ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2005, 69, 427–435 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.03.017 Worker nutrition and division of labour in honeybees A MY L. TOTH * & G ENE E. ROB IN SON *† *Program in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign yDepartment of Entomology and Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign (Received 4 September 2003; initial acceptance 4 December 2003; final acceptance 23 March 2004; published online 16 December 2004; MS. number: A9692) We determined whether there is an association between nutritional state (as indicated by stored abdominal lipid amounts) and division of labour in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. We found that foragers (typically older bees) had lower lipid amounts than did nurses (typically young bees). Results from experimental colonies that contained nurses and foragers of the same age showed that the lipid decline in foragers was not attributable to age. Analysis of bees with different amounts of foraging experience revealed little effect of the act of foraging on lipid stores. Lipid levels were low even on the first day of foraging, suggesting that the decline in stored lipid precedes the onset of foraging. We also found that bees that revert from foraging to nursing did not regain their lipid stores, indicating that high lipid stores are not required to sustain brood care behaviour. This demonstration of a robust association between reduced lipid stores and the transition to foraging suggests that worker nutritional state may be involved in the regulation of division of labour in honeybee colonies. Ó 2004 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Colonies of advanced social insects show two forms of division of labour: a division of labour between queens and workers for reproduction and a division of labour among workers for tasks related to colony growth and development (Robinson 1992). In the social Hymenoptera, reproductive division of labour between morphologically distinct queens and workers involves differences in nutrition during larval development (Wheeler 1986; Hartfelder & Engels 1998). Division of labour between morphologically distinct worker castes in ants also relates to differences in larval nutrition (Wheeler & Nijhout 1983). Nutritional differences have been shown to be correlated with behavioural division of labour among morphologically identical workers (Blanchard et al. 2000), but the role of nutrition in these systems is less understood than for systems that involve morphological differentiation. Many social insect species show age-related division of labour, whereby each worker progresses through a set of behavioural changes over her life, usually involving a transition from nest work to foraging (Wilson 1971). This involves not only drastic changes in behaviour, but sometimes also physiological changes such as the development of glands for brood feeding (Winston 1987) Correspondence: A. L. Toth, Department of Entomology, 505 S. Goodwin Avenue, 320 Morrill Hall, Urbana, IL 61801, U.S.A. (email: amytoth@life.uiuc.edu). 0003–3472/04/$30.00/0 and higher metabolism in foragers (Martin & Lieb 1979; Harrison 1986). Although patterns of temporal polyethism are robust, worker behavioural development in several species is flexible and can be accelerated, delayed, or reversed in response to colony needs (Robinson 1992). Since age can be uncoupled from behaviour, other factors take precedence in the control of worker behavioural development. Studies with honeybees have implicated social, endocrine, neurochemical and genetic factors (Robinson 2002). In several species of social insects, nutritional differences among workers appear to be associated with task performance. Porter & Jorgensen (1981) reported that Pogonomyrmex owyheei ant foragers have substantially less body weight than callow workers, and these authors further suggested that depletion of body energy reserves may stimulate foraging. Blanchard et al. (2000) found a negative correlation between amounts of energy reserves and foraging tendency in Leptothorax albipennis ants. The pattern of ‘lean forager–corpulent nest worker’ appears to be common in the social Hymenoptera, as it has been observed in at least eight species of ants and two wasps (see Tschinkel 1987, 1998; Blanchard et al. 2000; Markiewicz & O’Donnell 2001). Based on their findings and a review of other evidence from ants, wasps and bees, Blanchard et al. (2000) proposed that differences in the nutritional status of workers could be involved in the regulation of division of labour. Supporting this idea, 427 Ó 2004 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 428 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 69, 2 honeybees in starved colonies initiate foraging at significantly younger ages than well-fed bees (Schulz et al. 1998), suggesting that a change in worker nutritional state could result in a behavioural change. However, the extent to which differences in worker nutritional state influence regulation of age-related division of labour is unknown. Because nutrition can interact with numerous other physiological processes, a better understanding of possible nutritional factors is essential to the study of mechanisms that affect division of labour. The objective of this study was to determine whether there is an association between nutritional state and division of labour in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. We focused on two distinct honeybee behavioural states, brood care (nursing) and foraging, which represent easily identifiable tasks performed early and late in behavioural development, respectively. Nursing is typically performed during the first 2 weeks of