. K. Miller (2007). "Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices." Science 315(5820): 1860-1862. Busto, R., W. D. Dietrich, M. Y. Globus, I. Valdes, P. Scheinberg and M. D. Ginsberg (1987). "Small differences in intraischemic brain temperature critically determine the extent of ischemic neuronal injury." J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 7(6): 729-738. Cabanac, M. (1993). "Selective brain cooling in humans: "fancy" or fact?" FASEB J 7(12): 1143-1146; discussion 1146-1147. 131 Cardin, J. A., M. Carlen, K. Meletis, U. Knoblich, F. Zhang, K. Deisseroth, L. H. Tsai and C. I. Moore (2009). "Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses." Nature 459(7247): 663-667. Cardin, J. A., M. Carlen, K. Meletis, U. Knoblich, F. Zhang, K. Deisseroth, L. H. Tsai and C. I. Moore (2010). "Targeted optogenetic stimulation and recording of neurons in vivo using cell-typespecific expression of Channelrhodopsin-2." Nat Protoc 5(2): 247-254. Carter, M. E. and L. de Lecea (2011). "Optogenetic investigation of neural circuits in vivo." Trends in Molecular Medicine 17(4): 197-206. Carter, M. E., 0. Yizhar, S. Chikahisa, H. Nguyen, A. Adamantidis, S. Nishino, K. Deisseroth and L. de Lecea (2010). "Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons." Nat Neurosci 13(12): 1526-1533. Cavanaugh, J., W. M. Joiner and R. H. Wurtz (2012). "Suppressive Surrounds of Receptive Fields In Monkey Frontal Eye Field." The Journal of Neuroscience 32(35): 12284-12293. Cavanaugh, J., I. E. Monosov, K. McAlonan, R. Berman, M. K. Smith, V. Cao, K. H. Wang, E. S. Boyden and R. H. Wurtz (2012). "Optogenetic inactivation modifies monkey visuomotor behavior." Neuron 76(5): 901-907. Chafee, M. V. and P. S. Goldman-Rakic (2000). "Inactivation of Parietal and Prefrontal Cortex Reveals Interdependence of Neural Activity During Memory-Guided Saccades." Journal of Neurophysiology 83(3): 1550-1566. Chafee, M. V. and P. S. Goldman-Rakic (2000). "Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades." J Neurophysiol 83(3): 1550- 1566. Chan, A. W. S. (2009). "Transgenic Primate Research Paves the Path to a Better Animal Model: Are We a Step Closer to Curing Inherited Human Genetic Disorders?" Journal of Molecular Cell Biology 1(1): 13-14. Chan, A. W. S. and S.-H. Yang (2009). "Generation of transgenic monkeys with human inherited genetic disease." Methods 49(1): 78-84. Cheng, J., J. Zhang, C. Lu and L. Wang (2012). "Using optogenetics to translate the "inflammatory dialogue" between heart and brain in the context of stress." Neuroscience Bulletin 28(4): 435- 448. Chow, B. Y. and E. S. Boyden (2013). "Optogenetics and translational medicine." Sci Transl Med 5(177): 177ps175. Chow, B. Y., X. Han and E. S. Boyden (2012). "Genetically encoded molecular tools for light-driven silencing of targeted neurons." Prog Brain Res 196: 49-61. Chow, B. Y., X. Han, A. S. Dobry, X. Qian, A. S. Chuong, M. Li, M. A. Henninger, G. M. Belfort, Y. Lin, P. E. Monahan and E. S. Boyden (2010). "High-performance genetically targetable optical neural silencing by light-driven proton pumps." Nature 463(7277): 98-102. Christie, I. N., J. A. Wells, P. Southern, N. Marina, S. Kasparov, A. V. Gourine and M. F. Lythgoe (2012). "fMRI response to blue light delivery in the naive brain: Implications for combined optogenetic fMRI studies." Neurolmage 66C: 634-641. Chuong, A. S., Miri, M. L., Busskamp, V., Matthews, G.A.C., Acker, L.C., Soresnsen, A.T., Young, A., Klapoetke, N. C., Henninger, M.A., Kodandaramaiah, S.B., Ogawa, M., Ramanlal, S. B., Forest, C.R., Chow, B.Y., Han, X., Lin, Y., Tye, K.M., Roska, R., Cardin, J.A., Boyden, E.S. (2014). "Non-invasive optogenetic neural silencing." submitted. Ciocchi, S., C. Herry, F. Grenier, S. B. Wolff, J. J. Letzkus, I. Vlachos, I. Ehrlich, R. Sprengel, K. Deisseroth, M. B. Stadler, C. Muller