Psychoneuroendocrinology (2012) 37, 1531—1545 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / p s y n e u e n Psychosocial animal model of PTSD produces a long-lasting traumatic memory, an increase in general anxiety and PTSD-like glucocorticoid abnormalities Phillip R. Zoladz a, Monika Fleshner b, David M. Diamond c,d,* a Department of Psychology & Sociology, Ohio Northern University, Ada, OH, USA Department of Integrative Physiology & Center for Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, USA c Research and Development Service, VA Hospital, Tampa, FL, USA d Departments of Psychology and Molecular Pharmacology & Physiology, Center for Preclinical & Clinical Research on PTSD, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA b Received 10 August 2011; received in revised form 3 February 2012; accepted 14 February 2012 KEYWORDS Stress; Trauma; Glucocorticoids; HPA axis; Memory; Dexamethasone; Animal model; Fear conditioning Summary Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is characterized by a pathologically intense memory for a traumatic experience, persistent anxiety and physiological abnormalities, such as low baseline glucocorticoid levels and increased sensitivity to dexamethasone. We have addressed the hypothesis that rats subjected to chronic psychosocial stress would exhibit PTSD-like sequelae, including traumatic memory expression, increased anxiety and abnormal glucocorticoid responses. Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats were exposed to a cat on two occasions separated by 10 days, in conjunction with chronic social instability. Three weeks after the second cat exposure, the rats were tested for glucocorticoid abnormalities, general anxiety and their fear-conditioned memory of the two cat exposures. Stressed rats exhibited reduced basal glucocorticoid levels, increased glucocorticoid suppression following dexamethasone administration, heightened anxiety and a robust fear memory in response to cues that were paired with the two cat exposures. The commonalities in endocrine and behavioral measures between psychosocially stressed rats and traumatized people with PTSD provide the opportunity to explore mechanisms underlying psychological trauma-induced changes in neuroendocrine systems and cognition. # 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction * Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, 4202 E. Fowler Ave. (PCD 4118G), University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33620, USA. Tel.: +1 813 974 0480; fax: +1 813 974 4617. E-mail address: ddiamond@usf.edu (D.M. Diamond). Individuals exposed to intense, life-threatening trauma are at significant risk for developing post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). People who develop PTSD respond to a traumatic experience with intense fear, helplessness and horror 0306-4530/$ — see front matter # 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.02.007 1532 (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and subsequently endure chronic psychological distress by repeatedly reliving their trauma through intrusive, flashback memories (Ehlers et al., 2004; Hackmann et al., 2004; McFarlane, 1992; Reynolds and Brewin, 1998, 1999; Speckens et al., 2006, 2007). These intrusive memories are triggered by cues which were associated with the trauma and can, in extreme cases, lead to panic attacks. Therefore, PTSD patients make great efforts to avoid stimuli that remind them of their trauma. The re-experiencing and avoidance symptoms of the disorder can hinder everyday functioning in PTSD patients and foster the development of additional debilitating symptoms, including persistent anxiety, exaggerated startle, cognitive impairments and diminished extinction of conditioned fear (Brewin et al., 2000; Elzinga and Bremner, 2002; Geuze et al., 2009; Graham and Milad, 2011; Milad et al., 2009; Nemeroff et al., 2006; Newport and Nemeroff, 2000; Stam, 2007). PTSD is also characterized by an aberrant biological profile in different endocrine and physiological systems (Krystal and Neumeister, 2009; Pervanidou and Chrousos, 2010; Vidovic et al., 2011). One of the most extensively researched endocrine systems in people with PTSD is the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Empirical investigations of the adrenal hormone, cortisol, have often reported abnormally low baseline cortisol levels in people with PTSD (for reviews, see Yehuda, 2005, 2009). One explanation for the presence of low baseline cortisol levels in people with PTSD is that trauma induces an enhancement of negative feedback inhibition of the HPA axis. For example, studies have reported that people with PTSD display an increased number and sensitivity of glucocorticoid receptors (Rohleder et al., 2004; Stein et al., 1997; Yehuda et al., 1991, 1993a, 1995) and an increased suppression of cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) following the administration of dexamethasone, a synthetic glucocorticoid (Duval et al., 2004; Goenjian et al., 1996; Grossman et al., 2003; McFarlane et al., 2011; Newport et al., 2004; Stein et al., 1997; Yehuda et al., 1993b, 1995, 2002, 2004). Some studies have also observed greater increases in ACTH levels of PTSD patients, relative to controls, following the administration of metyrapone. This finding may be the result of metyrapone reducing the enhanced negative feedback inhibition present in PTSD patients (Otte et al., 2006; Yehuda et al., 1996). Studies have also employed the dexamethasone-corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) challenge paradigm to study abnormal HPA axis functioning in people with PTSD (de Kloet et al., 2006). An advantage of this paradigm is that the subjects are treated with dexamethasone prior to CRH administration, thereby activating negative feedback mechanisms before acute HPA axis stimulation. Studies have generally reported reduced ACTH levels in dexamethasonetreated PTSD patients who were subsequently treated with CRH. These findings support the notion that PTSD patients exhibit reduced sensitivity to CRH stimulation (Rinne et al., 2002; Strohle et al., 2008). Overall, extensive research indicates that PTSD is characterized by reduced basal levels of cortisol, reduced CRH receptor sensitivity and/or enhanced glucocorticoid negative feedback at the level of the pituitary. However, the literature in this area is not entirely consistent, which likely reflects the heterogeneity in the manifestation of trauma and the measurement of PTSD in different patient populations (Begic and P.R. Zoladz et al. Jokic-Begic, 2007; Bonne et al., 2003; Hamner et al., 2004; Klaassens et al., 2012; Marshall and Garakani, 2002; Metzger et al., 2008; Pitman and Orr, 1990; Radant et al., 2001; Shalev et al., 2008). Our understanding of how trauma affects the HPA axis in people may be enhanced by animal models that generate PTSD-like behavioral and physiological abnormalities. To this end, we have developed an animal model of PTSD that includes trauma induction procedures which are analogous to those that induce PTSD in people, including a threat to survival, a lack of control, an intrusive re-experiencing of a traumatic event and social instability (Roth et al., 2011; Zoladz et al., 2008; Zoladz and Diamond, 2010). Specifically, our animal model of PTSD is based on a combination of acute traumatic experiences (two 1-h periods of inescapable confinement of rats in close proximity to a cat) embedded within a 1 month-long period of social stress. We reported that rats administered this psychosocial stress regimen exhibited changes in physiology and behavior in common with people diagnosed with PTSD, including heightened anxiety, exaggerated startle, impaired cognition, increased cardiovascular reactivity and an exaggerated response to yohimbine administration (Brewin et al., 2000; Elzinga and Bremner, 2002; Nemeroff et al., 2006; Newport and Nemeroff, 2000; Stam, 2007). Moreover, we recently demonstrated that our psychosocial predator stress model of PTSD produced hippocampusspecific increases in DNA methylation (Roth et al., 2011), a finding which may be relevant toward understanding how traumatic memories can persist for a lifetime (Yehuda and Bierer, 2009). The purpose of the present experiments was to extend our animal model of PTSD to determine if rats administered psychosocial stress would exhibit two hallmark features of PTSD: (1) a long-lasting memory of the traumatic event (inescapable live cat exposure); and (2) abnormalities in glucocorticoid levels under baseline conditions and in response to stress and dexamethasone administration. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects Experimentally-naı̈ve adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (225— 250 g upon delivery) obtained from Charles River laboratories (Wilmington, MA) were used in all experiments. The rats were pair-housed on a 12-h light/dark schedule (lights on at 0700 h) in standard Plexiglas cages with free access to food and water. The colony room temperature and humidity were maintained at 20 1 8C and 60 3%, respectively. After the rats were given a 1-week vivarium acclimation period, their weights increased to 304 g (2.3 g), which was when all experimental manipulations began. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of South Florida. 