adult life, whereas foraging is performed from about 2–3 weeks of age and proceeds for the remainder of the typical 5–7-week life span (Winston 1987). As an indicator of nutritional state we measured abdominal lipid, since energy stores in adult social Hymenoptera are found mainly as lipid in the fat body (Ricks & Vinson 1972). In experiment 1 we determined whether honeybees show the lean forager–corpulent nest worker pattern observed in other social insects (Blanchard et al. 2000). In experiment 2 we examined whether differences in lipid stores are associated with worker age or behaviour. In experiment 3 we determined whether abdominal lipid is affected by foraging, which is an energetically demanding task. In experiment 4 we investigated whether bees that shift back from foraging to nursing experience a corresponding change in lipid levels. METHODS Bees Honeybees of a mix of European races were maintained at the University of Illinois Bee Research Facility in Urbana, Illinois, U.S.A. Experiments 1–3 were performed between June and August 2001. Experiment 4 was performed between June and July 2002. We collected 1-dayold bees (experiments 1–3) by removing frames of comb containing capped brood from source colonies with multiply mated queens. We placed frames in a 33 C incubator overnight and collected bees that had emerged over a period of approximately 24 h for experiments. We marked 1-day-old bees for later identification by painting a small spot of Testor’s enamel paint on the thorax. All bees collected for lipid analysis were killed by freezing in a 80 C freezer and kept frozen at 80 C until assayed. Lipid Assay For each bee, we removed the entire digestive tract, along with the sting apparatus and any wax scales observed on the outside of the bee. Each abdomen was freeze-dried at 300 mTorr (4 Pa) for 62 min, placed in 5 ml of 2:1 chloroform:methanol (Folch extraction method, Perkins 1975), homogenized using Kontes Tissue Grinders, and allowed to extract overnight. The extract was filtered with glass wool and evaporated in a Savant Speed Vac SC110 to a constant volume of 2 ml. We used a 100-ml subsample of each extract for the lipid assay (developed from Amenta 1964). We dried each sample completely, then added 1 ml of reagent (0.25% potassium dichromate in 80% sulphuric acid) and heated all samples for 20 min in a bath of boiling water. We then diluted 100 ml of the reaction product with 4 ml of distilled water and measured absorbance of the sample using a Molecular Devices SpectraMax 190 multiwell spectrophotometer at 350 nm. We converted absorbance measurements to milligrams of lipid using a cholesterol standard with each assay run. Histological Examination of Fat Bodies We examined fat bodies from individual bees histologically. We removed the abdomen, dissected out the digestive tract and sting, and cut a small square of cuticle with adhered fat body tissue (visible with a dissecting scope) from the middle of the second tergite of the gaster. The fat body tissue was smeared on a slide, fixed with formaldehyde, and stained with Oil Red O. We examined the tissue under a compound microscope at 400! and photographed it using an Olympus C3030 digital camera at F 2.8, 1/30 s. Lipid staining was quantified using Adobe Photoshop by selecting red pixels, which were counted with the Histogram tool. Experiment 1: do abdominal lipid stores of nurses and foragers differ? To compare lipid levels between nurse and forager bees, we paint-marked 1-day-old bees and introduced them into a typical field colony. Assessments of nursing and foraging behaviour were made as described in Huang et al. (1994). When marked bees were 7 days old, we collected a subset of the bees observed performing nursing behaviour (bees with their heads in cells containing larvae). When they were 21 days old, we collected another set of bees observed performing foraging behaviour (returning to the hive carrying pollen or with abdomens distended with nectar). Two trials were performed. In trial 1 only, fat bodies from nurses (N Z 10) and foragers (N Z 10) were stained for histology as described above. Experiment 2: are abdominal lipid stores associated with age or task performance? To examine whether abdominal lipid stores are related to age or task performance, we measured lipid stores of nurses and foragers in single-cohort colonies at 7 and 21 days of age. We used single-cohort colonies because a division of labour is established within a few days, producing bees of the same age performing different tasks (Robinson et al. 1989). To construct each colony, we placed 1100–1200 1-day-old bees into a small hive box with one empty frame of comb, one frame with an excess of honey and pollen, and a naturally mated queen. When bees were 7 days old, we collected precocious foragers and TOTH & ROBINSON: NUTRITION AND DIVISION OF LABOUR normal-age nurses. When bees were 21 days old, we collected normal-age foragers and overage nurses (only in trials 1 and 2). Three trials were performed. Experiment 3: do abdominal lipid stores vary with foraging experience? We examined lipid levels in bees with different amounts of flight experience. To control flight experience, we glued plastic tags to the thoraces of bees and attached a screen to the hive entrance to prevent tagged bees from leaving the hive (Withers et al. 1995). Three treatment groups were analysed: bees with unrestricted flight, partially restricted flight, and totally restricted flight. Unrestricted bees were not tagged but were paint-marked for later identification. Partially restricted bees bore plastic tags that increased their height by approximately 1.5 mm. Totally restricted bees bore plastic tags that increased their height by approximately 3 mm. One-day-old