Brain Research 800 Ž1998. 48–61 Research report Therapeutic effects of complex motor training on motor performance deficits induced by neonatal binge-like alcohol exposure in rats I. Behavioral results Anna Y. Klintsova a, ) , Rita M. Cowell a , Rodney A. Swain b , Ruth M.A. Napper c , Charles R. Goodlett d , William T. Greenough a,e,f,g,h a c Beckman Institute, UniÕersity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, 405 N. Mathews AÕe., Urbana IL 61801, USA b Department of Psychology, UniÕersity of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, USA Departments of Anatomy and Structural Biology, UniÕersity of Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand d Department of Psychology, Indiana UniÕersity–Purdue UniÕersity, Indianapolis, USA e Department of Psychology, UniÕersity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA f Departments of Cell and Structural Biology, UniÕersity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA g Department of Psychiatry, UniÕersity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA h Neuroscience Program, UniÕersity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA Accepted 28 April 1998 Abstract The effects of complex motor task learning on subsequent motor performance of adult rats exposed to alcohol on postnatal days 4 through 9 were studied. Male and female Long–Evans rats were assigned to one of three treatments: Ž1. alcohol exposure ŽAE. via artificial rearing to 4.5.g kgy1 dayy1 of ethanol in a binge-like manner Žtwo consecutive feedings., Ž2. gastrostomy control ŽGC. fed isocaloric milk formula via artificial rearing, and Ž3. suckling control ŽSC., where pups remained with lactating dams. After completion of the treatments, the pups were fostered back to lactating dams, and after weaning they were raised in standard cages Žtwo–three animals per cage. until they were 6 months old. Rats from each of the postnatal treatments then spent 20 days in one of three conditions: Ž1. inactive condition ŽIC., Ž2. motor control condition ŽMC. Žrunning on a flat oval track., or Ž3. rehabilitation condition ŽRC. Žlearning to traverse a set of 10 elevated obstacles.. After that all the animals were tested on three tasks, sensitive to balance and coordination deficits Žparallel bars, rope climbing and traversing a rotating rod.. On parallel bars, both male and female rats demonstrated the same pattern of outcomes: AE-IC rats made significantly more mistakes Žslips and falls. than IC rats from both control groups. After 20 days of training in the RC condition, there were no differences between AE and both SC and GC animals in their ability to perform on the parallel bars test. On rope climbing, female animals showed a similar pattern of abilities: AE-IC rats were the worst group; exercising did not significantly improve the AE rats’ ability to climb, whereas the RC groups ŽSC, GC and AE. all performed near asymptote and there were no significant differences among three neonatal treatment groups. There was a substantial effect of the male rats’ heavier body weight on climbing ability, and this may have prevented the deficits in AE rats behavior from being detected. Nevertheless, male animals from all three postnatal treatments ŽSC, GC and AE. were significantly better on this task after RC. Female and male rats from all three postnatal groups demonstrated significantly better performance on the rotarod task after 20 days of ‘rehabilitation’. These results suggest that complex motor skill learning improves some of the motor performance deficits produced by postnatal exposure to alcohol and can potentially serve as a model for rehabilitative intervention. q 1998 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Alcohol; Fetal alcohol effects; Motor learning; Plasticity; Cerebellum 1. Introduction Children with fetal alcohol syndrome ŽFAS. or fetal alcohol effects ŽFAE. exhibit numerous cognitive prob- ) Corresponding author. aklintso@s.psych.uiuc.edu Fax: q 1-217-244-5180; E-mail: 0006-8993r98r$19.00 q 1998 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PII S 0 0 0 6 - 8 9 9 3 Ž 9 8 . 0 0 4 9 5 - 8 lems, hyperactivity and motor deficits Že.g. Refs. w14,34,47,71,72,76x.. Some of the consequences of this prenatal exposure to alcohol appear to be lifelong while others may dissipate with age w46,69,75,76x. In cases lacking distinct facial abnormalities Žsometimes called FAE., a predominant behavioral characteristic that provides a basis for the diagnosis has been cognitive deficits w47x and deficits in motor development and performance w13,14,70x. A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 Not all mothers who consume alcohol during pregnancy produce children with FAS or FAE: the factors that are thought to determine the occurrence of the behavioral and anatomical pathology include the developmental stageŽs. when the drinking episodeŽs. occurred, the pattern of exposure and the peak blood alcohol concentration ŽBAC. reached during drinking episodes w23,35,68,73,93x. Social drinking during pregnancy or lactation has been reported to cause impaired motor development that lasted through adolescence w43,74x. Animal models of developmental exposure to alcohol exhibit many of the behavioral changes observed in children with FAS and FAE: memory and learning impairments w2,17,26,64,97x, developmental impairment of motor skills, poor locomotion and coordination, altered gait w1,20,22,31,39,49,50,56x and hyperactivity w8,9,65,67,82x. Poorly developed motor skills in prenatally and neonatally ethanol-exposed animals prevent them from successfully performing on balance-challenging tasks w20,22,39, 49,50,80x. The behavioral deficits appear to reflect underlying structural damage resulting from exposure to ethanol during development: while damage is widespread, impaired motor control appears to be associated primarily with cerebellar damage Že.g., Refs. w4,10,44,59,60,81x.. A few attempts have been made to reverse or mitigate behavioral incompetence resulting from developmental exposure to ethanol. Early behavioral experience Že.g., complex environment rearing, or familiarization with the radial maze. brought about improvement on learning tasks, such as the Morris water maze and the radial arm maze w30,32,57,58,83x and preweaning handling eliminated the deficit in response inhibition in prenatally alcohol-exposed rats w19x. These studies demonstrated that animals exposed to alcohol prenatally can benefit from the effects of an enriched postweaning environment or other behavioral experiences, and that postnatal factors can ameliorate some of the deficits resulting from prior exposure to alcohol. Rearing rats in an enriched environment after prenatal exposure to alcohol significantly improved behavioral performance, but failed to produce a detectable increase in the density of spines on CA1 pyramidal neurons in hippocampus w5x or an increase in the depth of the occipital cortex w83x, although these changes normally occur in control animals Že.g., Refs. w5,33,37,38,66,95x.. Berman et al.’s w5x findings were interpreted to reflect reduced neural plasticity after prenatal exposure to alcohol. This hypothesis was supported by the demonstration that prenatal alcohol exposure reduced reactive axonal sprouting in basal ganglia induced by nigrostriatal lesions w24x. In contrast, an increase of lesion-induced sprouting was reported in hippocampus of prenatally ethanol-exposed rats after enthorinal cortex lesions w15,89x. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity in the form of long-term potentiation exhibits long-lasting deficits after prenatal exposure to alcohol as shown by Swartzwelder et al. w78x and Sutherland et al. w77x. 49 In a previous report w42x, we demonstrated that Purkinje neurons, the sole output neurons of the cerebellum, retained a substantial capacity for synaptic plasticity after alcohol exposure on postnatal days 4–9, a model of human maternal binge alcohol consumption during the third trimester of pregnancy. Exposure to a program of complex motor skill training resulted in a significant increase in the number of parallel fiber synapses per Purkinje neuron. Furthermore, performance on the task improved across training such that by the end of 10 days, there were no significant differences between alcohol-exposed animals and controls in terms of time to complete the set of tasks used for the motor training. Because of the forced nature of the training, however, it was not possible to conclude that there was significant improvement in the specific behavioral performance of the ethanol-impaired animals. In the present study, we report that motor learning, but not simple exercise, produces a true therapeutic effect on balance and coordination impairments resulting from neonatal ethanol exposure. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects A total of 130 rats Ž65 female, 65 male. from 17 litters resulting from timed pregnancies of adult Long–Evans rats ŽSimonsen Labs, Gilroy, CA. bred in the Indiana University–Purdue University, Indianapolis ŽIUPUI. vivarium were used in this study. Gestational day 0 was identified by the presence of sperm in a vaginal smear taken the morning after an overnight mating. The day of birth was nearly always gestational day 22 Žpostnatal day 0., and litters were culled to 10 pups Ž5 males, 5 females whenever possible. on the day after birth. The breeders, their suckling litters, and the subsequently weaned rats were maintained in the IUPUI vivarium with ad libitum food and water on a 12 h: 12 h light–dark cycle with lights on at 0700 h. Offspring were weaned at 25 days of age and housed 2–4 per cage with same-sex animals. 2.2. Artificial rearing and alcohol exposure On PD 4, pups were assigned randomly within litter and sex to three groups ŽFig. 1.: alcohol-exposed ŽAE. —artificially reared pups given a 10.2% Žvrv. ethanol solution in milk formula on two consecutive feedings each day Ž4.5 g kgy1 dayy1 . from PD 4–9; gastrostomy control ŽGC. — artificially reared pups given matched isocaloric maltoserdextrin solutions on PD 4–9; suckle controls ŽSC. —reared normally by lactating dams. The rats assigned to the artificial rearing groups were surgically implanted with intragastric feeding tubes under methoxyflurane ŽPitman–Moore, Mundelein, IL. anesthe- 50 A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 Fig. 1. Design of the study: pups were evenly distributed across three postnatal treatment groups Žsuckling control—SC, gastrostomy control—GC, and alcohol-exposed—AE. on postnatal days 4–9, then the tubes were removed and animals were returned to their mothers. When the animals were 6 months old, they spent 20 days in one of three conditions: inactive ŽIC., motor ŽMC. or rehabilitation ŽRC.. After completion of this period, they were tested on three specific behavioral tasks. sia on PD 4 and reared using well-established procedures w10,11,21,22,90x. All intragastric feedings used a customized milk formula w90x delivered every 2 h using programmable Harvard Model 22 infusion pumps. Alcohol Žor maltoserdextrin. was provided on the first two feedings after 8:00 h each morning; all other feedings and all feedings on PD 10–12 used milk formula alone. Each day, the rats were fed a volume of formula Žin ml. equal to 33% of the mean body weight Žin g. of the litter being reared. Seventy minutes after the end of the second alcohol feeding on PD 6, a 20-m l sample of blood was collected in a heparinized capillary tube from a tail-clip of each artificially reared pup. The blood from the alcohol-treated pups was assayed with a enzymatic assay for ethanol content ŽSigma kit a332-BT, St. Louis, MO. using a Guilford Response spectrophotometer Žabsorbency at 340 nm. by comparison to a concurrently derived standard curve of five known alcohol concentrations Ž0–450 mgrdl.. Artificially reared offspring were fostered back to lactating dams on PD 12; all rats were weaned at 25 days of age, and housed 2–4 per cage with same-sex littermates thereafter. The rats were identified by a paw code Žon PD 12. by injection of a small amount of India ink into one or more of the paws for subject number, and by an ear punch code Žafter PD 60. designating litter number. Table 1 Female rats body weight at PD 180 and 220 Group Condition N rats Body weight PD 180 Body weight PD 200 SC IC MC RC 7 6 7 294"4 290"7 303"14 286"4 290"7 294"10 GC IC MC RC 6 6 7 291"9 292"14 290"9.5 292"11 277"9 269"8 AE IC MC RC 9 8 9 292"7.8 312"8 296"6 278"8 301"7 285"5 A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 Table 2 Male rats body weight at PD 180 and 200 Group Condition N rats Body weight PD 180 Body weight PD 200 SC IC MC RC 8 7 7 499"12 541"16 528"15 500"12 536"13 508"14 GC IC MC RC 7 7 6 491"24 515"10 482"10 496"24 508"11 472"11 AE IC MC RC 9 7 7 493"13 475"14 500"15 509"13 477"15 470"13 2.3. RehabilitatiÕe motor skill training procedure Rats were transferred to the University of Illinois vivarium at age 60–100 days. They were housed in same-sex pairs until they reached age 180 days. At that point, animals from each treatment group ŽSC, GC and AE. were assigned to one of three training conditions ŽFig. 1.: an inactive condition ŽIC., a motor control condition ŽMC. or a rehabilitative condition ŽRC.. At least 6 animals per grouprconditionrsex were involved in the study, a total of 65 female and 65 male animals in the nine experimental groups. All animals were coded so that the experimenters 51 were not aware of the early postnatal treatment. Whenever possible, littermates with the same postnatal treatment were assigned to IC, MC and RC; no more than 1 male and 1 female of the same treatment within each litter was assigned to a given training condition. On the first day of training, all rats were housed individually in standard laboratory cages. RC rats were given 5 trials per day on an elevated obstacle course for 20 days as described previously w6,41x. The obstacles included a horizontally placed wooden ladder, narrow rods, a link chain, barriers on narrow beams, a rope ladder, an elastic cord, ascending and