2.2. Psychosocial stress paradigm Following the 1-week acclimation phase, rats were brought to the laboratory and randomly assigned to ‘‘psychosocial stress’’ or ‘‘no psychosocial stress’’ groups. Rats in the psychosocial stress groups were given two 1-h cat exposures, Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats separated by 10 days, in conjunction with daily social stress in the form of randomized housing, as described previously (Zoladz et al., 2008). During each of the two cat exposures, rats in the psychosocial stress groups were immobilized in plastic DecapiCones (Braintree Scientific; Braintree, MA) and placed in a perforated wedge-shaped Plexiglas enclosure (Braintree Scientific; Braintree, MA; 20 cm 20 cm 8 cm). cm). The rats, still immobilized in the plastic DecapiCones within the Plexiglas enclosure, were taken to the cat housing room where they were placed in a metal cage (61 cm 53 cm 51 cm) with an adult female cat for 1 h. The Plexiglas enclosure prevented any physical contact between the cat and rats, but enabled the rats to be exposed to all non-tactile sensory stimuli associated with the cat. Canned cat food was smeared on top of the Plexiglas enclosure to increase cat motor activity because a moving cat provokes a greater fear response in rats than a non-moving cat (Blanchard et al., 1975). The two cat exposures were separated by 10 days, with the first exposure taking place during the light cycle (between 0800 h and 1300 h), and the second exposure taking place during the dark cycle (between 1900 h and 2200 h). Beginning on the day of the first cat exposure, rats in the psychosocial stress group were exposed to unstable housing conditions for the next 31 days. Rats in the psychosocial stress group were housed two rats per cage, with their cohort pair combination changed on a daily basis during the entire 31-day stress period. That is, rats in the stress groups were never housed with the same rat on consecutive days. Rats in the no psychosocial stress group remained in their home cages in the laboratory instead of receiving the 1-h acute stress sessions, and these rats were housed with the same cage cohort for the duration of the experiment. 2.3. Experiment 1: Traumatic memory expression and general anxiety Fig. 1 provides a summary of the timeline and procedures for the two experiments in this study. In Experiment 1, 20 rats were assigned to ‘‘psychosocial stress’’ (n = 10) or ‘‘no psychosocial stress’’ (n = 10) groups and exposed to the stress manipulations described above. Since a major component of PTSD is the persistent memory of a traumatic experience, we have developed a method with which to measure a rat’s memory for the cat exposure experiences. To accomplish this goal, rats in the psychosocial stress group were given a predator-based form of fear conditioning. The rats were placed in a chamber for 3 min immediately prior to each of the two cat exposures. The chamber (26 cm 30 cm 29 cm; Coulbourn Instruments; Allentown, PA) consisted of two aluminum sides, an aluminum ceiling and clear Plexiglas on the front and back walls and speaker on one wall. The floor of the chamber consisted of 18 stainless steel rods, spaced 1 cm apart. A 74 dB, 2500 Hz tone was presented during the last 30 s of each chamber exposure. Rats in the no psychosocial stress group were also given the 3-min chamber and tone exposures, but without subsequent immobilization and cat exposure. It is important to emphasize that the rats in both groups were not given any noxious stimulation, such as foot shock, while they were in the chamber. It is also important to emphasize that the tone was delivered to the rats while they 1533 were in the chamber, and then they were removed from the chamber and then immobilized and brought to a different room, where the traumatic experience (immobilization during cat exposure) occurred. Three weeks following the second cat exposure, the rats were tested for their conditioned fear memory by assessing their freezing response (degree of immobility) when they were returned to the chamber and exposed to the tone. Rats were placed in the chamber, which had been previously paired with the 2 cat exposures, for 5 min (context test). One hour later, the rats were placed in a novel box for 6 min, with a 74 dB, 2500 Hz tone presented during the last 3 min of the exposure (cue test). The novel box exposure consisted of a chamber (25 cm 23 cm 33 cm; Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA) with two aluminum sides, an aluminum ceiling and Plexiglas front and back. Unlike the conditioning chamber, which contained metal rods on the floor, the cue-testing chamber had a solid metal floor. Also, a house light was on while the rats were in the cue-testing chamber. During the context and cue tests, a 24-cell infrared activity monitor (Coulbourn Instruments; Allentown, PA) mounted on top of the chambers detected the infrared body heat image from the animals to detect their movement. Immobility in the chambers was operationally defined as continuous periods of inactivity lasting at least 7 s. Twenty-four hours after fear conditioning memory testing, the rats were placed on an elevated plus maze (EPM) for 5 min. The EPM is a routine test of anxiety in rodents (Korte and De Boer, 2003) and consists of two open arms (11 cm 50 cm) and two closed arms (11 cm 50 cm) that intersect each other to form the shape of a plus sign. At the beginning of each trial, the rats were placed in the intersection area of the EPM, facing one of the open arms. Rat behavior on the EPM was monitored by computer from 48 infrared photobeams located along the perimeter of the open and closed arms (Motor Monitor, Hamilton Kinder; San Diego, CA). The primary dependent measures of interest were the amount of time rats spent in the open arms and their overall ambulations. An arm entry was scored by the computer program only when a rat’s entire body moved from one arm into a new arm. An ambulation was scored by the computer program each time a rat crossed a photobeam sensor. 2.4. Experiment 2: Basal glucocorticoid levels and dexamethasone suppression test The rats used in Experiment 2 were not administered any post-psychosocial stress behavioral testing. Instead, these rats were used solely for the purpose of obtaining blood samples at the conclusion of the psychosocial stress regimen to evaluate corticosterone levels under undisturbed baseline, acute stress and dexamethasone injection conditions (see timeline; Fig. 1). Twenty days after the second cat exposure, the hind legs of all rats were shaved to allow access to their saphenous veins. The rats were then taken back to the housing room and left undisturbed for the remainder of the day. The hind legs of all rats were shaved 1 day prior to blood sampling to minimize the amount of time it took the experimenter to obtain baseline blood samples on the following day. 1534 P.R. Zoladz et al. Fig. 1 Experimental timeline and procedures. In Experiment 1, rats were placed in a chamber where a tone was delivered, followed by immobilization of the rat during cat exposure, which occurred in another room. Ten days later, the predator-based fear conditioning procedure was repeated. Beginning with the first day of cat exposure and continuing for a total of 31 days, the psychosocial stressed rats were given social instability, composed of pseudorandom daily changes in cage cohorts. The ‘‘no psychosocial stress’’ group was also administered chamber and tone exposure, which was then followed by placement in their home cages. These rats had the same cage cohorts throughout the testing period. On Day 32, all rats were re-exposed to the conditioning chamber and then administered the tone in a second chamber. On the following day, all rats were tested in the elevated plus maze (EPM). In Experiment 2, the ‘‘psychosocial stress’’ and ‘‘no psychosocial stress’’ groups received the same stress/no stress treatments as in Experiment 1 on Days 1— 32, except there was no chamber exposure included. On Day 32, the rats were administered no injection, vehicle or dexamethasone, followed 6 h later with by basal blood sampling, 20 min of subsequent restraint stress and 2 post-stress blood samples. Additional experimental details are provided in the Section 2. The next day, between 1100 h and 1400 h, a subset of the rats was administered subcutaneous injections of dexamethasone (10 mg/kg, 25 mg/kg, 50 mg/kg; 1 ml/kg) or vehicle. These doses of dexamethasone were chosen because previous work indicated that they produce a modest suppression of corticosterone levels in rats (Lurie et al., 1989). Ten rats from each of the psychosocial stress and no psychosocial stress groups were assigned to receive an injection of one of the three doses of dexamethasone or the vehicle solution. Dexamethasone (Sigma—Aldrich; St. Louis, MO) was dissolved in a vehicle solution consisting of sodium sulfite (1 mg/ml) and sodium citrate (19.4 mg/ml), which were both dissolved in distilled water. Immediately following the administration of dexamethasone or vehicle, the rats were returned to the housing room until the commencement of blood sampling. An additional ten rats from each of the psychosocial stress and no psychosocial stress groups were not injected to allow for the analysis of undisturbed baseline corticosterone levels. Six hours following the injections (or at an equivalent time point for uninjected rats, between 1700 h and 2000 h), the rats were taken individually to a procedure room for blood sampling (Lurie et al., 1989). Petroleum jelly was applied to each rat’s hind leg to reduce clotting while the blood sample was being collected. The saphenous vein of each rat was punctured with a sterile, 27-gauge syringe needle. A 0.2 cc sample of blood was then collected from each rat in a Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats microcentrifuge tube. The first blood sample was considered a baseline measure of corticosterone and was collected within 2 min after the rats were removed from the housing room. After obtaining this sample, the rats were immobilized in plastic DecapiCones for 20 min. The rats were then removed from the DecapiCones, and another 0.2 cc sample of blood was collected in a microcentrifuge tube via saphenous vein venipuncture. This blood sample served to examine the hormonal responses of rats to acute immobilization stress. We employed this manipulation because in previous work utilizing the single prolonged stress paradigm (Kohda et al., 2007), it was necessary to stress the rodents after dexamethasone administration to observe an exaggerated suppression of corticosterone in single prolonged stressexposed animals. In addition, examining stress-induced changes in corticosterone levels provides an assessment of HPA axis functioning comparable to the one used in dexamethasone-CRH challenge paradigms in PTSD patients. After the second blood sample was collected, the rats were returned to their home cages. One hour later, a final blood sample was collected from all rats to examine corticosterone levels following the termination of the acute immobilization stress. Once the blood samples had clotted at room temperature, they were centrifuged (3000 rpm for 8 min), and the serum was extracted and stored 80 8C until assayed for corticosterone with an Enzyme ImmunoAssay kit from Assay Design, Inc. (cat#901-097, Ann Arbor, MI). All samples were diluted 1:50 and assayed per kit instructions. 