bees were collected and randomly chosen to be in one of the three groups. All three groups of bees were placed into a single Langstroth hive box containing an already established colony with a typical age demography and approximately 10 000 bees. To restrict the flight of tagged bees, we used a modified hive entrance with two screens, the first with holes slightly larger than those in the second (Fig. 1). Unrestricted bees passed through both screens and had full flight experience. Partially restricted bees could pass through the first screen, but not the second. The second screen was removed every afternoon at 1300 hours, giving partially restricted bees approximately a half day for foraging. Totally restricted bees could not pass through the first screen, and thus had no flight experience. We collected bees from these three groups on the first, seventh and 14th day following the onset of foraging by unrestricted (control) and partially restricted bees. Marked 7-day-old nurse bees were also collected to provide a baseline comparison. To validate the effects of tagging on flight restriction, bees in the three flight experience Unrestricted Partially restricted groups were visually assessed for the presence or absence of noticeable wing wear. Wing wear was defined as tears in or small pieces missing from the wing margin. Honeybee wings become tattered during flight, and wing wear can be used to assess the extent of flight experience (Breed et al. 1990). One trial of this experiment was performed. To assure there were no residual effects of carrying tags on lipid levels, we performed a ‘control’ experiment in which tagged bees were not restricted by screened entrances. Large plastic tags (3 mm high) were glued to the backs of 1-day-old bees, and these bees were allowed unrestricted flight by placing them in a pre-established colony of approximately 10 000 bees with a normal hive entrance. Tagged and untagged paint-marked bees were collected for lipid analyses on the first, seventh and 14th day following the onset of foraging. Experiment 4: is behavioural reversion associated with an increase in abdominal lipid? To test whether foragers that revert to nursing behaviour experience an increase in abdominal lipid stores, we set up ‘reversion colonies’ (Robinson et al. 1992). In reversion colonies, a colony composed entirely of foragers establishes a division of labour; some bees continue to forage while others switch back to nursing behaviour (reverted nurses). Reversion colonies were made as follows. We collected 2000 foragers from a typical field colony (source colony). These bees were placed in a small hive box with an empty frame, a frame with open brood from the source colony, and the queen from the source colony (as in Bloch & Robinson 2001). The new reversion colony was moved several kilometres from the source colony to prevent bees from returning to the original colony. The colony entrance was left closed for 1 day (day 1 of the experiment) to allow bees to adjust to the new colony environment. On days 2–4, we opened the entrance and paint-marked all bees on the thorax that we observed performing nursing behaviour. On day 5, we Totally restricted 1 2 Figure 1. Design of experiment 3. Honeybee flight from the hive was either unrestricted (untagged bees), partially restricted (bees with 1.5mm tags) or totally restricted (bees with 3-mm tags) using two screens (1 and 2) that modified the size of the entrance. Unrestricted bees could pass through both screens, and had full flight experience. Partially restricted bees could pass through screen 1, but not screen 2. Screen 2 was removed each afternoon, allowing partially restricted bees half of the day for foraging. Totally restricted bees could not pass through screen 1, and thus had no flight experience. 429 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 69, 2 collected reverted nurses and foragers for lipid analysis. We identified reverted nurses as previously paint-marked bees performing nursing behaviour. Two trials of this experiment were performed. Statistical Analyses Statistical analyses were performed using SAS Software (SAS Institute 2000). Lipid level measurements were natural-log transformed to normalize the data. Extreme outliers were excluded from analyses, and were identified as those values with studentized residuals greater than 2.6. In experiments with multiple trials (experiments 1, 2 and 4), we examined results from each trial separately and in experiment-wide analyses. We used unpaired t tests (PROC GLM) to analyse the lipid assay and staining data for experiment 1, the trial 3 data for experiment 2, and all data for experiment 3. We used ANOVA (PROC GLM) to analyse the data for trials 1 and 2 of experiment 2 and for experiment 4 (experimental and control colonies were analysed separately). For experiment 2, behaviour (nursing versus foraging) and age (7 versus 21 days) were analysed as fixed effects. For experiment 4, behaviour (nursing versus all flight experience groups) was analysed as a fixed effect. After removing nurses from the model to allow comparisons between flight experience groups, we analysed the effects of treatment (control, partial restriction, total restriction) and days of foraging (1, 7, 14). For the effect of days of foraging, we also calculated power based on observed treatment means and sample sizes using SAS PROC MIXED (Stroup 2002). For pooled, experiment-wide analyses (experiments 1, 2 and 4), we performed nested ANOVAs with trial analysed as a random effect using PROC MIXED, and with bee nested within trial. For all post hoc comparisons, P values were adjusted for experiment-wise error using a Tukey’s adjustment. RESULTS Foragers had significantly lower lipid levels than nurse bees (unpaired two-tailed t tests: trial 1: t35 Z 3.80, 3 2 * ** 1 Trial 1 Trial 2 Figure 2. Lipid levels (means G SE) of foragers (-) and nurses ( from two typical field colonies. *P ! 