and A. Luthi (2010). "Encoding of conditioned fear in central amygdala inhibitory circuits." Nature 468(7321): 277-282. Claridge-Chang, A., R. D. Roorda, E. Vrontou, L. Sjulson, H. Li, J. Hirsh and G. Miesenb6ck (2009). "Writing Memories with Light-Addressable Reinforcement Circuitry." Cell 139(2): 405-415. Covington, H. E., M. K. Lobo, I. Maze, V. Vialou, J. M. Hyman, S. Zaman, Q. LaPlant, E. Mouzon, S. Ghose, C. A. Tamminga, R. L. Neve, K. Deisseroth and E. J. Nestler (2010). "Antidepressant 132 Effect of Optogenetic Stimulation of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex." The Journal of Neuroscience 30(48): 16082-16090. Cronemeyer, D. C. (1966). "Optical Absorption Characteristics of Pink Ruby." J. Opt. Soc. Am. 56(12): 1703-1705. Dai, J., Daniel I. Brooks and David L. Sheinberg (2014). "Optogenetic and Electrical Microstimulation Systematically Bias Visuospatial Choice in Primates." Current biology: CB 24(1): 63-69. Dias, E. C., M. Kiesau and M. A. Segraves (1995). "Acute activation and inactivation of macaque frontal eye field with GABA-related drugs." J Neurophysiol 74(6): 2744-2748. Dias, E. C. and M. A. Segraves (1996). "The primate frontal eye field and the generation of saccadic eye movements: comparison of lesion and acute inactivation/activation studies." Rev Bras Biol 56 Su 1 Pt 2: 239-255. Dias, E. C. and M. A. Segraves (1999). "Muscimol-Induced Inactivation of Monkey Frontal Eye Field: Effects on Visually and Memory-Guided Saccades." Journal of Neurophysiology 81(5): 2191- 2214. Diester, I., M. T. Kaufman, M. Mogri, R. Pashaie, W. Goo, 0. Yizhar, C. Ramakrishnan, K. Deisseroth and K. V. Shenoy (2011). "An optogenetic toolbox designed for primates." Nat Neurosci 14(3): 387-397. Ding, L. and J. I. Gold (2012). "Neural Correlates of Perceptual Decision Making before, during, and after Decision Commitment in Monkey Frontal Eye Field." Cerebral Cortex 22(5): 1052-1067. Dixson, A. F. and C. M. Nevison (1997). "The socioendocrinology of adolescent development in male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)." Horm Behav 31(2): 126-135. Dow, B. M., A. Z. Snyder, R. G. Vautin and R. Bauer (1981). "Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey." Exp Brain Res 44(2): 213-228. Eggert, H. R. and V. Blazek (1987). "Optical properties of human brain tissue, meninges, and brain tumors in the spectral range of 200 to 900 nm." Neurosurgery 21(4): 459-464. F., C. W. (1995). Summary of optical properties. Optical-Thermal Response of Laser-Irradiated Tissue. W. A. J. a. v. G. M. J. New York, Platinum Press. Felleman, D. J. and D. C. Van Essen (1991). "Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex." Cereb Cortex 1(1): 1-47. Flock, S. T., B. C. Wilson and M. S. Patterson (1987). "Total attenuation coefficients and scattering phase functions of tissues and phantom materials at 633 nm." Medical Physics 14(5): 835-841. Freire, M. A., E. Morya, J. Faber, J. R. Santos, J. S. Guimaraes, N. A. Lemos, K. Sameshima, A. Pereira, S. Ribeiro and M. A. Nicolelis (2011). "Comprehensive analysis of tissue preservation and recording quality from chronic multielectrode implants." PLoS One 6(11): e27554. Gattass, R., C. G. Gross and J. H. Sandell (1981). "Visual topography of V2 in the macaque." J Comp Neurol 201(4): 519-539. Gerits, A., R. Farivar, Bruce R. Rosen, Lawrence L. Wald, Edward S. Boyden and W. Vanduffel (2012). "Optogenetically Induced Behavioral and Functional Network Changes in Primates." Current Biology 22(18): 1722-1726. Gottschalk, W. (1992). Ein Messverfahren zur Bestimmung der optischen Parameter biologischer Gevebe in vitro. Ph.D., University of Karlsruhe. Gradinaru, V., M. Mogri, K. R. Thompson, J. M. Henderson and K. Deisseroth (2009). "Optical deconstruction of parkinsonian neural circuitry." Science 324(5925): 354-359. Gradinaru, V., K. R. Thompson and K. Deisseroth (2008). "eNpHR: a Natronomonas halorhodopsin enhanced for optogenetic applications." Brain Cell Biol 36(1-4): 129-139. Gradinaru, V., K. R. Thompson, F. Zhang, M. Mogri, K. Kay, M. B. Schneider and K. Deisseroth (2007). "Targeting and Readout Strategies for Fast Optical Neural Control In Vitro and In Vivo." The Journal of Neuroscience 27(52): 14231-14238. 