descending stairs, and a narrow v-shaped metal bridge. The rats were forced to traverse the obstacle course by gentle prodding of the hindquarters, while the tail was loosely held to prevent falls. The time to complete the entire set of 10 obstacles was recorded on each trial. The mean time to complete 5 trialsrday was computed for each animal. Each RC animal was pair matched with an animal in the motor control condition. MC rats were forced to traverse a flat, opaque, oval Plexiglas track equal in length to the acrobatic course. Both animals were placed onto their respective courses at the same time and removed each time that the AC animal had finished each of 5 trials. Animals received the same amount of prodding during training, such that the amount of handling stimulation was comparable for each rat. IC animals were housed individually and received no motor training or extra-cage motor Fig. 2. Mean daily latencies of female rats on the complex motor skill training. Both AE and GC animals were significantly worse than SC during the first 6 days. Data presented as a mean " S.E.M. 52 A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 activity but were handled for about 5 min each day. At the completion of the 20 days of rehabilitative motor skill training, the motor ability of each rat was tested on parallel bars, a rotating rod and a vertically suspended rope. 2.4. Parallel bar testing This task is particularly sensitive to the hindlimb coordination impairment that characterizes alcohol-affected rats Že.g., Ref. w80x.. The parallel bar apparatus consisted of two parallel wooden rods Ž0.48 cm diameter each, 91 cm long. connected to black Plexiglas platforms at each end Ž15 = 15 cm.. The rods were fastened to a wooden block that could be screwed to the platform. Three sets of parallel rods with an inter-rod distance of 2.5 cm, 5 cm or 6.25 cm were used for testing. On the first day, rats were introduced to the 2.5 cm parallel rods. On the second day, the 5 cm bars were used, and on the third day, the 6.25 cm bars were used. On the first day of testing, the subject was initially placed on the starting platform for 30 s. Approximately half of all rats started to traverse the parallel bars on their own initiative. Those that did not were carefully placed on the rods next to the platform, with both left paws on one bar and both right paws on the other bar. Four successive alternating steps with the hind legs on the rods constituted a successful traversal. The number of times the subject placed two hind paws on one rod, dropped a hind paw below the rod, stepped twice in succession with the same hind paw, fell or swung under the rods was recorded. Subjects were given three consecutive trials on parallel bars of a given distance. The subject was removed from the testing for the day and considered to have ‘failed’ if the number of errors was more than 5 per trial Žin which case the subject was assigned 5 errors for that trial and each remaining trial of the day.. For each day of testing, the following data were recorded: mean traversal time per trial, number of errors per trial, the type of error. 2.5. Rotating rod testing The rotating rod apparatus consisted of one of three 2 m long, 15 cm diameter, PVC rods, suspended 1 m above the polyurethane foam-covered bottom of a plywood enclosure. Individual rods either had a roughened surface Žby builders’ sand applied to the freshly painted surface of the rod.; hurdles Ž3.3 cm thick, foam strips wrapped and glued to the surface of the rod at about 40 cm intervals.; or were smooth. Twenty centimeter wide start and goal platforms were at the ends of the rod. These platforms could be extended by flip-down plywood platforms that covered the entire rod or portions of it. The animal’s task was to traverse the rod lengthwise while it was rotating at a preset RPM. Pretraining consisted of the animal’s traversing the apparatus for food reward with the rod entirely covered by the plywood flaps. On day 1 of testing, the rats traversed the roughened rod at speeds of 0, 6, 15 and 25 RPM. Rats were given 3 trials per speed, and hesitation time on the Fig. 3. Mean daily latencies of male rats on the complex motor skill training. AE male rats are significantly worse than both SC and GC rats on the first 8 days of training. Data presented as a mean " S.E.M. A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 platform and the time to traverse the rod were recorded. The number of slips and ‘falls’ Žrats were ‘caught’ by the experimenter such that they did not actually fall. was counted, and the subject was withdrawn from the task after a combination of 5 slips or falls. Days 2 and 3 of testing utilized the smooth rod and the rod with hurdles, respectively, at the speeds reported above. On day 4, the animals were tested on the smooth rod rotating at speeds of 6, 15, 25 and 30 rpm. 2.6. Rope climbing testing Generally, animals that cannot coordinate their forelimb and hind limb movements fail the rope climbing task, and thus the task is also sensitive to the forelimb–hindlimb coordination impairment that is characteristic of the alcohol-produced damage. A vertical rope Ž2.5 cm, 1.9 cm or 1.25 cm in diameter. was suspended from a platform 1 m above the base. During pretraining, the rat was taught by gentle encouragement to climb the upper half of the 2.5 cm thick rope. Following this pretraining session, the rats were tested on 53 the 2.5 cm, 1.9 cm and 1.25 cm thick ropes over the next three consecutive days Ž1 rope per day, 3 trials per day.. Time to climb the entire rope was recorded, and a subjective score was assigned for each trial as follows: 1—excellent performance, no prods or help needed, 2—a small amount of prodding Žusually in the beginning of climbing. was required, 3—the rat would climb up with extensive prodding, but would try to go down when the prodding was interrupted, 4—heavy prodding was required along the whole length of the rope, 5—could not climb or even hold the rope, but slid down. 2.7. Data analysis Although several variables were available for each of the testing procedures, the most straightforward measure of the behavior, i.e., number of failures, was chosen to represent the performance on both the parallel bar and rotating rod tasks. The subjective score was analyzed for the performance on the rope climb. Performance of male and female rats was analyzed separately, because obvious performance differences between the sexes were evident dur- Fig. 4. Results of female Žtop. and male Žbottom. rats’ performance on parallel bars: both male and female AE rats demonstrated a significantly higher number of slipsrfalls than their SC and GC littermates after spending 20 days in IC or MC. Learning the complex motor skill task ŽRC. resulted in significant improvement of performance Žright graphs. in all groups of animals, so that AE rats were no longer different from the two control groups. Data presented as a mean " S.E.M. 54 A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 ing behavioral testing. Statistical tests ŽANOVA. were run using the SAS general linear model procedure and were conducted at a significance level of alpha s 0.05. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA with GROUP Žpostnatal treatment: SC, GC or AE. and TRAINING ŽIC, MC or RC. as between-group factors was used to analyze the results of all three behavioral tests. The Student Newman—Keuls post-hoc Žalpha s 0.05. test was used for a posteriori comparisons among individual groups. 3. Results sex from the three neonatal treatment groups ŽSC, GC and AE. at PD 180 ŽTables 1 and 2., and was not significantly decreased after 20 days of training. 3.2. RehabilitatiÕe motor skill training During the course of the rehabilitative motor skill learning task employed in this study, it became obvious that animals in all groups significantly improved their motor performance. The performance of male and female AE rats on the obstacle course did not differ statistically from either control group by the end of the 20-day training period ŽFigs. 2 and 3.. 3.1. Blood alcohol concentration and body weight 3.3. Parallel bars (Fig. 4) The delivery of alcohol in two consecutive feedings resulted in an average peak blood alcohol concentration of 248 " 10 mgrdl in male animals and 278 " 14 mgrdl in female animals Žblood alcohol concentration was measured in 21 out of 23 male rats and in 20 out of 26 female rats.. At 6 months of age, the male rats were about 70% heavier than the females Žcompare Tables 1 and 2.. Body weight did not differ statistically among the animals of the same The mean number of slips on this task was significantly lower overall in female and male rats after complex motor training compared to inactive control and motor control animals. For females, there was a main effect of neonatal treatment ŽGROUP; F2,140 s 6.65, p - 0.01. and adult TRAINING Ž F2,140 s 29.90, p - 0.001., as well as an interaction ŽGROUP = TRAINING, F4,139 s 2.93, Fig. 5. Results of female Ža. and male Žb. rats testing on rope climbing task. Ža. AE rats Žboth those that remained in individual cages and those who ran on the track. had the least ability to climb and performed significantly more poorly than the rest of the animals. Rehabilitation training resulted in improvement of motor skills of all SC, GC and AE animals such that the groups no longer differed statistically. Žb. Untrained or track-trained male rats from all three ŽSC, GC and AE. groups demonstrated poor climbing abilities Ždue, most probably, to their excessive body weights.. Nevertheless, rehabilitation ŽRC. resulted in significantly better scores in all animals. Data presented as a mean" S.E.M. A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 p - 0.05.. Male rats did not demonstrate a significant interaction between these factors Ž F4,175 s 1.15, n.s.., although there was an effect of GROUP Ž F2,176 s 5.39, p - 0.01. and TRAINING Ž F2,176 s 21.53, p - 0.001.. Post-hoc comparisons showed that both female and male 55 IC and MC rats given neonatal ethanol exposure committed significantly more errors on the parallel bar task than rats from either control group ŽFig. 4a,b, IC and MC panels.. The 20 days of rehabilitative training significantly improved the abilities of the AE male and female rats on Fig. 6. Results of female rats on the rotating rod. AE animals that remained in individual cages had consistently more slips and falls on the test on each day of training Žleft column, open circles.. AE rats that ran on the track Žmiddle column. showed the same pattern of deficits in performance. After 20 days of rehabilitation, the difference in performance disappeared and the skills of all groups were improved Žright column.. Data presented as a mean" S.E.M. 56 A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 this test of hindlimb motor function such that they no longer differed from their SC and GC littermates ŽFig. 4a,b, RC panel.. 3.4. Rope climbing (Fig. 5) This task required animals to climb a series of three ropes of diminishing diameter. Performance on this task was scored from 1 to 5, such that the better the performance the lower the score. Rope climbing performance appeared to be affected by the excessive body weight of the males. For females there was a main effect of neonatal treatment ŽFig. 5a. ŽGROUP; F2,140 s 11.33, p - 0.001. and adult TRAINING Ž F2,140 s 22.66, p - 0.001., as well as GROUP= TRAINING interaction Ž F4,139 s 2.69, p - 0.05.. Male rats did not demonstrate a significant interaction between groups of training ŽFig. 5b. Ž F4,175 s 0.30, n.s.., nor was there an effect of neonatal treatment ŽGROUP; F2,176 s 0.53, n.s... Only Fig. 7. Male rats’ performance on the rotating rod was not as clear cut as in females: the damage produced by exposure to alcohol was not obvious in animals that remained in individual cages Žleft column., and was somewhat recognizable in animals after MC Žmiddle column.. After 20 days of RC training, the skills were improved in all groups of animals Žright column.. Data presented as a mean " S.E.M. A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 adult TRAINING produced a significant effect on males’ performance Ž F2,176 s 41.10, p - 0.001.. Post-hoc comparisons showed that ethanol-exposed IC and MC female rats scored significantly higher Ži.e., worse performance. on the rope climbing test than SC and GC counterparts of the same training conditions ŽFig. 5a.. Female rats from all three postnatal groups given 20 days of ‘rehabilitation’ demonstrated significantly better performance on the rope climbing task ŽFig. 5a, filled symbols.. Post-hoc comparisons for male rats showed no difference between the performance of IC and MC rats from all three postnatal groups ŽFig. 5b., although rehabilitative training resulted in significant improvement of animals from all three groups ŽFig. 5, filled symbols.. This indicates that rehabilitative intervention did offset the effects of alcohol exposure, but that the intervention had no statistically greater effect in AE rats than it had in controls. 3.5. Rotating rod (Figs. 6 and 7) For females, there was a main effect of early postnatal condition ŽGROUP; F2,50 s 3.68, p - 0.05. and adult TRAINING Ž F2,50 s 5.19, p - 0.05., but no GROUP= TRAINING interaction Ž F4,50 s 0.73, n.s... There was also a main effect of speed of rotation Ž F3,150 s 123.02, p 0.001. and a TRAINING= SPEED interaction Ž F6,150 s 3.49, p - 0.01.. Post-hoc comparisons showed that ethanol-exposed IC and MC female rats made significantly more errors on the rotating rod test than their control counterparts of the same adult training, especially at the higher speeds ŽFig. 6, first and second columns of graphs.. Female rats from all three postnatal groups demonstrated significantly better performance on the task ŽFig. 6, third column of graphs. after 20 days of ‘rehabilitation’. This result also indicates that rehabilitative intervention can offset the effects of alcohol exposure but that the intervention had no statistically greater effect than it had in controls. For males ŽFig. 7., there was a main effect of early postnatal condition ŽGROUP; F2,58 s 3.30, p - 0.05. and adult TRAINING Ž F2,58 s 7.23, p - 0.01., but the GROUP= TRAINING interaction only approached significance Ž F4, 58 s 2.24, p s 0.078.. There was also an interaction between the effects of neonatal treatment ŽGROUP., TRAINING and SPEED of rotation Ž F12,150 s 2.48, p 0.01.. Post-hoc comparisons showed that ethanol-exposed IC and MC male rats made significantly more mistakes during all four days of testing than their littermates that underwent the rehabilitative motor learning procedure. A major confound in the testing of the males that apparently contributed to these results was that the excessive body weights of the control males from IC and MC conditions interfered with their performance, making it difficult to detect the effects of early alcohol exposure. 