0.01; **P ! 0.001. P Z 0.0006; trial 2: t34 Z 2.71, P Z 0.01; Fig. 2). A pooled analysis of both trials showed a significant effect of behaviour (nested ANOVA: F1,71.3 Z 18.44, P ! 0.0001) and no difference between trials (Wald statistic: Z Z 0.24, P Z 0.41). Furthermore, Oil Red O staining of the fat bodies revealed markedly less red-stained (lipid-containing) tissue in foragers (Fig. 3a, b) than in nurses (Fig. 3c, d). The difference in lipid-staining intensity was statistically significant, with a significantly lower number of counted red pixels in forager abdominal tissue (unpaired two-tailed t test: trial 1: t18 Z 5.23, P ! 0.0001; Fig. 3e). Experiment 2: Are Abdominal Lipid Stores Associated with Age or Task Performance? In all three trials, lipid levels in both precocious foragers and normal-age foragers were lower than those of both groups of nurses (normal-age and overage). In all trials, there were significant differences in lipid levels due to behaviour (nursing versus foraging) (ANOVA: trial 1: F1,72 Z 12.05, P Z 0.0009, N Z 76; trial 2: F1,41 Z 12.34, P ! 0.001, N Z 45; unpaired two-tailed t test: trial 3: t16 Z 3.06, P Z 0.008; Fig. 4). The effect of age (7 versus 21 days) was significant in trial 1 (F1,72 Z 6.53, P Z 0.01), but not significant in trial 2 (F1,41 Z 0.02, P Z 0.90). There was no behaviour)age interaction in either trial (trial 1: F1,72 Z 1.68, P Z 0.20; trial 2: F1,41 Z 0.13, P Z 0.72). The significant age effect in trial 1 can be attributed to the fact that lipid levels in precocious foragers were higher in trial 1 than in the other two trials. In a pooled analysis of the three trials, there were significant differences in abdominal lipid levels due to behaviour (nested ANOVA: F1,134 Z 32.54, P ! 0.0001), but not age (F1,134 Z 1.33, P Z 0.25) or trial (Wald statistic: Z Z 0.69, P Z 0.25). The results of this experiment indicate that differences in abdominal lipid stores were strongly associated with differences in bee behaviour and not age. Experiment 3: Do Abdominal Lipid Stores Vary with Foraging Experience? Experiment 1: Do Abdominal Lipid Stores of Nurses and Foragers Differ? Lipid (mg) 430 ) To examine the effectiveness of our flight restriction treatments (unrestricted, partially restricted, totally restricted), we used wing wear as an indicator of flight experience and sampled bees (N Z 19 for each group) after 7 days of flight (for unrestricted and partially restricted bees). The unrestricted flight group had the highest proportion of bees with noticeable wing wear (21%), followed by the bees with partially restricted flight (10.5%). None of the bees in the totally restricted flight group showed noticeable wing wear. Although flight activity of each group of bees was not quantified, casual observations (A.L.T.) showed a steady stream of foragers throughout the morning and afternoon, with partially restricted bees ‘waiting’ to exit each day when the second screen was removed. We can safely conclude that our treatments caused partial and total restriction of flight. Foragers in all three flight groups regardless of the extent of flight experience, had low, forager-typical, lipid levels (Fig. 5). Nurse bees had significantly higher lipid TOTH & ROBINSON: NUTRITION AND DIVISION OF LABOUR (a) (c) (b) (d) Red pixels (%) 9 (e) ** 6 3 Foragers Nurses Figure 3. Representative examples of fat body stained with Oil Red O from two foragers (a, b) and two nurses (c, d) collected from typical field colonies. Lipid granules are stained red (darkly coloured). (e) Percentage of fat body stained red with Oil Red O in honeybee foragers and nurses. **P ! 0.001. levels than all three flight groups (ANOVA: F1,103 Z 16.85, N Z 127, P ! 0.0001). There was a significant, but much smaller, difference in lipid levels between the three flight groups (ANOVA: F2,103 Z 3.68, N Z 112, P Z 0.029), suggesting a possible effect of flight experience. There was no significant difference in lipid levels as a function of the number of days of foraging (F2,103 Z 3.68, 2.12, P Z 0.125). However, because the magnitude of the difference between mean lipid levels for days 1, 7 and 14 was small, the power of this test was low (38.7% power, or 61.3% probability of type II error). Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility that there may have been a small but statistically undetectable effect of days of foraging experience on lipid levels. None the less, lipid levels of bees with little or no flight experience (bees collected on day 1 of flight experience) were substantially lower than those of nurse bees. In the control experiment, we detected no side effects of the tagging treatment on bee lipid levels. Lipid levels in free-flying tagged bees were not significantly different from those of untagged, paint-marked bees (ANOVA: F1,54 Z 0.04, N Z 58, P Z 0.841). In addition, as in the previous case, there was no difference in lipid levels as a function of number of days of foraging (F2,54 Z 0.01, P Z 0.987). Experiment 4: Is Behavioural Reversion Associated with an Increase in Abdominal Lipid? Bees did not regain their lipid stores when reverting to nursing behaviour, at least not within the first 5 days. In both trials, we found no difference between lipid levels of 431 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 69, 2 2 b' b' 2 a a a 1 a' ** a' b 1.5 Age (days) 7 21 7 21 7 Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Figure 4. Lipid levels (means G SE) of precocious foragers (-) and normal-age nurses ( ) collected on day 7, and normal-age foragers (G) and overage nurses (,) collected on day 21 from single-cohort colonies. Lipid levels that were significantly different (within trials) are designated with different letters (a, b, a0 , b0 ). **P ! 