133 Gradinaru, V., F. Zhang, C. Ramakrishnan, J. Mattis, R. Prakash, I. Diester, I. Goshen, K. R. Thompson and K. Deisseroth (2010). "Molecular and Cellular Approaches for Diversifying and Extending Optogenetics." Cell 141(1): 154-165. Gregoriou, G. G., S. J. Gotts, H. Zhou and R. Desimone (2009). "High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention." Science 324(593 1): 1207-12 10. Gregory Keating, E. and S. G. Gooley (1988). "Saccadic disorders caused by cooling the superior colliculus or the frontal eye field, or from combined lesions of both structures." Brain Research 438(1-2): 247-255. Guatteo, E., K. K. Chung, T. K. Bowala, G. Bernardi, N. B. Mercuri and J. Lipski (2005). "Temperature sensitivity of dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra pars compacta: involvement of transient receptor potential channels." J Neurophysiol 94(5): 3069-3080. Gunaydin, L. A., 0. Yizhar, A. Berndt, V. S. Sohal, K. Deisseroth and P. Hegemann (2010). "Ultrafast optogenetic control." Nat Neurosci 13(3): 387-392. Gutierrez, D. V., M. D. Mark, 0. Masseck, T. Maejima, D. Kuckelsberg, R. A. Hyde, M. Krause, W. Kruse and S. Herlitze (2011). "Optogenetic Control of Motor Coordination by Gi/o Proteincoupled Vertebrate Rhodopsin in Cerebellar Purkinje Cells." Journal of Biological Chemistry 286(29): 25848-25858. Han, X. (2012). Chapter 11 - Optogenetics in the nonhuman primate. Progress in Brain Research. K. Thomas and S. B. Edward, Elsevier. Volume 196: 215-233. Han, X. and E. S. Boyden (2007). "Multiple-Color Optical Activation, Silencing, and Desynchronization of Neural Activity, with Single-Spike Temporal Resolution." PLoS ONE 2(3): e299. Han, X., B. Y. Chow, H. Zhou, N. C. Klapoetke, A. Chuong, R. Rajimehr, A. Yang, M. V. Baratta, J. Winkle, R. Desimone and E. S. Boyden (2011). "A high-light sensitivity optical neural silencer: development and application to optogenetic control of non-human primate cortex." Front Syst Neurosci 5: 18. Han, X., B. Y. Chow, H. Zhou, N. C. Klapoetke, A. Chuong, R. Rajimehr, A. Yang, M. V. Baratta, J. Winkle, R. Desimone and E. S. Boyden (2011). "A high-light sensitivity optical neural silencer: development, and application to optogenetic control of nonhuman primate cortex." Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 5. Han, X., X. Qian, J. G. Bernstein, H. H. Zhou, G. T. Franzesi, P. Stern, R. T. Bronson, A. M. Graybiel, R. Desimone and E. S. Boyden (2009). "Millisecond-timescale optical control of neural dynamics in the nonhuman primate brain." Neuron 62(2): 191-198. Hanes, D. P., W. F. Patterson, 2nd and J. D. Schall (1998). "Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity." J Neurophysiol 79(2): 817-834. Hanks, T. D., J. Ditterich and M. N. Shadlen (2006). "Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task." Nat Neurosci 9(5): 682-689. Hardingham, N. R. and A. U. Larkman (1998). "The reliability of excitatory synaptic transmission in slices of rat visual cortex in vitro is temperature dependent." The Journal of Physiology 507(1): 249-256. Haubensak, W., P. S. Kunwar, H. Cai, S. Ciocchi, N. R. Wall, R. Ponnusamy, J. Biag, H. W. Dong, K. Deisseroth, E. M. Callaway, M. S. Fanselow, A. Luthi and D. J. Anderson (2010). "Genetic dissection of an amygdala microcircuit that gates conditioned fear." Nature 468(7321): 270-276. Herculano-Houzel, S. (2009). "The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain." Front Hum Neurosci 3: 31. Horel, J. A., M. L. Voytko and K. G. Salsbury (1984). "Visual learning suppressed by cooling the temporal pole." Behav Neurosci 98(2): 310-324. Hubel, D. H. and T. N. Wiesel (1974). "Sequence regularity and geometry of orientation columns in the monkey striate cortex." J Comp Neurol 158(3): 267-293. Huber, D., L. Petreanu, N. Ghitani, S. Ranade, T. Hromadka, Z. Mainen and K. Svoboda (2008). "Sparse optical microstimulation in barrel cortex drives learned behaviour in freely moving mice." Nature 451(7174): 61-64. 