57 4. Discussion This study has demonstrated that acquisition of complex motor tasks, but not mere exercise, can rehabilitate the motor deficits occurring as a result of developmental exposure to alcohol. In this animal model of binge-drinking during the period of brain development comparable to that of the third trimester of human pregnancy, substantial loss of cerebellar neurons has been documented in previous studies reviewed in Ref. w23x. The significant improvement in motor performance produced by behavioral rehabilitation was evident despite the permanent loss of cerebellar neurons induced by the neonatal binge alcohol exposure. Behavioral testing of animals on a set of tasks sensitive to the deficits known to be induced by neonatal alcohol treatment Žtraversing parallel bars and a rotating rod, and rope climbing. demonstrated a generally consistent pattern of improved motor skills in alcohol-exposed animals after rehabilitative training ŽAE-RC. to the extent that they were no longer significantly different from the SC and GC rats. Simple physical exercise Žrunning on a track in the motor condition—MC. did not generally improve motor performance of alcohol-exposed animals. The females followed this pattern of changes quite closely across all tasks. One problem with testing the male animals was that, at the older ages used in this study, their excessive body weights interfered with testing across all experimental conditions. Thus, while males generally followed this pattern, results were not as clear cut as for females. Overall, the results of the post-training behavioral testing demonstrated that only those alcohol-exposed animals that underwent 20 days of complex motor learning procedure improved their motor skills up to the level of control animals, and that, in some cases, the ‘therapy’ had selectively more powerful effects upon the AE group; hence, was truly ‘therapeutic’ for the AE rats. The MC procedure did not statistically enhance the performance of ethanol-exposed female or male rats on any of the behavioral tests. The effects of alcohol on the developing CNS are known to depend on the particular period when the drinking episode occurred Ž so-called ‘ windows of vulnerability’., as well as on the dose and BAC reached Žsee w23x.. Animal studies of the behavioral effect of prenatal alcohol exposure have reported increased openfield activity and reduced locomotor habituation w7,8,18,45,55,58,64,65 x, increased swimming speed in a water maze w82x, and impaired performance in the radial arm maze w30,63x. Increased activity after gestational alcohol exposure may result in part from an alteration of the development of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis w25,79,84–86x, from retarded development of central cholinergic inhibitory systems w64x, as well as from damage to certain brain areas Žvisual and somatosensory cortex, hippocampus. that develop during this gestational period w51–54x and miscommunication and dysregulation between them Že.g., Ref. w88x.. 58 A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 Prior attempts to rehabilitate the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure have included early handling, cross-fostering with normal dams and postweaning exposure to a complex environment w19,32,83,87x. Significant behavioral improvements were established in these studies: Hannigan et al. w32x and Wainwright et al. w83x demonstrated that alcohol-exposed animals learned a Morris water maze task as well as control animals, after the complex environment experience. Gallo and Weinberg w19x reported elimination of the deficit in response inhibition in alcohol-exposed rats after early handling. Weinberg et al. w87x showed that prenatal alcohol-induced deficits in performance on a step-down avoidance test disappeared if the animals were extensively handled during the pre-weaning period. Although successful at the level of behavior, these interventions did not induce detectable morphological plasticity in ethanol-exposed rats w5,83x as they normally do in control animals. For example, Berman et al. w5x did not find increased dendritic plasticity in area CA1 of hippocampus in the animals exposed to enriched environment, and Wainwright et al. w83x reported no change in occipital cortex thickness after complex environment exposure, whereas exposure of normal animals to environmental novelty results in such plasticity w27–29,37,38,66x. These and other w24,77x studies suggest a reduced neural plasticity after prenatal alcohol exposure, though this may depend on brain region and type of measure of neuroplasticity w15,90x. Postnatal exposure of rats to alcohol is a model of drinking during the third trimester of human pregnancy, in terms of comparison based on the timing of developmental events in the brain Že.g., Ref. w16x.. Fig. 8 represents a comparative chart of time and sequence of events in the cerebellar morphogenesis in humans and rats based on Refs. w3,62,96x. Neonatal exposure results in a significant decrease in the weight of forebrain, cerebellum and brain- stem and loss of prenatally generated neurons in some of these areas Žcerebellum, hippocampus, olfactory bulb., suggesting that neurons in certain stages of differentiation Žnot only during neurogenesis. are also vulnerable to the effects of ethanol w11,12,61,80x. Although several brain areas are affected—both structurally and functionally—by exposure to alcohol during the first postnatal days Že.g., Ref. w59x. numerous studies have demonstrated a particular susceptibility of the cerebellum to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol during this period Že.g., w4,36,42,44,91,92,94x.. The degree of impairment of cerebellar structure and function is clearly time- and dose-dependent. Alcohol-induced damage to the development of the cerebellum is paralleled by the deficits in motor performance: poor performance on the rotating rod test w20x and decreased parallel bar traversal ability w22,48,49x. To our knowledge, no attempts to rehabilitate the effectŽs. of postnatal alcohol exposure had been made prior to Klintsova et al. w42x. There we showed that 10 days of learning a complex motor task results in