0.001. reverted nurses and foragers (unpaired two-tailed t tests: trial 1: t35 Z 1.71, P Z 0.095; trial 2: t37 Z 0.72, P Z 0.48; Fig. 6). A pooled analysis gave similar results (nested ANOVA: F1,74 Z 0.15, P Z 0.701) with no difference between trials (Wald statistic: Z Z 0.76, P Z 0.22). Bees in both groups had low, forager-typical lipid levels. DISCUSSION The results of this study show a strong association between nutrition and division of labour in honeybees. Foragers typically had half the lipid stores of nurses, paralleling the lean forager–corpulent nest worker pattern observed in other social insects. For example, in the ant Pogonomyrmex owyheei, foraging workers are 40% leaner than nest workers (Porter & Jorgensen 1981). In another species of this genus, Pogonomyrmex badius, young workers at the bottom of a nest have 30% fat, older workers near the top of a nest have less (w23%), and foragers always have less than 10% (Tschinkel 1998). Callow workers of Prenolepis imparis ants act as ‘corpulents’, with 60% abdominal fat; the following year they lose this fat almost entirely after brood rearing and become foragers (Tschinkel 1987). 2 c 1.5 b 1 ab a ab ab ab ab a a 0.5 1 7 14 Nurse Collection day Figure 5. Lipid levels (means G SE) of bees with unrestricted flight (-), partially restricted flight ( ) and totally restricted flight (G) collected on the first, seventh and 14th day following the onset of foraging by unrestricted and partially restricted bees, and lipid levels of nurses collected from the same colony. Lipid levels that were significantly different are designated with different letters (a, b, c). Lipid (mg) Lipid (mg) 3 Lipid (mg) 432 1 NS NS 0.5 Trial 1 Trial 2 Figure 6. Lipid levels (means G SE) of honeybee foragers (-) and reverted nurses ( ). Results from our single-cohort colony experiment show that nutritional differences are more closely related to behavioural state than to age. Overage nurses delayed foraging, but maintained high lipid levels. Conversely, bees that foraged precociously had low lipid levels despite their young age. These results suggest that changes in lipid stores are mediated by changes in lipid metabolism or feeding behaviour that are associated with some of the different tasks performed by a worker honeybee during her life. Because there are genotypic differences in foraging ontogeny in honeybees (e.g. Giray et al. 1999), the foragers we sampled from the single-cohort colonies may have shown a genotypic predisposition to early foraging. Thus, the differences in lipid levels seen in precocious foragers may have been a consequence of genotypic variation in lipid physiology unrelated to foraging. However, we consider this improbable because we obtained consistent results from three separate trials, each performed with bees of a different mix of genotypes, and thus, it is unlikely that the observed lipid differences could be attributed to repeated coincidences of the occurrence of early foraging genotypes with low-lipid genotypes. Furthermore, in all experiments we observed patterns of lipid levels that reflect behaviour and not age. There may well be genotypic differences in lipid physiology in honeybees, but this is not likely to confound our interpretation of the results. Results of experiment 3 suggest that foraging itself does not have a strong effect on lipid levels. The treatment effectively limited foraging activity, and a control experiment showed there was no additional lipid depletion from carrying a tag. In both experimental and control colonies, there was no detectable effect of days of foraging experience on lipid levels. Although experimental restriction of flight had a significant effect on lipid levels, this effect was small (on the order of tenths of a milligram of lipid) as opposed to the large difference (nearly 1 mg) found between foragers and nurses. Therefore, our results suggest slight lipid depletion may occur during foraging. However, lipid levels of foragers were already substantially lower than those of nurses on the first day of foraging. Furthermore, even bees that never made a single flight TOTH & ROBINSON: NUTRITION AND DIVISION OF LABOUR (totally restricted bees collected on day 1) had low lipid levels at an age when partially restricted and unrestricted bees foraged. Although only one trial of this experiment was performed, these results indicate that a substantial amount of lipid depletion can occur prior to foraging. This suggests that lipid stores are mainly metabolized during nest work activities such as nursing, feeding nestmates, or producing wax, perhaps in anticipation of the onset of foraging. Lipid depletion prior to foraging has also been observed in the wasp Polybia occidentalis (O’Donnell & Jeanne 1995). Our results suggest that honeybees rely on carbohydrates (not stored lipid) for energy when foraging. This is consistent with the finding that honeybee and Vespula vulgaris wasp foragers cease flight and quickly die after the crop is empty (Lorenz et al. 2001). In the migratory locust, Locusta migratoria, the initial 20–30 min of flight are fuelled by the disaccharide trehalose, with a switch to metabolism of lipid stores to sustain prolonged flight (Weis-Fogh 1952; Goldsworthy et al. 1979). In honeybees, foraging flights last about 10 min for pollen and 30–80 min for nectar (Winston 1987). If foragers use energy obtained from nectar/honey ingested in the hive (plus a portion of collected nectar to fuel return flights), it is feasible that honeybees are able to sustain most foraging flights from recently ingested