2.5. Body and organ weights 1535 test and the amount of time spent in the open arms of the EPM between the psychosocial stress and no psychosocial stress groups. A two-way mixed-model ANOVA was used to analyze the degree of immobility by rats during the cue test, with psychosocial stress (psychosocial stress, no psychosocial stress) serving as the between-subjects factor and tone (no tone, tone) serving as the within-subjects factor. In Experiment 2, two-way between-subjects ANOVAs were used to analyze the growth rates, adrenal gland weights and thymus weights, with psychosocial stress (psychosocial stress, no psychosocial stress) and injection condition (no injection, vehicle, 10 mg/kg, 25 mg/kg, 50 mg/kg) serving as the between-subjects factors. A three-way mixed-model ANOVA was used to analyze corticosterone levels, with the psychosocial stress and injection condition serving as the between-subjects factors and time point (0 min, 20 min, 80 min) serving as the within-subjects factor. 3. Results 3.1. Experiment 1: Fear conditioning and anxiety testing 3.1.1. Growth rates and organ weights The psychosocial stress group exhibited a significantly lower growth rate, t(17) = 3.43, significantly larger adrenal glands, t(14) = 2.24, and a significantly smaller thymus, t(16) = 2.78, than the no psychosocial stress group ( p’s < 0.05; Table 1). 2.6. Statistical analyses 3.1.2. Traumatic memory expression The psychosocial stress group exhibited significantly greater immobility than the no psychosocial stress group during the context test, t(14) = 4.55, p < 0.001. The analysis of the cue test revealed significant main effects of psychosocial stress, F(1,16) = 6.26, and tone, F(1,16) = 12.59, as well as a significant Psychosocial Stress Tone interaction, F(1,16) = 7.62 ( p’s < 0.05). The psychosocial stress group demonstrated significantly greater immobility than the no psychosocial stress group in the novel environment only when the tone was presented (Fig. 2). Alpha was set at 0.05 for all analyses, and Holm-Sidak post hoc comparisons were employed when an omnibus F test indicated a significant ANOVA. Outlier data points greater than 3 standard deviations from the exclusive group means were eliminated from the analyses (less than 1% of the data were outliers). In Experiment 1, independent samples t-tests were used to compare growth rates, adrenal glands weights, thymus weights, degree of immobility during the 5-min context 3.1.3. General anxiety The analysis of EPM behavior revealed that the psychosocial stress group spent significantly less time in the open arms than the no psychosocial stress group, t(15) = 2.44, p < 0.05, thus corroborating our previously-reported anxiogenic effects of the psychosocial stress paradigm (Zoladz et al., 2008). There was no significant difference between the two groups in their overall movement (i.e., number of ambulations) on the EPM, t(16) = 0.16, p > 0.05 (Fig. 3). In each experiment, body weights were recorded on the day of the first cat exposure and on the first day of behavioral/ physiological testing. Average growth rates (g/day) were calculated for statistical analysis. At the end of each experiment, the rats were euthanized, and the adrenals (left and right adrenals were pooled) and thymus were removed and weighed. Organ weights were expressed as mg/100 g body weight. Table 1 Average growth rates and organ weights (SEM) for Experiment 1. Group Growth rate (g/day) Adrenal gland weight (mg/100 g b.w.) Thymus gland weight (mg/100 g b.w.) No psychosocial stress Psychosocial stress 5.68 (0.53) 3.88* (0.13) 10.26 (0.25) 11.51* (0.50) 108.25 (8.03) 93.01* (5.28) * p < 0.05 relative to no psychosocial stress. 1536 P.R. Zoladz et al. Context Memory Cue Memory 80 60 * 60 50 40 30 20 Pre-Tone Tone 50 % Immobility % Immobility 70 * 40 30 20 10 10 0 No Psychosocial Stress 0 Psychosocial Stress No Psychosocial Stress Psychosocial Stress Fig. 2 Chronic psychosocial stress resulted in the expression of a traumatic memory. The psychosocial stress group exhibited significantly greater immobility than the no psychosocial stress group in response to the chamber (context memory; left) and tone (cue memory; right) that were previously paired with the two cat exposures. Context and cue fear memory were assessed three weeks following the second cat/home cage exposure. Data are presented as mean SEM. *p < 0.05 relative to the no psychosocial stress group. Open Arm Exploration 200 40 30 * 20 Locomotor Activity 150 100 50 10 0 Ambulations % Time in Open Arms 50 No Psychosocial Stress Psychosocial Stress 0 No Psychosocial Stress Psychosocial Stress Fig. 3 Chronic psychosocial stress resulted in heightened anxiety. The psychosocial stress group spent significantly less time in the open arms of the elevated plus maze than the no psychosocial stress group (left), despite there being no difference between the two groups in overall movement (right). Rat behavior on the elevated plus maze was examined 24 h after the context and cue fear memory assessments. Data are presented as mean SEM. *p < 0.05 relative to the no psychosocial stress group. 3.2. Experiment 2: Influence of psychosocial stress on basal glucocorticoid levels and dexamethasone suppression test 3.2.1. Growth rates and organ weights Each of the growth rate and organ weight analyses revealed significant main effects of psychosocial stress, indicating that the psychosocial stress groups exhibited significantly lower growth rates, F(1,90) = 13.65, significantly larger adrenal glands, F(1,88) = 16.53, and a significantly smaller thymus, F(1,86) = 36.77, than the no psychosocial stress groups ( p’s < 0.001; Table 2). 3.2.2. Serum corticosterone levels The analysis of serum corticosterone levels revealed a significant main effect of time point, F(2,158) = 122.58, p < 0.001. Rats demonstrated a significant increase in corticosterone levels following 20 min of immobilization, and these levels significantly declined, yet still remained elevated relative to baseline, 1 h later ( p’s < 0.001). There was a significant main effect of injection condition, F(4,79) = 54.99, p < 0.001, indicating that, relative to vehicle- and non-injected groups, dexamethasone led to significantly lower corticosterone levels. The Time Point Injection Condition, F(8,158) = 6.35, and Time Point Psychosocial Stress Injection Condition, F(8,158) = 3.10, interactions were also significant ( p’s < 0.01). The non-injected psychosocial stress group displayed significantly lower corticosterone levels than the non-injected no psychosocial stress group at the 0 min time point ( p < 0.01). Following administration of 10 mg/kg dexamethasone, the psychosocial stress group exhibited a greater suppression of post-immobilization corticosterone levels than the no psychosocial stress group. Additionally, 25 mg/kg dexamethasone prevented the immobilization-induced increase in Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats Table 2 1537 Average growth rates and organ weights (SEM) for Experiment 2. Group No psychosocial stress No injection Vehicle DEX — 10 mg/kg DEX — 25 mg/kg DEX — 50 mg/kg Psychosocial stress No injection Vehicle DEX — 10 mg/kg DEX — 25 mg/kg DEX — 50 mg/kg Growth Rate (g/day) Adrenal Gland Weight (mg/100 g b.w.) Thymus Gland Weight (mg/100 g b.w.) 4.75 5.88 5.23 5.69 4.76 (0.26) (0.24) (0.31) (0.39) (0.38) 7.45 9.57 9.20 9.98 9.35 (0.64) (0.86) (0.56) (0.45) (0.82) 115.18 122.22 122.29 105.73 105.66 (7.31) (6.94) (4.10) (6.35) (5.67) 3.73 4.75 4.95 4.52 4.38 (0.43) (0.13) (0.25) (0.35) (0.49) 10.66 11.28 10.57 12.27 11.15 (0.44) (1.06) (0.96) (0.42) (0.52) 96.47 87.52 95.13 97.87 80.31 (4.98) (4.18) (4.36) (5.28) (3.91) corticosterone levels in the psychosocial stress group only. Administration of 50 mg/kg dexamethasone suppressed baseline and post-immobilization corticosterone levels equivalently in the psychosocial stress and no psychosocial stress groups ( p > 0.05; Fig. 4). 4. Discussion The overall goal of our research program has been to develop an animal model of PTSD based on clinicallyrelevant features of the disorder. To this end, we previously demonstrated that two episodes of inescapable cat exposure, in conjunction with daily social instability, caused rats to exhibit heightened anxiety, exaggerated startle, impaired cognition, increased cardiovascular reactivity and an exaggerated response to yohimbine administration (Zoladz et al., 2008; Zoladz and Diamond, 2010), all of which are commonly observed symptoms in people with PTSD (Brewin et al., 2000; Elzinga and Bremner, 2002; Nemeroff et al., 2006; Newport and Nemeroff, 2000; Stam, 2007). In the present work, we have extended our animal model of PTSD by demonstrating that rats exposed to two acute predator stress experiences, embedded within a 31day period of social instability, resulted in a long-lasting, classically-conditioned fear memory for the predator exposure experiences. We have also shown that rats exposed to the psychosocial stress regimen exhibited increased general anxiety and a significant reduction of glucocorticoid levels at baseline and following dexamethasone administration. 