134 Jazayeri, M., Z. Lindbloom-Brown and G. D. Horwitz (2012). "Saccadic eye movements evoked by optogenetic activation of primate V1." Nat Neurosci 15(10): 1368-1370. Jing, W., W. Fabien, A. B. David, Z. Jiayi, 0. Ilker, D. B. Rebecca, V. N. Arto, W. Rick van, D. Ilka and D. Karl (2012). "Integrated device for combined optical neuromodulation and electrical recording for chronic in vivo applications." Journal of Neural Engineering 9(1): 016001. Johansen, J. P., H. Hamanaka, M. H. Monfils, R. Behnia, K. Deisseroth, H. T. Blair and J. E. LeDoux (2010). "Optical activation of lateral amygdala pyramidal cells instructs associative fear learning." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107(28): 12692-12697. Kahn, I., M. Desai, U. Knoblich, J. Bernstein, M. Henninger, A. M. Graybiel, E. S. Boyden, R. L. Buckner and C. I. Moore (2011). "Characterization of the functional MRI response temporal linearity via optical control of neocortical pyramidal neurons." J Neurosci 31(42): 15086-15091. Keating, E. G. and S. G. Gooley (1988). "Saccadic disorders caused by cooling the superior colliculus or the frontal eye field, or from combined lesions of both structures." Brain Res 438(1-2): 247-255. Keller, E. L., K.-M. Lee, S.-W. Park and J. A. Hill (2008). "Effect of Inactivation of the Cortical Frontal Eye Field on Saccades Generated in a Choice Response Paradigm." Journal of Neurophysiology 100(5): 2726-2737. Kirchner, H., E. J. Barbeau, S. J. Thorpe, J. Regis and C. Liegeois-Chauvel (2009). "Ultra-rapid sensory responses in the human frontal eye field region." J Neurosci 29(23): 7599-7606. Kiyatkin, E. A. (2004). "Brain hyperthermia during physiological and pathological conditions: causes, mechanisms, and functional implications." Curr Neurovasc Res 1(1): 77-90. Kiyatkin, E. A. (2005). "Brain hyperthermia as physiological and pathological phenomena." Brain Res Brain Res Rev 50(1): 27-56. Kiyatkin, E. A. (2007). "Physiological and pathological brain hyperthermia." Prog Brain Res 162: 219243. Kiyatkin, E. A. and P. L. Brown (2004). "Modulation of physiological brain hyperthermia by environmental temperature and impaired blood outflow in rats." Physiol Behav 83(3): 467-474. Kiyatkin, E. A., P. L. Brown and R. A. Wise (2002). "Brain temperature fluctuation: a reflection of functional neural activation." European Journal of Neuroscience 16(1): 164-168. Klapoetke, N. C., Y. Murata, S. S. Kim, S. R. Pulver, A. Birdsey-Benson, Y. K. Cho, T. K. Morimoto, A. S. Chuong, E. J. Carpenter, Z. Tian, J. Wang, Y. Xie, Z. Yan, Y. Zhang, B. Y. Chow, B. Surek, M. Melkonian, V. Jayaraman, M. Constantine-Paton, G. K. Wong and E. S. Boyden (2014). "Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations." Nat Methods 11(3): 338-346. Koeppen, B. M. and B. A. Stanton (2009). Berne & Levy Physiology, Updated Edition, Elsevier Health Sciences. Kravitz, A. V., B. S. Freeze, P. R. L. Parker, K. Kay, M. T. Thwin, K. Deisseroth and A. C. Kreitzer (2010). "Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry." Nature 466(7306): 622-626. Kubelka, P. (1948). "New Contributions to the Optics of Intensely Light-Scattering Materials. Part I." Journal of the Optical Society of America 38(5): 448-448. Lee, J. H., R. Durand, V. Gradinaru, F. Zhang, I. Goshen, D. S. Kim, L. E. Fenno, C. Ramakrishnan