increased synaptogenesis in the cerebellar cortex of rats that had been exposed to ethanol postnatally. In this study, the period of rehabilitative motor skill training was increased from 10 to 20 days to prolong the maintenance phase of the task for the AE animals. Several studies w21,40,59x have reported that exposure to ethanol does not completely prevent the animals from learning the tasks; they accomplish the tasks, but at much slower rate than the controls. We made similar observations during the learning period: although AE animals were significantly worse than controls in the beginning of the 20 days’ training, the difference disappeared across the course of training. Our study supports previous findings of long-term deficits in the motor behavior of alcohol-exposed animals w20,22,48–50,80x: both female and male alcohol-treated Fig. 8. Time chart of events in the development of rat Žtop. and human Žbottom. cerebellum: note that in man the PC dendritic tree orientation and synaptogenesis occur during the weeks 26–39 of gestation, whereas in rat this is an exclusively postnatal event. A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 rats showed significant impairment on parallel bar and rotating rod tests and females also were impaired in the rope climbing test. Most importantly, however, it demonstrates that the ability to learn motor skills after postnatal alcohol treatment can be dramatically improved by a challenging motor skill intervention. A further study is underway to assess morphological plasticity in the cerebellar cortex after 20 days of complex motor task learning. Acknowledgements We thank Stephanie Peterson for assistance with artificial rearing, Jennifer Anderson and Brad Weir for assistance in training and testing animals, and Dr. Ed Roy for permission to share his lab space. This work was supported by PHS AA09838. References w1x E.L. Abel, B.A. Dintcheff, Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on growth and development in rats, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 207 Ž1978. 916–921. w2x E.L. Abel, In utero alcohol exposure and developmental delay of response inhibition, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 6 Ž1982. 369–376. w3x J. Altman, S.A. Bayer, Development of the Cerebellar System, CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, 1997. w4x C. Bauer-Moffet, J. Altman, The effect of ethanol chronically administered to preweanling rats on cerebellar development: A morphological study, Brain Res. 119 Ž1977. 249–268. w5x R.F. Berman, J.H. Hannigan, M.A. Sperry, C.S. Zajac, Prenatal alcohol exposure and the effects of environmental enrichment on hippocampal dendritic spine density, Alcohol. Int. Biomed. J. 13 Ž1996. 209–216. w6x J.E. Black, K.R. Isaacs, B.J. Anderson, A.A. Alcantara, W.T. Greenough, Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 87 Ž1990. 5568–5572. w7x B.A. Blanchard, J.H. Hannigan, Prenatal ethanol exposure: effects on androgen and nonandrogen dependent behaviors and on gonadal development in male rats, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 16 Ž1994. 31–39. w8x N.W. Bond, E.L. DiGiusto, Effects of prenatal alcohol consumption on open-field behavior and alcohol preference in rats, Psychopharmacologia 46 Ž1976. 163–165. w9x N.W. Bond, Prenatal alcohol exposure and offspring hyperactivity: effects of physostigmine and neostigmine, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 10 Ž1988. 59–63. w10x D.J. Bonthius, J.R. West, Alcohol-induced neuronal loss in developing rats: increased brain damage with binge exposure, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 14 Ž1990. 107–118. w11x D.J. Bonthius, J.R. West, Permanent neuronal deficits in rats exposed to alcohol during the brain growth spurt, Teratology 44 Ž1991. 147–163. w12x D.J. Bonthius, N.E. Bonthius, R.M.A. Napper, J.R. West, Early postnatal alcohol exposure acutely and permanently reduces the number of granule cells and mitral cells in the rat olfactory bulb: a stereological study, J. Comp. Neurol. 324 Ž1992. 557–566. w13x L.S. Chandler, G.A. Richardson, J.D. Gallagher, N.L. Day, Prenatal exposure to alcohol and marijuana: effects on motor development of preschool children, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 20 Ž3. Ž1996. 455–461. w14x J. Conry, Neuropsychological deficits in fetal alcohol syndrome and fetal alcohol effects, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 14 Ž1990. 650–655. w15x S.L. Dewey, J.R. West, Evidence for altered lesion-induced sprout- w16x w17x w18x w19x w20x w21x w22x w23x w24x w25x w26x w27x w28x w29x w30x w31x w32x w33x w34x 59 ing in the dentate gyrus of adult rats exposed to ethanol in utero, Alcoholism 1 Ž1984. 81–88. J. Dobbing, J. Sands, Comparative aspects of the brain growth spurt, Early Human Dev. 3 Ž1. Ž1979. 79–83. C.D. Driscoll, A.P. Streissguth, E.P. Riley, Prenatal alcohol exposure: comparability of effects in human and animal models, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 12 Ž1990. 231–237. K. Fernandez, W.F. Caul, M. Haenlein, C.V. Vorhees, Effects of prenatal alcohol on homing behavior, maternal responding and open-field activity in rats, Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 5 Ž1983. 351–356. P.V. Gallo, J. Weinberg, Neuromotor development and response inhibition following prenatal ethanol exposure, Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 4 Ž1982. 505–513. C.R. Goodlett, J.D. Thomas, J.R. West, Long-term deficits in cerebellar growth and rotarod performance of rats following ‘binge-like’ alcohol exposure during the neonatal brain growth spurt, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 13 Ž1991. 69–74. C.R. Goodlett, S.D. Peterson, Sex differences in vulnerability to developmental spatial learning deficits induced by limited binge alcohol exposure in neonatal rats, Neurobiol. Learning Memory 64 Ž1995. 265–275. C.R. Goodlett, K.R. Lundahl, Temporal determinants of neonatal alcohol-induced cerebellar damage and motor performance deficits, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 55 Ž1996. 531–540. C.R. Goodlett, T.B. Johnson, Temporal windows of vulnerability to binge alcohol exposure in the third trimester equivalent: why ‘knowing when’ matters, in: J.H. Hannigan, L.P. Spear, N.E. Spear, C.R. Goodlett ŽEds.., Alcohol and Development: Brain and Behavior, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Englewood, NJ, in press. Z. Gottesfield, C.J. Garcia, R.B. Lingham, R.B. Chronister, Prenatal ethanol exposure impairs lesion-induced plasticity in a dopaminergic synapse after maturity, Neuroscience 29 Ž1989. 715–723. Z. Gottesfeld, Fetal alcohol exposure and functional implications of the neuroimmune-endocrine networks, in: E.L. Abel, ŽEd... Fetal Alcohol Syndrome: From Mechanism to Prevention, CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, 1996, pp. 113–144. P.L. Greene, J.L. Diaz-Granados, A. Amsel, Blood ethanol concentration from early postnatal exposure: Effects on memory-based learning and hippocampal neuroanatomy in infant and adult rats, Behav. Neurosci. 