4.1. Predator-based fear conditioning In Experiment 1, we exposed rats to a unique chamber (context) and tone (cue), which served as the two conditioned stimuli (CSs). The CSs were presented immediately prior to immobilization in conjunction with cat exposure, which served as the unconditioned stimulus (US) (Pavlov, 1928). The use of a live moving cat, alone, is well-established as a powerful and unlearned fear-provoking stimulus to rats (Blanchard et al., 1975, 1990; Blanchard et al., 1991; Pentkowski et al., 2006). Psychosocially stressed rats expressed a fear conditioned memory, as indicated by significant immobility, upon their re-exposure, three weeks later, to the context and cue that were temporally associated with the two cat exposures (Fig. 2). Importantly, during the cue test, the psychosocially stressed rats did not exhibit significant immobility in the novel environment until the tone was presented. This finding indicated that their fear response during the context test was not an indication of a nonspecific increase in anxiety, but was, instead, a selective anxiogenic response to the contextual stimuli that were associated with cat exposure. The expression of learned fear that the psychosocially stressed rats exhibited to the conditioned stimuli in this study can be considered analogous to the fear and panic that people with PTSD display when they are exposed to a cue that reminds them of their trauma (Garakani et al., 2006; Izquierdo et al., 2004; Redgrave, 2003). These findings confirm that our animal model of PTSD involves a fear conditioning (traumatic memory) component, which is central to the expression of PTSD in people (Bryant et al., 2007; Debiec and LeDoux, 2006; Elzinga and Bremner, 2002; Johnson et al., 2012; Jovanovic and Ressler, 2010; Kolb, 1984; Liberzon and Sripada, 2008). The present paradigm, therefore, may reveal mechanisms involved in the formation and persistence of intrusive memories in traumatized people. For example, in recent work, we reported that the same psychosocial stress regimen used here produced selective epigenetic alterations (methylation) of the hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene (Roth et al., 2011). We have also demonstrated that cat exposure impaired memory-related functions of the hippocampus (Diamond et al., 1999, 2007; Mesches et al., 1999; Vanelzakker et al., 2011; Vouimba et al., 2006), and, conversely, enhanced synaptic plasticity (Vouimba et al., 2006) and phosphorylation of calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII), a critical molecular component of memory formation and synaptic plasticity (Cammarota et al., 2002; Lisman et al., 2002), in the amygdala (Zoladz et al., 2012). These and related findings on an animal model of intrusive memories (Zoladz et al., 2010) are consistent with the view that traumatic memory reactivation involves impaired functioning of the hippocampus, in conjunction with enhanced functioning of the amygdala (Brewin, 2001; Debiec and LeDoux, 2006; Metcalfe and Jacobs, 1998; Milad et al., 2009; Nadel and Jacobs, 1998). 1538 P.R. Zoladz et al. Corticosterone (µg/dL) Undisturbed Baseline (No Injection) No Psychosocial Stress Psychosocial Stress 14 12 10 8 6 4 * 2 0 0 min 20 min 80 min Corticosterone (µg/dL) Dexamethasone/Vehicle Injection Vehicle 14 12 10 10 8 8 6 6 4 4 2 2 0 0 Corticosterone (µg/dL) 0 min 20 min 80 min 25 µg/kg 14 12 0 min 80 min 50 µg/kg 10 8 6 6 4 4 2 2 0 0 20 min 20 min 14 8 0 min * 12 * 10 10 µg/kg 14 12 80 min 0 min 20 min 80 min Fig. 4 Chronic psychosocial stress resulted in significantly lower corticosterone levels at baseline and following dexamethasone administration. The non-injected psychosocial stress group exhibited significantly lower corticosterone levels than the no psychosocial stress group at the 0 min time point (top). Following the administration of 10 mg/kg dexamethasone, the psychosocial stress group displayed a more rapid recovery of post-immobilization corticosterone levels than the no psychosocial stress group (middle right; 80 min time point). Following the administration of 25 mg/kg dexamethasone, there was a shift in the response pattern indicating that dexamethasone prevented the immobilization-induced increase in corticosterone levels in the psychosocial stress group only (bottom left; 20 min time point). Administration of 50 mg/kg dexamethasone resulted in an equivalent suppression of corticosterone levels in the two groups (bottom right). Blood sampling occurred three weeks following the second cat/home cage exposure between 1700 h and 2000 h. Data are presented as mean SEM. The dark bar from 0 min to 20 min indicates the time when rats were immobilized. *p < 0.05 relative to the psychosocial stress group. In related work, Cohen and colleagues (Cohen et al., 2006, 2008; Matar et al., 2009; Zohar et al., 2008) incorporated a fear conditioning-like component into their animal model of PTSD. These investigators exposed rats to well-soiled cat litter (predator scent stress) and subsequently measured their freezing behavior in response to fresh, unused cat litter. They found that predator scent stress-exposed rats exhibited significant freezing behavior upon exposure to unused cat litter, which suggested that the rats had a fear-provoking memory of the predator scent stress experience. Their work Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats is important because it potentially provides insight into how a fear-provoking experience produces a long-term change in rat behavior. However, their approach did not provide evidence that the stressed rats had formed an explicit association between a previously neutral cue (CS) and a biologically relevant, arousing stimulus (US), which is an essential feature of classical conditioning (Pavlov, 1928). The increased freezing their stressed rats exhibited in response to clean litter may have developed as a result of increased generalized fear to all novel stimuli. Thus, the possibility that the predator scent stress experience produced a non-associative sensitization of stress responses was not addressed in their work. Other investigators have also studied different forms of fear memory testing in rodents based on their forming an association between neutral stimuli and exposure to a predator (Adamec, 1997; Adamec et al., 2004, 2011) or predator odor (Blanchard et al., 2003a,b, 2001; Hubbard et al., 2004; Munoz-Abellan et al., 2009; Rosen et al., 2008; Takahashi et al., 2008). Unlike the other approaches, we embedded predator-based fear conditioning within a prolonged (31-day) period of chronic social stress. As such, our work mimics a clinically relevant situation in which a person is exposed to acute trauma as a component of prolonged periods of daily anxiety. The combination of acute traumatic experiences delivered in conjunction with chronic daily stress may explain why our model, unlike some predator odor-based work (Munoz-Abellan et al., 2009; Zangrossi and File, 1992), generates a persistent fear-based memory, as well as a longlasting increase in behavioral anxiety. It is important to note that unlike conventional classical conditioning training, in the current work, the CS (context/ cue) and US (immobilization during cat exposure) occurred in completely different locations. That is, in traditional classical conditioning, the CS and US are always presented together in the same context. For example, in typical fear conditioning training, rats are administered foot shock and a tone in the same context (Fanselow and Gale, 2003; Rudy et al., 2004). A rat’s memory for the shock is then tested by observing the rat’s behavior when it is returned to the same environment where the shock occurred. We have shown here that rats can associate two neutral stimuli (CSs; chamber and tone) with an aversive experience (US; immobilization during predator exposure) that occurred in two different places. That is, the rats were removed from the chamber and brought to another room, where cat exposure occurred, and yet, the rats exhibited fear when they were returned to the chamber or exposed to the tone. Thus, our demonstration that rats can associate cues that occurred across time and space is unique, and potentially relevant toward understanding how traumatic stress can produce powerful, long-lasting and context-independent intrusive memories and phobias in people (Bryant and Harvey, 1996; Cuthbert et al., 2003; Jacobs and Nadel, 1985; Wild et al., 2007). 4.2. PTSD-like HPA axis alterations In Experiment 2, psychosocially stressed rats exhibited abnormal, PTSD-like endocrine profiles, including significantly lower corticosterone levels at baseline and following dexamethasone administration (Fig. 4). Although investigations of HPA axis function in PTSD patients have produced 1539 mixed results (Bonne et al., 2003; Hawk et al., 2000; Klaassens et al., 2012; McFarlane et al., 1997, 2011; Metzger et al., 2008; Pitman and Orr, 1990; Shalev et al., 2008), numerous studies have reported that people with PTSD exhibit abnormally low baseline cortisol levels (for reviews, see Yehuda, 2005, 2009), an increased number and sensitivity of glucocorticoid receptors (Rohleder et al., 2004; Stein et al., 1997; Yehuda et al., 1991, 1993a, 1995) and increased suppression of cortisol and ACTH following the administration of dexamethasone (Duval et al., 2004; Goenjian et al., 1996; Grossman et al., 2003; Newport et al., 2004; Stein et al., 1997; Yehuda et al., 1993b, 1995, 2002, 2004). However, given the heterogeneity of findings in this area of research, some glucocorticoid abnormalities may be present only in a subtype of the disorder under restricted conditions (e.g., combat trauma in males versus sexual assault in females). It is also possible that such glucocorticoid abnormalities may occur as a result of trauma, per se, rather than being tied specifically to the diagnosis of PTSD (de Kloet et al., 2007), which may explain the difficulty in considering glucocorticoid abnormalities to be ubiquitous biomarkers of all forms of PTSD (Klaassens et al., 2012). Thus, inclusion of other biomarkers, perhaps at the molecular level (Kolassa et al., 2010; Su et al., 2009; Xie et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2008), may identify measures unique to trauma-induced psychopathology. Animal studies involving chronic stress have frequently reported significant elevations of baseline glucocorticoid levels (Blanchard et al., 1993; Kant et al., 1987; Lepsch et al., 2005; Marin et al., 2007; Mizoguchi et al., 2001; Patterson-Buckendahl et al., 2001; Touyarot and Sandi, 2002). Few studies, however, have produced abnormally low baseline glucocorticoid levels similar to those reported here. Those animal models that have reported significantly reduced baseline glucocorticoid levels have employed either the single prolonged stress or a stress—restress paradigm, consisting of situational reminders of the original stress experience (Diehl et al., 2007; Harvey et al., 2003; Harada et al., 2008; Iwamoto et al., 2007; Kohda et al., 2007; Liberzon et al., 1997; Takahashi et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2008). The present experiment, therefore, extends these findings by demonstrating that similar HPA axis abnormalities can be produced in rats by exposure to two acute predator stress episodes, in conjunction with daily social instability, three weeks after the last predator exposure occurred. It is important to note that we observed significantly lower baseline corticosterone levels only in the uninjected psychosocially stressed rats (Fig. 4). This baseline effect is contrasted with the lack of a psychosocial stress effect on ‘‘baseline’’ corticosterone in the rats that were injected with vehicle 6 h before the baseline blood sample was obtained (Fig. 4). These findings illustrate the importance of obtaining blood samples from completely undisturbed rats to obtain a true measure of baseline HPA axis activity. This finding also illustrates the sensitivity of corticosterone levels to relatively mild environmental manipulations, such as an intraperitoneal injection. This finding may also be relevant to inconsistent findings in the PTSD literature as single time point basal HPA and cardiovascular measures may be affected, for example, by how long subjects spend in an experimental (e.g., hospital) environment before a baseline measure is obtained (McFall et al., 1992). 