and K. Deisseroth (2010). "Global and local fMRI signals driven by neurons defined optogenetically by type and wiring." Nature 465(7299): 788-792. Little, C. C. and J. Pearsons (1940). "The Results of a "Functional Test" in a Strain of Mice (C57 Black) with a Low Breast Tumor Incidence." The American Journal of Cancer 38(2): 224-233. Llewellyn, M. E., K. R. Thompson, K. Deisseroth and S. L. Delp (2010). "Orderly recruitment of motor units under optical control in vivo." Nat Med 16(10): 1161-1165. Lue, N., J. Bewersdorf, M. D. Lessard, K. Badizadegan, R. R. Dasari, M. S. Feld and G. Popescu (2007). "Tissue refractometry using Hilbert phase microscopy." Opt Lett 32(24): 3522-3524. Madisen, L., T. Mao, H. Koch, J. M. Zhuo, A. Berenyi, S. Fujisawa, Y. W. Hsu, A. J. Garcia, 3rd, X. Gu, S. Zanella, J. Kidney, H. Gu, Y. Mao, B. M. Hooks, E. S. Boyden, G. Buzsaki, J. M. Ramirez, A. R. Jones, K. Svoboda, X. Han, E. E. Turner and H. Zeng (2012). "A toolbox of Cre-dependent 135 optogenetic transgenic mice for light-induced activation and silencing." Nat Neurosci 15(5): 793- 802. Masamizu, Y., T. Okada, H. Ishibashi, S. Takeda, S. Yuasa and K. Nakahara (2010). "Efficient gene transfer into neurons in monkey brain by adeno-associated virus 8." Neuroreport 21(6): 447-451. Maunsell, J. H. R. and S. Treue (2006). "Feature-based attention in visual cortex." Trends in Neurosciences 29(6): 317-322. McPeek, R. M., J. H. Han and E. L. Keller (2003). "Competition Between Saccade Goals in the Superior Colliculus Produces Saccade Curvature." Journal of Neurophysiology 89(5): 2577-2590. McPeek, R. M. and N. Takahashi (2006). "Deficits in covert attention after temporary inactivation of macaque frontal eye field." Journal of Vision 6(6): 823. Mesradi, M., A. Genoux, V. Cuplov, D. Abi Haidar, S. Jan, I. Buvat and F. Pain (2013). "Experimental and analytical comparative study of optical coefficient of fresh and frozen rat tissues." Journal of Biomedical Optics 18(11): 117010-117010. Mobley, J. a. V.-D., T. (2003). Optical Properties of Tissue. Biomedical Photonics Handbook. T. VoDinh. Boca Raton, Florida, CRC Press. 1. Mohler, C. W., M. E. Goldberg and R. H. Wurtz (1973). "Visual receptive fields of frontal eye field neurons." Brain Res 61: 385-389. Moore, T. and K. M. Armstrong (2003). "Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex." Nature 421(6921): 370-373. Moore, T. and M. Fallah (2004). "Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention." J Neurophysiol 91(1): 152-162. Moore, T. and M. Fallah (2004). "Microstimulation of the Frontal Eye Field and Its Effects on Covert Spatial Attention." Journal of Neurophysiology 91(1): 152-162. Moran, M. M., H. Xu and D. E. Clapham (2004). "TRP ion channels in the nervous system." Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14(3): 362-369. Murphey, D. K. and J. H. R. Maunsell (2008). "Electrical microstimulation thresholds for behavioral detection and saccades in monkey frontal eye fields." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(20): 7315-7320. Niu, Y., Y. Yu, A. Bernat, S. Yang, X. He, X. Guo, D. Chen, Y. Chen, S. Ji, W. Si, Y. Lv, T. Tan, Q. Wei, H. Wang, L. Shi, J. Guan, X. Zhu, M. Afanassieff, P. Savatier, K. Zhang, Q. Zhou and W. Ji (2010). "Transgenic rhesus monkeys produced by gene transfer into early-cleavage-stage embryos using a simian immunodeficiency virus-based vector." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107(41): 17663-17667. Ohayon, S., P. Grimaldi, N. Schweers and D. Y. Tsao (2013). "Saccade modulation by optical and electrical stimulation in the macaque frontal eye field." J Neurosci 33(42): 16684-16697. Osawa, S.