106 Ž1992. 51–61. W.T. Greenough, Chang, F.-L.F., Plasticity of synapse structure and pattern in the cerebral cortex, in: A. Peters, E.G. Jones ŽEds.., Cerebral Cortex 7, Plenum, New York, 1988, pp. 391–440. W.T., Greenough, G.S. Withers, C.S. Wallace, Morphological changes in the nervous system arising from behavioral experience: what is the evidence that they are involved in learning and memory? in: L.R. Squire, E. Lindenlaub ŽEds.., The Biology of Memory, Symposia Medica Hoechst 23, Schattauder Verlag, Stuttgart–New York, 1990, pp. 159–185. W.T. Greenough, J.E. Black, Induction of brain structure by experience: substrates for cognitive development, in: M. Gunnar, C. Nelson ŽEds.., Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychol. 24, 1992, pp. 155–200. J.L. Hall, M.W. Church, R.F. Berman, Radial arm maze deficits in rats exposed to alcohol during midgestation, Psychobiology 22 Ž1994. 181–185. J.H. Hannigan, E.P. Riley, Prenatal ethanol alters gait in rats, Alcohol 5 Ž1988. 451–459. J.H. Hannigan, R.F. Berman, C.S. Zajac, Environmental enrichment and the behavioral effects of prenatal exposure to alcohol in rats, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 15 Ž1993. 261–266. C.H. Horner, M. O’Regan, E. Arbuthnott, Neural plasticity of the hippocampal ŽCA1. pyramidal cell—quantitative changes in spine density following handling and injection for drug testing, J. Anat. 174 Ž1991. 229–238. S.W. Jacobson, J.L. Jacobson, R.J. Sokol, S.S. Martier, J.W. Ager, 60 w35x w36x w37x w38x w39x w40x w41x w42x w43x w44x w45x w46x w47x w48x w49x w50x w51x w52x w53x w54x A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 M.G. Kaplan-Estrin, Teratogenic effects of alcohol on infant development, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 17 Ž1993. 174–183. S.W. Jacobson, J.L. Jacobson, R.J. Sokol, Effects of prenatal exposure to alcohol, smoking and illicit drugs on postpartum somatic growth, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 18 Ž1994. 317. D.G. Jones, Influence of ethanol on neuronal and synaptic maturation in the central nervous system—morphological investigation, Prog. Neurobiol. 31 Ž1988. 171–197. J.M. Juraska, J.M. Fitch, D.L. Washburne, The dendritic morphology of pyramidal neurons in the rat hippocampal CA3 area: II. Effect of gender and experience, Brain Res. 479 Ž1989. 115–119. J.M. Juraska, Sex differences in ‘cognitive’ regions of the rat brain, Psychoneuroendocrinolgy 16 Ž1991. 105–119. S.J. Kelly, S.A. Hulsether, J.R. West, Alterations in sensorimotor development: relationship to blood alcohol concentration, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 9 Ž1987. 243–251. S.J. Kelly, T.D. Tran, Alcohol exposure during development alters social recognition and social communication in rats, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 19 Ž1997. 383–389. J.A. Kleim, E. Lussnig, E.R. Schwarz, T.A. Comery, W.T. Greenough, Synaptogenesis and FOS expression in the motor cortex of the adult rat after motor skill learning, J. Neurosci. 16 Ž1996. 4529–4535. A.Y. Klintsova, J.T. Matthews, C.R. Goodlett, R.M.A. Napper, W.T. Greenough, Therapeutic motor training increases parallel fiber synapse number per Purkinje neuron in cerebellar cortex of rats given postnatal binge alcohol exposure, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 21 Ž1997. 1257–1263. R.E. Little, K.W. Anderson, C.H. Ervin, B. Worthington-Roberts, S.K. Clarren, Maternal alcohol use during breast-feeding and infant motor development at one year, New Engl. J. Med. 321 Ž1989. 425–430. B.L. Marcussen, C.R. Goodlett, J.C. Mahoney, J.R. West, Developing rat Purkinje cells are more vulnerable to alcohol-induced depletion during differentiation than during neurogenesis, Alcohol 11 Ž1994. 147–156. J.C. Martin, D.C. Martin, G. Sigman, B. Radow, Maternal ethanol consumption and hyperactivity in cross-fostered offspring, Physiol. Psychol. 6 Ž1978. 362–365. S.N. Mattson, E.P. Riley, E.R. Sowell, T.L. Jernigan, D.F. Sobel, K.L. Jones, A decrease in the size of the basal ganglia children with fetal alcohol syndrome, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 20 Ž1996. 1088– 1093. S.N. Mattson, E.P. Riley, L. Gramling, D.C. Delis, K.L. Jones, Neurophysiological comparison of alcohol-exposed children with or without physical features of fetal alcohol syndrome, Neuropsychology 12 Ž1998. 146–153. T. Melcer, D. Gonzalez, C. Somes, E.P. Riley, Neonatal alcohol exposure and early development of motor skills in alcohol preferring and nonpreferring rats, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 17 Ž1995. 103–110. L.S. Meyer, L.E. Kotch, E.P. Riley, Alterations in gait following ethanol exposure during the brain growth spurt in rats, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 14 Ž1990. 23–27. L.S. Meyer, L.E. Kotch, E.P. Riley, Neonatal ethanol exposure: functional alterations associated with cerebellar growth retardation, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 12 Ž1990. 15–22. M.W. Miller, Effects of alcohol on the generation and migration of cerebral cortical neurons, Science 233 Ž1986. 1308–1311. M.W. Miller, Effects of prenatal exposure to ethanol on neocortical development: II. Cell proliferation in the ventricular and subventricular zones of the rat, J. Comp. Neurol. 287 Ž1989. 326–338. M.W. Miller, G. Potempa, Numbers of neurons and glia in mature rat somatosensory cortex: effects of prenatal exposure to ethanol, J. Comp. Neurol. 293 Ž1990. 92–102. M.W. Miller, S. Robertson, Prenatal exposure to ethanol alters the postnatal development and transformation of radial glia to astrocytes in the cortex, J. Comp. Neurol 337 Ž1993. 253–266. w55x H.K. Mothes, B. Opitz, R. Werner, P. Clausing, Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure and early experience on home-cage and open-field activity in mice, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 18 Ž1996. 59–65. w56x S. Norton, P. Terranova, J. Yol Na, M. Sancho-Tello, Early motor development and cerebral cortical morphology in rats exposed perinatally to alcohol, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 12 Ž1988. 130–136. w57x B. Opitz, H.K. Mothes, P. Clausing, Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure and early experience on radial maze performance and conditioned taste aversion in mice, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 19 Ž1997. 185–190. w58x G.L. Osborne, W.F. Caul, K. Fernandez, Behavioural effects of prenatal ethanol exposure and differential early experience in rats, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 12 Ž1980. 393–401. w59x J. Pauli, P. Wilce, K.S. Bedi, Acute exposure to alcohol during early postnatal life causes a deficit in the total number of cerebellar Purkinje cells in the rat, J. Comp. Neurol. 360 Ž1995. 506–512. w60x D.R. Pierce, C.R. Goodlett, J.R. West, Differential neuronal loss following early postnatal alcohol exposure, Teratology 40 Ž1989. 