1540 We did not find that corticosterone levels declined to baseline 60 min after the termination of 20 min of acute immobilization stress. The persistent elevation of corticosterone levels 1 h post-stress could have resulted, in part, because of the time of day that we initiated blood sampling. We began blood sampling in the early evening hours, at the time when lights are off and corticosterone levels rise in rats. This timing of blood sampling was designed to increase the likelihood that we could observe group differences in baseline corticosterone levels. The increase in corticosterone levels in the rat’s dark cycle could have interacted with the acute immobilization stress response to prevent a full recovery of corticosterone levels to baseline at the 80 min time point. As expected, dexamethasone-treated animals displayed significantly lower baseline corticosterone levels than vehicle-treated animals. However, there was no effect of psychosocial stress on baseline corticosterone levels following dexamethasone administration. The reason why we did not observe group differences in baseline corticosterone levels is because each of the three doses of dexamethasone produced an almost complete suppression of baseline corticosterone levels in the stress and control groups. Additional study with lower doses of dexamethasone may reveal a subtle influence of psychosocial stress on the dexamethasone-induced suppression of baseline corticosterone levels. In contrast to equivalent baseline corticosterone levels in dexamethasone-treated groups, we found that the 25 mg/kg dose of dexamethasone blocked the immobilization-induced increase in corticosterone levels in the psychosocial stress group. This finding replicates observations by Kohda et al. (2007), who found significantly lower corticosterone levels in rats exposed to the single prolonged stress paradigm following the administration of dexamethasone. Akin to the present study, Kohda et al. (2007) exposed rats to an acute stressor following dexamethasone administration to observe group differences in corticosterone levels. We also found that psychosocially stressed rats exhibited significantly lower post-immobilization (80 min time point) corticosterone levels than the no psychosocial stress group following administration of 10 mg/kg of dexamethasone. This dose of dexamethasone did not selectively blunt the immobilizationinduced increase of corticosterone levels in psychosocially stressed rats, as there were no significant group differences in corticosterone levels at the 20 min time point. However, following termination of the immobilization stress, psychosocially stressed rats displayed a significantly greater decline of corticosterone levels 60 min later. The finding of reduced corticosterone levels in psychosocially stressed animals following dexamethasone administration supports the hypothesis that our PTSD regimen generates enhanced negative feedback of the HPA axis. We may further speculate that dexamethasone prevented the immobilization-induced increase (25 mg/kg) and exacerbated the postimmobilization recovery (10 mg/kg) of corticosterone levels in the psychosocial stress groups because the rats in these groups had a greater number and/or sensitivity of glucocorticoid receptors or reduced sensitivity of CRH receptors in the pituitary. It is also possible that rather than enhanced negative feedback of the HPA axis, reduced adrenal output could explain the reduced corticosterone levels that were observed in the dexamethasone-treated stressed groups. Arguing against this interpretation is the finding that the psychosocial P.R. Zoladz et al. stress groups exhibited normal levels of corticosterone in response to acute stress in the uninjected and saline-treated groups (Fig. 4). Moreover, we found that psychosocial stress produced adrenal hypertrophy (Tables 1 and 2), which would seem inconsistent with adrenal insufficiency as an explanation of reduced corticosterone responses in the dexamethasone-treated psychosocial stress groups. However, previous work has demonstrated that chronic psychosocial stress in rodents can produce adrenal hypertrophy in conjunction with reduced ACTH responsiveness of the adrenal glands (Reber et al., 2007). Therefore, future work will need to be performed to identify the neuroendocrine basis of the psychosocial stress-induced disturbance of HPA axis function we have observed here, including an examination of CRH and glucocorticoid receptor expression, levels of circulating ACTH and adrenal gland responsiveness to ACTH. 4.3. Clinical relevance The importance of the current work is that our psychosocial stress regimen generated, in rats, three cardinal features of PTSD: evidence of a long-lasting traumatic memory, persistent anxiety and hormonal abnormalities. One feature of our work which may appear inconsistent with clinical research is that psychosocial stress produced PTSD-like sequelae in most, if not all, rats, but only a subset (10—25%) of traumatized people develops PTSD (Aupperle et al., 2012; Delahanty and Nugent, 2006; Marmar et al., 2006; Skelton et al., 2012; Yehuda and Flory, 2007). In this context it is important to point out that our animal model of PTSD was explicitly designed to expose rats to conditions which are known to maximize the likelihood of PTSD developing in people. Specifically, DSM-IV criteria for the diagnosis of PTSD includes the following three conditions: (1) PTSD can be triggered by an event that involves threatened death or a threat to one’s physical integrity; (2) a person’s response to the event involves intense fear, helplessness or horror; and (3) in the aftermath of the trauma, the person feels as if the traumatic event were recurring, including a sense of reliving the experience (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). All of these components of the DSM-IV criteria for PTSD have been incorporated into our animal model of PTSD. That is, rats exhibit an intense fear response when exposed to a cat (Adamec et al., 2005, 2011; Blanchard et al., 2005; Hubbard et al., 2004), which is a threat to their survival. The immobilization component of our animal model is a rodent analogue to a loss of control over traumatic conditions, which is known to exacerbate behavioral and physiological responses to stress (Amat et al., 2005; Bland et al., 2006, 2007; Kavushansky et al., 2006; Maier et al., 1993; Maier and Watkins, 2005; Shors et al., 1989). The re-experiencing the rats were exposed to with the second cat exposure was designed to be relevant to the increased likelihood of PTSD developing with multiple traumatic experiences (Koenen et al., 2002; Kolassa et al., 2010; Xie et al., 2009) and the unstable psychosocial component of our paradigm is based on the well-described finding of a link between a lack of social support reported by people who progress from short-term to long-lasting PTSD (Andrews et al., 2003; Boscarino, 1995; Brewin et al., 2000; Solomon et al., 1989; Ullman and Filipas, 2001; Vogt et al., 2011). Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats Taken together, the basis of the high rate of PTSD-like endocrine and behavioral effects we have reported here and in previous studies (Roth et al., 2011; Zoladz et al., 2008; Zoladz and Diamond, 2010) can be ascribed to the explicit design of our psychosocial stress manipulations to mimic clinical features of trauma that are known to increase the likelihood that persistent PTSD will occur in people. This approach, therefore, is a tool with which to identify mechanisms that are activated specifically in the subset of individuals who are exposed to environmental conditions that are most likely to progress from an acute stress state to chronic PTSD. 5. Concluding remarks We have developed an animal model of PTSD that includes trauma induction procedures which are analogous to those that induce PTSD in people, including a threat to survival, a lack of control, an intrusive reminder of a traumatic experience and social instability. Rats administered this psychosocial stress regimen exhibited changes in physiology and behavior in common with people diagnosed with PTSD, including heightened anxiety, exaggerated startle, impaired cognition, increased cardiovascular reactivity and an exaggerated response to yohimbine administration (Roth et al., 2011; Zoladz et al., 2008; Zoladz and Diamond, 2010). In the current work we have extended our model to demonstrate that psychosocially stressed rats exhibited a powerful and long-lasting fear memory for the context and cue which were temporally associated with immobilization and predator exposure. We also observed significant changes in HPA activity, specifically, reductions in basal glucocorticoid levels and enhanced dexamethasone-induced inhibition of glucocorticoid levels, which have been shown to occur in traumatized people. Overall, the numerous commonalities in stress-induced changes in physiology and behavior in traumatized people and in the psychosocially stressed rats studied here further validates the use of our animal model of PTSD to explore cognitive and neuroendocrine mechanisms underlying emotional trauma-induced changes in brain and behavior. Conflicts of interest All other authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. Contributors David Diamond designed the study and contributed to the editing of the manuscript. Phillip Zoladz contributed to the design of the study, conducted all laboratory procedures and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Monika Fleshner contributed to the design of the study, conducted the corticosterone assays and contributed to the editing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript. Role of the funding source Funding for this study was provided by the Department of Veterans Affairs; the VA had no role in study design, in the 1541 collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the paper for publication. Acknowledgements This research was supported by Research Career Scientist and Merit Review Awards from the Department of Veterans Affairs to David Diamond. References Adamec, R., 1997. Transmitter systems involved in neural plasticity underlying increased anxiety and defense-implications for understanding anxiety following traumatic stress. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21, 755—765. Adamec, R., Hebert, M., Blundell, J., 2011. Long lasting effects of predator stress on pCREB expression in brain regions involved in fearful and anxious behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 221, 118— 133. Adamec, R., Walling, S., Burton, P., 2004. Long-lasting, selective, anxiogenic effects of feline predator stress in mice. Physiol. Behav. 83, 401—410. Adamec, R.E., Blundell, J., Burton, P., 2005. Neural circuit changes mediating lasting brain and behavioral response to predator stress. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1225—1241. Amat, J., Baratta, M.V., Paul, E., Bland, S.T., Watkins, L.R., Maier, S.F., 2005. Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 365—371. American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV, 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC. Andrews, B., Brewin, C.R., Rose, S., 2003. Gender, social support, and PTSD in victims of violent crime. J. Trauma Stress 16, 421— 427. Aupperle, R.L., Melrose, A.J., Stein, M.B., Paulus, M.P., 2012. Executive function and PTSD: disengaging from trauma. Neuropharmacology 62, 686—694. Begic, D., Jokic-Begic, N., 2007. Heterogeneity of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in Croatian war veterans: retrospective study. Croat. Med. J. 48, 133—139. Blanchard, D.C., Canteras, N.S., Markham, C.M., Pentkowski, N.S., Blanchard, R.J., 2005. Lesions of structures showing FOS expression to cat presentation: effects on responsivity to a Cat, Cat odor, and nonpredator threat. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1243— 1253. Blanchard, D.C., Griebel, G., Blanchard, R.J., 2003a. Conditioning and residual emotionality effects of predator stimuli: some reflections on stress and emotion. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 1177—1185. Blanchard, D.C., Markham, C., Yang, M., Hubbard, D., Madarang, E., Blanchard, R.J., 2003b. Failure to produce conditioning with lowdose trimethylthiazoline or cat feces as unconditioned stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 360—368. Blanchard, D.C., Sakai, R.R., McEwen, B., Weiss, S.M., Blanchard, R.J., 1993. Subordination stress: behavioral, brain, and neuroendocrine correlates. [Review] [28 refs] Behav. Brain Res. 58, 113— 121. Blanchard, R.J., Blanchard, D.C., Agullana, R., Weiss, S.M., 1991. Twenty-two kHz alarm cries to presentation of a predator, by laboratory rats living in visible burrow systems. Physiol. Behav. 50, 967—972. Blanchard, R.J., Blanchard, D.C., Rodgers, J., Weiss, S.M., 1990. The characterization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior. [Review] [42 refs] Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 14, 463—472. 1542 Blanchard, R.J., Mast, M., Blanchard, D.C., 1975. Stimulus control of defensive reactions in the albino rat. J. Compar. Physiol. Psychol. 88, 81—88. Blanchard, R.J., Yang, M., Li, C.I., Gervacio, A., Blanchard, D.C., 2001. Cue and context conditioning of defensive behaviors to cat odor stimuli. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 587—595. Bland, S.T., Schmid, M.J., Greenwood, B.N., Watkins, L.R., Maier, S.F., 2006. Behavioral control of the stressor modulates stressinduced changes in neurogenesis and fibroblast growth factor-2. NeuroReport 17, 593—597. Bland, S.T., Tamlyn, J.P., Barrientos, R.M., Greenwood, B.N., Watkins, L.R., Campeau, S., Day, H.E., Maier, S.F., 2007. Expression of fibroblast growth factor-2 and brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA in the medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus after uncontrollable or controllable stress. Neuroscience 144, 1219— 1228. Bonne, O., Brandes, D., Segman, R., Pitman, R.K., Yehuda, R., Shalev, A.Y., 2003. Prospective evaluation of plasma cortisol in recent trauma survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 119, 171—175. Boscarino, J.A., 1995. Post-traumatic stress and associated disorders among Vietnam veterans: the significance of combat exposure and social support. J. Trauma Stress 8, 317—336. Brewin, C.R., 2001. A cognitive neuroscience account of posttraumatic stress disorder and its treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 373—393. Brewin, C.R., Andrews, B., Valentine, J.D., 2000. Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. J. Consult Clin. Psychol. 68, 748—766. Bryant, R.A., Harvey, A.G., 1996. Visual imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma Stress 9, 613—619. Bryant, R.A., Salmon, K., Sinclair, E., Davidson, P., 2007. Heart rate as a predictor of posttraumatic stress disorder in children. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 29, 66—68. Cammarota, M., Bevilaqua, L.R., Viola, H., Kerr, D.S., Reichmann, B., Teixeira, V., Bulla, M., Izquierdo, I., Medina, J.H., 2002. Participation of CaMKII in neuronal plasticity and memory formation. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 22, 259—267. Cohen, H., Kaplan, Z., Matar, M.A., Loewenthal, U., Kozlovsky, N., Zohar, J., 2006. Anisomycin, a protein synthesis inhibitor, disrupts traumatic memory consolidation and attenuates posttraumatic stress response in rats. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 767—776. Cohen, H., Matar, M.A., Buskila, D., Kaplan, Z., Zohar, J., 2008. Early post-stressor intervention with high-dose corticosterone attenuates posttraumatic stress response in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 708—717. Cuthbert, B.N., Lang, P.J., Strauss, C., Drobes, D., Patrick, C.J., Bradley, M.M., 2003. The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology 40, 407—422. de Kloet, C.S., Vermetten, E., Geuze, E., Kavelaars, A., Heijnen, C.J., Westenberg, H.G., 2006. Assessment of HPA-axis function in posttraumatic stress disorder: pharmacological and non-pharmacological challenge tests, a review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 550— 567. de Kloet, C.S., Vermetten, E., Heijnen, C.J., Geuze, E., Lentjes, E.G., Westenberg, H.G., 2007. Enhanced cortisol suppression in response to dexamethasone administration in traumatized veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 215—226. Debiec, J., LeDoux, J.E., 2006. Noradrenergic signaling in the amygdala contributes to the reconsolidation of fear memory: treatment implications for PTSD. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 521—524. Delahanty, D.L., Nugent, N.R., 2006. Predicting PTSD prospectively based on prior trauma history and immediate biological responses. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 27—40. Diamond, D.M., Campbell, A.M., Park, C.R., Halonen, J., Zoladz, P.R., 2007. The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing: a synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress- P.R. Zoladz et al. induced amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-Dodson Law. Neural Plast. 2007, 60803. Diamond, D.M., Park, C.R., Heman, K.L., Rose, G.M., 1999. Exposing rats to a predator impairs spatial working memory in the radial arm water maze. Hippocampus 9, 542—552. Diehl, L.A., Silveira, P.P., Leite, M.C., Crema, L.M., Portella, A.K., Billodre, M.N., Nunes, E., Henriques, T.P., Fidelix-da-Silva, L.B., Heis, M.D., Goncalves, C.A., Quillfeldt, J.A., Dalmaz, C., 2007. Long lasting sex-specific effects upon behavior and S100b levels after maternal separation and exposure to a model of posttraumatic stress disorder in rats. Brain Res. 1144, 107—116. Duval, F., Crocq, M.A., Guillon, M.S., Mokrani, M.C., Monreal, J., Bailey, P., Macher, J.P., 2004. Increased adrenocorticotropin suppression after dexamethasone administration in sexually abused adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. N. Y Acad. Sci. 1032, 273—275. Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., Michael, T., 2004. Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory 12, 403—415. Elzinga, B.M., Bremner, J.D., 2002. Are the neural substrates of memory the final common pathway in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)? J. Affect. Disord. 70, 1—17. Fanselow, M.S., Gale, G.D., 2003. The amygdala, fear, and memory. Amygdala Brain Funct.: Basic Clin. Approach. 985, 125—134. Garakani, A., Mathew, S.J., Charney, D.S., 2006. Neurobiology of anxiety disorders and implications for treatment. Mt. Sinai J. Med. 73, 941—949. Geuze, E., Vermetten, E., de Kloet, C.S., Hijman, R., Westenberg, H.G., 2009. Neuropsychological performance is related to current social and occupational functioning in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress. Anxiety. 26, 7—15. Goenjian, A.K., Yehuda, R., Pynoos, R.S., Steinberg, A.M., Tashjian, M., Yang, R.K., Najarian, L.M., Fairbanks, L.A., 1996. Basal cortisol, dexamethasone suppression of cortisol, and MHPG in adolescents after the 1988 earthquake in Armenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 153, 929—934. Graham, B.M., Milad, M.R., 2011. The study of fear extinction: implications for anxiety disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 1255—1265. Grossman, R., Yehuda, R., New, A., Schmeidler, J., Silverman, J., Mitropoulou, V., Sta, M.N., Golier, J., Siever, L., 2003. Dexamethasone suppression test findings in subjects with personality disorders: associations with posttraumatic stress disorder and major depression. Am. J Psychiatry 160, 1291—1298. Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., Speckens, A., Clark, D.M., 2004. Characteristics and content of intrusive memories in PTSD and their changes with treatment. J. Trauma Stress 17, 231—240. Hamner, M.B., Robert, S., Frueh, B.C., 2004. Treatment-resistant posttraumatic stress disorder: strategies for intervention. CNS Spectr. 