-i., M. Iwasaki, R. Hosaka, Y. Matsuzaka, H. Tomita, T. Ishizuka, E. Sugano, E. Okumura, H. Yawo, N. Nakasato, T. Tominaga and H. Mushiake (2013). "Optogenetically Induced Seizure and the Longitudinal Hippocampal Network Dynamics." PLoS ONE 8(4): e60928. Ozden, I., J. Wang, Y. Lu, T. May, J. Lee, W. Goo, D. J. O'Shea, P. Kalanithi, I. Diester, M. Diagne, K. Deisseroth, K. V. Shenoy and A. V. Nurmikko (2013). "A coaxial optrode as multifunction writeread probe for optogenetic studies in non-human primates." Journal of Neuroscience Methods 219(1): 142-154. Parthasarathy, H., J. Schall and A. Graybiel (1992). "Distributed but convergent ordering of corticostriatal projections: analysis of the frontal eye field and the supplementary eye field in the macaque monkey." The Journal of Neuroscience 12(11): 4468-4488. Quintana, J., J. M. Fuster and J. Yajeya (1989). "Effects of cooling parietal cortex on prefrontal units in delay tasks." Brain Research 503(1): 100-110. Raimondo, J. V., L. Kay, T. J. Ellender and C. J. Akerman (2012). "Optogenetic silencing strategies differ in their effects on inhibitory synaptic transmission." Nat Neurosci 15(8): 1102-1104. Rengachary, S. S. a. E., R.G (2005) "Principles of Neurosurgery." 136 Roberts, T. F., S. M. H. Gobes, M. Murugan, B. P. Olveczky and R. Mooney (2012). "Motor circuits are required to encode a sensory model for imitative learning." Nat Neurosci 15(10): 1454-1459. Robles, F. E., S. Chowdhury and A. Wax (2010). "Assessing hemoglobin concentration using spectroscopic optical coherence tomography for feasibility of tissue diagnostics." Biomed Opt Express 1(1): 310-317. Ruiz, 0., B. R. Lustig, J. J. Nassi, A. Cetin, J. H. Reynolds, T. D. Albright, E. M. Callaway, G. R. Stoner and A. W. Roe (2013). "Optogenetics through windows on the brain in the nonhuman primate." Journal of Neurophysiology 110(6): 1455-1467. Schafer, R. J. and T. Moore (2011). "Selective attention from voluntary control of neurons in prefrontal cortex." Science 332(6037): 1568-1571. Schall, J. D., A. Morel, D. J. King and J. Bullier (1995). "Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams." J Neurosci 15(6): 4464-4487. Schiller, P. H. and I. Chou (2000). "The effects of anterior arcuate and dorsomedial frontal cortex lesions on visually guided eye movements: 2. Paired and multiple targets." Vision Res 40(10-12): 16271638. Schiller, P. H. and I. H. Chou (1998). "The effects of frontal eye field and dorsomedial frontal cortex lesions on visually guided eye movements." Nat Neurosci 1(3): 248-253. Schiller, P. H. and E. J. Tehovnik (2003). "Cortical inhibitory circuits in eye-movement generation." Eur J Neurosci 18(11): 3127-3133. Schone, C. and D. Burdakov (2012). "Glutamate and GABA as rapid effectors of hypothalamic "peptidergic" neurons." Front Behav Neurosci 6: 81. Sharp, A. A., A. M. Ortega, D. Restrepo, D. Curran-Everett and K. Gall (2009). "<emphasis emphasistype="italic">In Vivo</emphasis> Penetration Mechanics and Mechanical Properties of Mouse Brain Tissue at Micrometer Scales." Biomedical Engineering, IEEE Transactions on 56(1): 45-53. Skinner, J. G. (1964). "Pumping Energy Distribution in Ruby Rods." Appl. Opt. 3(8): 963-965. Smith, K. S., A. Virkud, K. Deisseroth and A. M. Graybiel (2012). "Reversible online control of habitual behavior by optogenetic perturbation of medial prefrontal cortex." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109(46): 18932-18937. Sohal, V. S., F. Zhang, 0. Yizhar and K. Deisseroth (2009). "Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance." Nature 459(7247): 698-702. Sommer, M. A. and E. J. Tehovnik (1997). "Reversible inactivation of macaque frontal eye field." Experimental Brain Research 116(2): 229-249. Sommer, M. A. and R. H. Wurtz (1998). "Frontal eye field neurons orthodromically activated from the superior colliculus." Journal of Neurophysiology 80: 3331-3335. Sommer, M. A. and R. H. Wurtz (2001). "Frontal eye field sends delay activity