carbohydrates. In contrast to foraging, there does not appear to be as close an association between levels of abdominal lipid stores and nursing behaviour in honeybees. We found that bees that reverted to nursing did not regain their lipid stores, but we do not know how active these bees were at nursing. We did notice substantial mortality of larvae cared for by reverted nurses in the reversion colonies (A.L.T., personal observation), consistent with the results of a previous study (Robinson et al. 1992). This observation suggests that reverted nurses may be less effective, due either to physiological and/or behavioural deficits. The limited lipid stores of reverted nurses may result in a lower quality and/or quantity of brood food produced by the hypopharyngeal glands, leading to reduced nursing ability. This suggestion is consistent with recent findings that the lipoprotein vitellogenin, which is produced by the fat body, is involved in brood food production by worker bees (Amdam et al. 2003). Although our experiments addressed one component of nutritional state (lipid stores), the fat body is also an important site of storage of protein reserves. Paralleling our results, foragers also show low protein content in fat body cells (Fluri & Bogdanov 1987), and synthesis of the storage protein vitellogenin is shut down (Pinto et al. 2000). The consumption of pollen, bees’ only dietary source of fat and amino acids, is extremely high in nurse bees and minimal in foragers (Crailsheim et al. 1992). It is likely that protein-based and carbohydrate-based nutritional patterns are closely linked since vitellogenin, the most abundant storage protein in female insects, is a lipoprotein whose synthesis depends on amino acid and lipid reserves (Chapman 1998). Our finding of nutritional depletion prior to foraging is consistent with the possibility that nutritional depletion can influence the onset of foraging behaviour. Nutrition could affect worker behavioural development through food input (changes in the quality and quantity of the diet), intrinsic physiological changes (e.g. metabolic and neuroendocrine shifts), and/or social input (food exchange). Adult honeybees switch from high consumption of pollen during the first weeks of life to a mostly carbohydrate-based diet later in life as foragers (Crailsheim et al. 1992). This dietary change could potentially affect the behavioural and physiological shifts that accompany worker honeybee behavioural development. For example, nutrient depletion in honeybees might lead to an increase in circulating titres of juvenile hormone (Kaatz et al. 1994; Pinto et al. 2000), and an increase in titres of this hormone is associated with the onset of foraging (reviewed by Bloch et al. 2002). Starvation can cause increased expression of a neuropeptide that induces juvenile hormone production in Manduca sexta larvae (Bhaskaran & Jones 1980; Lee & Horodyski 2002). Starvation also causes an increase in octopamine in some insects (Hirashima et al. 1992), and this neurochemical has also been shown to influence the onset of foraging in honeybees (Schulz & Robinson 2001). However, to evaluate the possibility that nutrition affects division of labour, it is necessary to go beyond the correlations reported here and determine whether experimental depletion of lipid stores can actually cause an earlier onset of foraging. The observed changes in worker nutrition may be related to the regulation of division of labour by social inhibition (Huang & Robinson 1999). Young bees are inhibited from becoming foragers in part by direct social contact with older bees (Huang & Robinson 1992, 1996; Huang et al. 1998). Because one of the primary forms of social contact in honeybees is trophallaxis (social food exchange), a connection may exist between trophallaxis, nutrition and social inhibition. Blanchard et al. (2000) suggested that the ‘unidirectional food flow’ from foragers to nurses (Korst & Velthuis 1982; Leibig et al. 1997) may contribute to the lean forager–corpulent nest worker pattern found in many social insects. If this is the case, then food exchange between foragers that tend to donate food and young bees that tend to receive food (Free 1955) could lead to an enhanced nutritional state in young bees, and perhaps inhibition of foraging behaviour. We suggest that a thorough investigation of the connection between social interactions and nutrition could have important implications for the social inhibition model. The ‘lean forager–corpulent nest worker’ pattern has been observed numerous times in the literature (see Introduction). Thus, there must be strong selective pressure causing repeated convergence on this pattern across taxa of social insects. Porter & Jorgensen (1981) suggested that foragers serve as a ‘disposable caste’ for the colony. They argue that since foraging is the most energetically costly and dangerous task a worker can perform, it is reserved until the last portion of a worker’s life. The disposable caste idea has implications for colony organization as well as worker ergonomics (Porter & Jorgensen 1981; O’Donnell & Jeanne 1995). By reserving tasks that incur high mortality risk for the most nutritionally depleted workers, the amount of energy lost by the colony is minimized. From the perspective of foraging energetics, 433 434 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 69, 2 lean foragers save energy by having less weight (e.g. fat stores) to carry on foraging trips. Blanchard et al. (2000) also pointed out that the reduction of nutritional reserves in the abdomen, especially by limiting the volume of the fat body, leaves more room for foragers to store liquid food in the crop. As a corollary to the disposable forager theory, one can also