9, 740—752. Harada, K., Yamaji, T., Matsuoka, N., 2008. Activation of the serotonin 5-HT2C receptor is involved in the enhanced anxiety in rats after single-prolonged stress. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 89, 11—16. Harvey, B.H., Naciti, C., Brand, L., Stein, D.J., 2003. Endocrine, cognitive and hippocampal/cortical 5HT 1A/2A receptor changes evoked by a time-dependent sensitisation (TDS) stress model in rats. Brain Res. 983, 97—107. Hawk, L.W., Dougall, A.L., Ursano, R.J., Baum, A., 2000. Urinary catecholamines and cortisol in recent-onset posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Psychosom. Med. 62, 423—434. Hubbard, D.T., Blanchard, D.C., Yang, M., Markham, C.M., Gervacio, A., Chun, I., Blanchard, R.J., 2004. Development of defensive behavior and conditioning to cat odor in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 80, 525—530. Iwamoto, Y., Morinobu, S., Takahashi, T., Yamawaki, S., 2007. Single prolonged stress increases contextual freezing and the expression Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats of glycine transporter 1 and vesicle-associated membrane protein 2 mRNA in the hippocampus of rats. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 642—651. Izquierdo, I., Cammarota, M., Vianna, M.M., Bevilaqua, L.R., 2004. The inhibition of acquired fear. Neurotox. Res. 6, 175—188. Jacobs, W.J., Nadel, L., 1985. Stress-induced recovery of fears and phobias. Psychol. Rev. 92, 512—531. Johnson, L.R., McGuire, J., Lazarus, R., Palmer, A.A., 2012. Pavlovian fear memory circuits and phenotype models of PTSD. Neuropharmacology 62, 638—646. Jovanovic, T., Ressler, K.J., 2010. How the neurocircuitry and genetics of fear inhibition may inform our understanding of PTSD. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 648—662. Kant, G.J., Leu, J.R., Anderson, S.M., Mougey, E.H., 1987. Effects of chronic stress on plasma corticosterone, ACTH and prolactin. Physiol. Behav. 40, 775—779. Kavushansky, A., Vouimba, R.M., Cohen, H., Richter-Levin, G., 2006. Activity and plasticity in the CA1, the dentate gyrus, and the amygdala following controllable vs. uncontrollable water stress. Hippocampus 16, 35—42. Klaassens, E.R., Giltay, E.J., Cuijpers, P., van Veen, T., Zitman, F.G., 2012. Adulthood trauma and HPA-axis functioning in healthy subjects and PTSD patients: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 317—331. Koenen, K.C., Harley, R., Lyons, M.J., Wolfe, J., Simpson, J.C., Goldberg, J., Eisen, S.A., Tsuang, M., 2002. A twin registry study of familial and individual risk factors for trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 190, 209—218. Kohda, K., Harada, K., Kato, K., Hoshino, A., Motohashi, J., Yamaji, T., Morinobu, S., Matsuoka, N., Kato, N., 2007. Glucocorticoid receptor activation is involved in producing abnormal phenotypes of single-prolonged stress rats: a putative post-traumatic stress disorder model. Neuroscience 148, 22—33. Kolassa, I.T., Ertl, V., Eckart, C., Glockner, F., Kolassa, S., Papassotiropoulos, A., de Quervain, D.J., Elbert, T., 2010. Association study of trauma load and SLC6A4 promoter polymorphism in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence from survivors of the Rwandan genocide. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 543—547. Kolb, L.C., 1984. The post-traumatic stress disorders of combat: a subgroup with a conditioned emotional response. Mil. Med. 149, 237—243. Korte, S.M., De Boer, S.F., 2003. A robust animal model of state anxiety: fear-potentiated behaviour in the elevated plus-maze. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 463, 163—175. Krystal, J.H., Neumeister, A., 2009. Noradrenergic and serotonergic mechanisms in the neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder and resilience. Brain Res. 1293, 13—23. Lepsch, L.B., Gonzalo, L.A., Magro, F.J., DeLucia, R., Scavone, C., Planeta, C.S., 2005. Exposure to chronic stress increases the locomotor response to cocaine and the basal levels of corticosterone in adolescent rats. Addict. Biol. 10, 251—256. Liberzon, I., Krstov, M., Young, E.A., 1997. Stress-restress: effects on ACTH and fast feedback. Psychoneuroendocrinology 22, 443— 453. Liberzon, I., Sripada, C.S., 2008. The functional neuroanatomy of PTSD: a critical review. Prog. Brain Res. 167, 151—169. Lisman, J., Schulman, H., Cline, H., 2002. The molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic and behavioural memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 175—190. Lurie, S., Kuhn, C., Bartolome, J., Schanberg, S., 1989. Differential sensitivity to dexamethasone suppression in an animal model of the DST. Biol. Psychiatry 26, 26—34. Maier, S.F., Grahn, R.E., Kalman, B.A., Sutton, L.C., Wiertelak, E.P., Watkins, L.R., 1993. The role of the amygdala and dorsal raphe nucleus in mediating the behavioral consequences of inescapable shock. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 377—388. Maier, S.F., Watkins, L.R., 2005. Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: the roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and 1543 corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 829— 841. Marin, M.T., Cruz, F.C., Planeta, C.S., 2007. Chronic restraint or variable stresses differently affect the behavior, corticosterone secretion and body weight in rats. Physiol. Behav. 90, 29—35. Marmar, C.R., McCaslin, S.E., Metzler, T.J., Best, S., Weiss, D.S., Fagan, J., Liberman, A., Pole, N., Otte, C., Yehuda, R., Mohr, D., Neylan, T., 2006. Predictors of posttraumatic stress in police and other first responders. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 1—18. Marshall, R.D., Garakani, A., 2002. Psychobiology of the acute stress response and its relationship to the psychobiology of post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 25, 385—395. Matar, M.A., Zohar, J., Kaplan, Z., Cohen, H., 2009. Alprazolam treatment immediately after stress exposure interferes with the normal HPA-stress response and increases vulnerability to subsequent stress in an animal model of PTSD. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 19, 283—295. McFall, M.E., Veith, R.C., Murburg, M.M., 1992. Basal sympathoadrenal function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 1050—1056. McFarlane, A.C., 1992. Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 180, 439—445. McFarlane, A.C., Atchison, M., Yehuda, R., 1997. The acute stress response following motor vehicle accidents and its relation to PTSD. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 821, 437—441. McFarlane, A.C., Barton, C.A., Yehuda, R., Wittert, G., 2011. Cortisol response to acute trauma and risk of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 720—727. Mesches, M.H., Fleshner, M., Heman, K.L., Rose, G.M., Diamond, D.M., 1999. Exposing rats to a predator blocks primed burst potentiation in the hippocampus in vitro. J. Neurosci. 19, RC18. Metcalfe, J., Jacobs, W.J., 1998. Emotional memory: the effects of stress on ‘‘cool’’ and ‘‘hot’’ memory systems. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 38, 187—222. Metzger, L.J., Carson, M.A., Lasko, N.B., Paulus, L.A., Orr, S.P., Pitman, R.K., Yehuda, R., 2008. Basal and suppressed salivary cortisol in female Vietnam nurse veterans with and without PTSD. Psychiatry Res. 161, 330—335. Milad, M.R., Pitman, R.K., Ellis, C.B., Gold, A.L., Shin, L.M., Lasko, N.B., Zeidan, M.A., Handwerger, K., Orr, S.P., Rauch, S.L., 2009. Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 1075—1082. Mizoguchi, K., Yuzurihara, M., Ishige, A., Sasaki, H., Chui, D.H., Tabira, T., 2001. Chronic stress differentially regulates glucocorticoid negative feedback response in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 443—459. Munoz-Abellan, C., Daviu, N., Rabasa, C., Nadal, R., Armario, A., 2009. Cat odor causes long-lasting contextual fear conditioning and increased pituitary-adrenal activation, without modifying anxiety. Horm. Behav. 56, 465—471. Nadel, L., Jacobs, W.J., 1998. Traumatic memory is special. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 7, 154—157. Nemeroff, C.B., Bremner, J.D., Foa, E.B., Mayberg, H.S., North, C.S., Stein, M.B., 2006. Posttraumatic stress disorder: a state-of-thescience review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 1—21. Newport, D.J., Heim, C., Bonsall, R., Miller, A.H., Nemeroff, C.B., 2004. Pituitary-adrenal responses to standard and low-dose dexamethasone suppression tests in adult survivors of child abuse. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 10—20. Newport, D.J., Nemeroff, C.B., 2000. Neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 211—218. Otte, C., Muhtz, C., Daneshkhah, S., Yassouridis, A., Kiefer, F., Wiedemann, K., Kellner, M., 2006. Mineralocorticoid receptor function in posttraumatic stress disorder after pretreatment with metyrapone. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 784—787. Patterson-Buckendahl, P., Rusnak, M., Fukuhara, K., Kvetnansky, R., 2001. Repeated immobilization stress reduces rat vertebral bone 1544 growth and osteocalcin. Am. J Physiol Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 280, R79—R86. Pavlov, I.P., 1928. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press, London. Pentkowski, N.S., Blanchard, D.C., Lever, C., Litvin, Y., Blanchard, R.J., 2006. Effects of lesions to the dorsal and ventral hippocampus on defensive behaviors in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 2185— 2196. Pervanidou, P., Chrousos, G.P., 2010. Neuroendocrinology of posttraumatic stress disorder. Prog. Brain Res. 182, 149—160. Pitman, R.K., Orr, S.P., 1990. Twenty-four hour urinary cortisol and catecholamine excretion in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 245—247. Radant, A., Tsuang, D., Peskind, E.R., McFall, M., Raskind, W., 2001. Biological markers and diagnostic accuracy in the genetics of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 102, 203—215. Reber, S.O., Birkeneder, L., Veenema, A.H., Obermeier, F., Falk, W., Straub, R.H., Neumann, I.D., 2007. Adrenal insufficiency and colonic inflammation after a novel chronic psycho-social stress paradigm in mice: implications and mechanisms. Endocrinology 148, 670—682. Redgrave, K., 2003. Brain function and conditioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. R Soc. Promot. Health 123, 120—123. Reynolds, M., Brewin, C.R., 1998. Intrusive cognitions, coping strategies and emotional responses in depression, post-traumatic stress disorder and a non-clinical population. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 135—147. Reynolds, M., Brewin, C.R., 1999. Intrusive memories in depression and posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 37, 201—215. Rinne, T., de Kloet, E.R., Wouters, L., Goekoop, J.G., DeRijk, R.H., van den, B.W., 2002. Hyperresponsiveness of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis to combined dexamethasone/corticotropinreleasing hormone challenge in female borderline personality disorder subjects with a history of sustained childhood abuse. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 1102—1112. Rohleder, N., Joksimovic, L., Wolf, J.M., Kirschbaum, C., 2004. Hypocortisolism and increased glucocorticoid sensitivity of proInflammatory cytokine production in Bosnian war refugees with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 745—751. Rosen, J.B., Pagani, J.H., Rolla, K.L., Davis, C., 2008. Analysis of behavioral constraints and the neuroanatomy of fear to the predator odor trimethylthiazoline: a model for animal phobias. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1267—1276. Roth, T.L., Zoladz, P.R., Sweatt, J.D., Diamond, D.M., 2011. Epigenetic modification of hippocampal Bdnf DNA in adult rats in an animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 919—926. Rudy, J.W., Huff, N.C., Matus-Amat, P., 2004. Understanding contextual fear conditioning: insights from a two-process model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 675—685. Shalev, A.Y., Videlock, E.J., Peleg, T., Segman, R., Pitman, R.K., Yehuda, R., 2008. Stress hormones and post-traumatic stress disorder in civilian trauma victims: a longitudinal study. Part I: HPA axis responses. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 11, 365— 372. Shors, T.J., Seib, T.B., Levine, S., Thompson, R.F., 1989. Inescapable versus escapable shock modulates long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Science 244, 224—226. Skelton, K., Ressler, K.J., Norrholm, S.D., Jovanovic, T., BradleyDavino, B., 2012. PTSD and gene variants: new pathways and new thinking. Neuropharmacology 62, 628—637. Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., Benbenishty, R., 1989. Locus of control and combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder: the intervening role of battle intensity, threat appraisal and coping. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 28, 131—144. Speckens, A.E., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., Clark, D.M., 2006. Changes in intrusive memories associated with imaginal reliving in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Anxiety. Disord. 20, 328—341. P.R. Zoladz et al. Speckens, A.E., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., Ruths, F.A., Clark, D.M., 2007. Intrusive memories and rumination in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: a phenomenological comparison. Memory 15, 249—257. Stam, R., 2007. PTSD and stress sensitisation: a tale of brain and body Part 1: human studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 530—557. Stein, M.B., Yehuda, R., Koverola, C., Hanna, C., 1997. Enhanced dexamethasone suppression of plasma cortisol in adult women traumatized by childhood sexual abuse. Biol. Psychiatry 42, 680— 686. Strohle, A., Scheel, M., Modell, S., Holsboer, F., 2008. Blunted ACTH response to dexamethasone suppression-CRH stimulation in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 1185—1188. Su, T.P., Zhang, L., Chung, M.Y., Chen, Y.S., Bi, Y.M., Chou, Y.H., Barker, J.L., Barrett, J.E., Maric, D., Li, X.X., Li, H., Webster, M.J., Benedek, D., Carlton, J.R., Ursano, R., 2009. Levels of the potential biomarker p11 in peripheral blood cells distinguish patients with PTSD from those with other major psychiatric disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 1078—1085. Takahashi, L.K., Chan, M.M., Pilar, M.L., 2008. Predator odor fear conditioning: current perspectives and new directions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1218—1227. Takahashi, T., Morinobu, S., Iwamoto, Y., Yamawaki, S., 2006. Effect of paroxetine on enhanced contextual fear induced by single prolonged stress in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 189, 165—173. Touyarot, K., Sandi, C., 2002. Chronic restraint stress induces an isoform-specific regulation on the neural cell adhesion molecule in the hippocampus. Neural Plast. 9, 147—159. Ullman, S.E., Filipas, H.H., 2001. Predictors of PTSD symptom severity and social reactions in sexual assault victims. J. Trauma Stress 14, 369—389. Vanelzakker, M.B., Zoladz, P.R., Thompson, V.M., Park, C.R., Halonen, J.D., Spencer, R.L., Diamond, D.M., 2011. Influence of PreTraining Predator Stress on the Expression of c-fos mRNA in the Hippocampus, Amygdala, and Striatum Following Long-Term Spatial Memory Retrieval. Front Behav. Neurosci. 5, 30. Vidovic, A., Grubisic-Ilic, M., Kozaric-Kovacic, D., Gotovac, K., Rakos, I., Markotic, A., Rabatic, S., Dekaris, D., Sabioncello, A., 2011. Exaggerated platelet reactivity to physiological agonists in war veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 161—172. Vogt, D., Smith, B., Elwy, R., Martin, J., Schultz, M., Drainoni, M.L., Eisen, S., 2011. Predeployment, deployment, and postdeployment risk factors for posttraumatic stress symptomatology in female and male OEF/OIF veterans. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 819—831. Vouimba, R.M., Munoz, C., Diamond, D.M., 2006. Differential effects of predator stress and the antidepressant tianeptine on physiological plasticity in the hippocampus and basolateral amygdala. Stress 9, 29—40. Wang, W., Liu, Y., Zheng, H., Wang, H.N., Jin, X., Chen, Y.C., Zheng, L.N., Luo, X.X., Tan, Q.R., 2008. A modified single-prolonged stress model for post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 441, 237—241. Wild, J., Hackmann, A., Clark, D.M., 2007. When the present visits the past: updating traumatic memories in social phobia. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 386—401. Xie, P., Kranzler, H.R., Poling, J., Stein, M.B., Anton, R.F., Brady, K., Weiss, R.D., Farrer, L., Gelernter, J., 2009. Interactive effect of stressful life events and the serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR genotype on posttraumatic stress disorder diagnosis in 2 independent populations. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 1201—1209. Yehuda, R., 2005. Neuroendocrine aspects of PTSD. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol. 169, 371—403. Yehuda, R., 2009. Status of glucocorticoid alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1179, 56—69. Yehuda, R., Bierer, L.M., 2009. The relevance of epigenetics to PTSD: implications for the DSM-V. J. Trauma Stress 22, 427—434. Psychosocial stress produces PTSD-like sequelae in rats Yehuda, R., Boisoneau, D., Lowy, M.T., Giller Jr., E.L., 1995. Doseresponse changes in plasma cortisol and lymphocyte glucocorticoid receptors following dexamethasone administration in combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 583—593. Yehuda, R., Boisoneau, D., Mason, J.W., Giller, E.L., 1993a. Glucocorticoid receptor number and cortisol excretion in mood, anxiety, and psychotic disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 18—25. Yehuda, R., Flory, J.D., 2007. Differentiating biological correlates of risk, PTSD, and resilience following trauma exposure. J. Trauma Stress 20, 435—447. Yehuda, R., Golier, J.A., Halligan, S.L., Meaney, M., Bierer, L.M., 2004. The ACTH response to dexamethasone in PTSD. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 1397—1403. Yehuda, R., Halligan, S., Grossman, R., Golier, A., Wong, C., 2002. The cortisol and glucocorticoid receptor response to low dose dexamethasone administration in aging combat veterans and holocaust survivors with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 393. Yehuda, R., Levengood, R.A., Schmeidler, J., Wilson, S., Guo, L.S., Gerber, D., 1996. Increased pituitary activation following metyrapone administration in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21, 1—16. Yehuda, R., Lowy, M.T., Southwick, S.M., Shaffer, D., Giller Jr., E.L., 1991. Lymphocyte glucocorticoid receptor number in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 148, 499—504. Yehuda, R., Southwick, S.M., Krystal, J.H., Bremner, D., Charney, D.S., Mason, J.W., 1993b. Enhanced suppression of cortisol following dexamethasone administration in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 150, 83—86. 1545 Zangrossi Jr., H., File, S.E., 1992. Behavioral consequences in animal tests of anxiety and exploration of exposure to cat odor. Brain Res. Bull. 29, 381—388. Zhang, L., Li, H., Su, T.P., Barker, J.L., Maric, D., Fullerton, C.S., Webster, M.J., Hough, C.J., Li, X.X., Ursano, R., 2008. p11 is upregulated in the forebrain of stressed rats by glucocorticoid acting via two specific glucocorticoid response elements in the p11 promoter. Neuroscience 153, 1126—1134. Zohar, J., Matar, M.A., Ifergane, G., Kaplan, Z., Cohen, H., 2008. Brief post-stressor treatment with pregabalin in an animal model for PTSD: Short-term anxiolytic effects without long-term anxiogenic effect. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, 653—666. Zoladz, P.R., Conrad, C.D., Fleshner, M., Diamond, D.M., 2008. Acute episodes of predator exposure in conjunction with chronic social instability as an animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Stress 11, 259—281. Zoladz, P.R., Diamond, D.M., 2010. An animal model of PTSD which integrates inescapable predator exposure and social instability. Cult. Psychol. Neurosci. 15, 6—7. Zoladz, P.R., Park, C.R., Halonen, J.D., Salim, S., Alzoubi, K.H., Srivareerat, M., Fleshner, M., Alkadhi, K.A., Diamond, D.M., 2012. Differential expression of molecular markers of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex and amygdala in response to spatial learning, predator exposure and stress-induced amnesia. Hippocampus 22, 577—589. Zoladz, P.R., Woodson, J.C., Haynes, V.F., Diamond, D.M., 2010. Activation of a remote (1-year old) emotional memory interferes with the retrieval of a newly formed hippocampus-dependent memory in rats. Stress 13, 36—52.