related to movement, memory, and vision to the superior colliculus." Journal of Neurophysiology 85(4): 1673-1685. Sommer, M. A. and R. H. Wurtz (2002). "A pathway in primate brain for internal monitoring of movements." Science 296(5572): 1480-1482. Stanton, G. B., C. J. Bruce and M. E. Goldberg (1995). "Topography of projections to posterior cortical areas from the macaque frontal eye fields." The Journal of Comparative Neurology 353(2): 291305. Stanton, G. B., S. Y. Deng, E. M. Goldberg and N. T. McMullen (1989). "Cytoarchitectural characteristic of the frontal eye fields in macaque monkeys." The Journal of Comparative Neurology 282(3): 415-427. Star, W. M. (2011). Chapter 6: Diffusion Theory of Light Transport. Optical-Thermal Response of LaserIrradiated Tissue. A. J. Welch and M. J. C. van Gemert, Springer. Star, W. M., J. P. Marijnissen and M. J. van Gemert (1987). "Light dosimetry: status and prospects." J Photochem Photobiol B 1(2): 149-167. 137 Sterenborg, H. J., A. E. Saarnak, R. Frank and M. Motamedi (1996). "Evaluation of spectral correction techniques for fluorescence measurements on pigmented lesions in vivo." J Photochem Photobiol B 35(3): 159-165. Stirman, J. N., M. M. Crane, S. J. Husson, A. Gottschalk and H. Lu (2012). "A multispectral optical illumination system with precise spatiotemporal control for the manipulation of optogenetic reagents." Nat. Protocols 7(2): 207-220. Story, G. M., A. M. Peier, A. J. Reeve, S. R. Eid, J. Mosbacher, T. R. Hricik, T. J. Earley, A. C. Hergarden, D. A. Andersson, S. W. Hwang, P. McIntyre, T. Jegla, S. Bevan and A. Patapoutian (2003). "ANKTM1, a TRP-like channel expressed in nociceptive neurons, is activated by cold temperatures." Cell 112(6): 819-829. Tamura, K., Y. Ohashi, T. Tsubota, D. Takeuchi, T. Hirabayashi, M. Yaguchi, M. Matsuyama, T. Sekine and Y. Miyashita (2012). "A glass-coated tungsten microelectrode enclosing optical fibers for optogenetic exploration in primate deep brain structures." Journal of Neuroscience Methods 211(1): 49-57. Tehovnik, E. J. and M. A. Sommer (1997). "Effective spread and timecourse of neural inactivation caused by lidocaine injection in monkey cerebral cortex." J Neurosci Methods 74(1): 17-26. Tehovnik, E. J., M. A. Sommer, I. H. Chou, W. M. Slocum and P. H. Schiller (2000). "Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates." Brain Research Reviews 32(2-3): 413-448. Thomas, N. A. and L. J. Elias (2011). "Upper and lower visual field differences in perceptual asymmetries." Brain Research 1387(0): 108-115. Thompson, K. G., K. L. Biscoe and T. R. Sato (2005). "Neuronal Basis of Covert Spatial Attention in the Frontal Eye Field." The Journal of Neuroscience 25(41): 9479-9487. Tomita, H., E. Sugano, Y. Fukazawa, H. Isago, Y. Sugiyama, T. Hiroi, T. Ishizuka, H. Mushiake, M. Kato, M. Hirabayashi, R. Shigemoto, H. Yawo and M. Tamai (2009). "Visual properties of transgenic rats harboring the channelrhodopsin-2 gene regulated by the thy-1.2 promoter." PLoS One 4(11): e7679. Tomita, H., E. Sugano, H. Isago, T. Hiroi, Z. Wang, E. Ohta and M. Tamai (2010). "Channelrhodopsin-2 gene transduced into retinal ganglion cells restores functional vision in genetically blind rats." Experimental Eye Research 90(3): 429-436. Tonnesen, J., A. T. Sorensen, K. Deisseroth, C. Lundberg and M. Kokaia (2009). "Optogenetic control of epileptiform activity." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(29): 12162-12167. Tonnesen, J., A. T. Sorensen, K. Deisseroth, C. Lundberg and M. Kokaia (2009). "Optogenetic control of epileptiform activity." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106(29): 12162-12167. Tootell, R. B., E. Switkes, M. S. Silverman and S. L. Hamilton (1988). "Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. II. Retinotopic organization." J Neurosci 8(5): 1531-1568. Trubel, H. K. F., L. I. Sacolick and F. Hyder (2005). "Regional temperature changes in the brain during somatosensory stimulation." J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 26(1): 68-78. Tsai, H. C., F. Zhang, A. Adamantidis, G. D. Stuber, A. Bonci, L. de Lecea and K. Deisseroth (2009). "Phasic firing in dopaminergic neurons is sufficient for behavioral conditioning." Science 324(5930): 1080-1084. Tsunematsu, T., T. S. Kilduff, E. S. Boyden, S. Takahashi, M. Tominaga and A. Yamanaka (2011). "Acute optogenetic silencing of orexin/hypocretin neurons induces slow-wave sleep in mice." J Neurosci 31(29): 10529-10539. Van Essen, D. C., W. T. Newsome and J. H. Maunsell (1984). "The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability." Vision Res 24(5): 429-448. Vanhoutte, G., M. Verhoye and A. Van der Linden (2006). "Changing body temperature affects the T2* signal in the rat brain and reveals hypothalamic activity." Magn Reson Med 55(5): 1006-1012. Volgushev, M., I. Kudryashov, M. Chistiakova, M. Mukovski, J. Niesmann and U. T. Eysel (2004). "Probability of Transmitter Release at Neocortical Synapses at Different Temperatures." Journal of Neurophysiology 92(1): 212-220. 138 Williams, H. P. (2001). "Sir Harold Ridley's vision." British Journal of Ophthalmology 85(9): 1022-1023. Wilson, B., M. Patterson and D. Burns (1986). "Effect of photosensitizer concentration in tissue on the penetration depth of photoactivating light." Lasers in Medical Science 1(4): 235-244. Wise, S. P., D. Boussaoud, P. B. Johnson and R. Caminiti (1997). "PREMOTOR AND PARIETAL CORTEX: Corticocortical Connectivity and Combinatorial Computations 1." Annual Review of Neuroscience 20(1): 25-42. Witten, I. B., S.-C. Lin, M. Brodsky, R. Prakash, I. Diester, P. Anikeeva, V. Gradinaru, C. Ramakrishnan and K. Deisseroth (2010). "Cholinergic Interneurons Control Local Circuit Activity and Cocaine Conditioning." Science 330(6011): 1677-1681. Xu, H., I. S. Ramsey, S. A. Kotecha, M. M. Moran, J. A. Chong, D. Lawson, P. Ge, J. Lilly, I. SilosSantiago, Y. Xie, P. S. DiStefano, R. Curtis and D. E. Clapham (2002). "TRPV3 is a calciumpermeable temperature-sensitive cation channel." Nature 418(6894): 181-186. Yaroslavsky, A. N., P. C. Schulze, I. V. Yaroslavsky, R. Schober, F. Ulrich and H. J. Schwarzmaier (2002). "Optical properties of selected native and coagulated human brain tissues in vitro in the visible and near infrared spectral range." Phys Med Biol 47(12): 2059-2073. Yizhar, 0., L. E. Fenno, T. J. Davidson, M. Mogri and K. Deisseroth (2011). "Optogenetics in neural systems." Neuron 71(1): 9-34. Zhang, F., M. Prigge, F. Beyriere, S. P. Tsunoda, J. Mattis, 0. Yizhar, P. Hegemann and K. Deisseroth (2008). "Red-shifted optogenetic excitation: a tool for fast neural control derived from Volvox carteri." Nat Neurosci 11(6): 631-633. Zhang, Y.-P., N. Holbro and T. G. Oertner (2008). "Optical induction of plasticity at single synapses reveals input-specific accumulation of aCaMKII." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(33): 12039-12044. Zhang, Y., E. Ivanova, A. Bi and Z.-H. Pan (2009). "Ectopic Expression of Multiple Microbial Rhodopsins Restores ON and OFF Light Responses in Retinas with Photoreceptor Degeneration." The Journal of Neuroscience 29(29): 9186-9196. Zhao, S., C. Cunha, F. Zhang, Q. Liu, B. Gloss, K. Deisseroth, G. Augustine and G. Feng (2008). "Improved expression of halorhodopsin for light-induced silencing of neuronal activity." Brain Cell Biology 36(1): 141-154. Zhou, H. and R. Desimone (2011). "Feature-Based Attention in the Frontal Eye Field and Area V4 during Visual Search." Neuron 70(6): 1205-1217. Zorzos, A. N., E. S. Boyden and C. G. Fonstad (2010). "Multiwaveguide implantable probe for light delivery to sets of distributed brain targets." Opt Lett 35(24): 4133-4135. 139