consider the adaptive value of the corpulent nest worker. Nest workers may function as living vessels for the storage of colony energy (Porter & Jorgensen 1981). This occurs to an extreme degree in some ants, in which replete workers remain in the nest and store huge amounts of liquid food in their crops (Wilson 1971). ‘Fat body repletes’ have also been found in several species of ants, in which lipid stores in the fat body are hypertrophied to an extreme degree (Burgett & Young 1974; Tschinkel 1987; Borgesen 2000). In cases such as these, but also in less extreme cases such as honeybees, fat nest workers may serve as a valuable sink of colony energy. This work provides a first step into the study of nutritional influences on patterns of worker division of labour. Further studies of the physiological and molecular pathways underlying nutritional changes in worker honeybees can improve our understanding of the mechanisms of honeybee division of labour, via interactions with other known factors such as juvenile hormone titres, octopamine levels and gene expression patterns (Ben-Shahar et al. 2002; Robinson 2002; Whitfield et al. 2003). Furthermore, there is a need for parallel studies in other social insects to understand the evolutionary significance of this pattern in a comparative context. Additional studies of worker nutrition have the potential to provide important new insights into both the mechanisms and evolution of division of labour in the social insects. Acknowledgments We thank K. Pruiett for expert assistance with the bees; M. Sakashita for dissections and lipid analyses; S. E. Fahrbach and R. A. Velarde for advice on histology and lipid assays; F. E. Miguez for assistance with statistics; and J. L. Beverly, S. A. Cameron, S. E. Fahrbach and members of the Robinson laboratory for comments that improved the manuscript. Supported by Clark Research Grant, Program in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Research Grants from SIB/PEEB to A.L.T. and grants from the National Science Foundation, the Burroughs–Wellcome Trust, and the University of Illinois Research Board to G.E.R. References Amdam, G. V., Norberg, K., Hagen, A. & Omholt, S. W. 2003. Social exploitation of vitellogenin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 100, 1799–1802. Amenta, J. S. 1964. A rapid chemical method for quantification of lipids separated using thin-layer chromatography. Journal of Lipid Research, 5, 270–272. Ben-Shahar, Y., Robichon, A., Sokolowski, M. & Robinson, G. E. 2002. Influence of gene action across different time scales on behavior. Science, 296, 636–639. Bhaskaran, G. & Jones, G. 1980. Neuroendocrine regulation of the corpus allatum activity in Manduca sexta: the endocrine basis for starvation-induced supernumerary larval moult. Journal of Insect Physiology, 26, 431–440. Blanchard, G. B., Orledge, G. M., Reynolds, S. E. & Franks, N. R. 2000. Division of labour and seasonality in the ant Leptothorax albipennis: worker corpulence and its influence on behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 59, 723–738. Bloch, G. & Robinson, G. E. 2001. Reversal of honeybee behavioral rhythms. Nature, 410, 1048. Bloch, G., Wheeler, D. E. & Robinson, G. E. 2002. Endocrine influences on the organization of insect societies. In: Hormones, Brain and Behavior (Ed. by D. W. Pfaff), pp. 195–235. San Diego: Academic Press. Borgesen, L. W. 2000. Nutritional function of replete workers in the pharaoh’s ant, Monomorium pharaonis (L.). Insectes Sociaux, 47, 141–146. Breed, M. D., Robinson, G. E. & Page, R. E., Jr. 1990. Division of labor during honey bee colony defense. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27, 395–402. Burgett, D. M. & Young, R. G. 1974. Lipid storage by honey ant repletes. Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 67, 743–744. Chapman, R. F. 1998. The Insects: Structure and Function. 4th edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crailsheim, K., Schneider, L. H. W., Hrassnigg, N., Bühlmann, G., Brosch, U., Gmeinbauer, R. & Schöffmann, B. 1992. Pollen consumption and utilization in worker honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica): dependence on individual age and function. Journal of Insect Physiology, 38, 409–419. Fluri, P. & Bogdanov, S. 1987. Age dependence of fat body protein in summer and winter bees (Apis mellifera). In: Chemistry and Biology of Social Insects (Ed. by K. Eder & H. Rembold), pp. 170– 171. Munich: Verlag J. Peperny. Free, J. B. 1955. The transmission of food between worker honeybees. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2, 41–47. Giray, T., Guzman-Novoa, E., Aron, C. W., Zelinsky, B., Fahrbach, S. E. & Robinson, G. E. 1999. Genetic variation in worker temporal polyethism and colony defensiveness in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 44–55. Goldsworthy, G. J., Jutsum, A. R. & Robinson, N. L. 1979. Substrate utilization and flight speed during tethered flight in the locust. Journal of Insect Physiology, 25, 183–185. Harrison, J. M. 1986. Caste-specific changes in honeybee flight capacity. Physiological Zoology, 59, 175–187. Hartfelder, K. & Engels, W. 1998. Social insect polymorphism: hormonal regulation of plasticity in development and reproduction in the honey bee. Current Topics in Developmental Biology, 40, 45–77. Hirashima, A., Uenoi, R. & Eto, M. 1992. Effects of various stressors of larval growth and whole-body octopamine levels of Tribolium castaneum. Pesticide Biochemistry and Physiology, 44, 217–225. Huang, Z.-Y. & Robinson, G. E. 1992. Honey bee colony integration: worker–worker interactions mediate hormonally regulated plasticity in division of labor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 89, 11726–11729. Huang, Z.