IMPLICIT MEMORY: A “HIDDEN WORLD?” • Tasks and terms – “indirect” (vs. direct) memory tests: no memory judgments; assess effects of prior exposure on • Fragment completion • Perceptual identification • Repetition and “feature” priming • Other decisions and actions – Implicit (vs. explicit) memory: the memory systems and/ or processes that (largely) mediate performance in indirect memory tests • Contrast to: – Incidental learning: no reference to memory test during study – Implicit learning: of patterns or correlations without intent or awareness Anecdotal Examples of Implicit Memory • Cases of “unconscious plagiarism” – George Harrison and the Chiffons – Freud’s “discovery” of universal bisexuality, and Fliess’ reaction • Use of expert knowledge – Peter Bonyhard: helped IBM develop mag-resist disk drives, barred from working with competitor Seagate • Implicit memory for traumatic events – Amnesia for rape on a brick path, but words “brick” and “path” come to mind – Global amnesia, home is unfamiliar, but “recently dreamed of that house” • Implicit memory for words spoken during anesthesia – Kilstrohm & Schacter (1990) THE SEARCH FOR DISSOCIATIONS • Stochastic – Performance in IM and EM tasks given same study is uncorrelated • Functional – Weak: variable X influences one kind of test, (not) the other • Levels of processing • Modality – Strong: variable X has opposite effects on IM and EM tests • Read versus generate (Jacoby 83) • Population – A functional dissociation where X is a group factor (amnestics vs. controls) • Reverse Association – X affects A and B the same, Y has opposite effects on A and B, in same data set (Dunn & Kirsner, 1988) A CAPSULE HISTORY of IMPLICIT MEMORY • Late 19th century – Dissociations in the clinic (Dunn, 1845; Claparede, 1889) – Savings without explicit memory (Ebbinghaus, 1885) – Habit versus memory (James, 1890; Bergson, 1911) • 1970’s – Controlled studies of priming in amnestics • HM can learn motor skills • Amnestics show normal fragment-completion priming (Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1970) recogn fragment ID Amnestics .42 .46 Controls .75 .45 • Demonstrations of implicit memory in normals – Jacoby & Dallas (1981): • Depth affects recognition, not priming • Modality affects priming, not recog – Tulving, Schacter & Stark (1982): • much less forgetting for implicit tasks – Jacoby (1983): • Opposite effects of context and generation on implicit and explicit tasks No context XXX-COLD context HOT-COLD generate HOT-XXX Jacoby, 1983 Probability correct 0.85 Identification 0.8 Recognition 0.75 0.7 0.65 0.6 0.55 0.5 no context context Study Task generate • Demonstrations of implicit memory in normals (cont’d) – Graf & Schacter (1987): • Little interference with implicit tasks Word pairs studied (AB) RI: PI: AB AD AD AB --- AB AB Control group learns CD RI PI Ctl Exp Ctl Exp Cued recall Fragment .55 .40 .67 .45 Completion .34 .32 .32 .35 THEORETICAL ACCOUNTS OF IMPLICIT MEMORY • The activation view (Graf & Mandler, 1987) – IM as a subset of EM processes • IM reflects activation of prior memories • EM requires integration / elaboration – Problems: • Amnestics can learn new associations • Priming can last for months • The systems view (Tulving, 1985; Schacter, 1987) – IM based on procedural system, EM on declarative system • EM more advanced • Explains neuroanatomic dissociations – Problems: • A system for every dissociation? • Lack of consensus about criteria • The processing view (Roediger, Weldon & Challis, 1987) – Transfer-appropriate memory tests • IM : data-driven processing • EM: conceptually-driven processing – Dissociations can be TAP-based (Blaxton, 1989) “generate” (vs. read) gives better memory for conceptually-driven tests free recall (EM) semantic cued recall (EM) Jeopardy question-answers (IM) and worse memory for data-driven tests fragment completion (IM) graphemically-cued recall (EM) – Problems: • Fuzzy bounds of processes • Can become circular • Doesn’t handle amnestic data well THE PROCESS-DISSOCIATION APPROACH (Jacoby, 1991) • The problem of “process-impure tests” – Jacoby’s process-dissociation technique – Assumes indendent concious (C) and unconscious (U) contributions to memory – To dissociate these: two sets of items presented (e.g., some read, some heard) inclusion task: recall all exclusion task: recall only heard items p[corr]inclusion = p[C] + p[U] – p[U] x p[C] = p[C] + p[U] x p[1-C] p[corr]exclusion = p[U] x p[1-C] so: p[C] = inclusion – exclusion then solve first equation for U • Applying Process Dissociation: Jacoby, Toth & Yonelinas (1993) study presentation Read Incl Excl Heard Incl Excl Full attn .61 .36 .47 .34 Divided .46 .46 .42 .46 Estimated contributions of C and U to memory: C(conscious) U(automatic) Full attn .25 .47 Divided .00 .46 Controversies about independence and other assumptions