113–126. w61x D.R. Pierce, D.C. Serbus, K.E. Light, Intragastric intubation of alcohol during postnatal development of rats results in selective cell loss in the cerebellum, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 17 Ž1993. 1275– 1280. w62x P. Rakic, R.L. Sidman, Histogenesis of cortical layers in human cerebellum, particularly the lamina dissecans, J. Comp. Neurol. 139 Ž1970. 473–500. w63x E. Reyes, J. Wolfe, D.D. Savage, The effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on radial-arm maze performance in adult rats, Physiol. Behav. 46 Ž1989. 45–48. w64x E.P. Riley, E.A. Lochry, N.R. Shapiro, Lack of response inhibition in rats prenatally exposed to alcohol, Psychopharmacology 62 Ž1979. 47–52. w65x E.P. Riley, S. Barron, C.D. Driscoll, R.T. Hamlin, The effects of physostigmine on open-field behavior in rats exposed to alcohol prenatally, Alcohol Drug Res. 6 Ž1986. 351–360. w66x M.R. Rosenzweig, E.L. Bennett, M. Hebert, H. Morimoto, Social grouping cannot account for cerebral effects of enriched environments, Brain Res. 153 Ž1978. 563–576. w67x B.A. Shaywitz, G.G. Griffieth, J.B. Warshaw, Hyperactivity and cognitive defects in developing rat pups born to alcoholic mothers: an expanded fetal alcohol syndrome ŽEFAS., Neurobehav. Toxicol. 1 Ž1979. 113–122. w68x D.E. Smith, A. Foundas, J. Canale, Effect of perinatally administered ethanol on the development of the cerebellar granule cell, Exp. Neurol. 92 Ž1986. 491–501. w69x H.L. Spohr, J. Willms, H.C. Steinhausen, Prenatal alcohol exposure and long-term developmental consequences, Lancet 341 Ž1993. 907–910. w70x A.P. Streissguth, C.S. Herman, D.W. Smith, Intelligence, behaviour and dysmorphogenesis in the fetal alcohol syndrome: a report on 20 patients, J. Pediatr. 92 Ž1978. 363–367. w71x A.P. Streissguth, S. Landesman-Dwyer, J.C. Martin, D.W. Smith, Teratogenic effects of alcohol in humans and laboratory animals, Science 209 Ž1980. 353–361. w72x A.P. Streissguth, The behavioural teratology of alcohol; performance, behavioral and intellectual deficits in prenatally exposed children, in: J.R. West ŽEd.., Alcohol and Brain Development, Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1986, 3–44. w73x A.P. Streissguth, P.D. Sampson, H.M. Barr, Neurobehavioral doseresponse effects of prenatal alcohol exposure in humans from infancy to adulthood, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 562 Ž1989. 145–158. w74x A.P. Streissguth, H.M. Barr, P.D. Sampson, Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure: effects on child IQ and learning problems at age 7 1r2 years, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 14 Ž1990. 662–669. w75x A.P. Streissguth, J.M. Aase, S.K. Clarren, S.P. Randels, R.A. LaDue, D.F. Smith, Fetal alcohol syndrome in adolescents and adults, J. Am. Med. Ass. 265 Ž1991. 1961–1967. A.Y. KlintsoÕa et al.r Brain Research 800 (1998) 48–61 w76x A.P. Streissguth, P.D. Sampson, H. Carmichael Olson, F.L. Bookstein, H.M. Barr, M. Scott, J. Feldman, A.F. Mirsky, Maternal drinking during pregnancy: attention and short-term memory in 14-year-old offspring—A longitudinal prospective study, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 18 Ž1994. 202–218. w77x R.J. Sutherland, R.J. McDonald, D.D. Savage, Prenatal exposure to moderate levels of ethanol can have long-lasting effects on hippocampal synaptic plasticity in adult offspring, Hippocampus 7 Ž1997. 232–238. w78x H.S. Swartzwelder, K.L. Farr, W.A. Wilson, D.D. Savage, Prenatal exposure to ethanol decreases physiological plasticity in the hippocampus of the adult rat, Alcohol 5 Ž1988. 121–124. w79x A.N. Taylor, B.J. Branch, S.H. Liu, N. Kokka, Long-term effects of fetal ethanol exposure on pituitary–adrenal response to stress, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 16 Ž1982. 585–589. w80x J.D. Thomas, E.A. Wasserman, J.R. West, C.R. Goodlett, Behavioral deficits induced by bingelike exposure to alcohol in neonatal rats: importance of developmental timing and number of episodes, Dev. Psychobiol. 29 Ž1996. 433–452. w81x B. Volk, Cerebellar histogenesis and synaptic maturation following pre- and postnatal alcohol administration. An electron-microscopic investigation of the rat cerebellar cortex, Acta Neuropathol., Berlin 63 Ž1984. 57–65. w82x C.V. Vorhees, K. Fernandez, Effects of short-term prenatal alcohol exposure on maze activity, and olfactory orientation performance in rats, Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 8 Ž1986. 23–28. w83x P.E. Wainwright, S. Levesque, L. Krempulec, B. Bulman-Fleming, D. McCutcheon, Effects of environmental enrichment on cortical depth and Morris–Maze performance in B6D2F2 mice exposed prenatally to ethanol, Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 15 Ž1993. 11–20. w84x J. Weinberg, Effects of ethanol and maternal nutritional status on fetal development, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 9 Ž1985. 49–55. w85x J. Weinberg, Hyperresponsiveness to stress: differential effects of prenatal ethanol on males and females, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 12 Ž1988. 647–652. w86x J. Weinberg, Prenatal ethanol exposure alters adrenocortical development of offspring, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 13 Ž1989. 73–83. 61 w87x J. Weinberg, C.K. Kim, W. Yu, Early handling can attenuate adverse effects of fetal alcohol exposure, Alcohol 12 Ž1995. 317– 327. w88x J.R. West, C.A. Hodges, A.C. Black, Prenatal ethanol exposure alters the organization of hippocampal mossy fibers, Science 211 Ž1981. 957–959. w89x J.R. West, S.L. Dewey, M.D. Cassell, Prenatal ethanol exposure alters the post-lesion reorganization Žsprouting. of acetylcholinesterase staining in the dentate gyrus of adult rats, Dev. Brain Res. 12 Ž1984. 83–95. w90x J.R. West, K. Hamre, D.R. Pierce, Delay in brain growth induced by alcohol in artificially reared rat pups, Alcohol 1 Ž1984. 213–222. w91x J.R. West, K.M. Hamre, M.D. Cassel, Effects of ethanol exposure during the third trimester equivalent on neuron number in rat hippocampus and dentate gyrus, Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 10 Ž1986. 190–197. w92x J.R. West, Fetal alcohol-induced brain damage and the problem of determining temporal vulnerability: a review, Alcohol Drug Res. 7 Ž1987. 423–441. w93x J.R. West, C.R. Goodlett, D.J. Bonthius, D.R. Pierce, Manipulating peak blood alcohol concentrations in neonatal rats: review of an animal model for alcohol-related developmental effects, Neurotoxicology 10 Ž1989. 347–366. w94x J.R. West, C.R. Goodlett, Teratogenic effects of alcohol on brain development, Ann. Med. 22 Ž1990. 319–325. w95x R.W. West, W.T. Greenough, Effect of environmental complexity on cortical synapses of rats: preliminary results, Behav. Biol. 7 Ž1972. 279–284. w96x N. Zecevic, P. Rakic, Differentiation of Purkinje cells and their relationship to other components of developing cerebellar cortex in man, J. Comp. Neurol. 167 Ž1976. 27–48. w97x B. Zimmerberg, H.L. Sukel, J.D. Stekler, Spatial learning of adult rats with fetal alcohol exposure: deficits are sex-dependent, Behav. Brain Res. 42 Ž1991. 49–56.