-Y. & Robinson, G. E. 1996. Regulation of honey bee division of labor by colony age demography. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 39, 147–158. Huang, Z.-Y. & Robinson, G. E. 1999. Social control of division of labor in honey bee colonies. In: Information Processing in Social Insects (Ed. by C. Detrain, J. Pasteels & J. L. Deneubourg), pp. 165–186. Basel: Birkhauser Verlag. Huang, Z.-Y., Robinson, G. E. & Borst, D. 1994. Physiological correlates of division of labor independent of age in honey bee colonies. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 174, 731–739. TOTH & ROBINSON: NUTRITION AND DIVISION OF LABOUR Huang, Z.-Y., Plettner, E. & Robinson, G. E. 1998. Effects of social environment and worker mandibular glands on endocrinemediated behavioral development in honey bees. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 183, 143–152. Kaatz, H., Eichmuller, S. & Kreissl, S. 1994. Stimulatory effect of octopamine on juvenile hormone biosynthesis in honey bees (Apis mellifera): physiological and immunocytochemical evidence. Journal of Insect Physiology, 40, 865–872. Korst, P. J. A. M. & Velthuis, H. H. W. 1982. The nature of trophallaxis in honey bees. Insectes Sociaux, 29, 209–221. Lee, K.-Y. & Horodyski, F. M. 2002. Restriction of nutrient intake results in the increase of a specific Manduca sexta allaltotropin (Manse-AT) mRNA in the larval nerve cord. Peptides, 23, 653–661. Leibig, J., Heinze, J. & Hölldobler, B. 1997. Trophallaxis and aggression in the ponerine ant, Ponera arctica: implications for the evolution of liquid food exchange in the Hymenoptera. Ethology, 103, 707–722. Lorenz, M. W., Kellner, R., Volkl, W., Hoffmann, K. & Woodring, J. 2001. A comparative study on hypertrehalosemic hormones in the Hymenoptera: sequence determination, physiological actions and biological significance. Journal of Insect Physiology, 47, 563–571. Markiewicz, D. A. & O’Donnell, S. 2001. Social dominance, task performance, and nutrition: implications for reproduction in eusocial wasps. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 187, 327– 333. Martin, M. M. & Lieb, T. J. 1979. Patterns of fuel utilization by the thoracic muscles of adult worker ants: the use of lipid by a hymenopteran. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology B, 64, 387–390. O’Donnell, S. & Jeanne, R. L. 1995. Worker lipid stores decrease with outside-nest performance in wasps: implications for the evolution of age polyethism. Experientia, 51, 749–752. Perkins, E. G. 1975. Analysis of Lipids and Lipoproteins. Champaign, Illinois: American Oil Chemist’s Society. Pinto, L. Z., Bitondi, M. M. G. & Simões, Z. L. P. 2000. Inhibition of vitellogenin synthesis in Apis mellifera workers by a juvenile hormone analogue, pyriproxyfen. Journal of Insect Physiology, 46, 153–160. Porter, S. D. & Jorgensen, C. D. 1981. Foragers of the harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex owyheei: a disposable caste? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 247–256. Ricks, B. L. & Vinson, S. B. 1972. Changes in nutrient content during one year in workers of the imported fire ant. Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 65, 135–138. Robinson, G. E. 1992. Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annual Review of Entomology, 37, 637–665. Robinson, G. E. 2002. Genomics and integrative analyses of division of labor in honeybee colonies. American Naturalist, Supplement, 160, 160–172. Robinson, G. E., Page, R. E., Strambi, C. & Strambi, A. 1989. Hormonal and genetic control of behavioral integration in honey bee colonies. Science, 246, 109–112. Robinson, G. E., Page, R. E., Strambi, C. & Strambi, A. 1992. Colony integration in honey bees: mechanisms of behavioral reversion. Ethology, 90, 336–338. SAS Institute. 2000. SAS/STAT User’s Guide. Cary, North Carolina: SAS Institute. Schulz, D. J. & Robinson, G. E. 2001. Octopamine influences division of labor in honey bee colonies. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 187, 53–61. Schulz, D. J., Huang, Z.-Y. & Robinson, G. E. 1998. Effects of colony food shortage on behavioral development in honey bees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42, 295–303. Stroup, W. W. 2002. Power analysis of spatial mixed models. Journal of Agricultural, Biological and Environmental Statistics, 7, 491–511. Tschinkel, W. R. 1987. Seasonal life history and nest architecture of a winter-active ant, Prenolepis imparis. Insectes Sociaux, 34, 143– 164. Tschinkel, W. R. 1998. Sociometry and sociogenesis of colonies of the harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex badius: worker characteristics in relation to colony size and season. Insectes Sociaux, 45, 385–410. Weis-Fogh, T. 1952. Fat combustion and metabolic rate of flying locusts (Schistocerca gregaria Forskal). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 237, 1–36. Wheeler, D. E. 1986. Developmental and physiological determinants of caste in social Hymenoptera: evolutionary implications. American Naturalist, 128, 13–43. Wheeler, D. E. & Nijhout, H. F. 1983. Soldier determination in the ant Pheidole bicarinata: inhibition by adult soldiers. Journal of Insect Physiology, 30, 127–135. Whitfield, C. W., Cziko, A.-M. & Robinson, G. E. 2003. Gene expression profiles in the brain predict behavior in individual honey bees. Science, 302, 296–299. Wilson, E. O. 1971. The Insect Societies. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Winston, M. L. 1987. The Biology of the Honey Bee. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Withers, G. S., Fahrbach, S. E. & Robinson, G. E. 1995. Effects of experience and juvenile hormone on the organization of the mushroom bodies of honey bees. Journal of Neurobiology, 26, 130–144. 435